Skip to main content
The Journal of Neuroscience logoLink to The Journal of Neuroscience
. 2018 Mar 7;38(10):2413–2415. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3224-17.2018

Movement Variability Is Processed Bilaterally by Inferior Parietal Lobule

David Marc Anton Mehler 1,*,, Sasha Reschechtko 2,*,
PMCID: PMC6705896  PMID: 30995608

Robots with artificial intelligence have made enormous progress in solving complex cognitive tasks; but when it comes to learning and executing coordinated, smooth, and complex movements, humans and animals still excel. To ensure consistent skilled movements, the motor system needs to learn how to control and exploit movement variability.

While some movement variability is “noise” that results from stochastic neural and muscle activity and can reduce task success (Faisal et al., 2008), other movement variability does not harm performance. For instance, when one is playing tennis, multiple combinations of movement of the shoulder, elbow, and wrist joint can result in a successful hit. This abundance of possible movements constitutes “task-irrelevant” motor variability, so-called because it does not affect successful task completion; the motor system can, however, exploit task-irrelevant variability to optimize motor performance (Sternad et al., 2011; Wolpert et al., 2011). Moreover, it can learn through such variability that certain body positions minimize the impact of unexpected perturbations and how to flexibly switch to a different movement to compensate for muscle fatigue or injuries (Latash, 2012). Understanding the neural control of both forms of movement variability is hence central for the study of human movement control in health and disease.

Noninvasive brain stimulation can probe neural sources of motor variability, but current techniques are limited by relatively poor control over perturbation site and intensity (Siebner et al., 2009; Horvath et al., 2014). Invasive recordings from implanted electrodes allow researchers to correlate movement kinematics with neural activity in small populations of neurons (Churchland et al., 2006; Kaufman et al., 2014), but these methods are too invasive for use in healthy humans and they do not allow measurement of simultaneous neural activity from many sites in the brain. In contrast, noninvasive neuroimaging with fMRI can record the neural correlates of motor variability across the brain. The BOLD signal captured by fMRI reflects the ratio of oxygenated to deoxygenated blood, which largely depends on energy consumption by local neural populations and resulting increases in local blood flow due to neurovascular coupling, but it is also affected by other, non-neural factors. Potential methodological concerns for fMRI include the slowness of vascular responses to neural activity and the presence of non-neural variability (e.g., heartbeat, breathing, and head motion) that can confound measurements. However, by modeling the delay and correcting for non-neural noise, fMRI signals can reveal brain activity during fast-evolving behavior, such as arm reaches.

In a recent study, Haar et al. (2017b) reported intriguing correlations between intertrial neural variability (measured with fMRI) and intertrial movement variability during arm reaches. They instructed 32 healthy adults to perform out-and-back reaching movements to near and far target locations using a pen stylus on a digital drawing tablet while fMRI was recorded. No visual feedback about the endpoint position or trajectory was provided during or after movements to minimize neural variability stemming from visual feedback. Hence, subjects did not know whether their reaches were accurate.

The authors quantified the trial-by-trial neural variability (fMRI variability around individual mean response) and variability in reach extent, direction, and velocity for each subject, and they tested whether subjects exhibited consistent magnitudes of neural variability in multiple cortical motor regions of interest during reaches to different targets and during reaches with each arm. Neural variability in several motor and premotor regions of interest in each hemisphere were correlated across reaches to different targets by the right arm (Haar et al., 2017b, their Fig. 5A) and by the left arm (Haar et al., 2017b, their Fig. 5B). In addition, variability in the premotor cortex, superior parietal lobule, and supplementary motor area in each hemisphere was correlated for right and left arm reaches (Haar et al., 2017b, their Fig. 5C). Together, these results indicate that subjects exhibited consistent magnitudes of neural variability regardless of the arm used to perform the movements or the target to which they reached.

To study the neural control of movement variability, Haar et al. (2017b) tested whether neural variability was correlated with any of their three measures of movement variability: reach extent, direction, or velocity. A link between neural variability and movement variability was found bilaterally in the inferior parietal lobule (IPL), which explained ∼24% of between-subject differences of variability in movement extent (Haar et al., 2017b, their Fig. 6). This finding was corroborated by an exploratory searchlight analysis of the cortical surface. The searchlight identified additional clusters in a medial area of the superior parietal lobule (SPL), the precuneus (Haar et al., 2017b, their Fig. 7). These results extend previous reports of effector-invariant encoding of movement directions (Haar et al., 2017a) to effector-invariant encoding of movement variability in the IPL and SPL.

These results raise a question: why does the motor system involve both hemispheres in processing movement variability of either arm? One possible reason is that the motor system integrates knowledge of movement variability across arms to maximize error reduction and thereby facilitate motor learning. Movement extent variability is critical for task success and provides a crucial learning signal that has been extensively studied in perturbation experiments (Wolpert et al., 2011). Encoding movement variability across both arms may come at a higher computational cost for the motor system, but it allows both hemispheres to learn from errors made by either limb. Transfer of learning between hands and limbs has been described in simple and complex motor tasks (Lee et al., 2010; Dickins et al., 2015), and the IPL and precuneus may facilitate this process. Furthermore, effector-invariant encoding of reaching directions has been shown in an identical task for several ipsilateral and contralateral cortical motor areas, but barely for the IPL (Haar et al., 2017a). Together, these findings suggest that the IPL might mainly encode movement variability during reaching when no task feedback is provided. However, the function of the IPL (and precuneus) in this task setting remains an open and intriguing question.

Another interpretation for the observed bilateral IPL and precuneus activity relates to processes that were emphasized in the present experiment because of the lack of visual task feedback. While the interpretation above relies on the role of the IPL in motor planning and preparation (Cohen and Andersen, 2002), these processes typically require knowledge of task success from previous motor actions. Without visual feedback about task success, however, motor planning in the present experiment was limited. As a result, two other processes likely interacted to guide the reaches subjects performed in the experiment: (1) shifting attention toward the action space (informed by reappearing target locations); and (2) monitoring joint configurations (informed by proprioception). The findings of Haar et al. (2017b) are in line with the expected neural bases of these processes: the IPL is critically involved in processing and attending to peripersonal space (Fogassi and Luppino, 2005); that is, the action space that immediately surrounds the body, and the IPL's role in spatial attention has previously been demonstrated (Mattingley et al., 1998). Similarly, the precuneus, which receives input from premotor regions and the IPL (Margulies et al., 2009), is also involved in shifting spatial attention between different target locations in the absence of visual feedback (Wenderoth et al., 2005) and updating postural representations of the upper limb during reaching (Pellijeff et al., 2006). The specific experimental design used in the present study likely recruited these IPL and precuneus functions; therefore, it remains to be tested whether these regions also process movement variability when visual feedback is provided.

Another open question is which brain regions process task-irrelevant variability. The stylus pen recorded movement data only from the tip and thus only quantified task-relevant variability, but the whole movement required complex coordination in multiple joints of the arm. Therefore, the same position of the stylus tip may have been executed using many different joint configurations across trials, leading to task-irrelevant variability. While task-irrelevant joint variability tends to be larger than task-relevant endpoint variability for healthy subjects (Wolpert et al., 2011), high task-relevant endpoint variability dominates when joint control is impaired (e.g., due to stroke) (Cirstea and Levin, 2000). Neural correlates of joint-based control over reaching directions for an identical task included activity in the IPL and SPL (Haar et al., 2017a). Extending this work to task-irrelevant variability could yield valuable insights into the neural mechanisms contributing to both the benefits (Sternad et al., 2011; Wolpert et al., 2011; Latash, 2012) and challenges (Cirstea et al., 2003) that arise from movement variability in movement control.

Haar et al. (2017b) demonstrated that humans show consistent magnitudes of neural variability across hemispheres regardless of the movements performed. Additionally, they reported that bilateral IPL, a key region in motor planning, processes movement extent variability regardless of arm use. This suggests that both hemispheres cooperate in controlling movement extent variability, a metric critical for task success and motor learning. However, the function of IPL and the generalizability of findings to tasks that involve visual feedback and quantify task-irrelevant variability remain to be tested.

Footnotes

Editor's Note: These short reviews of recent JNeurosci articles, written exclusively by students or postdoctoral fellows, summarize the important findings of the paper and provide additional insight and commentary. If the authors of the highlighted article have written a response to the Journal Club, the response can be found by viewing the Journal Club at www.jneurosci.org. For more information on the format, review process, and purpose of Journal Club articles, please see http://jneurosci.org/content/preparing-manuscript#journalclub.

D.M.A.M. was supported by Health Care and Research Wales PhD studentship. We thank Johannes Algermissen for insightful discussions.

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

References

  1. Churchland MM, Afshar A, Shenoy KV (2006) A central source of movement variability. Neuron 52:1085–1096. 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.10.034 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Cirstea MC, Levin MF (2000) Compensatory strategies for reaching in stroke. Brain 123:940–953. 10.1093/brain/123.5.940 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Cirstea MC, Mitnitski AB, Feldman AG, Levin MF (2003) Interjoint coordination dynamics during reaching in stroke. Exp Brain Res 151:289–300. 10.1007/s00221-003-1438-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Cohen YE, Andersen RA (2002) A common reference frame for movement plans in the posterior parietal cortex. Nat Rev Neurosci 3:553–562. 10.1038/nrn873 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Dickins DS, Sale MV, Kamke MR (2015) Intermanual transfer and bilateral cortical plasticity is maintained in older adults after skilled motor training with simple and complex tasks. Front Aging Neurosci 7:73. 10.3389/fnagi.2015.00073 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Faisal AA, Selen LP, Wolpert DM (2008) Noise in the nervous system. Nat Rev Neurosci 9:292–303. 10.1038/nrn2258 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Fogassi L, Luppino G (2005) Motor functions of the parietal lobe. Curr Opin Neurobiol 15:626–631. 10.1016/j.conb.2005.10.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Haar S, Dinstein I, Shelef I, Donchin O (2017a) Effector-invariant movement encoding in the human motor system. J Neurosci 37:1663–1671. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1663-17.2017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Haar S, Donchin O, Dinstein I (2017b) Individual movement variability magnitudes are explained by cortical neural variability. J Neurosci 37:9076–9085. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1650-17.2017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Horvath JC, Carter O, Forte JD (2014) Transcranial direct current stimulation: five important issues we aren't discussing (but probably should be). Front Syst Neurosci 8:1–8. 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Kaufman MT, Churchland MM, Ryu SI, Shenoy KV (2014) Cortical activity in the null space: permitting preparation without movement. Nat Neurosci 17:440–448. 10.1038/nn.3643 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Latash ML. (2012) The bliss (not the problem) of motor abundance (not redundancy). Exp Brain Res 217:1–5. 10.1007/s00221-012-3000-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Lee M, Hinder MR, Gandevia SC, Carroll TJ (2010) The ipsilateral motor cortex contributes to cross-limb transfer of performance gains after ballistic motor practice. J Physiol 588:201–212. 10.1113/jphysiol.2009.183855 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Margulies DS, Vincent JL, Kelly C, Lohmann G, Uddin LQ, Biswal BB, Villringer A, Castellanos FX, Milham MP, Petrides M (2009) Precuneus shares intrinsic functional architecture in humans and monkeys. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106:20069–20074. 10.1073/pnas.0905314106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Mattingley JB, Husain M, Rorden C, Kennard C, Driver J (1998) Motor role of human inferior parietal lobe revealed in unilateral neglect patients. Nature 392:179–182. 10.1038/32413 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Pellijeff A, Bonilha L, Morgan PS, McKenzie K, Jackson SR (2006) Parietal updating of limb posture: an event-related fMRI study. Neuropsychologia 44:2685–2690. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.01.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Siebner HR, Hartwigsen G, Kassuba T, Rothwell JC (2009) How does transcranial magnetic stimulation modify neuronal activity in the brain? Implications for studies of cognition. Cortex 45:1035–1042. 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.02.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Sternad D, Abe MO, Hu X, Müller H (2011) Neuromotor noise, error tolerance and velocity-dependent costs in skilled performance. PLoS Comput Biol 7:e1002159. 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002159 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Wenderoth N, Debaere F, Sunaert S, Swinnen SP (2005) The role of anterior cingulate cortex and precuneus in the coordination of motor behaviour. Eur J Neurosci 22:235–246. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04176.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Wolpert DM, Diedrichsen J, Flanagan JR (2011) Principles of sensorimotor learning. Nat Rev Neurosci 12:739–751. 10.1038/nrn3112 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from The Journal of Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Society for Neuroscience

RESOURCES