Abstract
During selective attention, subjects voluntarily focus their cognitive resources on a specific stimulus while ignoring others. Top-down filtering of peripheral sensory responses by higher structures of the brain has been proposed as one of the mechanisms responsible for selective attention. A prerequisite to accomplish top-down modulation of the activity of peripheral structures is the presence of corticofugal pathways. The mammalian auditory efferent system is a unique neural network that originates in the auditory cortex and projects to the cochlear receptor through the olivocochlear bundle, and it has been proposed to function as a top-down filter of peripheral auditory responses during attention to cross-modal stimuli. However, to date, there is no conclusive evidence of the involvement of olivocochlear neurons in selective attention paradigms. Here, we trained wild-type and α-9 nicotinic receptor subunit knock-out (KO) mice, which lack cholinergic transmission between medial olivocochlear neurons and outer hair cells, in a two-choice visual discrimination task and studied the behavioral consequences of adding different types of auditory distractors. In addition, we evaluated the effects of contralateral noise on auditory nerve responses as a measure of the individual strength of the olivocochlear reflex. We demonstrate that KO mice have a reduced olivocochlear reflex strength and perform poorly in a visual selective attention paradigm. These results confirm that an intact medial olivocochlear transmission aids in ignoring auditory distraction during selective attention to visual stimuli.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT The auditory efferent system is a neural network that originates in the auditory cortex and projects to the cochlear receptor through the olivocochlear system. It has been proposed to function as a top-down filter of peripheral auditory responses during attention to cross-modal stimuli. However, to date, there is no conclusive evidence of the involvement of olivocochlear neurons in selective attention paradigms. Here, we studied the behavioral consequences of adding different types of auditory distractors in a visual selective attention task in wild-type and α-9 nicotinic receptor knock-out (KO) mice. We demonstrate that KO mice perform poorly in the selective attention paradigm and that an intact medial olivocochlear transmission aids in ignoring auditory distractors during attention.
Keywords: attention, auditory distractors, auditory efferent, nicotinic receptors, olivocochlear, top-down
Introduction
In the brain of a behaving animal, there is a permanent balance between the activity of top-down (voluntary or goal directed) and bottom-up (involuntary or stimulus directed) attention networks. Paradigms studying selective attention are models of predominant top-down attention processing in which subjects voluntarily focus their cognitive resources on a specific stimulus while ignoring others (Fritz et al., 2007). The filter theory of attention (Broadbent, 1958) has been proposed as one of the neural substrates for top-down attention mechanisms. According to this model, attention-related changes in the neural activity of higher stages of the brain can modulate the activity of more initial structures in the nervous system, acting as biological filters of the responses to irrelevant stimuli (Gilbert and Sigman, 2007).
A prerequisite to accomplish top-down modulation of the activity of peripheral structures is the presence of corticofugal pathways. The mammalian auditory efferent system is a unique neural network that originates in the auditory cortex and projects to the cochlear receptor through the olivocochlear (OC) bundle (Robles and Delano, 2008; Terreros and Delano, 2015). The OC system comprises medial (MOC) and lateral (LOC) olivocochlear neurons located in the brainstem (Warr and Guinan, 1979). MOC neurons make synapses with auditory receptor cells, the outer hair cells (OHC) of the cochlea, and LOC neurons make synapses with type I auditory nerve neurons (Guinan, 1996). MOC neurons release acetylcholine and activate nicotinic receptors assembled from α9 and α10 subunits located in OHCs (Elgoyhen et al., 1994, 2001). Importantly, for the CNS, they are the final and mandatory pathway to regulate OHC responses (Murugasu and Russell, 1996; Cooper and Guinan, 2003). Therefore, all corticofugal modulation of OHC responses must be transmitted through MOC synapses (Katz and Elgoyhen, 2014).
The auditory efferent system has been proposed to function as a top-down filter of peripheral auditory responses during attention to cross-modal stimuli (Hernandez-Peón et al., 1956; Oatman, 1971; Delano et al., 2007; Wittekindt et al., 2014; Walsh et al., 2015). For instance, Wittekindt et al. (2014) showed in human subjects that, during periods of visual attention to Gabor patches, there is a reduction in the amplitude of distortion product otoacoustic emissions (DPOAE), indicative of a reduction of OHC-based cochlear amplification. Similarly, reductions in the amplitude of auditory nerve responses to click and tone (C+T) distractors during selective attention to visual stimuli have been obtained in cats and chinchillas (Oatman, 1971; Delano et al. 2007). In the latter work, we found cochlear receptor potential increases (cochlear microphonics, CM) concomitant to reductions of auditory-nerve compound action potentials (CAP) during visual attention. These attentional effects were most likely produced by activation of MOC neurons because the electrical stimulation of MOC fibers produces opposite effects in CAP and CM amplitudes (Elgueda et al., 2011). However, to date, there is no conclusive evidence of the involvement of MOC neurons in selective attention paradigms.
In the present work, we trained wild-type (WT) and α-9 nicotinic receptor subunit (α9-nAChR) knock-out (KO) mice, which lack cholinergic transmission between MOC and OHC (Vetter et al., 1999), in a two-choice visual discrimination task and studied the behavioral consequences of adding auditory distractors. We demonstrate that α9-nAChR KO mice perform poorly in a visual discrimination task, showing that an intact MOC efferent–OHC synaptic transmission aids in efficiently reducing cochlear sensitivity during selective attention to visual stimuli.
Materials and Methods
Animals.
Visual discrimination data were obtained from 17 α9-nAChR KO and 15 WT adult male mice, aged between 60 and 80 d and weighing between 22 and 30 g at the start of the behavioral training. α9-nAChR KO mice on a 129S6/SvEv backcrossed to CBA/CaJ background (Vetter et al., 1999) and WT littermates were generously provided by Dr. Douglas Vetter from the University of Mississippi. Genotypes were confirmed before and after the behavioral training by PCR screening of genomic DNA extracted and purified from the tail. All mice were housed in groups of two in polycarbonate cages (27.5 cm long, 16.5 cm wide, and 13.0 cm high) for the complete duration of the experiment with a 12 h light/dark cycle (lights on at 0800 h) in a temperature-controlled room (22 ± 2°C). During all of the experimental period, mice were given water ad libitum and were food deprived, maintaining 84–92% of their free-feeding weight. Mice were manipulated and weighed every day by the same investigator. All procedures were approved by the local committee of Bioethics (Comité de Bioética Animal permit number #0548 Facultad de Medicina, Universidad de Chile) and were performed in accordance with the Guidelines for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (publication number 86–23, National Institutes of Health, revised 1996).
Training procedures.
Two experimenters blinded to the genotype of the animals performed the behavioral procedures. Mice were trained in a two-choice visual discrimination task similar to those that we used previously to assess visual attention in rats (Hamame et al., 2006, 2008) and chinchillas (Delano et al., 2007). The experimental protocol was performed in an operant mesh cage (17 cm long, 20 cm wide, and 28 cm high) located inside a double-walled sound-attenuating room 1 m above the room floor and 1.4 m from the ceiling. The front panel of the cage had three different lights: a central light (neutral cue) above a magazine food dispenser and two lateral lights (targets) under which there were two levers located 1 cm above the cage floor (Fig. 1A). The operant cage, stimulus presentation, and data acquisition were controlled by a custom-made computer program written in the C programming language (developed in LabWindows/CVI 9.0 environment; National Instruments) and a digital interface.
Each trial began with the onset of a neutral cue (central light or warning period) with a duration of 2 s, after which the right or left target light was turned on randomly for a period of 0.5 s (Fig. 1B). Mice were trained to respond by pressing the corresponding lever. A response in the lever located below the target light made during the response period (within 5 s from the onset of the target light) was defined as a correct response and was rewarded with a 15 mg precision pellet (Bioserv). Incorrect responses were defined as responses made during the response period in the lever opposite to the target light. The intertrial interval (ITI) period varied randomly between 5 and 11 s. Central light, incorrect, or ITI responses were punished with a 12 s timeout period during which all lights were turned off. Trials in which mice did not respond to the cue or target lights were defined as omission trials and were not punished. The behavioral variables measured were as follows: latency of correct lever-pressing responses (time between the onset of the target light and the lever-pressing response), accuracy ([correct responses/(correct responses + incorrect responses)] × 100), number of responses during the warning and ITI periods, and number of omitted trials. To initiate the experimental protocol phase, all mice were trained until they reached a behavioral criterion of at least 70% accuracy for 3 consecutive days. The mean age of WT and KO mice at the beginning of the experimental protocol was 144.7 ± 8.0 (mean ± SD) and 145.4 ± 10.3 d, respectively (WT: 4.82 ± 0.26 months; KO: 4.85 ± 0.34 months).
Experimental protocol.
After mice reached the behavioral criterion, they were recruited in the experimental protocol. Behavioral performance was evaluated for 12 d divided into 4 periods of 3 d (Fig. 1C). Mice performed 110 trials each day of the protocol. For the analysis of possible effects along sessions, trials were divided into five blocks of 22 trials. In the first 3 d (predistractor period, PRE), mice performed the 2-choice visual discrimination task without auditory distractors. During days 4–9, mice did the visual discrimination task in the presence of two different auditory distractors: (1) days 4–6: clicks and 15 kHz tones presented at ∼65 dB SPL (C+T) and (2) days 7–9: broadband noise presented at ∼90 dB SPL (BBN). Finally, in the last 3 d of the protocol (days 10–12, postdistractor period, POST), mice were again evaluated in the visual discrimination task without auditory distractors (Fig. 1B,C).
Auditory distractors.
Two different auditory distractors were used in the behavioral protocol: (1) clicks (100 μs wide) and 15 kHz tones and (2) BBN (5–40 kHz). All acoustic stimuli were digitally generated at 100 kHz with a National Instruments Board (PC-MIO-16E4). Tones and BBN had a 5 ms ramp (rise/fall) and a total duration of 150 ms. To diminish habituation to auditory distractors, auditory stimuli were delivered at irregular interstimulus intervals that were centered at 400 ms (2.5 Hz rate) and pseudorandomly ranged between 667 and 286 ms (1.5–3.5 Hz rate, uniformly distributed intervals; Fig. 1B). Clicks, tones, and BBN were delivered through a tweeter (Realistic super tweeter; Radioshack; frequency response 5–40 kHz) located 120 cm above the floor of the operant cage. Sound pressure calibrations inside the operant cage were performed with a 1/2 inch Bruel and Kjaer microphone that revealed <10 dB of variation in different positions of the behavioral cage.
The C+T compound stimuli consisted of a click and a 15 kHz tone (presented 40 ms after the click) that were always presented together. These stimuli were chosen to assess a selective attention paradigm similar to that used by Delano et al. (2007) in chinchillas. The 15 kHz tone was selected because there is evidence of a higher density of olivocochlear innervation at this position of the cochlea in mice (Maison et al., 2003). To increase auditory distractibility progressively along the behavioral protocol, the order of presentation of the auditory distractors was designed to be from moderate to high sound pressure levels. Therefore, C+T stimuli at ∼65 dB SPL were presented during days 4–6 and the BBN at ∼90 dB SPL was presented during days 7–9.
Open-field measurements.
To measure the locomotor activity of mice before the experimental protocol, a group of 10 WT and nine α9-KO mice were evaluated in an open-field apparatus. The apparatus was an acrylic box of 40 cm length × 40 cm width × 22 cm height with a luminous intensity of 40 lux (Prut and Belzung, 2003). Each mouse was placed in the center of the open-field apparatus and was allowed to explore for 5 min. The spontaneous locomotor activity was monitored with a digital video camera. Data were stored and analyzed offline by a researcher blinded to the genotype of mice using the software ANY-maze v4.70 (Stoelting). The analyzed variables were as follows: total distance, number of center entries, and time spent at the center.
Electrophysiological procedures.
All electrophysiological procedures were conducted by one experimenter blinded to the genotype of the animals and were performed in a soundproof chamber after the behavioral protocol was completed. The electrophysiological recordings were performed in 16 KO mice and in 13 WT mice because two WT mice and one KO mouse died before assessing the electrophysiological evaluation. To perform recordings, all mice were anesthetized with xylazine (10 mg/kg, IP) and ketamine (100 mg/kg, IP) and body temperature was maintained at 37–38°C by means of a heating pad.
Two different measures were obtained from auditory brainstem responses (ABR) to ipsilateral 15 kHz tone bursts: (1) wave V ABR thresholds (using different sound pressure levels, from 0–90 dB SPL) and (2) contralateral BBN suppression of wave I obtained with 15 kHz ABR at 80 dB SPL. Wave V was chosen to obtain auditory thresholds because it is the most constant response to auditory stimuli; wave I was chosen in the contralateral noise evaluation as a measure of the suppressive strength of the olivocochlear reflex in auditory nerve responses (Lichtenhan et al., 2016).
The auditory stimuli used in the estimation of ABR thresholds and in contralateral suppression of wave I were as follows: (1) ipsilateral (right) 15 kHz tone bursts of alternating polarity (5 ms duration, 0.5 ms rise/fall time) delivered at different sound pressure levels and (2) contralateral (left) BBN (5–40 kHz), 200 ms, delivered at 60 dB SPL. Tone bursts and broad band noise were presented at 4 Hz rate and were digitally generated by two synchronized National Instruments PCI boards (6071-E) at 100,000 samples/s, attenuated by PA-5 programmable attenuators (Tucker Davis Technologies system III) and were delivered with two tweeters (one for ipsilateral and the second for contralateral auditory stimuli; Realistic super tweeter; Radioshack; frequency response 5–40 kHz) through tubes sealed to the external auditory meatus.
ABR signals were acquired with a technique similar to that described by Melki et al. (2014). Two subdermal needle electrodes were inserted percutaneously to the left and right ear canal in direction to the round window and a third electrode was located in the midline of the animal cranium. The electrophysiological signals were amplified 10,000–100,000× and filtered between 0.3 and 3 kHz using a BMA-200 differential preamplifier (CWE) and a second stage filter (Krohn-Hite, model 3323). Signals were acquired and digitized at 40,000 samples/s with a National Instruments board (6071-E). Evoked potentials (ABR waves I and V) amplitudes were obtained by averaging 512 trials with and without contralateral noise using a custom-made C program (LabWindows). ABR thresholds were defined as the lowest tone intensity (dB SPL) that evokes an averaged response evaluated by visual inspection of ABR waveforms by an expert observer. ABR amplitudes were calculated using a peak to peak method. Amplitude suppression of ABR wave I produced by contralateral noise in WT and KO mice were calculated in dB referenced to wave I amplitude without contralateral noise: (dBamplitude suppression = 20 * Log10[wave I amplitude with noise/wave I amplitude without noise]).
Statistical analyses.
Because mice were evaluated during different months, before data pooling, the effect of different time assessment (months) was evaluated using Kruskal–Wallis one-way ANOVA. The effects of time assessment of experiments was not significant for any measured variable. Shapiro–Wilk tests were performed to evaluate the distribution of data. In the cases of normal distributed data, two-way repeated-measures ANOVA (RM-ANOVA) was used to analyze differences between days [genotypes: (WT, KO) × days (1–12)] periods [genotypes: (WT, KO) × period (PRE, C+T, BBN, POST)] or blocks along sessions [genotypes (WT, KO) × blocks (1, 2, 3, 4, 5)]. Non-normal distributed data were transformed to [Log (X +1)] to satisfy requirements of the ANOVA model. All statistical analyses were followed by Bonferroni post hoc comparison tests.
In addition, to measure the day-to-day variability of accuracy, correct, and omitted trials along the experimental protocol, we calculated the individual differences of these variables produced in consecutive days in WT and KO mice (e.g., accuracy in day 2 minus day 1; day 3 minus day 2; up to day 12 minus day 11). These analyses yielded 11 differential values per genotype per variable that were evaluated with a bootstrapped routine with 1000 iterations using the R programming language (R version 3.2.5; R Core Team, 2014). This procedure allowed us to obtain the median, interquartile range, and 95% confidence interval (CI) of these variables.
Fisher's exact tests were used to compare the distributions of KO and WT mice with the two arbitrary behavioral criteria. Spearman correlation tests were used to evaluate possible correlations between variables. Multivariate logistic regressions were calculated for modeling data, using forward and backward methods to isolate significant variables. ABR wave V thresholds and the magnitude of contralateral noise suppression of wave I between WT and KO mice were evaluated by unpaired t tests. A p-value <0.05 was considered a significant difference in all statistical tests.
Results
A summary of the behavioral performance evaluated for the 12 d of the experimental protocol, including 17 KO and 15 WT mice, is shown in Figure 2. No accuracy differences between KO and WT mice were obtained in the first 3 d of protocol (visual discrimination without auditory distractors). After that, a significant progressive increase along days in accuracy (two-way RM-ANOVA, F(1,11) = 10.846; p < 0.001) and decrease in the latency of correct responses along days were found in both genotypes (Fig. 2A,B). The post hoc analysis of the daily performance along the 12 d protocol showed a trend toward a differential effect in accuracy between genotypes in day 4; that is, in the first day of presentation of the C+T distractors (Bonferroni post hoc test, t = 1.89, p = 0.06). Significant differences in the number of correct responses between KO and WT mice were obtained comparing daily performance (two-way RM-ANOVA, F(1,11) = 3.066; p < 0.001). The Bonferroni post hoc test showed that these differences were statistically significant in days 4 and 6 of the protocol during the presentation of C+T distractors for correct responses (day 4 WT = 68.73 ± 12.71 mean ± SD; KO = 54.64 ± 17.18; day 6 WT = 75.27 ± 11.39; KO = 61.05 ± 17.15; Fig. 2C), whereas a tendency was obtained in day 5 (WT = 74.67 ± 11.32; KO = 63.41 ± 17.34; p = 0.055). No significant differences were obtained for omitted trials during the 12 d analysis (two-way RM ANOVA, F(1,11) = 1.650; p = 0.083). Regarding other behavioral variables, there were no significant differences between the number of incorrect, warning and ITI responses, and mice weight between WT and KO mice along the protocol (data not shown).
Next, we analyzed the behavioral performances comparing the four periods of the experimental protocol, including baseline (PRE), C+T, BBN, and POST periods (Fig. 3). Two-way repeated-measures ANOVAs followed by Bonferroni post hoc tests showed that, during the period of presentation of the C+T distractors, there was a significant decrease in the number of correct responses in KO mice (F(1,3) = 14.950, p < 0.001; WT = 72.89 ± 11.04; KO = 59.55 ± 15.71; Fig. 3B) and a significant increase in the number of omitted trials in KO mice (F(1,3) = 6.70, p < 0.001; WT = 18.62 ± 11.58; KO = 33.98 ± 14.04; Fig. 3D). Conversely, whereas there were no significant differences in the number of correct responses and omitted trials during BBN and POST periods between KO and WT mice, there was a difference tendency between genotypes in the number of omission trials during the PRE period (Bonferroni post hoc test, t = 2.015, p = 0.05). In addition, there were no significant differences between KO and WT in other behavioral variables, including accuracy and warning, incorrect, and ITI responses (Fig. 3A,C,E,F). Together, the day-by-day- (Fig. 2) and the period (Fig. 3)-based analyses showed that the number of omitted trials and correct responses during the C+T distractor period were the only behavioral variables that were significantly different between KO and WT mice. However, we also found strong tendencies for behavioral differences between KO and WT mice in the number of omission trials during baseline (PRE) and in accuracy during experimental day 4 (C+T).
To study whether the behavioral differences in accuracy, correct, and omitted responses between WT and KO mice were produced by the introduction of the auditory distractors, we used a measure of the individual daily change in accuracy, correct, and omitted responses produced during 2 consecutive days of the experimental protocol. These behavioral differences allowed us to study whether the baseline day-to-day variability (e.g., day 2 minus day 1, day 3 minus day 2) was different from that with auditory distractors (e.g., day 4 minus day 3, day 5 minus day 4). Figure 4 shows that the magnitudes of the interquartile ranges (IQRs) for accuracy, omission, and correct day-to-day differences increase during the C+T period in KO mice (differences between day 4 and 3 and day 5 and 4). The bootstrap analysis showed that the 95% CI of the upper values of the IQR of the accuracy difference between WT and KO mice between days 5 and 4 did not overlap: WT: median 3.0 (95% CI: 2.1–6.1); KO: median 12.9 (95% CI: 7.5–19.5). Conversely, IQR differences in omissions and correct responses were not statistically different. These results show that the introduction of the C+T distractor generates a behavioral change in the visual discrimination accuracy of α9-nAChR-KO mice.
Subsequently, we developed two behavioral criteria based on the variables that showed significant differences or strong tendencies between genotypes in the previous analyses. These criteria allowed us to separate individuals from WT and KO genotypes. The first criterion was based on the daily number of correct and omitted trials: <55 correct responses or >36 omitted trials in daily sessions. Nine of 17 KO and two of 15 WT mice complied with this criterion during the C+T period (Fig. 5). This differential distribution between KO and WT mice was only significant during the C+T period (Fisher's exact test, p = 0.028) because there were no significant differences in the other 3 periods (PRE, Fisher's exact test, p = 0.308; BBN, Fisher's exact test, p = 0.265; and POST, Fisher's exact test, p = 0.147, Fig. 5A,C,D). To identify the subset of WT and KO mice with poor behavior in the PRE period, in Figure 5, these animals are plotted with red border symbols. Figure 5 shows that those animals classified as bad performers in the PRE period are not necessarily the same individuals with altered behavior in the C+T period.
The second criterion was based on accuracy in day 4 (<77%) and was developed to detect changes in the behavioral performance between the baseline period and the first day of presentation of the C+T distractors. Figure 6 shows individual accuracy plotted against omitted and correct trials in the first 4 d of the behavioral protocol. Bad KO performers according to the first criterion are identified with yellow symbols. According to this second criterion, there was a significant number of KO mice affected by the C+T distractor (eight KO mice and only one WT mouse, identified by the symbols below the dotted lines during day 4 in Fig. 6, Fisher's exact test, p = 0.018). Moreover, as evident from the figure, bad KO performers according to this second criterion are not necessarily the same individuals as those identified by the first criterion. Taking both behavioral criteria together, we were able to identify 13 of 17 KO mice. Only four KO mice were indistinguishable from WT mice (four green symbols with a black cross in Fig. 6).
Next, we built a multivariate logistic regression model using genotype as the dependent variable (WT = 0 and KO = 1) and the number of correct and omitted trials during the C+T period and accuracy during day 4 as the 3 independent variables. Forward and backward stepwise methods show that the number of correct responses could be removed from the model because they covary with the number of omissions. The independent variables included in the final model were accuracy during day 4 and the number of omissions during the C+T period. This analysis yielded a significant behavioral model to predict genotype (logit p = 7.175 − (0.117 * accuracy day 4) + (0.107 * omissions), likelihood ratio test, p < 0.001; accuracy day 4, 95% CI: 0.89–1.02, p = 0.083; omissions, 95% CI: 1.03–1.11, p = 0.01). Using this multivariate logistic regression model to predict the genotype of mice during the C+T period, we were able to identify 14 of 17 KO mice. These 14 KO mice included the 13 mice identified by the two previous behavioral criteria.
Although the main differences found between WT and KO mice were obtained during the C+T distractor period, there was a tendency in KO mice to have more omitted trials during the baseline period. For that reason, we evaluated the locomotor activity and anxiety-like behavior in another group of nine α9-KO and 10 WT mice using the open-field test before the experimental protocol (Prut and Belzung, 2003). There were no significant differences in the total distance traveled, the number of center entries, or the time spent in the center between WT and KO mice (Fig. 7). These results indicate that the deletion of α9-nAChR does not affect the basal locomotor activity and suggest no anxiety differences between genotypes. Therefore, any possible difference tendency in the baseline number of omitted responses between WT and KO mice is not due to differences in locomotor activity at the start of the experimental protocol.
To evaluate whether the individual variability of the olivocochlear reflex strength in WT and KO mice could explain the behavioral results, we measured amplitude suppression of ABR-wave I by contralateral BBN stimulation. This brainstem reflex is a widely used method to evaluate olivocochlear activity on auditory nerve responses (ABR wave I; Lichtenhan et al., 2016). In addition, we evaluated auditory thresholds by measuring ABR wave V to 15 kHz tone bursts. Figure 8, A and B, shows that, whereas there were no differences in ABR thresholds to 15 kHz tones between WT (32.1 ± 1.9 dB SPL) and KO (31.8 ± 1.3 dB SPL) mice, a significant difference (unpaired t test, t(27) = 13.98, p < 0.001) was obtained in the magnitude of contralateral noise suppression of wave I (WT: 2.99 ± 0.40 dB amplitude suppression, KO: 0.83 ± 0.43 dB amplitude suppression). We also compared wave I suppression for 2 subsets of KO mice using the behavioral criterion 1 (<55 correct responses and >36 omitted trials during the C+T period) and found no differences between altered (n = 9) and compensated (n = 8) KO mice (Fig. 8C). Finally, we correlated single-day performance of WT and KO mice considering the number of correct responses and omitted trials in the C+T period and accuracy in day 4 of the behavioral protocol with the individual magnitude of contralateral noise suppression of wave I (Fig. 8D,E,F). The effects of contralateral noise suppression of wave I in WT and KO mice were analyzed together (n = 29) and significant positive and negative correlations between the magnitude of contralateral noise suppression of wave I and the number of correct responses (Spearman r(29) = 0.407, p = 0.0286) and omitted trials (Spearman r(29)= −0.556, p = 0.00186) during the period of C+T distractors were found. However, there were no significant correlations analyzing each genotype alone. In addition, no significant correlations were found for accuracy in day 4 and wave I suppression (Fig. 8F).
To test whether correct responses and omitted trials changed along daily sessions, we divided the behavioral performance into five blocks of 22 trials for each day (each daily session is comprised of 110 trials; Fig. 9). This analysis allowed us to study a possible differential decrement of sustained attention to visual stimuli between WT and KO mice. A two-way RM- ANOVA was performed for days 4–6 because these days showed significant differences in the previous analyses. Significant effects of genotype and blocks were obtained for the number of correct responses along sessions for day 4 (F(1,4) = 2.46, p = 0.048) and a Bonferroni post hoc test showed significant differences in correct responses between genotypes for the 2 first blocks (trials 1–44) of day 4 (Fig. 9A). Omitted trials had significant effects for genotype and blocks in day 4 (F(1,4) = 4.066; p < 0.05) and for genotype in day 6 (F(1,150) = 7208, p < 0.05). Bonferroni post hoc analyses showed differences between genotype for the first 66 trials in day 4 and for the last 44 trials in day 6 (Fig. 9B). Figure 9, C and D, shows the distribution of individual mice performance sorted by correct responses and omissions in the first 22 trials of day 4 (Fig. 8C) and in the last 22 trials of day 6 (Fig. 9D). Considering a behavioral criterion of 50% of the correct responses (≤11 correct responses, for 22 trials), we found that KO and WT mice have different distributions in the first 22 trials of day 4 (Fisher's exact test, p = 0.018) and in the last trials of day 6 (Fisher's exact test, p = 0.028). In this figure, bad KO performers in the first 22 trials of day 4 are identified with red border symbols, showing that KO mice that were affected in day 4 are not necessarily the same affected in day 6. These results suggest the presence of two different effects with C+T distractors in KO mice: (1) a day 4 effect, which seems to be related to first moment of auditory distraction, and (2) a day 6 effect, which could be related to sustained attention deficits in the α9-KO mice.
To determine whether there are genotype differences in sustained attention to visual stimuli with auditory distractors, we analyzed the slope of the regression line fitted to the omissions in the first and last 22 trials of each day during the four periods (PRE, C+T, BBN, POST) of the experimental protocol (Fig. 9B). A two-way RM ANOVA showed no statistically significant differences in the slope of omissions between KO and WT mice (F(1,3) = 1.469, p = 0.235; WTomission slope = 0.468 ± 0.231 (mean ± SEM); KOomission slope = 0.853 ± 0.217), whereas significant differences were obtained for the slopes in the different periods of the experimental protocol (F(1,3) = 5.200, p = 0.002; PREomission slope = 0.421; T+Comission slope = 0.484; BBNomission slope = 0.835; POSTomission slope = 0.901). A Bonferroni post hoc test showed significant differences between the POST and the C+T and PRE periods. These analyses show no genotype differences in sustained attention to visual stimuli with auditory distractors, but a general effect along days showing that omissions slopes between the first and last blocks of the experimental protocol became steeper in the POST period in both genotypes.
Discussion
In the present study, we show that α9-nAChR KO mice, which lack cholinergic transmission between MOC and OHC, have worse behavioral performance than WT mice during selective attention to visual stimuli using auditory distractors. These results demonstrate that an intact medial olivocochlear transmission aids in reducing cochlear sensitivity during selective attention to visual stimuli.
Behavioral differences between KO and WT mice during tone and click distractors
KO mice made significantly fewer correct responses and more omissions during the presentation of the C+T distractors at 65 dB SPL. Moreover, KO mice had a significant increased variability range in the day-to-day difference in accuracy during the C+T distractor period. These results can be related to our previous work in chinchillas, in which we used a similar behavioral paradigm to assess selective attention to visual stimuli with C+T distractors (Delano et al., 2007). In that study, we found CAP (auditory nerve) amplitude reductions concomitant to CM increases during correct trials, whereas no differences were obtained during omitted trials. These results from chinchilla experiments suggested that MOC would be activated during correct trials, whereas there might be less or no MOC activation during omissions. This was confirmed in the present work because α9-nAChR KO mice, which lack MOC activity (Vetter et al., 1999), indeed have more omitted trials and fewer correct responses with C+T distractors.
In addition to the significant effects obtained during the C+T period, there was a tendency toward more omissions in the baseline period in KO mice that was not due to locomotor differences between genotypes (Fig. 7). A speculative explanation to this behavioral tendency is that MOC activity could be also needed to suppress sounds generated by mice during visual discrimination.
Physiological mechanisms of the behavioral differences observed in the KO mice
Alpha-9 nicotinic receptor subunits assemble with α10 subunits to form the receptor that mediates synaptic transmission between MOC fibers and OHC (Elgoyhen et al., 2001; Vetter et al., 2007). They are also present in type II vestibular hair cells as targets of vestibular efferents (Kong et al., 2006). Other sites of α9 expression include the skin, nasal epithelium, the pars tuberalis of the pituitary gland, human and rat urothelium, human and rat placenta, rat heart, dorsal root ganglia, and a variety of immune cells (for review, see McIntosh et al., 2009). However, as reported by Elgoyhen et al. (1994) and confirmed by searching the Allen Brain Atlas (2012), α9 is not expressed in the brain. Given these expression pattern of the α9 nicotinic subunit, the most likely conclusion is that peripheral responses to auditory distractors during visual attention are filtered through the activation of corticofugal pathways (Leon et al., 2012; Dragicevic et al., 2015), which in turn recruit MOC neurons that suppress peripheral responses.
Lack of behavioral differences between KO and WT mice during BBN distractors
Although KO mice have altered behavior compared with WT mice during selective attention to visual stimuli in the presence of C+T distractors, no differences were obtained when using BBN as a distractor. Because, in our experimental protocol, BBN was presented in the days after the C+T period, the lack of behavioral differences in WT and KO mice could be explained by the fact that adaptive training along days can diminish distractibility (Mishra et al., 2014). Therefore, the behavioral differences observed between WT and KO mice during the C+T period could have been compensated for by central mechanisms produced by the behavioral training along days of the experimental protocol. Another possibility to explain the lack of behavioral differences between genotypes during the BBN period could be the activation of the middle ear reflex (MEM; Borg and Counter, 1989). Evidence obtained in mice shows that the MEM activation threshold with contralateral BBN is ∼80 dB SPL (Valero et al., 2016). Because, in the present study, the sound levels of BBN distractors were ∼90 dB SPL, it is probable that MEMs were active and suppressing BBN distractors during attention to visual stimuli in WT and KO mice. However, because the sound pressure of C+T distractors were ∼65 dB SPL, it is unlikely that MEM activation contributes to suppression of these acoustic stimuli.
Two populations of KO mice: altered and compensated behavior
Although we found clear behavioral differences between the groups of WT and KO mice, single-day analyses of the individuals and the multivariate logistic regression analysis revealed the presence of two different populations of KO mice during selective attention to visual stimuli with auditory distractors: altered and compensated KO. A similar variability was observed by Lauer and May (2011) when evaluating a similar group of α9-nAChR KO mice in a behavioral task designed to measure auditory temporal acuity (gap-induced inhibition of the acoustic startle reflex). These investigators found two groups of KO mice, one with altered temporal auditory responses and a second with compensated responses similar to that of their WT mice. Therefore, both behavioral studies show that, at least in a subset of α9-nAChR KO mice, compensatory mechanisms are sufficient to reverse the behavioral deficits.
A possible explanation for the behavioral compensation observed in a fraction of KO mice may derive from attention mechanisms needed to suppress irrelevant stimuli involving corticocortical or corticostriatal networks (Aboitiz et al., 2014) and not from lower brainstem pathways. The fact that we did not find any KO mice that compensated for the OC reflex strength deficits (Fig. 8) and the lack of compensation using electrical stimulation of MOC fibers in α9-nAChR KO mice (Vetter et al., 1999) and in frequency selectivity of brainstem neurons (Clause et al., 2014) support the conclusion that brainstem functions cannot be compensated in α9-nAChR KO mice, whereas cognitive functions such as filtering irrelevant stimuli during attention can be compensated for by other brain circuits (May et al., 2002).
Contralateral noise suppression of wave I in WT and KO mice
In agreement with behavioral auditory thresholds obtained in α9-nAChR KO and WT mice (Prosen et al., 2000) and with previous ABR measurements at 8 and 16 kHz (Lauer and May, 2011), in the present study, ABR thresholds at 15 kHz tones were similar in WT and KO mice. Therefore, behavioral differences in the auditory distractors across genotypes cannot be attributed to differences in auditory sensitivity.
In our experiments performed in anesthetized animals, we found that WT mice have larger contralateral noise suppressions of ABR wave-I compared with KO mice (Fig. 8). Although this result is in agreement with the effects obtained with contralateral suppression of DPOAEs in awake WT and α9-nAChR KO mice (Chambers et al., 2012), our effects are larger than those reported in anesthetized mice with DPOAEs (Chambers et al., 2012). Because it is known that anesthesia reduces the effects of contralateral noise on cochlear and auditory nerve responses (Chambers et al., 2012; Aedo et al., 2015), differences between the effects obtained in the anesthetized condition in Chambers et al. (2012) and in our work could be attributed to the fact that olivocochlear effects are larger when evaluated with neural responses compared with DPOAEs (Puria et al., 1996; Lichtenhan et al., 2016). Therefore, the measurement of ABR-wave I suppression with contralateral noise in anesthetized mice can be proposed as an easier technique for testing the olivocochlear reflex in anesthetized animals than measuring DPOAEs in awake conditions (Chambers et al., 2012).
There was a negative correlation between the strength of the olivocochlear reflex and the number of omitted responses during the period of selective attention to visual stimuli with C+T distractors (Fig. 8E). This could suggest that, when MOC activity is strong during the presentation of auditory distractors, these auditory stimuli can be efficiently ignored and thus mice have fewer omitted trials. Another possibility to explain this correlation is that normal MOC activity could help to reduce the overall effort needed to suppress auditory distractors (Shinn-Cunningham and Best, 2008). However, the latter mechanism seems less probable because there were no significant differences between genotypes in the slopes of the omissions along blocks, showing that the effort was probably similar between genotypes. Finally, the lack of difference between the strength of the OC reflex in altered and compensated KO (Fig. 8C) suggest that the behavioral compensation observed in a subset of KO mice is not due to the release of other MOC neurotransmitters, but rather to other mechanisms.
Conclusions
We demonstrate that α9-nAChR KO mice have a diminished strength of the olivocochlear reflex and perform poorly in a visual selective attention paradigm with auditory distractors. These results show that an intact medial olivocochlear transmission aids in ignoring auditory stimuli during selective attention to visual stimuli.
Footnotes
This work was supported by Fondecyt (Grant 3130635 to G.T.) and Fundación Guillermo Puelma. We thank Luis Robles for valuable comments, Felipe Moreno-Gómez for the bootstrap analysis of data, Fernando Vergara for technical assistance, and Dr. Douglas Vetter for providing WT and α9-nAChR KO mice.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
References
- Aboitiz F, Ossandón T, Zamorano F, Palma B, Carrasco X. Irrelevant stimulus processing in ADHD: catecholamine dynamics and attentional networks. Front Psychol. 2014;5:183. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00183. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aedo C, Tapia E, Pavez E, Elgueda D, Delano PH, Robles L. Stronger efferent suppression of cochlear neural potentials by contralateral acoustic stimulation in awake than in anesthetized chinchilla. Front Syst Neurosci. 2015;9:21. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2015.00021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Allen Institute for Brain Science. Allen developing mouse brain atlas. 2012. Available from: http://www.brain-map.org.
- Borg E, Counter SA. The middle-ear muscles. Sci Am. 1989;261:74–80. doi: 10.1038/scientificamerican0889-74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Broadbent D. Perception and communication. London: Pergamon; 1958. [Google Scholar]
- Chambers AR, Hancock KE, Maison SF, Liberman MC, Polley DB. Sound-evoked olivocochlear activation in unanesthetized mice. J Assoc Res Otolaryngol. 2012;13:209–217. doi: 10.1007/s10162-011-0306-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clause A, Kim G, Sonntag M, Weisz CJ, Vetter DE, Rűbsamen R, Kandler K. The precise temporal pattern of prehearing spontaneous activity is necessary for tonotopic map refinement. Neuron. 2014;82:822–835. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.04.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cooper NP, Guinan JJ., Jr Separate mechanical processes underlie fast and slow effects of medial olivocochlear efferent activity. J Physiol. 2003;548:307–312. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2003.039081. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delano PH, Elgueda D, Hamame CM, Robles L. Selective attention to visual stimuli reduces cochlear sensitivity in chinchillas. J Neurosci. 2007;27:4146–4153. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3702-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dragicevic CD, Aedo C, León A, Bowen M, Jara N, Terreros G, Robles L, Delano PH. The olivocochlear reflex strength and cochlear sensitivity are independently modulated by auditory cortex microstimulation. J Assoc Res Otolaryngol. 2015;16:223–240. doi: 10.1007/s10162-015-0509-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elgoyhen AB, Johnson DS, Boulter J, Vetter DE, Heinemann S. Alpha 9: an acetylcholine receptor with novel pharmacological properties expressed in rat cochlear hair cells. Cell. 1994;79:705–715. doi: 10.1016/0092-8674(94)90555-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elgoyhen AB, Vetter DE, Katz E, Rothlin CV, Heinemann SF, Boulter J. Alpha10: a determinant of nicotinic cholinergic receptor function in mammalian vestibular and cochlear mechanosensory hair cells. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2001;98:3501–3506. doi: 10.1073/pnas.051622798. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elgueda D, Delano PH, Robles L. Effects of electrical stimulation of olivocochlear fibers in cochlear potentials in the chinchilla. J Assoc Res Otolaryngol. 2011;12:317–327. doi: 10.1007/s10162-011-0260-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fritz JB, Elhilali M, David SV, Shamma SA. Auditory attention–focusing the searchlight on sound. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2007;17:437–455. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2007.07.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilbert CD, Sigman M. Brain states: top-down influences in sensory processing. Neuron. 2007;54:677–696. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.05.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guinan JJ., Jr . Physiology of olivocochlear efferents. In: Dallos P, Popper A, Fay R, editors. The cochlea. New York: Springer; 1996. pp. 435–502. [Google Scholar]
- Hamame CM, Delano PH, Robles L. Relevance of a neutral cue in a two-choice detection task in the rat. Biol Res. 2006;39:259–267. doi: 10.4067/s0716-97602006000200008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamame CM, Delano PH, Robles L. A neutral cue facilitates detection of a visual target by modulating attention. Biol Res. 2008;41:473–479. doi: 10.4067/S0716-97602008000400012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hernández-Peón R, Scherrer H, Jouvet M. Modification of electric activity in cochlear nucleus during attention in unanesthetized cats. Science. 1956;123:331–332. doi: 10.1126/science.123.3191.331. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Katz E, Elgoyhen AB. Short-term plasticity and modulation of synaptic transmission at mammalian inhibitory cholinergic olivocochlear synapses. Front Syst Neurosci. 2014;8:224. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00224. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kong WJ, Cheng HM, van Cauwenberge P. Expression of nicotinic acetylcholine receptor subunit alpha9 in type II vestibular hair cells of rats. Acta Pharmacol Sin. 2006;27:1509–1514. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-7254.2006.00423.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lauer AM, May BJ. The medial olivocochlear system attenuates the developmental impact of early noise exposure. J Assoc Res Otolaryngol. 2011;12:329–343. doi: 10.1007/s10162-011-0262-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- León A, Elgueda D, Silva MA, Hamamé CM, Delano PH. Auditory cortex basal activity modulates cochlear responses in chinchillas. PLoS One. 2012;7:e36203. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0036203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lichtenhan JT, Wilson US, Hancock KE, Guinan JJ., Jr Medial olivocochlear efferent reflex inhibition of human cochlear nerve responses. Hear Res. 2016;333:216–224. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2015.09.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maison SF, Adams JC, Liberman MC. Olivocochlear innervation in the mouse: immunocytochemical maps, crossed versus uncrossed contributions, and transmitter colocalization. J Comp Neurol. 2003;455:406–416. doi: 10.1002/cne.10490. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- May BJ, Prosen CA, Weiss D, Vetter D. Behavioral investigation of some possible effects of the central olivocochlear pathways in transgenic mice. Hear Res. 2002;171:142–157. doi: 10.1016/S0378-5955(02)00495-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McIntosh JM, Absalom N, Chebib M, Elgoyhen AB, Vincler M. Alpha9 nicotinic acetylcholine receptors and the treatment of pain. Biochem Pharmacol. 2009;78:693–702. doi: 10.1016/j.bcp.2009.05.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Melki SJ, Li Y, Semaan MT, Zheng QY, Megerian CA, Alagramam KN. A mouse model validates the utility of electrocochleography in verifying endolymphatic hydrops. J Assoc Res Otolaryngol. 2014;15:413–421. doi: 10.1007/s10162-014-0445-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mishra J, de Villers-Sidani E, Merzenich M, Gazzaley A. Adaptive training diminishes distractibility in aging across species. Neuron. 2014;84:1091–1103. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.10.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murugasu E, Russell IJ. The effect of efferent stimulation on basilar membrane displacement in the basal turn of the guinea pig cochlea. J Neurosci. 1996;16:325–332. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-01-00325.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oatman LC. Role of visual attention on auditory evoked potentials in unanesthetized cats. Exp Neurol. 1971;32:341–356. doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(71)90003-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prosen CA, Bath KG, Vetter DE, May BJ. Behavioral assessments of auditory sensitivity in transgenic mice. J Neurosci Methods. 2000;97:59–67. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0270(00)00169-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prut L, Belzung C. The open field as a paradigm to measure the effects of drugs on anxiety-like behaviors: a review. Eur J Pharmacol. 2003;463:3–33. doi: 10.1016/S0014-2999(03)01272-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Puria S, Guinan JJ, Jr, Liberman MC. Olivocochlear reflex assays: effects of contralateral sound on compound action potentials versus ear-canal distortion products. J Acoust Soc Am. 1996;99:500–507. doi: 10.1121/1.414508. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- R Core Team. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2014. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Available from: http://www.R-project.org. [Google Scholar]
- Robles L, Delano PH. Efferent system. In: Dallos P, Oertel D, editors. The senses: a comprehensive reference. London: Academic; 2008. pp. 413–445. [Google Scholar]
- Shinn-Cunningham BG, Best V. Selective attention in normal and impaired hearing. Trends Amplif. 2008;12:283–299. doi: 10.1177/1084713808325306. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Terreros G, Delano PH. Corticofugal modulation of peripheral auditory responses. Front Syst Neurosci. 2015;9:134. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2015.00134. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Valero MD, Hancock KE, Liberman MC. The middle ear muscle reflex in the diagnosis of cochlear neuropathy. Hear Res. 2016;332:29–38. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2015.11.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vetter DE, Liberman MC, Mann J, Barhanin J, Boulter J, Brown MC, Saffiote-Kolman J, Heinemann SF, Elgoyhen AB. Role of alpha9 nicotinic ACh receptor subunits in the development and function of cochlear efferent innervation. Neuron. 1999;23:93–103. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80756-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vetter DE, Katz E, Maison SF, Taranda J, Turcan S, Ballestero J, Liberman MC, Elgoyhen AB, Boulter J. The alpha10 nicotinic acetylcholine receptor subunit is required for normal synaptic function and integrity of the olivocochlear system. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007;104:20594–20599. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0708545105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walsh KP, Pasanen EG, McFadden D. Changes in otoacoustic emissions during selective auditory and visual attention. J Acoust Soc Am. 2015;137:2737–2757. doi: 10.1121/1.4919350. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Warr WB, Guinan JJ., Jr Efferent innervation of the organ of corti: two separate systems. Brain Res. 1979;173:152–155. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(79)91104-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wittekindt A, Kaiser J, Abel C. Attentional modulation of the inner ear: a combined otoacoustic emission and EEG study. J Neurosci. 2014;34:9995–10002. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4861-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]