Abstract
Background/Objectives
Body composition (BC) does not always vary as a function of exercise induced energy expenditure (exercise EE – resting EE). Energy balance variables were measured to understand energy compensation (EC) in response to an exercise intervention performed at low (LOW) or moderate (MOD) intensity.
Subjects/Methods
Twenty-one women with overweight/obesity (33 ± 5 kg/m2; 29 ± 10 yrs; 31 ± 4 ml O2/kg/min) were randomized to a 3-month LOW or MOD (40 or 60% of VȮ2reserve, respectively) matched to expend 1500 kcal/week (compliance = 97 ± 5%). Body energy stores (DXA), energy intake (EI) (food menu and food diaries), resting EE (indirect calorimetry), total EE (doubly-labeled water), time spent in different activities (accelerometers), appetite (visual analog scale), eating behavior traits and food reward (liking and wanting) were assessed at baseline, after weeks 1 and 2 and at the end of the 3-month exercise intervention.
Results
EC based on BC changes (fat mass and fat-free mass) was 49 ± 79% and 161 ± 88% in LOW and MOD groups, respectively (p = 0.010). EI did not change significantly during the intervention. However, eating behavior traits and food reward had changed by the end of the 3-month supervised exercise. Non-structured physical activity (NSPA) decreased across the intervention (p < 0.002), independent of the intensity of the exercise training.
Conclusion
Women with overweight/obesity training at LOW presented lower EC for a given energy cost of exercise. Our results strongly suggest that NSPA plays a major role in mediating the effects of exercise on energy balance and ultimately on changes in BC.
Clinical Trial Registration
www.ClinicalTrials.gov, identifier ISRCTN31641049.
Keywords: exercise intensity, energy compensation, body composition, non-structured physical activity, doubly-labeled water, obesity
Background/Objectives
The impact of exercise EE on weight loss is often less than expected (Miller et al., 1997). The limited dose of prescribed exercise (Thomas et al., 2012), the lack of compliance, the reduction of resting EE, the increase of EI and/or the decrease in NSPA following exercise are among the reasons that have been proposed to explain this observation (Thomas et al., 2012; Caudwell et al., 2013). We recently reported results from a systematic review, which showed an overall EC from exercise interventions of ∼ 20%, with EC being closer to 100% for longer interventions (Riou et al., 2015a).
Evidence suggests little EI compensation after acute exercise bouts [(King et al., 1994, 1997; Imbeault et al., 1997), and there are several reviews on this topic (King, 1999; Martins et al., 2008; King et al., 2012)]. However, following 14 days, EI compensation seems to hover around 30% (Stubbs et al., 2002a; Blundell et al., 2003; Whybrow et al., 2008), a phenomenon potentially influenced by the sex of subjects (Andersson et al., 1991; Meijer et al., 1991; King et al., 1996; Todd Alan Hagobian, 2012), eating behavior traits and hedonic processes (Lluch et al., 1998, 2000; Finlayson et al., 2009) as well as adiposity levels (Woo et al., 1982a, b; Kissileff et al., 1990; Westerterp-Plantenga et al., 1997; George and Morganstein, 2003; Hagobian et al., 2009) [for reviews see Hill et al. (1995) and Martins et al. (2008)]. EI following exercise is also subject to large inter-individual variability (Hopkins et al., 2013). Finlayson et al. (2009) showed that lean women who presented a higher EI following an acute bout of exercise also displayed a higher preference for high-fat sweet food and generally found foods more palatable. Similarly, it has been reported that compensators (individuals with a weight loss smaller than predicted) present an increase in EI with a greater intake from fat and increased hunger (King et al., 2008).
Some literature supports an increase in sedentary activities after exercise, which could ultimately result in no change in total EE, and may in part explain why the effects of exercise on BW are dampened (Goran and Poehlman, 1992; Morio et al., 1998; Meijer et al., 1999, 2000; Colley et al., 2009). However, NSPA (transportation, work-related movement and/or activity of daily living) (Levine, 2004; Levine et al., 2006; Colley et al., 2009) has also been found to remain unchanged (Blaak et al., 1992; McLaughlin et al., 2006; Rangan et al., 2011; Martin et al., 2019; Myers et al., 2019), or to increase (Meijer et al., 1991; Westerterp et al., 1992; Hollowell et al., 2009) in response to exercise interventions.
Exercise intensity could also influence changes in body energy stores. Results from lean and overweight individuals suggest that BW decreased to a greater extent in response to lower intensity (LOW) than from higher intensity (HI) exercise intervention (Grediagin et al., 1995; Mougios et al., 2006). In contrast, no changes in BW and BC were noted following exercise training performed at either LOW or HI in individuals living with obesity (Hinkleman and Nieman, 1993; van Aggel-Leijssen et al., 2001, 2002). Collectively, these observations suggest that multiple factors may hinder exercise-induced weight loss (Myers et al., 2014). There is a need to more clearly understand the impact of exercise on factors that could ultimately influence EI and total EE.
The objective of this study was to provide a comprehensive investigation of the impact of an equicaloric (1500 kcal/week) 3-month exercise intervention performed at LOW or moderate (MOD) intensity (40% VȮ2reserve or 60% VȮ2reserve, respectively) on EC. It was hypothesized that women training at MOD would have higher EC across the intervention due to greater EI and to a decrease in NSPA, when compared to women training at LOW.
Subjects and Methods
Participants
Twenty-five premenopausal women with overweight/obesity aged between 19 and 51 years were recruited to participate in a 3-month supervised exercise intervention. The inclusion criteria were as follows: (1) regular menstrual cycle; (2) non-smoker; (3) body mass index (BMI) higher than 27 kg/m2; (4) reported weight stability (±2 kg) for ≥2 months prior to enrolment in the study; and (5) physically inactive (practice of exercise <150 min/week). All participants were healthy with no orthopedic limitations, were not taking any medications that had the potential to impact study outcomes and had no history of alcohol (<2 drinks/day) or drug abuse. As presented in Figure 1, 4 women withdrew from the study due to personal reasons (n = 2), dieting (n = 1) and irregular menstrual cycle (n = 1). As such, twenty-one participants were included in the analyses. Participants were not aware of the true purpose of this study. They were told that the objective was to determine how the program will increase their physical fitness. This study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki and the University of Ottawa ethics committees approved all procedures involving human subjects. Written informed consent was obtained from all subjects.
Experimental Procedures
This study had three data collection phases: 7-day phase at baseline, which was completed 1 month before the exercise intervention (wk -4); a 14-day phase at the onset of the exercise intervention (wk 1, wk 2); and a 7-day phase at the end of the 3-month exercise intervention (wk 12) (Figure 2). Women were tested between days 1–9 of the follicular phase of the menstrual cycle at all sampling points. Due to the different length of the menstrual cycle, the testing at the end of the exercise intervention varied between wk 12 and wk 14. This is also the reason for which 1 month elapsed between baseline testing and the onset of the exercise intervention.
In Laboratory Morning Testing (wk -4, wk 1, wk 12–14)
On the first day of each phase, BW and BC (7:50 am) and resting EE (8:00–9:00 am) were measured. Anthropometric measurements are described elsewhere (McNeil et al., 2012). BC was measured with a DXA system (GE-LUNAR Prodigy module; GE Medical Systems, Madison, WI, United States). Coefficients of variation and correlation for %BF measured in twelve subjects tested in our laboratory were 1.8% and r = 0.99, respectively.
Resting energy expenditure (REE) was assessed by indirect calorimetry using the ventilated hood technique and the Vmax Encore 29 N metabolic cart (SensorMedics Corporation, Yorba Linda, CA, United States). The metabolic carts were calibrated against 95% O2/5% CO2 reference gas at the beginning of each day. Concentrations of CO2 and O2 were measured during 30 min (5 min of acclimatization and the average of the following 6th–25th min were included in the analysis) and Weir’s equation (Weir, 1949) was used to determine the caloric equivalents of oxygen consumption. Measurements were taken between 7:00 and 9:30 am after 12-h fasting period, while also abstaining from exercise for at least 24 h. This measurement was always done in the same room, which was kept dark and at an ambient temperature of ∼21° Celsius. After a 20–30-min resting period in the supine position, 30 min of measurements were conducted, whereas only the last 20 min were used in the calculation of REE. Ethanol burning tests are routinely performed to control the quality of measurements, as previously described (Cooper et al., 2009; Blond et al., 2011). Our most recent analyses demonstrated −5% and 4.76% difference in CO2 and O2 measures, respectively.
Appetite (i.e., desire to eat, hunger, fullness and prospective consumption) was measured using visual analog scales before (9:10 am) and after (9:30 am) breakfast (Hill et al., 1984; Marsh-Richard et al., 2009).
Breakfast (9:15 am) included ad libitum whole wheat bread, strawberry jam, peanut butter, cheddar cheese and orange juice. The amount (kcal) and the food selected at wk -4 were used for the following two sampling points (wk 1 and week 12–14). As such, breakfast was standardized for each individual but differed from one individual to the next.
Eating behavior traits (9:35 am) were measured with the Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire (Stunkard and Messick, 1985). The LFPQ, a forced choice computer task (9:45 am) was used to evaluate the implicit hedonic wanting (speed and frequency of choice) and the explicit hedonic liking (subjective VAS rating) for different visual food cues, which varied in both fat and sugar content (Finlayson et al., 2007).
Instructions for urine sample collection (DLW) and for food diaries were then provided to participant (10:00 am). At baseline (wk -4), the hour that followed (10:45–11:45 am) comprised of a sedentary session (e.g., writing, reading, studying, etc.), while exercise was performed on the first day of wk 1 and wk 12–14.
In Laboratory Afternoon Testing (wk -4, wk 1, wk 12–14)
Appetite was measured before and after lunch (11:45 am and 1:00 pm), while the LFPQ was performed before lunch (11:50 am).
Ad libitum intake was measured with a food menu that was presented to participants (12:00 pm), so that they could choose the foods and beverages that they wanted to consume (McNeil et al., 2012). After lunch, participants were asked to choose (1:15 pm) foods and beverages offered ad libitum from the same food menu for the rest of the day as well as for the following day (1.5 days).
Free-Living Environment (wk -4, wk 1, wk 12–14)
Participants received the foods they had selected (2:00 pm) and were instructed to bring back all leftovers and wrappers. Participants were also instructed as to how to complete the 7-day food diaries.
Participants were fitted with two triaxial accelerometers around the arm and thigh (SenseWear Pro 3 Armbands©, HealthWear Bodymedia, Pittsburgh, PA, United States). Data from the accelerometers were used to measure the time spent in different activities (i.e., time spent walking, standing, sitting and lying down). The following method was developed and validated in our laboratory (Riou et al., 2015b). Briefly, the data (acceleration axes and step counts) were obtained from accelerometers worn by participants. The INNERVIEW software (version 4.02; Bodymedia, Pittsburgh, PA) was used to extract the data obtained from accelerometers. Training data were exported in two Comma-Separated Values (CSV) files: one file for the accelerometer worn on the arm and one for the accelerometer worn on the thigh. Activity Recognition software was used to combine and to synchronize these two training data files, which produced a single file containing a sequence of training data samples. Then, the SVM model was used to classify each data sample as an activity (e.g., classified as variants of lying down, standing and sitting). The total time spent performing each activity was determined as the product of the sampling rate (5 s) and the number of occurrences of the different activities.
Urine samples were collected on the following morning as well as on the morning of day eight in order to assess total EE with DLW (Schoeller and van Santen, 1982). As previously described by St-Onge et al. (2007), we used differential loss of the 2H and 18O isotopes of water to integrate carbon dioxide production over time in free-living subjects. Following the administration of deuterated water, 2H is lost from the body water at an exponential rate. When 18O-water is administered, the 18O is also lost with body water turnover (as per the 2H isotope) and with each molecule of carbon dioxide produced because carbonic anhydrase in the body rapidly facilitates the equilibrium exchange of water and carbon dioxide/carbonic acid oxygen (Speakman et al., 1993; Racette et al., 1994). The difference between the rates of disappearance of 2H and 18O corresponds to the total carbon dioxide production over that period. The rates of disappearance are determined from urine samples taken at the start and at the end of the measurement period. Based on results from food journals administered at baseline and after the intervention, the food quotient was calculated and used to establish oxygen consumption and to consequently obtain a value for daily EE. Daily energy spent performing physical activity was calculated by subtracting REE from daily EE, while assuming that the thermic effect of food (TEF) remained stable (at 10% of daily EE). The DLW measurement periods generated 5 urine samples per subject: a pre-dose sample, 2 samples obtained after the 2H218O dose has initially equilibrated in the body (post-dose samples 1 and 2), and 2 at the end of the collection period (post-dose samples 3 and 4). Post-dose samples 1 and 2 were collected on day 1, 16–24 h after the dose of DLW on day 0. There was a minimum of 30 min and a maximum of 4 h between post-dose samples 1 and 2 as well as between post-dose samples 3 and 4. All samples were measured in triplicate for 18O-water and for 2H-water. An Isoprime Stable Isotope Ratio Mass Spectrometer connected to a Multiflow-Bio module for Isoprime and a Gilson 222XL Autosampler (GV Instruments, Manchester, United Kingdom) was used. Data processing was performed with MassLynx 3.6 software (Waters Corp, Milford, MA). Stability tests were performed each day before testing, which yielded an SD of 0.026% for deuterium and 0.004% for 18O. Known reference materials—Vienna-Standard Mean Ocean Water (V-SMOW), Greenland Ice Sheet Program (GISP), Standard Light Antarctic Precipitation (SLAP), and International Atomic Energy Agency standards (IAEA-304A and IAEA-304B)—were used for calibration and data normalization. Isotope ratio analysis results were reported as delta relative to a reference gas.
Free-Living Environment (wk 2)
For the first 2 days of wk 2, participants were asked to self-select foods from the menu and received two lunch boxes containing foods selected. They were instructed to wear the armbands and to fill out the food diary diaries for a second week. Urine samples were collected on the last day of this second week in order to assess total EE with the DLW method.
Cardiorespiratory Fitness (VȮ2peak)
A progressive exercise stress test was performed to measure participants’ peak maximal oxygen consumption (VȮ2peak) at wk -4, 6 wk after the onset of exercise and at wk 12–14. Breath-by-breath samples of expired air were collected with a mouthpiece, and measurements of VO2 and respiratory exchange ratio were automatically collected using a Vmax 229 series metabolic cart (SensorMedics Corporation, Yorba Linda, CA, United States). Each test lasted between 8 and 12 min and we followed the ramp medium protocol test on a treadmill until exhaustion. The ramp medium protocol test on a treadmill was used until exhaustion. The test was terminated when at least 2 of the following criteria were reached (Whaley et al., 2006): (1) predicted maximal HR reached, (2) respiratory quotient >1.1, (3) oxygen consumption remained stable or decreased with an increase in workload, or (4) rate of Borg-type scale reached ≥19. The VȮ2reserve = [(VȮ2peak–resting O2 ml min–1) × Intensity (40% (LOW) or 60% (MOD)) + resting O2 ml min–1] was used to prescribe the corresponding target HR for the exercise intervention (HR-VȮ2reserve regressions).
Exercise Intervention
The 21 participants were randomly assigned to one of the two intensity exercise interventions. Eleven participants trained at LOW (40% of the VȮ2reserve) while 10 participants trained at MOD (60% of the VȮ2reserve). The intensity of each training session was precisely monitored with a HR monitor (Polar RS300) to achieve 300 kcal per exercise session (as described above), which is consistent with the recommendations given by the ACSM for weight loss and health benefits (Donnelly et al., 2009). Participants took part in 3 supervised training sessions in the laboratory and performed 2 by themselves. Most trained on a treadmill and while 2 participants (MOD) used a cycle-ergometer. HR was recorded (Polar RS300) for all sessions for the entire intervention. The target HR was set to ± 10 beats of the prescribed HR. When HR deviated outside of this range, treadmill speed and/or incline was adjusted accordingly (resistance and/or cadence for the cycle ergometer). HR data was downloaded weekly to track compliance and to determine average HR/session. The average HR/session was used to determine the VO2 value derived from the HR-VO2 relationship collected during the VO2max. The total net energy cost of exercise, which includes every training session, and is calculated as “exercise EE – resting EE,” was then calculated using the Weir formula (Weir, 1949). To account for training adaptation, VȮ2peak performed at wk -4 was used to adjust the intensity of the exercise intervention for the first 6 weeks while the VȮ2peak performed at 6 wk after the onset of exercise was used to adjust for the remainder of the study. Due to the variable length of the menstrual cycle and since all testing were performed during the follicular phase, women trained on average for 14 ± 2 weeks.
Energy Compensation
EC was calculated from the total exercise EE (kcal) and changes in FM and FFM (kg converted to kcal) over the course of the intervention (wk 12–14 – wk 1) (Riou et al., 2015a). Due to technical problems with measurements obtained from the DXA at wk 1 (n = 1), data were taken from wk -4. Changes in body energy were calculated as follows: a gain/loss of 1 kg of FM corresponds to 9500 kcal, while it corresponds to 1200 kcal for FFM (Hall, 2008). A decrease in FM and/or FFM was entered as a negative value, whereas a gain in these variables was entered as a positive value. EC (%) was thus calculated as follows:
A compensation of 0% represents BC variations perfectly aligned with exercise EE. In contrast, a compensation of 100% indicates that BC remained the same despite exercise EE. Finally, when compensation is negative body energy stores are reduced beyond what is expected from the amount of energy spent during exercise.
Statistical Analysis
Descriptive variables are presented as means ± SD. Normality was assessed with a Shapiro-Wilk test with variables and residuals. Q-Q plots were also generated to investigate the normality of the distribution. Differences between LOW and MOD for baseline characteristics (Table 1), exercise intervention (Table 2) and EC were assessed using an independent sample t-test. In the case of violations of normality, a Mann-Whitney U-Test was performed. A two-way mixed model ANOVA with a within-subject factor of “phase” (wk 1 and wk 12–14) and a between-subjects factor of “intensity” [2 levels: LOW (± 40% of VȮ2reserve and MOD: ± 60% of VȮ2reserve)] was used to analyze the dependent variables of BC (Figure 3). A two-way mixed model ANOVA with a within-subject factor of “phase” (wk -1, wk 1, wk 2, and wk 12–14) and a between-subject factor of “intensity” [2 levels: LOW (± 40% of VȮ2reserve and MOD: ± 60% of VȮ2reserve)] was used to analyze the dependent variables of EI and macronutrients, subjective indicators of appetite, eating behavior traits and food reward as well as EE (Figure 4). When significant differences were found, post hoc test analyses were performed with paired t-tests and Bonferroni corrections were applied for multiple comparisons.
TABLE 1.
LOW | MOD | P-values | Cohen’s d | |
n | 11 | 10 | ||
Age (y) | 27 ± 9 | 31 ± 11 | 0.281 | 0.40 |
Body composition | ||||
Body weight (kg) | 88.1 ± 12.0 | 94.9 ± 21.1 | 0.37 | 0.40 |
BMI (kg⋅m-2) | 32.3 ± 3.8 | 35.1 ± 6.2 | 0.22 | 0.55 |
Fat mass (kg) | 41.9 ± 8.2 | 45.0 ± 14.2 | 0.55 | 0.27 |
Percent fat mass (%) | 47.9 ± 3.4 | 47.1 ± 5.3 | 0.71 | 0.18 |
Fat free mass (kg) | 45.0 ± 4.3 | 48.5 ± 6.7 | 0.17 | 0.63 |
Energy Intake (kcal) | ||||
n | 11 | 8 | ||
Breakfast | 648 ± 166 | 746 ± 256 | 0.33 | 0.47 |
Lunch | 772 ± 322 | 676 ± 330 | 0.54 | 0.30 |
Day 1 | 2620 ± 780 | 2842 ± 959 | 0.59 | 0.26 |
Day 2 | 2418 ± 659 | 2443 ± 1050 | 0.95 | 0.03 |
Day 3–7 | 2048 ± 378 | 2290 ± 718 | 0.40 | 0.44 |
Day 1–7 | 2182 ± 410 | 2391 ± 714 | 0.48 | 0.38 |
Energy Expenditure (kcal/day) | ||||
n | 11 | 10 | ||
REE | 1469 ± 185 | 1623 ± 285 | 0.16 | 0.65 |
NSPA | 701 ± 272 | 985 ± 371 | 0.06 | 0.88 |
TEE | 2411 ± 289 | 2898 ± 596 | 0.042 | 1.06 |
Eating behaviors traits | ||||
n | 11 | 8 | ||
Dietary restraint | 8.6 ± 3.2 | 9.4 ± 2.7 | 0.61 | 0.27 |
Flexible dietary restraint | 2.0 ± 0.8 | 1.8 ± 1.0 | 0.481 | 0.23 |
Rigid dietary restraint | 1.6 ± 1.4 | 2.5 ± 0.5 | 0.671 | 0.80 |
Disinhibition | 8.5 ± 2.9 | 8.5 ± 1.5 | 0.97 | 0.02 |
Hunger | 7.0 ± 2.6 | 7.5 ± 2.9 | 0.70 | 0.18 |
Values are mean ± SD. LOW, lower intensity; MOD, moderate intensity; BMI, body mass index; REE, resting EE; NSPA, non-structured physical activity; TEE, total EE 1A Mann Whitney U-Test was performed due to the non-normal distribution of the variables. 2When Total EE was expressed as a function of BW, no significant difference was noted between both groups (LOW = 30.12 ± 3.97 (n = 11) vs. MOD = 28.08 ± 4.08 (n = 10); p = 0.26, cohen’s d = 0.51).
TABLE 2.
LOW | MOD | P-values | Cohen’s d | |
n | 11 | 10 | ||
Characteristics of the exercise EE intervention | ||||
Weeks of training | 14 ± 1 | 15 ± 2 | 0.241 | 0.64 |
Estimated training sessions | 68 ± 6 | 75 ± 12 | 0.16 | 0.75 |
Training sessions completed | 68 ± 6 | 71 ± 12 | 0.58 | 0.32 |
Compliance (%) | 100 ± 2 | 95 ± 6 | 0.031 | 1.14 |
Exercise EE per session (kcal) | 352 ± 20 | 343 ± 27 | 0.40 | 0.38 |
Exercise above REE per session (kcal) | 290 ± 18 | 292 ± 25 | 0.87 | 0.09 |
Total exercise EE for the intervention (kcal) | 23902 ± 2584 | 24015 ± 4173 | 0.94 | 0.03 |
Time spent exercising (min)/session | 62 ± 6 | 46 ± 6 | <0.000 | 2.67 |
VȮ2peak (ml/kg/min) | 32 ± 4 | 30 ± 4 | 0.38 | 0.5 |
Mean HR during exercise (bpm) | 117 ± 10 | 134 ± 15 | 0.006 | 1.35 |
Values are mean ± SD. NS, not significant; LOW, lower intensity; MOD, moderate intensity; EE, energy expenditure; REE, Resting EE; VȮ2peak, participants’ peak maximal oxygen consumption; HR, heart rate. 1A Mann Whitney U-Test was performed due to the non-normal distribution of the variables.
Prior to the analyses, imputations (Elliott and Hawthorne, 2005) (i.e., mean of LOW and mean MOD group at a specific time) were performed for 6 samples: 3 for the DLW (i.e., sampling limited in isotopes, problem with the time of urine collected), 2 for exercise EE and 1 for resting EE. NSPA was obtained by subtracting resting EE, TEF (estimated at 10% of total EE) and exercise EE from total EE. Data obtained from accelerometers were used to measure the time spent performing different activities (i.e., time spent walking, standing, sitting and lying down). To be included into the analyses, accelerometers had to be worn for more than 65% of the day (936 min over 1440 min). For these analyses, 8 and 6 participants were part of the LOW and MOD groups. Data obtained across the intervention (wk -4, wk 1, wk 2 and wk 12–14) were presented as the mean of time spent in different activities on the day where a training was or was not performed.
Results
Baseline Participants’ Characteristics and BC Changes
Baseline characteristics are shown in Table 1, while exercise data are presented in Table 2. No main effects, no group effects and no interactions were noted for maximal oxygen consumption (data not shown). The analyses revealed a combined EC of 51 ± 173% and no significant difference between LOW and MOD participants, respectively [49 ± 79% (n = 11) and 53 ± 244% (n = 10) (cohen’s d = 0.02)]. However, further analyses, based on median comparison (p = 0.086) and boxplot, demonstrated extreme variables. When removed, a significant difference was noted for EC between groups [LOW: 49 ± 79% (n = 11) and MOD: 161 ± 88% (n = 8), p = 0.010, cohen’s d = 1.35]. Analyses suggested significant group interactions for body FM (p = 0.020, cohen’s f2 = 0.278) and BW (p = 0.019, cohen’s f2 = 0.283) (Figure 3). Since the extreme negative compensation of the two outliers was almost certainly explained by the addition of a caloric restriction, these participants were not included in the analyses of EI, subjective indicators of appetite, eating behavior traits and food reward (LOW, n = 11 and MOD, n = 8). However, all participants, including the outliers for EC, were included in the analyses of EE variables (LOW, n = 11 and MOD, n = 10).
Energy Intake, Macronutrient and Subjective Indicators of Appetite
No significant differences were noted between women training at LOW and MOD for EI and macronutrient at breakfast. No main effect of phase, group, or group by phase interactions were noted for EI and macronutrient at lunch, on day 1 and 2 or for the mean value over 7 days (i.e., 2 days of food menu and 5 days of food diary).
After the exercise session, women training at MOD had a higher degree of fullness and had lower mean prospective food consumption when compared to women training at LOW (data not shown). After lunch, a trend (p = 0.083, cohen’s f2 = 0.162) was notice for mean prospective food consumption during the intervention (i.e., 4 ± 5 mm, 6 ± 7 mm, 13 ± 22 mm, respectively). Appreciation of the foods consumed from the food menu after breakfast and after lunch was similar across the intervention with no significant effect of group, or group by phases interaction (82 ± 18 mm and 89 ± 11 mm, respectively).
Eating Behavior Traits (TFEQ) and Food Reward
Hunger and external hunger significantly increased (p = 0.002, cohen’s f2 = 0.315 and p = 0.000, cohen’s f2 = 0.436, respectively for both traits) from wk -4 to wk 1 (p = 0.032 and p = 0.000, respectively) and from wk -4 to wk 12 (p = 0.004 and p = 0.002, respectively). No significant difference was noticed for other eating behavior traits. Implicit wanting for fat decreased from wk 1 to wk 12–14 (p = 0.034, cohen’s f2 = 0.237). Implicit wanting for fat (p = 0.000, cohen’s f2 = 0521) and explicit liking for fat (p = 0.025, cohen’s f2 = 0.261) significantly decreased after the exercise session when compared to before. Women in the LOW group demonstrated higher implicit wanting for fat when compared to women training in the MOD group (p = 0.054, cohen’s f2 = 0.202). Following the exercise session, a trend was noticed for implicit wanting for savory foods increased (vs. sweet) (p = 0.063, cohen’s f2 = 0.189). An interaction between phase and time (p = 0.002, cohen’s f2 = 0.443) was shown for explicit liking for savory foods. When compared to before the exercise session, explicit liking for savory foods increased after the exercise session at wk 1, while it decreased at wk 12–14.
Energy Expenditure and Time Spent Performing Activities
Components of total EE (resting EE, NSPA, exercise induced EE) are presented in Figure 4. No significant difference was noted for resting EE and total EE across the intervention. A trend (p = 0.055, cohen’s f2 = 0.181) between LOW and MOD group was found for total EE, where MOD participants tended to have greater total EE when compared to LOW participants. No interaction was observed. Significant main effects of phase (p = 0.002, cohen’s f2 = 0.299) (Figure 4) were noted for NSPA. Post hoc comparisons revealed significant decreases between wk -4 and wk 1 (p = 0.006), wk -4 and wk 2 (p = 0.036) and wk -4 and wk 12–14 (p < 0.000).
Time spent walking significantly increased (p < 0.000, cohen’s f2 = 0.446) from wk -4 to wk 1 (p < 0.003), wk -4 to wk 2 (p = 0.003), wk 12–14 (p = 0.021). LOW participants spent significantly more time walking then MOD participants (p = 0.046, cohen’s f2 = 0.292). An interaction was noted for the time spent lying down (p = 0.044, cohen’s f2 = 0.199). Across the intervention, MOD participants significantly increased their time spent lying down while LOW participants decreased it. No other differences were observed for posture allocation (i.e., sitting, standing, running).
Discussion
This study measured EC in women living with overweight/obesity training at either LOW or MOD over the course of a 3-month (12–14 wks) intervention. Our data suggest that on average, exercise EE was almost entirely compensated (96%), an effect that was much greater in women training at MOD (161%) when compared to women training at LOW (49%). DLW samples were taken during 4 of the 12 wks of the protocol, which provide comprehensive coverage of the effects of exercise on EE. During the same period, EI was monitored with lunch boxes and 7-day dietary records, while body energy changes were measured with DXA. The close monitoring of EE from the exercise revealed a high level of compliance to the exercise sessions over the course of the study (∼97%). While EI measures did not change across the intervention, our results emphasized that total EE, measured with DLW, did not increase during the exercise intervention in either of the groups. What is more, this effect was noted as early as the onset of the exercise intervention (wk 1). As well, NSPA decreased across the intervention, independently of the intervention group.
Our study suggest that total EE remained stable across the intervention. More specifically, NSPA decreased early and remained lower during the entire exercise intervention. Results from previous studies have shown a decrease in NSPA in elderly subjects (Goran and Poehlman, 1992) in response to an exercise intervention as well as in overweight boys (Paravidino et al., 2016) following an acute bout of exercise. On the other hand, others have found that individuals living with overweight and obesity do not decrease the time spent performing sedentary activities (Willis et al., 2014). Our results also demonstrate that the amount of time spent lying down increased across the intervention for MOD while it decreased for LOW. In addition, the time spent walking increases in both group and the analyses suggested that the amount was significantly higher in LOW when compared to MOD. Nevertheless, our characterization of posture allocation did not align with the changes in NSPA measured with DLW. Even if our classification model was found to be accurate and reproducible (Riou et al., 2015b), it could be speculated that our wear time cut-off (65%) could have been too low to fully capture differences in posture allocation time.
Our results are in agreement with literature as far as showing that EI does not increase following acute exercise or over a 2-day period when compared to baseline values [(King et al., 1994, 1997; Imbeault et al., 1997); for reviews, see King (1999), Martins et al. (2008), King et al. (2012)]. However, for long-term exercise performed at a HI, we could have anticipated an increase in EI due to the documented high level of EI compensation under similar conditions (Stubbs et al., 2002b; Pomerleau et al., 2004; Whybrow et al., 2008; Martin et al., 2019; Myers et al., 2019). However, no significant effect was noted for time or group during the intervention. Similar results were also obtained in men and women living with overweight/obesity expending 1500 kcal/week or 3000 kcal/week during a 12 week intervention (Flack et al., 2018) and in an intervention designed to expend 300 kcal and 600 kcal for 13 wks in men with overweight (Rosenkilde et al., 2012).
When considering other important contributors to EI variability (King, 1999; King et al., 2012), such as hedonic responses (i.e., subjective indicators of appetite and eating behaviors traits), results are in line with the relatively stable EI observed during the intervention. Our analyses suggest that implicit hedonic wanting for fat and explicit hedonic liking for fat decreased after the exercise session, a result that persisted over time for implicit hedonic wanting for fat. Interestingly, comparable results were found in non-compensators after an acute exercise (Finlayson et al., 2009). The decrease for explicit liking for sweet before the exercise session, when compared to the increase after the exercise session between wk 1 and wk 12–14 is in accordance with an increased preference for carbohydrate following acute exercise (Westerterp-Plantenga et al., 1997). Additionally, subjective indicators of appetite revealed that women training at HI presented a higher acute degree of fullness and had lower mean prospective food consumption after the exercise session when compared to women training at LOW (Panissa et al., 2016). Finally, our results suggest that hunger and external hunger as assessed with the TFEQ, increased across the intervention. However, we previously demonstrated that this result was associated with EI among overweight women performing a lower level of physical activity but not related to EI for women with a higher physical activity level (Riou et al., 2011).
Because our results, EI measurements did not change significantly over the course of the study. It would seem logical to conclude that the compensation from exercise-induced EE was not mediated by variations EI. This perception is supported by the fact that we assessed dietary intake over 7 days at four different time points during the entire study with a combination of objective food intake (McNeil et al., 2012) and self-report. However, it is important to remember that any changes in EE, including those in NSPA, can only impact body energy stores if they create a prolonged energy imbalance. Along these lines, our results show that from baseline to the end of the intervention mean total EE for LOW essentially remained the same (∼+5 kcal/day or +490 kcal for the intervention), whereas a non-significant decrease in mean total EE was noted for MOD (∼−173 kcal/day or −18165 kcal for the intervention). Mean calculated body energy changes for the intervention were −8502 and +10379 kcal for LOW and MOD, respectively. This means that changes in EE cannot by themselves explain the changes in body energy stores that were measured, even if NSPA was significantly reduced for both groups. If we assume that our calculations are accurate, this means that EI was decreased by approximately ∼8012 kcal or 82 kcal/day (−8502+490 kcal) for LOW and by ∼7786 kcal or 74 kcal/day (10379–18165 kcal) for MOD during the entire trial. Arguably, such subtle daily differences are extremely challenging to reveal considering the sensitivity of the tools used to measure free-living EI in this study and elsewhere. These numbers also highlight the inherent challenges related to the precise evaluation of EI in the context of comprehensive energy balance studies (Schoeller et al., 2013).
Since the BMI at inclusion was >27, these conclusions cannot be extended to the general population. The high quantity and quality of the variables measured lead to the inclusion of a relatively smaller number of participants. Consequently, the results have a lower statistical power. Nevertheless, the low effect size noted for important primary outcomes suggests that a higher number of participants would have very likely led to similar results. It has been suggested that greater weight loss could have been obtained with a larger dose of exercise (Jakicic et al., 2003). However, it should be emphasized that three out of five sessions were performed and monitored in the laboratory and that two other sessions were carefully scrutinized with a HR monitor. Exercise compliance was 97% for this study. Since measurement with indirect calorimetry would have been too laborious (21 participants × 5 trainings per week× 3 months), the estimation of exercise EE is a limitation of this study. As well, exercise-induced increases in RMR (EPOC effect) was not considered in this study. Nevertheless, as discussed in the review from LaForgia et al. (2006), a sufficient stimulus (for example, a 50 min at 70% of VO2max) would be enough to create an additional deficit of 6–15% of the net total oxygen cost of the exercise. Considering these values, the EPOC from our study would be between 17 and 44 kcal, and would have very little impact on EC. Regarding EC, it is possible that LBM and total body water gains early into an exercise program can mask healthy BC changes. However, it is important to note that the values of BC that we used in our calculations were obtained at baseline and after the completion of the study, 3 months later. It is reasonable to assume that shifts in body water, would have had time to re-equilibrate over this time period. Additionally, two participants mostly trained on an ergometer, which could have reduced their EE across the intervention. The absence of change for the fitness level (VO2peak) across the intervention might suggest that the intensity was not high enough to cause an increase in oxygen consumption. Nevertheless, exercise intensities between 40 and 59% is normally recommended for sedentary individuals (2006). As well, the ramp type protocol used to prescribe the training program does not provide the steady state data necessary for accurately predict the HR/VO2 relationship. However, as mentioned earlier, the subject burden in this study was already very high. This would have entailed to add 3 additional exercise tests during the study, which was not feasible from a logistical standpoint. In addition, we used the same approach in both groups, so we can assume that the inflation of the caloric cost of exercise was the same for both groups. Finally, even though data were collected with the most available objective measures (i.e., DLW, DXA and accelerometry) and included both sides of the energy balance, the limitations associated with the non-continuous measures of EI and EE should not be underestimated, as previously discussed.
Data suggested a total EC of 96 % and a higher compensation in women training at MOD when compared to women training at LOW. This study adds new perspectives by suggesting that NSPA should be taken into consideration when investigating changes in body energy stores in response to exercise interventions. Finally, our study highlighted the limitations associated with the use of repeated acute measures to predict long-term outcomes, especially when the measurement of EI is involved.
Author Contributions
M-ÈR and ÉD designed the research. M-ÈR, SJ-T, and J-CG conducted the research. M-ÈR, GF, JB, and ÉD conceptualized the analysis of the data. M-ÈR and GL analyzed the data. M-ÈR wrote the manuscript. SJ-T, GL, GF, JB, LD-S, J-CG, and ÉD critically appraised and approved the final version of the manuscript. ED had primary responsibility for final content.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank the participants for their devoted participation. We especially want to thank Dr. François Rioux for his contribution to the analysis of the time spent in different activities.
Abbreviations
- BC
body composition
- BW
body weight
- DLW
doubly labeled water
- DXA
dual-energy X-ray absorptiometry
- EC
energy compensation
- EE
energy expenditure
- EI
energy intake
- FFM
fat free mass
- FM
fat mass
- HR
heart rate
- LFPQ
leeds food preference questionnaire
- LOW
low intensity
- MOD
moderate intensity
- NSPA
non-structured physical activity
- TFEQ
three-factors eating questionnaire.
Footnotes
Funding. M-ÈR was a recipient of the Frederick Banting and Charles Best Doctoral Award (CIHR) for the entire duration of the study.
References
- Andersson B., Xu X. F., Rebuffe-Scrive M., Terning K., Krotkiewski M., Bjorntorp P. (1991). The effects of exercise, training on body composition and metabolism in men and women. Int. J. Obes. 15 75–81. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blaak E. E., Westerterp K. R., Bar-Or O., Wouters L. J., Saris W. H. (1992). Total energy expenditure and spontaneous activity in relation to training in obese boys. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 55 777–782. 10.1093/ajcn/55.4.777 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blond E., Maitrepierre C., Normand S., Sothier M., Laville M. (2011). A new indirect calorimeter is accurate and reliable for measuring basal energy expenditure, thermic effect of food and substrate oxidation in obese and healthy subjects. E. Spen. Eur. E. J. Clin. Nutr. Metab. 6 e7–e15. 10.1016/j.eclnm.2010.12.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Blundell J. E., Stubbs R. J., Hughes D. A., Whybrow S., King N. A. (2003). Cross talk between physical activity and appetite control: does physical activity stimulate appetite? Proc. Nutr. Soc. 62 651–661. 10.1079/pns2003286 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caudwell P., Finlayson G., Gibbons C., Hopkins M., King N., Naslund E., et al. (2013). Resting metabolic rate is associated with hunger, self-determined meal size, and daily energy intake and may represent a marker for appetite. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 97 7–14. 10.3945/ajcn.111.029975 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Colley R. C., Hills A. P., King N. A., Byrne N. M. (2009). Exercise-induced energy expenditure: implications for exercise prescription and obesity. Patient Educ. Couns. 79 327–332. 10.1016/j.pec.2010.03.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cooper J. A., Watras A. C., O’Brien M. J., Luke A., Dobratz J. R., Earthman C. P., et al. (2009). Assessing validity and reliability of resting metabolic rate in six gas analysis systems. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 109 128–132. 10.1016/j.jada.2008.10.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Donnelly J. E., Blair S. N., Jakicic J. M., Manore M. M., Rankin J. W., Smith B. K. (2009). American college of sports medicine position stand. Appropriate physical activity intervention strategies for weight loss and prevention of weight regain for adults. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 41 459–471. 10.1249/MSS.0b013e3181949333 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elliott P., Hawthorne G. (2005). Imputing missing repeated measures data: how should we proceed? Aust. N. Z. J. Psychiatry 39 575–582. 10.1111/j.1440-1614.2005.01629.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Finlayson G., Bryant E., Blundell J. E., King N. A. (2009). Acute compensatory eating following exercise is associated with implicit hedonic wanting for food. Physiol. Behav. 97 62–67. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.02.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Finlayson G., King N., Blundell J. E. (2007). Is it possible to dissociate ’liking’ and ’wanting’ for foods in humans? A novel experimental procedure. Physiol. Behav. 90 36–42. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.08.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Flack K. D., Ufholz K. E., Johnson L. K., Fitzgerald J. S., Roemmich J. N. (2018). Energy compensation in response to aerobic exercise training in overweight adults. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 315 R619–R626. 10.1152/ajpregu.00071.2018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- George V. A., Morganstein A. (2003). Effect of moderate intensity exercise on acute energy intake in normal and overweight females. Appetite 40 43–46. 10.1016/s0195-6663(02)00146-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goran M. I., Poehlman E. T. (1992). Endurance training does not enhance total energy expenditure in healthy elderly persons. Am. J. Physiol. 263(5 Pt 1), E950–E957. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grediagin A., Cody M., Rupp J., Benardot D., Shern R. (1995). Exercise intensity does not effect body composition change in untrained, moderately overfat women. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 95 661–665. 10.1016/s0002-8223(95)00181-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hagobian T. A., Sharoff C. G., Stephens B. R., Wade G. N., Silva J. E., Chipkin S. R., et al. (2009). Effects of exercise on energy-regulating hormones and appetite in men and women. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 296 R233–R242. 10.1152/ajpregu.90671.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hall K. D. (2008). What is the required energy deficit per unit weight loss? Int. J. Obes. 32 573–576. 10.1038/sj.ijo.0803720 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hill A. J., Magson L. D., Blundell J. E. (1984). Hunger and palatability: tracking ratings of subjective experience before, during and after the consumption of preferred and less preferred food. Appetite 5 361–371. 10.1016/s0195-6663(84)80008-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hill J. O., Melby C., Johnson S. L., Peters J. C. (1995). Physical activity and energy requirements. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 62(5 Suppl.), 1059S–1066S. 10.1093/ajcn/62.5.1059S [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hinkleman L. L., Nieman D. C. (1993). The effects of a walking program on body composition and serum lipids and lipoproteins in overweight women. J. Sports Med. Phys. Fitness 33 49–58. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hollowell R. P., Willis L. H., Slentz C. A., Topping J. D., Bhakpar M., Kraus W. E. (2009). Effects of exercise training amount on physical activity energy expenditure. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 41 1640–1644. 10.1249/MSS.0b013e31819c71a4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hopkins M., Blundell J. E., King N. A. (2013). Individual variability in compensatory eating following acute exercise in overweight and obese women. Br. J. Sports Med. 48 1472–1476. 10.1136/bjsports-2012-091721 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Imbeault P., Saint-Pierre S., Almeras N., Tremblay A. (1997). Acute effects of exercise on energy intake and feeding behaviour. Br. J. Nutr. 77 511–521. 10.1079/bjn19970053 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jakicic J. M., Marcus B. H., Gallagher K. I., Napolitano M., Lang W. (2003). Effect of exercise duration and intensity on weight loss in overweight, sedentary women: a randomized trial. JAMA 290 1323–1330. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- King N. A. (1999). What processes are involved in the appetite response to moderate increases in exercise-induced energy expenditure? Proc. Nutr. Soc. 58 107–113. 10.1079/pns19990015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- King N. A., Burley V. J., Blundell J. E. (1994). Exercise-induced suppression of appetite: effects on food intake and implications for energy balance. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 48 715–724. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- King N. A., Hopkins M., Caudwell P., Stubbs R. J., Blundell J. E. (2008). Individual variability following 12 weeks of supervised exercise: identification and characterization of compensation for exercise-induced weight loss. Int. J. Obes. 32 177–184. 10.1038/sj.ijo.0803712 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- King N. A., Horner K., Hills A. P., Byrne N. M., Wood R. E., Bryant E., et al. (2012). Exercise, appetite and weight management: understanding the compensatory responses in eating behaviour and how they contribute to variability in exercise-induced weight loss. Br. J. Sports Med. 46 315–322. 10.1136/bjsm.2010.082495 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- King N. A., Lluch A., Stubbs R. J., Blundell J. E. (1997). High dose exercise does not increase hunger or energy intake in free living males. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 51 478–483. 10.1038/sj.ejcn.1600432 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- King N. A., Snell L., Smith R. D., Blundell J. E. (1996). Effects of short-term exercise on appetite responses in unrestrained females. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 50 663–667. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kissileff H. R., Pi-Sunyer F. X., Segal K., Meltzer S., Foelsch P. A. (1990). Acute effects of exercise on food intake in obese and nonobese women. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 52 240–245. 10.1093/ajcn/52.2.240 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LaForgia J., Withers R. T., Gore C. J. (2006). Effects of exercise intensity and duration on the excess post-exercise oxygen consumption. J. Sports Sci. 24 1247–1264. 10.1080/02640410600552064 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levine J. A. (2004). Nonexercise activity thermogenesis (NEAT): environment and biology. Am. J. Physiol. Endocrinol. Metab. 286 E675–E685. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levine J. A., Vander Weg M. W., Hill J. O., Klesges R. C. (2006). Non-exercise activity thermogenesis: the crouching tiger hidden dragon of societal weight gain. Arterioscler. Thromb. Vasc. Biol. 26 729–736. 10.1161/01.atv.0000205848.83210.73 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lluch A., King N. A., Blundell J. E. (1998). Exercise in dietary restrained women: no effect on energy intake but change in hedonic ratings. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 52 300–307. 10.1038/sj.ejcn.1600555 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lluch A., King N. A., Blundell J. E. (2000). No energy compensation at the meal following exercise in dietary restrained and unrestrained women. Br. J. Nutr. 84 219–225. 10.1017/s0007114500001458 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marsh-Richard D. M., Hatzis E. S., Mathias C. W., Venditti N., Dougherty D. M. (2009). Adaptive visual analog scales (AVAS): a modifiable software program for the creation, administration, and scoring of visual analog scales. Behav. Res. Methods 41 99–106. 10.3758/BRM.41.1.99 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin C. K., Johnson W. D., Myers C. A., Apolzan J. W., Earnest C. P., Thomas D. M., et al. (2019). Effect of different doses of supervised exercise on food intake, metabolism, and non-exercise physical activity: the E-MECHANIC randomized controlled trial. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 108 1–10. 10.1093/ajcn/nqz054 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martins C., Morgan L., Truby H. (2008). A review of the effects of exercise on appetite regulation: an obesity perspective. Int. J. Obes. 32 1337–1347. 10.1038/ijo.2008.98 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McLaughlin R., Malkova D., Nimmo M. A. (2006). Spontaneous activity responses to exercise in males and females. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 60 1055–1061. 10.1038/sj.ejcn.1602417 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McNeil J., Riou M. E., Razmjou S., Cadieux S., Doucet E. (2012). Reproducibility of a food menu to measure energy and macronutrient intakes in a laboratory and under real-life conditions. Br. J. Nutr. 108 1316–1324. 10.1017/s0007114511006672 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meijer E. P., Westerterp K. R., Verstappen F. T. (1999). Effect of exercise training on total daily physical activity in elderly humans. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. Occup. Physiol. 80 16–21. 10.1007/s004210050552 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meijer E. P., Westerterp K. R., Verstappen F. T. (2000). Effect of exercise training on physical activity and substrate utilization in the elderly. Int. J. Sports Med. 21 499–504. 10.1055/s-2000-7419 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meijer G. A., Janssen G. M., Westerterp K. R., Verhoeven F., Saris W. H., ten Hoor F. (1991). The effect of a 5-month endurance-training programme on physical activity: evidence for a sex-difference in the metabolic response to exercise. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. Occup. Physiol. 62 11–17. 10.1007/bf00635626 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller W. C., Koceja D. M., Hamilton E. J. (1997). A meta-analysis of the past 25 years of weight loss research using diet, exercise or diet plus exercise intervention. Int. J. Obes. Relat. Metab. Disord. 21 941–947. 10.1038/sj.ijo.0800499 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morio B., Montaurier C., Pickering G., Ritz P., Fellmann N., Coudert J., et al. (1998). Effects of 14 weeks of progressive endurance training on energy expenditure in elderly people. Br. J. Nutr. 80 511–519. 10.1017/s0007114598001603 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mougios V., Kazaki M., Christoulas K., Ziogas G., Petridou A. (2006). Does the intensity of an exercise programme modulate body composition changes? Int. J. Sports Med. 27 178–181. 10.1055/s-2005-865625 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Myers A., Dalton M., Gibbons C., Finlayson G., Blundell J. (2019). Structured, aerobic exercise reduces fat mass and is partially compensated through energy intake but not energy expenditure in women. Physiol. Behav. 199 56–65. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.11.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Myers C. A., Johnson W. D., Earnest C. P., Rood J. C., Tudor-Locke C., Johannsen N. M., et al. (2014). Examination of mechanisms (E-MECHANIC) of exercise-induced weight compensation: study protocol for a randomized controlled trial. Trials 15:212. 10.1186/1745-6215-15-212 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Panissa V. L., Julio U. F., Hardt F., Kurashima C., Lira F. S., Takito M. Y., et al. (2016). Effect of exercise intensity and mode on acute appetite control in men and women. Appl. Physiol. Nutr. Metab. 41 1083–1091. 10.1139/apnm-2016-0172 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paravidino V. B., Mediano M. F., Hoffman D. J., Sichieri R. (2016). Effect of exercise intensity on spontaneous physical activity energy expenditure in overweight boys: a crossover study. PLoS One 11:e0147141. 10.1371/journal.pone.0147141 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pomerleau M., Imbeault P., Parker T., Doucet E. (2004). Effects of exercise intensity on food intake and appetite in women. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 80 1230–1236. 10.1093/ajcn/80.5.1230 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Racette S. B., Schoeller D. A., Luke A. H., Kushner R. F. (1994). Relative dilution spaces of 2H- and 18O-labeled water in humans. Am. J. Physiol. 267 585–590. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rangan V. V., Willis L. H., Slentz C. A., Bateman L. A., Shields A. T., Houmard J. A., et al. (2011). Effects of an eight-month exercise training program on off-exercise physical activity. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 43 1744–1751. 10.1249/mss.0b013e3182148a7e [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riou M. E., Doucet E., Provencher V., Weisnagel S. J., Piche M. E., Dube M. C., et al. (2011). Influence of physical activity participation on the associations between eating behaviour traits and body mass index in healthy postmenopausal women. J. Obes. 2011:465710. 10.1155/2011/465710 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riou M. E., Jomphe-Tremblay S., Lamothe G., Stacey D., Szczotka A., Doucet E. (2015a). Predictors of energy compensation during exercise interventions: a systematic review. Nutrients 7 3677–3704. 10.3390/nu7053677 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riou M. E., Rioux F., Lamothe G., Doucet E. (2015b). Validation and reliability of a classification method to measure the time spent performing different activities. PLoS One 10:e0128299. 10.1371/journal.pone.0128299 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosenkilde M., Auerbach P., Reichkendler M. H., Ploug T., Stallknecht B. M., Sjodin A. (2012). Body fat loss and compensatory mechanisms in response to different doses of aerobic exercise–a randomized controlled trial in overweight sedentary males. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 303 R571–R579. 10.1152/ajpregu.00141.2012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schoeller D. A., Thomas D., Archer E., Heymsfield S. B., Blair S. N., Goran M. I., et al. (2013). Self-report-based estimates of energy intake offer an inadequate basis for scientific conclusions. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 97 1413–1415. 10.3945/ajcn.113.062125 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schoeller D. A., van Santen E. (1982). Measurement of energy expenditure in humans by doubly labeled water method. J. Appl. Physiol. 53 955–959. 10.1152/jappl.1982.53.4.955 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Speakman J. R., Nair K. S., Goran M. I. (1993). Revised equations for calculating CO2 production from doubly labeled water in humans. Am. J. Physiol. 264 912–917. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- St-Onge M., Mignault D., Allison D. B., Rabasa-Lhoret R. (2007). Evaluation of a portable device to measure daily energy expenditure in free-living adults. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 85 742–749. 10.1093/ajcn/85.3.742 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stubbs R. J., Sepp A., Hughes D. A., Johnstone A. M., Horgan G. W., King N., et al. (2002a). The effect of graded levels of exercise on energy intake and balance in free-living men, consuming their normal diet. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 56 129–140. 10.1038/sj.ejcn.1601295 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stubbs R. J., Sepp A., Hughes D. A., Johnstone A. M., King N., Horgan G., et al. (2002b). The effect of graded levels of exercise on energy intake and balance in free-living women. Int. J. Obes. Relat. Metab. Disord. 26 866–869. 10.1038/sj.ijo.0801874 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stunkard A. J., Messick S. (1985). The three-factor eating questionnaire to measure dietary restraint, disinhibition and hunger. J. Psychosom. Res. 29 71–83. 10.1016/0022-3999(85)90010-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thomas D. M., Bouchard C., Church T., Slentz C., Kraus W. E., Redman L. M., et al. (2012). Why do individuals not lose more weight from an exercise intervention at a defined dose? An energy balance analysis. Obes. Rev. 13 835–847. 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2012.01012.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Todd Alan Hagobian N. E. (2012). Exercise and weight loss: what is the evidence of sex differences? Curr. Obes. Rep. 2 86–92. 10.1007/s13679-012-0035-6 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- van Aggel-Leijssen D. P., Saris W. H., Wagenmakers A. J., Hul G. B., van Baak M. A. (2001). The effect of low-intensity exercise training on fat metabolism of obese women. Obes. Res. 9 86–96. 10.1038/oby.2001.11 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Aggel-Leijssen D. P., Saris W. H., Wagenmakers A. J., Senden J. M., van Baak M. A. (2002). Effect of exercise training at different intensities on fat metabolism of obese men. J. Appl. Physiol. 92 1300–1309. 10.1152/japplphysiol.00030.2001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weir J. B. (1949). New methods for calculating metabolic rate with special reference to protein metabolism. J. Physiol. 109 1–9. 10.1113/jphysiol.1949.sp004363 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Westerterp K. R., Meijer G. A., Janssen E. M., Saris W. H., Ten Hoor F. (1992). Long-term effect of physical activity on energy balance and body composition. Br. J. Nutr. 68 21–30. 10.1079/bjn19920063 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Westerterp-Plantenga M. S., Verwegen C. R., Ijedema M. J., Wijckmans N. E., Saris W. H. (1997). Acute effects of exercise or sauna on appetite in obese and nonobese men. Physiol. Behav. 62 1345–1354. 10.1016/s0031-9384(97)00353-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whaley M. H., Brubaker P. H., Otto R. M., Armstrong L. E. (2006). American College of Sports Medicine. ACSM’s Guidelines for Exercise Testing and Prescription, 7th Edn, Vol. 10 Baltimore, MD: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, 141–149. [Google Scholar]
- Whybrow S., Hughes D. A., Ritz P., Johnstone A. M., Horgan G. W., King N., et al. (2008). The effect of an incremental increase in exercise on appetite, eating behaviour and energy balance in lean men and women feeding ad libitum. Br. J. Nutr. 100 1109–1115. 10.1017/S0007114508968240 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Willis E. A., Herrmann S. D., Honas J. J., Lee J., Donnelly J. E., Washburn R. A. (2014). Nonexercise energy expenditure and physical activity in the Midwest Exercise Trial 2. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 46 2286–2294. 10.1249/MSS.0000000000000354 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woo R., Garrow J. S., Pi-Sunyer F. X. (1982a). Effect of exercise on spontaneous calorie intake in obesity. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 36 470–477. 10.1093/ajcn/36.3.470 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woo R., Garrow J. S., Pi-Sunyer F. X. (1982b). Voluntary food intake during prolonged exercise in obese women. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 36 478–484. 10.1093/ajcn/36.3.478 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]