Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2019 Aug 28.
Published in final edited form as: Depress Anxiety. 2012 May;29(5):449–459. doi: 10.1002/da.21892

BRAIN ACTIVATION TO FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN YOUTH WITH PTSD SYMPTOMS

Amy S Garrett 1,2,*, Victor Carrion 2, Hilit Kletter 2, Asya Karchemskiy 1, Carl F Weems 3, Allan Reiss 1,2
PMCID: PMC6712984  NIHMSID: NIHMS1041937  PMID: 22553009

Abstract

Objective:

This study examined activation to facial expressions in youth with a history of interpersonal trauma and current posttraumatic stress symptoms (PTSS) compared to healthy controls (HC).

Design and analysis:

Twenty-three medication-naive youth with PTSS and 23 age- and gender-matched HC underwent functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRi) while viewing fearful, angry, sad, happy, and neutral faces. Data were analyzed for group differences in location of activation, as well as timing of activation during the early versus late phase of the block. Using SPM5, significant activation (P <.05 FWE [Family-Wise Error] corrected, extent = 10 voxels) associated with the main effect of group was identified. Activation from selected clusters was extracted to SPSS software for further analysis of specific facial expressions and temporal patterns of activation.

Results:

The PTSS group showed significantly greater activation than controls in several regions, including the amygdala/hippocampus, medial prefrontal cortex, insula, and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, and less activation than controls in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC). These group differences in activation were greatest during angry, happy, and neutral faces, and predominantly during the early phase of the block. Post hoc analyses showed significant Group × Phase interactions in the right amygdala and left hippocampus.

Conclusions:

Traumatic stress may impact development of brain regions important for emotion processing. Timing of activation may be altered in youth with PTSS. Depression and Anxiety 29:449–459, 2012.

Keywords: fMRI, PTSD, pediatric, emotion, faces

INTRODUCTION

Traumatic stress is a common experience in youth. A recent study found that more than 68% of youth had experienced a traumatic event by 16 years of age and more than 20% of these children subsequently suffered from problems in school, emotional difficulties, and physical ailments.[1] Other studies have also found adverse effects of trauma on youth, including elevated cortisol levels,[2] emotional distress, and impaired daily function.[3] Posttraumatic stress symptoms (PTSS) refer to the psychiatric condition suffered by those who have experienced a significant trauma and are experiencing a severe and prolonged stress response, but do not meet full criteria for Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD). Our previous research indicates significant functional impairment in children with PTSS[3] and research by others underscores the importance of studying these children.[4]

Neuroimaging studies of youth with PTSS provide a valuable opportunity to better understand the effects of traumatic stress on neural function during development, and may lead to improved treatments for this age group. Structural MRI studies of brain volume in youth with PTSD have indicated reduced corpus callosum area, whereas adult studies of PTSD have reported deficits in the hippocampus and anterior cingulate cortex, although not consistently (see reviews of brain structure in pediatric PTSD[5] and adult PTSD.[6]) There are few neuroimaging studies in pediatric PTSD that use functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data. An early study of five adolescents with earthquake-related PTSD reported increased parahippocampal gyrus and decreased anterior cingulate activation to trauma-related tasks compared to six trauma-exposed youth who did not develop PTSD.[7] Previous fMRI studies from our lab have shown aberrant activation of ventral prefrontal regions during a response inhibition task[8] and reduced hippocampal activation during a verbal memory task[9] in youths with PTSS. fMRI studies in adults with PTSD may suggest hypotheses about pediatric PTSS. Recent reviews of adult PTSD report relatively consistent findings of increased amygdala activation and decreased medial prefrontal activation to emotion-related tasks (recent reviews include[10,11]). Amygdala activation is associated with perceiving the emotional saliency of stimuli, and has a central role in fear conditioning and memory,[12] thus it has been studied extensively in the context of PTSD[13] as well as other psychiatric disorders. Medial prefrontal cortex activation may be related to self-reflection,[14] and is strongly associated with fear extinction paradigms.[15] More fMRI studies are clearly needed in youths to determine the effects of trauma on the developing brain, particularly in emotion processing regions such as the amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex.

In the study described here, emotional facial expressions were presented to youth with PTSS, as faces reliably activate emotion-related brain regions in both psychiatric and healthy control (HC) populations.[16] Some facial expressions may be trauma-mimetic stimuli for youth with a history of interpersonal trauma. Behavioral studies have found that children of hostile parents recognize expressions of anger more quickly (using less perceptual information) than children of parents with low hostility.[17] This suggests a priming effect, such that neural systems become more efficient at recognizing salient information in order to avoid harm. Similarly, several behavioral studies have indicated that traumatized children process threat-related stimuli more quickly, indicating attention bias toward threatening stimuli,[18,19] whereas others respond more slowly, suggesting attention bias away from threatening stimuli, which can be interpreted as avoidance or difficulty disengaging from trauma-related stimuli.[20,21] Based on these behavioral observations, we predicted that children with PTSS would demonstrate aberrant timing in addition to aberrant magnitude of activation to emotional face expressions. Aberrant timing may be observed as greater early-phase activation, suggesting priming, greater late-phase activation, suggesting failure to habituate, or greater increase from the early to late phase, indicating increased sensitization. Adults with PTSD have greater early-phase activation of the amygdala to trauma-related words, and lack of habituation to panic-related words[22] and facial expressions.[13] Understanding the chronometry of activation in youth with PTSS would contribute to our understanding of the effects of trauma on brain function during development.

METHODS

PARTICIPANTS

Participants included 56 medication-naive youth between 10 and 16 years of age. All subjects reported no neurological disorders, major medical illness, or contraindications for the MRI procedure. This study was approved by Stanford University IRB and parent consent and child assent were obtained for all participants.

For the PTSS group, 30 youths (19 females) with a history of interpersonal trauma (>6 months prior) and posttraumatic symptoms were recruited from local social service departments and mental health clinics. Inclusion in the PTSS group required at least one exposure to inter-personal trauma as defined by DSM-IV, including sexual abuse, physical abuse, and/or witnessing violence. The participants were required to currently live in a safe environment (confirmed during Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale for Children and Adolescents [CAPS-CA] interview) and to have no history of alcohol or drug abuse or dependence. A protocol was in place to contact Child Protective Services if necessary, but no contacts were needed. Twenty-six HC youth (13 females) with no history of trauma (as reported by child and caregiver) were recruited by advertisement in the community.

CLINICAL ASSESSMENT

PTSS subjects were required to score at least 13 on the CAPS-CA[23]). A PTSD symptom was considered present if it received scores of at least 1 in frequency and 2 in intensity, totalling 3 in severity.[3,4] The Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Youth-Present and Lifetime Version (K-SADS-PL) was used to identify comorbid Axis I DSM-IV disorders.[24] A child psychiatrist (VC) or psychologist (CW) administered all interviews. Our previous publication provides additional information on standardization of clinical interviews.[8]

The caregivers of H C participants confirmed the absence of psychiatric symptoms by completing the Child Behavior Checklist.[25] The Childhood Trauma Questionnaire[26] was used to rule out a history of trauma. For all subjects, the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence[27] and the Edinburgh Inventory determined handedness.[28] Ethnic composition, family income, and other demographic information were assessed by self-report questionnaire.

fMRI PROCEDURES

IMPLICIT EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS TASK

Validation of these affective faces stimuli has been reported in a previous publication.[29] Briefly, a set of 96 pictures were taken from several sources and edited to be monochromatic and of the same size. The faces displayed either a happy, sad, fearful, angry, or neutral expression. Blocks of scrambled pictures, created by randomly rearranging the voxels into an unrecognizable pattern, were included as a baseline comparison condition. Half of the pictures showed male models, and half showed female models. Pictures were matched (across types of emotion) for intensity of emotional expression and gender of model. Pictures were rated by an independent group of subjects to verify that they were perceived as intended.

During fMRI, the faces were presented in a block design, each containing eight pictures having the same expression. None of the stimuli were repeated. Each individual picture was shown for 3 s with no interstimulus interval. Therefore, each block lasted 24 s. The blocks were ordered such that a nonaffective block (neutral or scrambled pictures or rest) always preceded and followed each affective block to avoid carryover effects.[30] To lessen the probability of head movement during the scan, the task was presented in two runs with approximately 1 min of rest between runs. Happy, sad, neutral, and scrambled blocks were presented in one run, and angry, fearful, neutral, and scrambled blocks in the other run. Each condition was presented for four (nonsequential) blocks total. In order to minimize order effects, there were four different sequences of blocks presented in the study. To assure attention to the stimuli, subjects judged the gender of the model in each picture and pressed button 1 with the right index finger to indicate female and button 2 with the right second digit to indicate male pictures. During scrambled blocks, subjects alternated pushing buttons 1 and 2. Correct and incorrect responses and response times were recorded. The task was presented using Psyscope software (http://poppy.psy.cmu.edu/psyscope). Onset of scanner and task were synchronized using a trigger pulse delivered to the scanner. Stimuli were projected onto a screen attached to the headcoil, and viewed using a mirror.

IMAGE ACQUISITION

Images were acquired on a 3T GE Signa scanner using a standard GE whole head coil (General Electric, Milwaukee, WI). A custom-built head stabilization system prevented head movement. The entire brain was imaged in 28 axial slices (4-mm thick, 0.5 mm skip) parallel to the Anterior Commisure-Posterior Comissure (AC-PC) line. Functional images were acquired using a T2*-weighted gradient echo spiral pulse sequence (Relaxation Time [TR] = 2,000 ms, Echo Time [TE] = 30 ms, flip angle = 89°, 1 interleave; Field of View (FOV) = 20 cm2; matrix = 64 × 64; in-plane resolution = 3.125 mm; slice thickness = 4 mm, 0.5 mm skip; 28 slices). Spiral fMRI optimizes signal to noise and minimizes susceptibility effects, therefore improving Blood Oxygenation Level Dependent (BOLD) signal in regions such as the ventral prefrontal cortex and the medial temporal lobe.[31] A high-resolution T1-weighted Spoiled Grass Gradient Recalled (SPGR) 3Danatomical image was acquired during the same session (TR = 35 ms; TE = 6 ms; flip angle = 45°; FOV = 24 cm2; 124 slices; 256 × 192 matrix; acquired resolution = 1.5 × 0.9 × 1.2 mm3).

DATA ANALYSIS

fMRI DATA ANALYSIS

Images were processed using SPM5 (Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, London, UK). The images were spatially realigned, and motion was corrected using ArtRepair toolbox for SPM (http://cibsr.stanford.edu/tools/ArtRepair/ArtRepair.htm). The functional images were spatially normalized to an age-appropriate stereotactic template (CCHMC: http://irc.cchmc.org/software/pedbrain.php) and smoothed with an 8-mm full-width half maximum isotropic Gaussian filter. Low-frequency noise was removed from the time series using a high-pass filter set to 0.5 cycles per minute and applied to the fMRI time series at each voxel.

ANALYSIS OF EARLYAND LATE PHASES

A fixed-effects model was used to identify activation associated with each facial expression compared to scrambled images. Each block of faces was divided into an early and late phase, so that the first 12 s of the faces block was modeled as the early phase, and the last 12 s was modeled as the late phase. The early phase of each facial expression block was contrasted with the early phase of the scrambled images block, and the late phase was contrasted with the late phase of the scrambled block, collapsing across all blocks within each facial expression condition. Voxel-wise t-statistics were normalized to Z scores.

The group analysis involved two steps. First, a random-effects voxel-wise whole brain analysis was conducted in SPM5 using a multivariate ANOVA. The ANOVA included the factors group (PTSS and HC), and face (angry, fearful, happy, sad, neutral) and phase (early and late). Variance associated with IQ was removed by modeling IQ as a nuisance covariate. Voxels significantly activated for the main effect of group and the interaction of Group × Phase were identified, using a stringent threshold of P =.05 FWE corrected, and minimum cluster extent = 10. Regions of activation were localized with reference to the stereotaxic atlas of Talairach and Tournoux.[32] The software program Rfxplot (rfxplot.sourceforge.net) was used to plot time-courses to verify that group differences were not attributed to the ‘scrambled’ baseline condition.

The second step of the group analysis determined the relative contributions of each facial expression and phase to the results obtained from the SPM analysis of the main effect of group. Only clusters showing significant activation in step 1 and having prominence in the PTSD literature were further analyzed, including the amygdala, insula, ventral medial prefrontal, ventrolateral prefrontal, and dorsolateral prefrontal cortices (DLPFC). The MARSBAR (http://marsbar.sourceforge.net/) toolbox in SPM5 was used to extract mean activation for export to SPSS (http://www.spss.com/) for further analysis. In SPSS, independent group t-tests were conducted for each brain region, comparing group activation to each facial expression and phase, for example PTSS versus HC in the amygdala/hippocampus for the early phase of angry faces, etc. We also assessed habituation and sensitization of activation (significant decreases or increases in activation from the early to late phase of the block, respectively). Group differences surpassing a corrected αof .01 were flagged as contributing substantially to the main effect of group.

Because no significant clusters were identified by the interaction of Group × Phase at this threshold, we also investigated the interaction of Group × Phase using a small volume correction confined to the bilateral amygdalae and hippocampi (using masks from the AAL Atlas[33]), as these regions have been strongly implicated in the literature. Clusters of activation passing threshold (P= .05 FWE corrected) were plotted for qualitative analysis of timing of activation.

RESULTS

DESCRIPTIVE GROUP DIFFERENCES

Six of the PTSS and three of the HC subjects were rejected for excessive movement during the scan (for both groups, this was defined as greater than 25% of the time points having motion exceeding a signal threshold of 3% from the mean, or 0.5 mm per temporal frame). For the remaining subjects, the number of time points corrected for motion did not differ between groups (P >.05). One of the PTSS subjects was removed for missing IQ data, as all fMRI analyses were covaried for group differences in IQ. This left 23 PTSS and 23 HC subjects.

Table 1 shows the demographic descriptors of the groups. There were no group differences in age, gender distribution, or handedness. The PTSS group had significantly lower IQ scores than the HC group, so all fMRI analyses were covaried for IQ. Although 17 of the 23 PTSS subjects had no comorbid diagnoses, six subjects had depression, anxiety, and behavioral disorders characteristic of youth with PTSS.

TABLE 1.

Clinical, demographic, and behavioral descriptors of the participants

Descriptor Youth with PTSS (PTSS: N = 23) Healthy control (HC: N= 23)
Mean SD Range Mean SD Range
Age 14.1 2.0 14–17 14.6 1.8 14–17
WASIIQ** 104.7 15.6 71–136 115.1 11.2 92–133
CAPS total 39.4 17.8 13–72 NA
Left handed 3 - - 1
Gender 13 F/10M - - 12F/11M - -
Description Description
Trauma 5 SA NA
SPA
4WV
4SA/PA
4PA/WV
1SA/PA/WV
Comorbid diagnoses 2 MDD only None
1 MDD + panic
1 sep anxiety
1 enuresis + MDD
1 MDD, ADHD, CD
17 no comorbid Dx
Ethnic group 13 White 16 White
3 Hispanic 2 Hispanic
3 mixed 2 mixed
2 Black 1 Black
1 Pacific Islander 1 Pacific Islander
1 native American 1 Asian
Family education level* 1 junior high
5 high school
6 partial college 3 partial college
8 college grad 3 college grad
8 grad degree
2 missing 9 missing
Family income* 10 (10–50K)
3 (5O–7SK) 5 (5O–75K)
2 (75–1OOK) 1 (75–1OOK)
3 (1OO–125K) 2 (1OO–125K)
2 (125–200K)
5 over 200K
3 missing 10 missing
Mean SD Mean SD
Percent correct happy face 89 6 88 8
Response time happy face (ms) 916.4 184.7 929.2 170.3
Percent correct sad face 86 9 87 7
Response time sad face (ms) 932.9 194.6 954.7 136.6
Percent correct fear face*** 80 9 85 9
Response time fear face (ms) 960.8 161.1 976.8 162.8
Percent correct angry face 81 11 85 12
Response time angry face (ms) 992.2 176.4 1,017.4 177.8
Percent correct neutral face 86 9 88 8
Response time neutral face (ms) 931.7 172.6 948.7 157.0
*

The family education level and income of the posttraumatic stress symptoms (PTSS) subjects corresponds to the current caretaker, in most cases this was not the biological parents; CAPS, Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale for Children and Adolescents; SA, sexual abuse; PA, physical abuse; WV, witnessing violence; meds, medication; MDD, major depressive disorder; sep anxiety, separation anxiety; ADHD, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder; CD, conduct disorder; Dx, diagnosis.

**

P = .014.

***

Trend for decreased percent correct in PTSS group for fear faces; P = .06.

There were no significant group differences in task accuracy or response time, although there was a trend for the PTSS group to identify gender less accurately than the HC group for the fearful faces only (P= .06).

BRAIN ACTIVATION GROUP DIFFERENCES

Significant clusters of activation from the main effect of group are given in Table 2. Results of follow-up analyses of these significant clusters are summarized in Figure 3: Group differences in amygdala/hippocampal activation were greatest during the late phase of angry faces (P= .003) and the early (P= .002) and late (P= .007) phases of neutral faces; activation to neutral faces increased significantly from the early to late phase of the block for all subjects together (P= .003) and for control subjects alone (P= .005). Group differences in the medial prefrontal cortex cluster were greatest during the early phase of the angry faces (P= .006). Group differences in insula activation were greatest during the early phase of angry faces (P= .002) and the early phase of neutral faces (P= .005). The HC group, but not PTSS group, shows sensitization of activation to happy (P= .008) and neutral faces (P= .003) in the insula. Group differences in left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex activation were greatest during the early phase of happy faces (P= .002) and the early phase of neutral faces (P= .006), and both groups together show sensitization of activation to fear faces (P= .008). Group differences in the DLPFC were greatest during the late phase of happy faces (P= .002), and sensitize to neutral faces (P= .009) in the PTSS group only. Post hoc analyses of group differences in timing of activation using a small volume corrected voxel-wise analysis of the Group × Phase interaction found significant activation in the right amygdala and left hippocampus. The average time-courses in these regions are shown in Figure 2. The graphs suggest that the PTSS group showed greater right amygdala activation predominantly in the early (but not late) phase of the block of angry and fearful faces. For the left hippocampus, the PTSS group appears to show greater activation for angry and fear faces in the early phase of the block, but decreased activation compared to controls in response to happy and sad faces in the late phase of the block. The group × face and the Group × Face × Phase interactions were not significant.

TABLE 2.

Significantly activated clusters from the main effect of group

No. Region BA k Peak Z score TAL:X TAL:Y TAL:Z
1 L cuneus 17/18 719 7.32 −2 −73 18
L precuneus 23/31 6.70 −2 −75 11
2* L amygdala NA 252 6.01 −26 −3 −22
L hippocampus NA 5.36 −26 −10 −8
L globus pallidus NA 5.95 −24 −8 −6
L putamen NA 5.93 −26 −8 −6
L entorhinal cortex 28 5.52 −30 5 −22
3 R inferior parietal cortex 40 241 6.15 61 −33 42
R postcentral gyrus 2/3 6.03 61 −27 42
R precentral gyrus 6/4 5.35 63 −12 36
4 L superior temporal gyrus 22 214 6.92 −63 −42 11
L middle temporal gyrus 21 6.60 −61 −48 8
5 L precentral gyrus 6/4 140 6.81 −61 −8 34
L inferior frontal gyrus 9 5.62 −61 9 25
6* L inferior frontal gyrus 45/44 149 6.55 −57 22 12
7 L inferior temporal gyrus 37/20/21 88 6.46 −59 −53 −9
L middle temporal gyrus 37 5.83 −57 −51 −9
8 R inferior temporal gyrus 20 71 6.46 40 −11 −33
R middle temporal gyrus 21 5.82 40 −7 −33
9 R middle occipital gyrus 19/18 266 6.45 44 −83 13
10 L inferior parietal lobule 40 164 6.12 −57 −39 42
L postcentral gyrus 2 4.94 −61 −25 36
11* Rinsula(l) 13 111 6.02 46 −34 18
R superior temporal gyrus 41 5.78 44 −34 20
12 L inferior temporal gyrus (2) 20 76 5.71 −61 −22 −21
L middle temporal gyrus (2) 21 4.99 −63 −22 −14
13 R insula (2) 13 159 5.82 44 −16 −1
R superior temporal gyrus (2) 22 5.35 46 −17 1
R claustrum na 4.93 38 −11 4
R superior temporal gyrus 38 37 4.94 32 10 −26
14 L paracentral lobule 6 52 5.34 −12 −23 49
15 R superior temporal gyrus 22 42 5.61 57 −48 12
R middle temporal gyrus 39 4.67 57 −52 10
16* R DLPFC (superior frontal) 9 27 5.26 24 56 25
R middle frontal gyrus 10 4.84 24 55 21
17 L precuneus 7 29 5.21 −14 −66 33
18* R medial frontal gyrus 10/11 21 5.02 6 52 −11

Threshold of P = .05 FWE corrected, and minimum cluster size of 10 voxels. Clusters with “*” were extracted for follow-up analyses (shown in the table).

R, right hemisphere; L, left hemisphere; k, number of voxels in cluster; BA, Brodmann area; p, corrected cluster-wise p; TAL, Talairach Atlas Coordinate; NA, not applicable.

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Significant correlation between CAPS score and activation in the ventral medial prefrontal cortex during the late phase of the fearful faces block: for total CAPS score, ρ = .55, P = .008; for the CAPS-B (re-experiencing ) subscale (ρ = .54, P = .008). Green datapoints indicate subjects with a full PTSD diagnosis, whereas blue are subthreshold.

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Time-courses of activation in the right amygdala (top) and left hippocampus (bottom) displaying a significant Group × Phase interaction. The posttraumatic stress symptoms (PTSS) group time-course is shown in red and the HC group is shown in green. The x-axis is seconds, and the early phase (0–12 s) and late phase (12–24 s) are indicated (both shifted 6 s forward to account for the hemodynamic lag). For the right amygdala, the PTSS activation is greater than the HC particularly in the early phases of the angry and fear blocks. For the left hippocampus, PTSS activation is greater during early and late phase of the angry blocks, but less during the late phase of the happy and sad blocks.

CORRELATIONS WITH CAPS SCORES IN THE PTSS GROUP

A Spearman’s nonparametric correlation (threshold of P= .01 corrected for multiple comparisons) showed that increasing activation in the medial prefrontal cortex during the late phase of the fear block was associated with both total CAPS score (ρ= .55, P= .008) and the B (re-experiencing) subscale (ρ= .54, P= .008). Scatter plots of these associations are given in Figure 3. No other regions were significantly correlated with symptom severity.

We did not collect detailed information about severity of trauma exposure that would allow correlations with brain activation. However, we attempted to examine associations with trauma severity by correlating activation with number of types of trauma (e.g., physical abuse + sexual abuse + witnessing violence = 3). No correlations reached significance (P= .05/5 regions = .01).

COMORBID DIAGNOSES

To investigate effects of comorbid diagnoses on our findings, we removed the six subjects from the PTSS group who had a comorbid diagnosis. Between-group differences in extracted values were reduced along with the reduced power, but α remained less than .03.

EFFECTS OF A DIAGNOSIS OF PTSD

We directly compared Reign of Interest (ROI) activation in subjects with PTSS (N = 12) to those with a full diagnosis of PTSD (N = 11). The only difference was greater activation in the left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex to happy faces in subjects with PTSD compared to those with PTSS (P= .028).

DISCUSSION

This study examined brain activation to emotional facial expressions in youth with a history of interpersonal trauma and PTSS. Individuals in the PTSS group showed a significant degree of subsyndromal symptoms, and our previous studies of this sample have reported significant abnormalities in brain structure[34] and function.[8,9] The current study adds to the previous reports by finding abnormalities in emotion processing brain circuitry. Regions showing abnormal activation are consistent with previous neuroimaging studies of adults with PTSD, including the amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Notably, youth with PTSS show aberrant activation to neutral faces as well as emotional expressions, and show altered timing of activation in the amygdala and hippocampus. Therefore, traumatic stress in youth may impact the development of brain regions important for emotion perception.

Group differences in activation of the amygdala/hippocampus, medial prefrontal cortex, and insula were associated with angry face blocks but not fearful or sad. It is possible that angry facial expressions have particular relevance to this group of children who have been exposed to interpersonal trauma. If so, a previous trauma and/or current PTSD symptoms (such as hypervigilance) may prime the amygdala to respond to trauma-related stimuli more quickly. Magnetoencephalography shows that priming enhances amygdala theta activity as soon as 150–400 ms after presentation of the subsequent target word.[35] Therefore we would expect that priming would enhance early-phase amygdala activation, and we did observe exaggerated early-phase activation of the right amygdala in the PTSS group to angry and fearful faces. It is also possible that individuals with PTSS displayed hypersensitivity to threat even before experiencing trauma.

Greater activation in the PTSS group in response to neutral faces was noted in the amygdala, along with reduced activation in the DLPFC. The reactivity of the amygdala to neutral faces may reflect the ambiguity or a negative interpretation of these stimuli for this young age group[36] and has been reported in neuroimaging studies of children with anxiety and depression,[37] as well as adults with mild PTSD.[38]

Activation to happy faces shows an interesting pattern. For the PTSS group compared to HC, activation to happy faces is increased in the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex but decreased in the DLPFC, suggesting altered emotion regulation in response to these stimuli.[3941] Interestingly, happy faces are the only stimuli that elicit marked sensitization of activation from early to late phase in the insula in the HC group, but elicit no change from early to late phase in the PTSS group. In light of the somewhat consistent trend for sensitization of activation to the other stimuli, lack of sensitization to happy faces may be related to anhedonia or numbing symptoms reported in PTSS.

Severity of PTSD symptoms, as measured by the CAPS-CA score, was significantly correlated with medial prefrontal cortex activation during the late phase of the fearful faces block. In general, the PTSS group showed greater activation in this region compared to the HC group. This region has been consistently implicated in the PTSD neuroimaging literature[10,10] and is important for emotion regulation[42,43] and extinction of conditioned fear.[15] Interestingly, we observed a trend (P= .06) for decreased task accuracy during fearful faces trials only, even though the gender identification task was easy and proficiently performed for all other faces. Taken together, these data may suggest subtle avoidance of processing fearful faces in the PTSS group. This interpretation is consistent with previous studies finding attention bias away from threat-related stimuli in PTSD.[21] Thus, increased medial prefrontal activation during the fearful faces may reflect attention regulation to avoid processing fearful faces.

We did not observe habituation (a decrease in activation from early to late phase) in any of the regions in either group. This may have occurred because we did not repeat any of the faces, which is typically the method used in studies reporting rapid habituation of amygdala response.[44,45] The previous study of adult PTSD that examined early and late phases of activation[22] found increased early-phase activation to trauma-related words in the left amygdala in PTSD compared to HC, and a lack of habituation to panic-related words. However, that study defined the early and late phases as the first two and last two epochs of the experiment, whereas our late and early phases were the first and last 12 s of a block.

This study was strengthened by including a large sample of unmedicated subjects with few comorbid diagnoses. Limitations of the study include group differences in IQ, which we controlled by covarying statistically. Both adults and children with PTSD symptoms tend to have lower IQ, particularly lower verbal IQ, but not visual IQ. It is possible that symptoms of PTSD interfere with the cognitive functions that contribute to verbal IQ skills, such as memory and attention. If so, we would expect the IQ of these children to increase when symptoms are relieved. It is also possible that lower verbal IQ exists before trauma, and places children at risk for PTSD following a trauma, because it makes them less likely to process the experience verbally. A third possibility is that early trauma and symptoms of PTSD interfere with the ability of children to acquire verbal skills.

As we did not require a PTSD diagnosis, our findings may not generalize to studies of more severe PTSD symptoms in youth. However, the similarity of our findings to studies in adults suggests that our sample shows neurofunctional abnormalities representative of PTSD.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Main effect of group broken down by facial expression and phase. Significant group differences in left amygdala/hippocampus activation (at crosshairs) were greatest in the late phase of angry faces, and early and late phases of neutral faces; group differences in activation of the medial prefrontal cluster (at crosshairs) were greatest in the early phase of angry faces; group differences in activation in the insula were greatest during the early phase of angry faces, and early phase of neutral faces; group differences in activation in the left ventrolateral cluster (at crosshairs) were greatest during the early phase of happy faces, and the early phase of neutral faces; group differences in activation in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) were greatest in the late phase of happy faces.

Acknowledgments

Contract grant sponsor: National Institute of Mental Health; Contract grant number: MH63893; Contract grant sponsor: National Association for Research in Schizophrenia and Depression (NARSAD); Contract grant sponsor: American Foundation for Suicide Prevention (AFSP); Contract grant sponsor: Evans Foundation, Contract grant sponsor: Klingenstein Third Generation Foundation.

Footnotes

Conflict of interest: None declared.

REFERENCES

  • 1.Copeland WE, et al. Traumatic events and posttraumatic stress in childhood. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2007;64(5):577–584. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Carrion VG, et al. Diurnal salivary cortisol in pediatric posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2002;51(7):575–582. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Carrion VG, et al. Toward an empirical definition of pediatric PTSD: the phenomenology of PTSD symptoms in youth. J Am Acad ChildAdolesc Psychiatry 2002;41(2):166–173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.King NJ, et al. Treating sexually abused children with posttraumatic stress symptoms: a randomized clinical trial. J Am Acad ChildAdolesc Psychiatry 2000;39(11):1347–1355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Jackowski AP, et al. Neurostructural imaging findings in children with post-traumatic stress disorder: brief review. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2009;63(1):1–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Woon FL, Sood S, Hedges DW. Hippocampal volume deficits associated with exposure to psychological trauma and posttraumatic stress disorder in adults: a meta-analysis. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2010;34(7):1181–1188. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Yang P, et al. Evidence of early neurobiological alternations in adolescents with posttraumatic stress disorder: a functional MRI study. Neurosci Lett 2004;370(1):13–18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Carrion VG, et al. Posttraumatic stress symptoms and brain function during a response-inhibition task: an fMRI study in youth. Depress Anxiety 2008;25(6):514–526. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Carrion VG, et al. Reduced hippocampal activity in youth with posttraumatic stress symptoms: an fMRI study. J Pediatr Psychol 2009;35(5):559–569. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Hughes KC, Shin LM. Functional neuroimaging studies of post-traumatic stress disorder. Expert Rev Neurother 2011;11(2):275–285. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Robinson BL, Shergill SS. Imaging in posttraumatic stress disorder. Curr Opin Psychiatry 2011;24(1):29–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Phelps EA. Emotion and cognition: insights from studies of the human amygdala. Annu Rev Psychol 2006;57:27–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Shin LM, et al. A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex responses to overtly presented fearful faces in posttraumatic stress disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005;62(3):273–281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.van der Meer L, et al. Self-reflection and the brain: a theoretical review and meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies with implications for schizophrenia. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2010;34(6):935–946. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Rougemont-Bucking A, et al. Altered processing of contextual information during fear extinction in PTSD: an fMRI study. CNS Neurosci Ther 2010;17(4):227–36. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Fusar-Poli P, et al. Functional atlas ofemotional faces processing: a voxel-based meta-analysis of105 functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. J Psychiatry Neurosci 2009;34(6):418–432. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Scrimin S, et al. Recognition of facial expressions of mixed emotions in school-age children exposed to terrorism. Dev Psychol 2009;45(5):1341–1352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Dalgleish T, et al. An experimental investigation ofhypervigilance for threat in children and adolescents with post-traumatic stress disorder. PsycholMed 2001;31(3):541–547. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Watts SE, Weems CF. Associations among selective attention, memory bias, cognitive errors and symptoms of anxiety in youth. J Abnorm Child Psychol 2006;34(6):841–852. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Bar-Haim Y, et al. Life-threatening danger and suppression of attention bias to threat. Am J Psychiatry 2010;167(6):694–698. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Pine DS, et al. Attention bias to threat in maltreated children: implications for vulnerability to stress-related psychopathology. Am J Psychiatry 2005;162(2):291–296. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Protopopescu X, et al. Differential time courses and specificity of amygdala activity in posttraumatic stress disorder subjects and normal control subjects. Biol Psychiatry 2005;57(5):464–473. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Nader K, et al. Clinician administered PTSD Scale, Child and Adolescent Version. White River Junction, VT: National Center for PTSD; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Kaufman J, et al. Schedule forAffective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children-Present and Lifetime Version (K-SADS-PL): initial reliability and validity data. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1997;36(7):980–988. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Achenbach T Manual for the child behavior checklist and revised child behavior profile. Burlington: University of Vermont Department of Psychiatry; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Scher CD, et al. The childhood trauma questionnaire in a community sample: psychometric properties and normative data. J Trauma Stress 2001;14(4):843–857. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Wechsler D WAIS-III Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. New York: Psychological Corporation; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 1971;9(1):97–113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Yang TT, et al. Amygdalar activation associated with positive and negative facial expressions. Neuroreport 2002;13(14):1737–1741. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Garrett AS,Maddock RJ. Time course ofthe subjective emotional response to aversive pictures: relevance to fMRI studies. PsychiatryRes 2001;108(1):39–48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Glover GH, Lai S. Self-navigated spiral fMRI: interleaved versus single-shot. Magn Reson Med 1998;39(3):361–368. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Talairach J, Tournoux. Co-planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain. New York: Thieme; 1988. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Tzourio-Mazoyer N, et al. Automated anatomical labeling ofactivations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNIM RIsingle-subject brain. Neuroimage 2002;15(1):273–289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Carrion VG, et al. Attenuation of frontal asymmetry in pediatric posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2001;50(12):943–951. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Garolera M, et al. Amygdala activation in affective priming: a magnetoencephalogram study. Neuroreport 2007;18(14):1449–1453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Neta M, Whalen PJ. The primacy of negative interpretations when resolving the valence of ambiguous facial expressions. Psychol Sci 2010;21(7):901–907. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Thomas KM, et al. Amygdala response to fearful faces in anxious and depressed children. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2001;58(11):1057–1063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Brunetti M, et al. Elevated response ofhuman amygdala to neutral stimuli in mild post traumatic stress disorder: neural correlates of generalized emotional response. Neuroscience 2010;168(3):670–679. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Light SN, et al. Reduced right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex activity while inhibiting positive affect is associated with improvement in hedonic capacity after 8 weeks of antidepressant treatment in major depressive disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2011;70(10):962–968. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Mitchell DG. The nexus between decision making and emotion regulation: a review of convergent neurocognitive substrates. Behav Brain Res 2011;217(1):215–231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Passarotti AM, et al. Neural correlates of incidental and directed facial emotion processing in adolescents and adults. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2009;4(4):387–398. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Kanske P, et al. How to regulate emotion? Neural networks for reappraisal and distraction. Cereb Cortex 2011;21(6):1379–1388. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Koenigsberg HW, et al. Neural correlates of using distancing to regulate emotional responses to social situations. Neuropsychologia 2010;48(6):1813–1822. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Breiter HC, et al. Response and habituation of the human amygdala during visual processing of facial expression. Neuron 1996;17(5):875–887. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Strauss MM, et al. fMRI of sensitization to angry faces. Neuroimage 2005;26(2):389–413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES