Skip to main content
The International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity logoLink to The International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity
. 2019 Sep 3;16:77. doi: 10.1186/s12966-019-0844-z

Nudging to move: a scoping review of the use of choice architecture interventions to promote physical activity in the general population

S Forberger 1,, L Reisch 2,3, T Kampfmann 1, H Zeeb 1,4
PMCID: PMC6724306  PMID: 31481090

Abstract

Background

Nudges are used to alter lifestyles and thus curb the rise of non-communicable diseases. Physical activity is a core prevention strategy to reduce the burden of non-communicable diseases. This paper aims to (1) give an overview of the scope of interventions using choice architecture techniques to promote physical activity at the population levels and (2) identify research gaps by analysing the different approaches in terms of class and type of intervention used.

Methods

A systematic electronic database search was combined with snowball citation sampling of a starter set of publications to search for studies published through October 2018 reporting interventions to promote physical activity at the population level using choice architecture techniques. The methodology of the Joanna Briggs Institute for Scoping Reviews was applied.

Results

In all, 35 publications were included. Most of the interventions used point-of-choice prompts tested at railway stations, shopping malls and airports (N = 27). Eight studies were online studies. While all studies were aimed at the general population, details, if reported at all, were vague and basic. All studies focused on individual-level lifestyle behaviour. None of the studies attempted to alter population-based lifestyle behaviour. Online and “real-world” approaches were rarely combined. Neither, interventions targeting meso- and macro-level structures nor combinations of individual-level and specific meso- or macro-level interventions were found.

Conclusion

Nudging is in principle an effective approach to promote physical activity within the general population. However, there are large gaps in research. Available opportunities have not yet been exhausted. Further research is needed that is explicitly based on behavioural insights and covering the full range of nudging approaches, particularly focussing on theoretical developments, practical feasibility tests and scale-up activities.

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article (10.1186/s12966-019-0844-z) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

Keywords: Physical activity, General population, Nudge, Choice architecture, Behavioural insights, Behaviourally informed policy

Background

Since the publication of the seminal book Nudge by Thaler and Sunstein in 2008 [1], the use of behavioural insights in general and nudges in particular has gained increasing interest among public and private institutions. The Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development (OECD) concludes in its 2017 report that the use of behavioural insights goes beyond a trend towards widespread dissemination [2] as it is studied in various areas and integrated into policy making [3, 4]. The Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development defines behavioural insights as a mix of “traditional economic strategies with insights from psychology, cognitive sciences and other social sciences to discover the many irrational factors that influence decision making” [5]. Governments are increasingly interested in using behaviourally informed policies to shape citizens’ behaviour as a complement to or replacement of traditional instruments such as bans or mandates [24, 6]. Behavioural insights help to understand why people often fail to act in their best interest, to follow well-informed preferences or to achieve their set goals. Nudges can help to overcome these challenges by using the same habits, biases or boundaries to alter our decision-making in favour of the more preferred behaviour [7]. According to Thaler and Sunstein, nudges aim to “alter people’s behaviour in a predictable way without forbidding any option or significantly change their economic incentives. To count as a mere nudge, [an] intervention must be easy and cheap to avoid. Nudges are not mandates” [1] and “allow [s] them [people] to go their own way” [8]. Nudges alter the underlying “choice architecture”, the context in which the decision is made. Their appeal lies in the idea of influencing individual decision-making with minimal effort in order to support or achieve behavioural changes [9]. The concept has attracted the attention of governments at all levels interested in influencing lifestyles at the population level by focusing directly or indirectly on the physical and social environment [10, 11], especially with the constraint of limited public resources. This is attractively combined with the preservation of individual freedom of choice at a time when individualisation is prominent and the one-fits-all approach increasingly rejected [12]. The promising aspect of choice architecture approaches is the idea that once the right stimulus is found, individuals will automatically choose a “better” (e.g. healthier, more sustainable, more environmentally friendly, more financially attractive) alternative [13, 14] rendering expensive enforcement structures obsolete.

It is increasingly apparent that systematic changes in people’s environments (micro-, meso-, and macro-level) where people make decisions can be important catalysts for changing behaviour at individual and population-level [1517]. Micro-, meso- and macro-level refer to a general approach to analysis with micro-level (individual-level) covering the individual in his or her social setting or a small group of individuals in a particular context. Examples include but are not limited to: persons, citizens, families and households. Meso-level (social-organisational level) analyses fall between the micro- and macro-levels, normally covering the community or an organisation. It could also be a whole village, town or federal state. The macro-level, sometimes referred to as institutional level, includes among others the analytical units: states, nations, or societies [18, 19]. However, systematic research analysing the use of choice architecture approaches to effect change on the micro-, meso- and macro-levels is only slowly emerging, especially for meso- and macro-level interventions. Since their introduction the terms choice architecture and nudge have been used in different ways, across a wide range of interventions, multiple behaviours, environmental contexts (e.g. physical activity [2022], and food consumption [2325] or levels of analysis [15, 2630]). In most cases, intervention approaches were grouped based on common mechanisms like behavioural economics, gamification elements, or point-of-choice [24, 26, 3136]. To date, there have been few systematic overviews on the effectiveness of selected choice architecture interventions in specific areas such as food consumption [23, 25, 36, 37], pro-environmental behaviour [38], or lifestyle risk factors [32]. Szaszi et al. gave a domain-general overview of the used choice architecture intervention in the domains consumer choice, health, sustainability, education, transport, finance, health, and other [39]. To categorize the intervention used, Szaszi and his colleagues used the classification suggested by Münscher et al. [40].

Münscher and his colleagues distinguish between three categories of choice architecture techniques: (A) decision information covering various techniques that target the presentation and provision of decision-relevant information without changing existing options. The second category (B) decision structure refers to techniques aimed at designing options and associated consequences such as pre-selected options for organ donations or pension schemes (default option) or 5-cent taxes for shopping bags. The last category (C) decision assistance includes techniques to support and implement intentions to change a given behaviour by encouraging engagement, feedback, or reminders (Table 1).

Table 1.

Choice architecture categories and techniques according to Münscher et al. (2016) with examples

Category Technique Examples
A Decision information

A1 Translate information

Includes: reframe, simplify

Reframing call for blood donations as death-preventing rather than life-saving

A2 Make information visible

Includes: make own behaviour visible (feedback), make external information visible

Feedback about one’s own behaviour (fitness tracker), information in the form of graphics, etc. about e.g. house insulation, credit card statements, calorie intake

A3 Provide social reference point

Includes: refer to descriptive norm, refer to opinion leader

Information about the behaviour of people from one’s own peer group or people who are valued for special purposes, experts, or role models
B Decision structure

B1 Change choice defaults

Includes: set no-action default, use prompted choice

Pre-selected options that leave the freedom to select a different option (or not) such as done for organ donation or pension savings in some countries (default options), poster/banners to use stairs

B2 Change option-related effort

Includes: increase/decrease physical/financial effort

(Re) arrangements of food items in grocery stores, of menu cards in restaurants, or the presentation of food dishes at buffets so that the healthier choices are easier to reach/to choose

B3 Change range or composition of options

Includes: change categories, change grouping of options

Segregating healthy options into diverse categories

B4 Change option consequences

Includes: connect decision to benefit/cost, change social consequences of the decision

5-cent tax for a shopping bag, possibility to take part in a lottery when complying with medication or taking part in a survey
C Decision assistance C1 Provide reminders Get reminders

C 2 Facilitate commitment

Includes: support self-commitment/public commitment

www.stickk.com, browser apps, blocking the internet access for specific items; agreements between parents and schools

Hollands et al. present another approach to classify choice architecture interventions aiming to alter micro environments in order to induce certain behaviours [15, 41]. They differentiate six intervention types: availability, position, functionality, presentation, size, and information with an intervention focus on product related objects or the wider environment. While the first version covered food, alcohol, tobacco, and physical activity, the final version of TIPPME does not cover physical activity (Table 2). Sunstein gave an overview of the ten main nudges currently used [8] (Table 3).

Table 2.

TIPPME intervention typology for environments to change behaviour according to Hollands et al. (2017)

Class Intervention type Examples for intervention focus
Product Related objects Wider environment
Placement Availability Adding non-alcoholic options to a bar’s range of drinks, or removing less healthy snack options from a vending machine Add baskets, trolleys or trays to a shop or restaurant to increase the number of products that people can select and carry Removing some of the entrance doors leading to a bar or cafeteria
Position Place less healthy options further away from seating, entrance, or main thoroughfare Move refrigerators containing sugary drinks to a less convenient location in a supermarket Move dividing walls or fixed furniture to alter layout of a supermarket, restaurant, or bar
Properties Functionality Allowing easier opening or pouring or demarcate plate to provide guidance for amounts of vegetables vs. meat selected Demarcate shopping trolley space to indicate designated space for fruit and vegetables Alter functionality of entrance and exit doors (e.g. change their opening mechanism)
Presentation Plain packaging for cigarettes or alcohol products Colours, textures, and visual design of shelf displays, menus, and other related object Indoor climate: temperature, humidity, air pressure, lighting
Size Change size of portions, plates, packages Change size of shopping trolleys or baskets, cafeteria trays, or food and drink storage equipment Size and shape of windows, or fixed furniture
Information Health warnings on cigarette packets, alcohol consumption units on glasses Nutritional information on menus or menu boards Information on posters, leaflets, or computer screens, in the wider environment

Table 3.

The 10 most important Nudges (Sunstein, 2014)

Nudge Example
1 Default rules Automatic enrollment in programs, including education, health, savings
2 Simplification In part to promote participation in existing programs
3 Uses of social norms “Most people pay their taxes on time.”, “Nine out of ten hotel guests reuse their towels.”
4 Increases in ease and convenience Making low-cost options or healthy foods visible
5 Disclosure Economic or environmental costs associated with energy use, or the full cost of certain credit cards
6 Warnings, graphic or otherwise Pictures on cigarette packages
7 Pre-commitment strategies Pre-commit to engaging in certain activities such as smoking cessation
8 Reminders Email or text message, as for overdue bills and coming obligations or appointments
9 Eliciting implementation intentions “Do you plan to vaccinate your child?”
10 Informing people of the nature and consequences of their own past choices Expenditures on health care or on electric bills

This scoping review aims to give an overview of work in a specific part of this growing field of research: the application of choice architecture interventions to increase physical activity in the general population, i.e. not in a specific setting or target group but in the population as a whole. The specific feature of this scoping review is to provide an overview of research in the field of physical activity promotion for the general population, to map the literature, and to identify knowledge gaps. Objectives of systematic reviews such as the feasibility, appropriateness, rationality, or effectiveness of interventions will not be addressed. In our scoping review, we have included studies explicitly stating that they use choice architecture interventions. In addition, we searched grey literature including reports of the Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development (OECD), the World Health Organisation (WHO), the European Union (EU), and the United Nations (UN) to focus on population-level physical activity interventions. Although physical activity has been shown to be effective in preventing many chronic diseases [42, 43] physical activity in the population remains low [44]. Population-based approaches which explicitly include context and environmental factors in the decision-making process could represent a valid option to increase physical activity in daily life. However, little is known so far about choice architecture interventions to target physical activity in the general population.

To date, in the field of physical activity most work has been done for specific target groups [45, 46], specific settings [46, 47], disease prevention [4850], and technology use [5153]. There have been activities focusing on public policy evolution to promote physical activity [5456] and some studies were conducted with a special focus on financial incentives without a focus on nudging [5762]. In 2010, Nocon et al. and Soler et al. published papers analysing the effectiveness of point-of-choice prompts [22, 63]. Zimmerman et al. [21] examined behavioural economics with the aim to promote physical activity. He suggested looking beyond the default option, i.e. pre-selected options, and to focus instead on “anchors” that are reference points (e.g. norms, framing, habits) influencing subsequent judgements in order to see how they interact with the context and influence preferences [21]. However, little is known beyond the application of point-of-choice nudges in terms of population-level interventions for the promotion of physical activity. This scoping review aims to address this knowledge gap by [1] providing an overview of the scope of choice architecture interventions to promote physical activity within the general population and [2] analysing the different approaches in terms of class and type of intervention used.

Methods

The scoping review is based on the Joanna Briggs Institute methodology for Scoping Reviews [64] using the preferred reporting items for systematic reviews [65] and the transparent reporting of systematic reviews and meta-analyses “PRISMA” flowchart. Although scoping reviews follow a similar approach to systematic reviews [66], they differ in terms of objectives and key characteristics. They answer broader questions that go beyond those related to the effectiveness of treatments or interventions and aim to (a) map existing literature in terms of nature, features and volume, (b) clarify work definitions and conceptual boundaries and (c) identify gaps in the existing literature and research [67].

Literature search

Medline, PsycInfo, different Web of Science databases (Science Citation Index, Arts & Humanities Citation Index, Conference Proceedings Citation Index, Social Science & Humanities Index, Book Citation Index–Science, Book Citation Index–Social Science & Humanities), CINHAL, Econ. Lit and ASSIA were used to search for studies published through October 2018 reporting interventions to promote physical activity in the population using choice architecture techniques. Databases and publication period covered for each database are shown in Table 4.

Table 4.

List of databases and their characteristics

Database Provider Time span
1 Medline PubMed 1982–10/2018
2 PsycInfo Ovid 1806–10/2018
3 Science Citation Index Expanded Web of Science 1900–10/2018
Social Science Citation Index 1956–10/2018
Arts & Humanities Citation Index 1975–10/2018
Conference Proceedings Citation Index–Science 2013–10/2018
Conference Proceedings Citation Index–Social Science & Humanities 2013–10/2018
Book Citation Index–Science 2013–10/2018
Book Citation Index–Social Science & Humanities 2013–10/2018
4

CINHAL

Econ.Lit

EBSCO

1937–10/2018

1886–10/2018

5 ASSIA Proquest 1987–10/2018

Inclusion was restricted to full-text papers and to studies published in English or German

Search strategy

The search strategy consisted of a combination of keywords, Medical Subject Headings (MeSH-Terms) and Thesaurus of Psychological Index Terms. We combined five search themes with Boolean operators (Table 5). An example of the search is available in Additional file 1. The search strategy was developed and tested in collabrotation with a research librarian beforehand.

Table 5.

List of search themes and terms used for the search strategy

Search themes Search terms Search type
1 Behavioural insights Behavioural insight, nudging, nudge, behavioural economic, behavioural public policy, choice architecture, choice intervention, behavioural informed Title/Abstract
2 Physical activity Sport, sporting, exercise, physical fitness, physical activity, aerobic, training Title/Abstract
3 Physical activity Exercise, sports, physical education and training MeSH-Terms
4 Walkability Walkability MeSH-Terms
5 Active transport Active transport MeSH-Terms

Consideration of: British and American English, singular/plural

The reference lists of the included studies were searched to identify additional studies. Hand-searched grey literature was included if eligible (see “Screening procedure” below). The starter-set publications for the grey literature research were the reports of the Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development (OECD), the United Nations, the European Commission, and the World Bank about using behavioural-informed strategies [2, 4, 6870], as well as the World Health Organization Health-enhancing physical activity (HEPA) policy audit tool (PAT) (WHO HEPA PAT) country reports. Following a snowball approach, we first used backward snowballing, using the reference lists to identify new papers. The identified papers were examined based on inclusion and exclusion criteria for the systematic literature search. Second, forward snowball sampling was applied to include studies based on papers citing the paper under examination [71]. The process was repeated until no more new papers were found. For the forward snowball sampling system, Google Scholar was used.

Screening procedure and inclusion/exclusion criteria

Following the literature search, the screening procedure was based on predefined inclusion and exclusion criteria (Table 6) and consisted of two consecutive phases. All publications were independently screened by two reviewers. First, titles and abstracts were screened to exclude articles that did not meet the eligibility criteria. In a second step, full texts were independently reviewed and evaluated by two reviewers. Any disagreements were either resolved by consensus or by discussion with a third reviewer. In terms of publications, we included those that applied choice architecture techniques. We included full-text papers in English and German. We included peer-reviewed publications as well as grey literature to incorporate results that may not yet have been published. If the full text was not available we contacted the authors. We excluded review articles but screened the reference lists of the papers to identify suitable publications. Study designs included in the review were: randomised controlled trials (RCTs), non-randomised controlled trials as well as observational studies aimed at the general population. Studies that did not aim for the general population, e.g. those targeted at specific groups such as students or staff or settings such as schools, workplaces, day care centres, or nursing homes, were not considered. We included online studies, i.e., studies that recruited their participants through open online platforms, provided they excluded only persons under 18 years of age and had no further restrictions.

Table 6.

Inclusion and exclusion criteria

Inclusion criteria
 1 Full-text paper.
 2 The language of the paper was English or German.
 3 The paper was published in a peer-reviewed journal or as grey literature.
 4 The studies in the paper investigated one or more behaviourally informed intervention techniques attributed to a nudge or connected to the choice-architecture literature.
 5 The target group was the general population.
Exclusion Criteria
 1 Review articles, conference abstracts and conference papers. However, we screened the review articles for suitable publications.
 2 Intervention targeting specific settings (schools, kindergartens, workplaces), specific target groups (children, workplace staff members, women), or disease prevention/weight-management programs.

Data extraction

Only full texts were analysed for data extraction. Retrieved publications were organised via a reference management software and later managed with Rayyan [72]. All included publications were analysed and data were extracted into an a priori developed extraction form (Table 7), which was tested beforehand on three studies to ensure all relevant results were extracted [73, 74]. Data extraction was conducted independently by two authors. In case of disagreement a third author was consulted.

Table 7.

Predefined data extraction form with categories of the extracted data and characteristics of the categories

Category Characteristics
1 Author All authors of the publication
2 Year Year of publication
3 Publication type Journal article, report, grey literature
4 Domain/subdomain While the domain of physical activity (PA) is growing the category summarises the aim of the intervention indicating specific subdomains if needed, such as active transport, cycling, walking and stair use.
5 Origin The country in which the intervention was carried out
6 Aim of the intervention Aim of the intervention
7 Study design Study design used
8 Intervention Short description of the intervention
9 Setting Setting of the intervention
10 Target group specifications Details reported about the target group (inclusion/exclusion of persons, children, disabled persons)
11 Approach Population-wide, individual-based
12 Analytical level Micro, meso, macro
Choice-architecture category and intervention techniques

Based on the taxonomy of Münscher, Vetter & Scheuerle (2015) the interventions were sorted into three choice-architecture categories with nine techniques:

(A) Decision information

(1) translate information (e.g. framing techniques and simplification of information),

(2) make information visible (e.g. using feedback techniques and presenting external information),

(3) provide social reference point (e.g. referring to a descriptive norm or to an opinion leader)

(B) Decision structure (1) change choice defaults (e.g. opt-in, opt-out techniques or prompted choice),

(2) change option-related effort (e.g. financial or physical effort),

(3) change range or composition of options (e.g. changing categories or grouping of options),

(4) change option consequences (e.g. changing social consequences of the decision or connecting it to benefit or cost)

(C) Decision assistance

(1) provide reminders (e.g. making information more salient or easier to access), and

(2) facilitate commitment (e.g. supporting self or public commitment to choice).

14 Typology of interventions with special focus on micro environment

Typology of intervention in accordance with TIPPME [26]

Placement

 Availability

 Position

Properties

 Functionality

 Presentation

 Size

 Information

15 Intervention results Study results

The extracted data were synthesised and presented in a table mapping the publications included against the predefined categories (Table 7). To categorise the interventions two taxonomies were combined. The choice architecture techniques used were categorised by the taxonomy developed by Münscher et al. [40] and sorted into three choice-architecture categories with nine techniques. The categories include: (A) decision information ((A1) translate information (A2) make information visible, (A3) provide social reference), (B) decision structure ((B1) change choice defaults, (B2) change option-related effort, (B3) change range or composition of options, (B4) change option consequences), (C) decision assistance, ((C1) provide reminders, (C2) facilitate commitment) (Table 1). In addition, interventions explicitly used to alter micro environments, that means the context in which the intervention is applied, were categorised in accordance with TIPPME [15, 41] which focuses on the categorisation of interventions aiming to change micro environments in order to change behaviour. The interventions were classified by placement and properties with six interventions types (availability, position, functionality, presentation, size, information) (Table 2).

Results

Search results

In the database search phase, 549 articles were retrieved. A total of 192 records were found by an additional search beginning with the starter-set publications. After controlling for duplicates, 611 records were included in the screening of abstracts and titles. During that screening, 485 articles were excluded because they did not meet the inclusion criteria. In the next step, 135 full-text articles were assessed for eligibility. At the end of screening, 35 articles met the selection criteria for the review (Fig. 1, Additional file 2).

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Study Flow Diagram

Characteristics of the selected articles

All studies described in the selected articles focus on the increase of physical activity with the following specifications: increase of stair use [75100], exercise commitment [101103], physical activity in general [104107], and walkability (step count) [108]. All articles focused on the general population. However, in the studies that reported data on the general population, children and persons with children, disabled persons, persons with luggage and, in online studies, persons under the age of 18 were excluded. Most studies were conducted in the UK [77, 81, 8489, 92, 95, 105], followed by online studies mostly without geographical focus [101104, 106108], and studies from the USA [76, 78, 79, 97, 98, 109], and Hong Kong [75, 80]. There was one study each from Singapore [96], Japan [91], Denmark [83], Sweden [82], Germany [90], Belgium [100], France [99], South Africa [93], and Spain [94]. Most studies were conducted in train stations/underground train stations [78, 79, 8183, 8992, 94, 96, 99, 100], followed by studies in malls/shopping malls [7678, 8488, 95, 100], airports [79, 97, 98, 109], pedestrian transit systems [80, 82], two public buildings [79, 93], and one bus station [78]. Eight studies were conducted online only [101108]. Most of the studies used an observational study design [7683, 8590, 92, 9597, 100, 109]. Experimental study [99, 101, 104, 107], post-test study [102], interrupted time-series, randomised controlled trial [75, 103, 105], interview study [84], quasi-experimental [94, 98, 108], and cross-sectional survey [91] designs were represented as well. All interventions targeted the micro level. None of the studies reported any involvement of communal, regional, or national authorities (Table 8).

Table 8.

Characteristics of included articles (n = 35)

Characteristic Number of articles Percent
Geographical region
 UK 10 31
 Online 8 20
 USA 7 17
 Hong Kong 2 6
 Japan 1 3
 Denmark 1 3
 Sweden 1 3
 Germany 1 3
 Belgium 1 3
 South Africa 1 3
 Spain 1 3
 France 1 3
 Singapore 1 3
Setting (multiple settings possible)
 Train station/underground train station 12 34
 Mall/shopping mall 10 29
 Airport 4 11
 Pedestrian transit system 2 6
 Library (public building) 2 6
 Bus station 1 3
 Online only 8 23
Study design
 Observational study 21 60
 Experimental study 4 11
 2 × 2 Post-test study 1 3
 Interrupted time-series design 1 3
 Randomised controlled trial 2 6
 Interviews 1 3
 Quasi-experimental 3 9
 Cross-sectional survey 1 3
 Not specified 1 3
Intervention aim
 Increased stair use 27 77
 Increase in exercise commitment 3 9
 PA in general 4 11
 Walking (step count) 1 3
Intervention level (micro, meso, macro)
 Micro 35 100
Choice-architecture category (Taxonomy following Münscher et al. 2016; multiple categories possible)
 A. Decision information category
  A1 Translation information (e.g. framing techniques) 3 9
  A2 Making information visible 3 9
  A3 Provide social reference points 2 6
 B. Decision structure
  B1 Change choice defaults 30 86
  B2 Change option-related effort 0 0
  B3 Change range or composition of option 0 0
  B4 Change option consequences 6 17
 C. Decision assistance
  C1 Provide reminders 1 3
  C2 Facilitate commitment 3 9

Use of nudges for physical activity promotion in the general population

Most of the interventions used were nudges from the choice-architecture category B1: “Change choice defaults”. Within this category, 27 studies used point-of-choice prompts like placing banners or posters at stairs or escalators with different messages to encourage stair use [7594, 96100, 109, 110]. Three studies used default options [8991]. Three studies used A1: “Translate information” with gain/loss message framing [102104]. Three studies used the choice-architecture category A2: “Making information visible”, like using bio-feedback with the help of an external monitoring device [108], computer-generated tailored feedback of participants’ physical activity levels [107], or feedback on daily individual goal performance [105]. Two studies used A3: “Provided social reference points”, such as whether or not the personal physical activity level is in accordance with recommendations [107] or whether individuals using financial stakes are more successful [103]. Six studies used B4 “Change option consequences” by connecting options to small benefits or costs [101, 103106, 108]. One study used reminders (C1) [107]. Three studies used C2: “Facilitate commitment” by giving the option to use challenges between participants, and allowing participants to track success or failure to comply or designate a referee [101, 105, 106].

While the studies using point-of-choice prompts mostly used one nudge, the eight online studies combined several choice-architecture categories. Four studies used two choice categories by:

  • combining gain/loss message framing (A1) with a default option (B1) [102]

  • combining framing (A1) and a “change option consequences” (B4; getting a greater reward when spending more time being physically active) [104];

  • giving feedback with an external monitoring device (A2) and offering an opportunity to earn cents when reaching personalized daily step goals (B4) [108]; and

  • combining reward (B4) and commitment structures (C2) by allowing challenge [106].

Two studies utilized three combinations:

  • “change choice default” (B1), “change option consequences” (B4) and “facilitate commitment” (C2) [101]; and

  • “make information visible” (feedback) (A2), “change option consequences” (B4) and “facilitate commitment” (C2) [105].

Two studies combined four nudges [103, 107].

In order to examine interventions altering micro environments in more detail we used the TIPPME typology that classifies interventions into “properties” and “placement” (Table 9). There were 22 studies that used only the intervention class properties and intervention type information by placing posters or banners to prompt stair use. One study used the intervention type placement and analysed the role of an opportunity for increased stair use by modifying the environment in favour of either escalator use (two escalators ascending) or stair use (only one escalator ascending) [82]. Six studies combined the two intervention classes. One intervention used a combination of two intervention types, information and size, using big letters for the information material [80]. Two studies alternated between posters and stair banners (information and position) [92]. Three studies used information and presentation, explicitly mentioning the use of colourful designs or yellow background [75, 80, 88, 95].

Table 9.

Analysing prompted choice following TIPPME (Hollands et al., 2017)/ n = 27 (n = 8 online-only studies, taxonomy not applicable)

Intervention class Intervention type Number of studies
Properties Information 22
Placement Availability 1
Properties & placement Information & position 2
Information & presentation 3
Information & size 1

n = 27 studies. However, Eves et al. [78] reported three sub-studies using point-of-decision prompts, one properties and information, two information and presentation in one paper, which were counted here separately

Almost all studies using point-of-choice prompts reported positive effects and an increase in stair use under intervention conditions; except for four studies [75, 80, 90, 92]. Eves and Masters found no effect in one of their studies in Hong Kong [80]. Three other studies [75, 90, 92] found mixed results. Nine studies reported post-intervention data: five studies reported no effect under the post-intervention condition [75, 78, 83, 96, 99] and four studies reported higher stair use after intervention compared to baseline [85, 90, 93, 94]. Six of the eight online studies reported positive effects of the interventions [101, 103, 105108]. One study found no effect [102] and one had mixed effects [104]. Two online studies reported post-intervention data: Patel et al. reported a decrease in staircase use, but higher rates compared to baseline [105], and van’t Riet et al. reported no post-intervention effect [107].

Discussion

The aim of this scoping review was to provide an overview of interventions for physical activity promotion that [1] use choice architecture approaches within the general population and [2] to classify the approaches used. Although we were able to identify several studies using this approach, the number of studies is surprisingly small compared to the attention given to nudging or physical activity. Further, there are no studies targeting meso- or macro-level structures to promote physical activity. This may be due in part to the complexity of the issue of physical activity promotion at the population level, encompassing governmental structures on various levels to target obesogenic environmental aspects [111].

The interventions reported used mostly point-of-choice prompts targeting the individual and encouraging pedestrians to use stairs. However, population characteristics were rarely reported in detail, which is also due to the focus of the studies. The studies used observations or unobtrusive technology targeting the general population in order to examine behaviour in real life that is important to analyse real-world effects. Exclusions were made on the basis of observations, which are subject to mistakes. When reported, children and persons with children, impaired persons, and persons with large bags were excluded, and the physical-activity behaviour of participants was not monitored. In light of habit formation and the change of a population norm or reframing physical activity within a culture as a whole, these excluded populations are of interest, as well [21]. It is known that physical activity is not only performed by individuals simply for the sake of health but more importantly to manage everyday life such as commuting to work, grocery shopping, visiting doctors, bringing up children, visiting friends or leisure time activities [112]. In a review, Westerterp reported that habitual physical activity in early life and during growth had a significant independent influence on the growth of a lean body mass, which by implication has a long-term effect on body mass and calorie expenditure at a later age [113]. Following this line, all population groups, also persons with children (role-model function) and children themselves should be considered when designing nudges for physical activity promotion. Further, in terms of participation and health equality, disabled persons have to be considered as well; otherwise the interventions used are not sufficient. Designs have to be used that allow the observation of all population groups and the combination of different types of intervention that work for the whole population.

The online studies were able to apply more than one nudge and combined, for example, “change choice defaults” with “change in option consequences”. However, we did not find any studies that utilised a combination of interventions targeting stair use at an individual level with posters/banners and supporting this with online tools, or using meso- or macro-level support for long-term interventions. Nor could we find work with a combination of targeting environmental cues for physical activity with nudges at the meso- or macro-level and technology support. There is no information in the studies about the support and cooperation of, for example, a mall, an airport, or a city management that allowed stair-use interventions. Also, there is no reference to governmental programs or actions that used behaviourally informed approaches. It could be argued that this gap exists because there are currently no such interventions combining these approaches or because they are not being researched. However, in order to more broadly influence the environmental context to foster daily physical activity as a cultural or socially accepted norm, structures at the organisational, community, federal, and/or state level have to be targeted to effect a change of attitudes and norms in favour of habitual physical activity [112, 114, 115]. This is also important because the studies have shown that point-of-choice prompts are mostly effective and therefore a cost-effective way to promote physical activity in everyday life as long as the interventions continue. However, few studies report data on stair use after the end of the intervention. Most studies reported that stair use decreased after the prompt was removed, and most of the stair-use interventions had no long-term effects [22, 63]. More research is needed to answer questions such as: What is the timeframe for a stair use intervention until habituation/tolerance will occur? Can the habituation effect be delayed? How can the effect be delayed? How can different intervention approaches be meaningfully combined (poster, banner, music, digital, technical installation for stair use games)? How can results based on this research be scaled-up? How can meso- and macro-level structures be addressed and which combinations of micro- (individual-level interventions) meso- and macro-level approaches affect the behaviour of the population and alter habitual physical activity? These questions open up a much broader field for further research using choice architecture techniques to promote physical activity for the general population [13].

With their work Münscher et al. [40] and Hollands et al. [15, 41] further developed initial taxonomies categorising choice-architecture approaches. However, while Münscher et al. is very useful to categorise choice architectures, TIPPME allows much more precision in defining the micro environments. A combination of both taxonomies may be advantageous to classify physical activity promotion. While the development of an own classification system for physical activity taking into account the combination of individual characteristics and role of environment should be considered, the use of two existing classifications allows for comparison between the research fields as soon as sufficient data is available.

The currently small number of studies makes it impossible to compare the results with other areas like food, alcohol use, or smoking [15, 32, 39] or to determine whether certain countries prefer and support specific behaviourally informed approaches [3, 116]. States and social systems may differ when it comes to the acceptance of behaviour-informed interventions and to the extent of their intrusiveness in everyday life. Initial work has been done to analyse the level of approval of nudges between groups of nations and the circumstances in which they are supported [117120]. However, more work is needed to test which nudging approaches are most effective in combination with which state structures and related (social) norms for physical activity promotion, as the use of these approaches is always contextual and culture specific.

Our search and analysis aimed to be broad and inclusive in order to obtain an overview of approaches to physical activity promotion using choice architecture interventions and to concepts surrounding this effort. To our knowledge, no comparable work has been done so far. For the interpretation of the results, some points should be considered. First of all, interventions or broader policies that use nudging implicitly without stating so were not included. Only studies related to behavioural insights, choice architecture, nudging, behavioural economics and behaviourally informed intervention were included. Second, specific approaches relevant to physical activity promotion such as active transport and urban planning may have been overlooked with our search strategy and were not included in the scoping review if they did not explicitly refer to nudging or choice architecture. Furthermore, research reporting e.g. on the use of public transport or the role of urban planning for traffic management purposes or environmental reasons not related to physical activity was not included, although increased use of public transport could increase daily physical activity. Here, more sensitive research strategies that take into account the complexity of the issue, indirect effects and inter-dependencies of the different research fields through e.g. nested research strategies might be more appropriate. Finally, Embase was not included in the research because we did not have access. The scoping review was not pre-registered in PROSPERO, which covers only systematic reviews and a research protocol was not published beforehand. In order to ensure that reporting on the process and the results was as objective as possible the authors previously agreed on a publication proposal that included an abstract, the search strategy, and predefined inclusion and exclusion criteria. Furthermore, all screenings were carried out by two reviewers and the results were discussed throughout the group.

Conclusion

Given the complexity of health intervention and the interplay among micro-, meso- and macro-level factors, the full potential of choice architecture interventions has yet to be realized. The results of this scoping review show that studies use nudging for physical activity promotion, but mostly on an individual level. There are large gaps in the approaches and instruments for increasing daily physical activity in the population. Studies are concentrating mainly on point-of-decision nudges. A few online studies combine several nudges but are limited to the online domain. A combination of online and “real-world” approaches has not yet been implemented. Furthermore, we found no interventions targeting meso- and macro-level structures, and a combination and linkage of individual-level intervention and specific meso- or macro-level intervention to nudge at the population level has not been applied so far. There is a lack of studies testing the use of behaviourally informed public policy making to enable sustainable changes in meso- and macro-level environmental factors. It is rarely attempted to scale-up the intervention from the individual level to the meso- or macro-level. Further tests of such approaches and the exploration of the full potential of choice architecture interventions for physical activity and more active mobility are needed.

Additional files

Additional file 1: (43.4KB, docx)

Characteristics of studies included. (DOCX 43 kb)

Additional file 2: (16.4KB, docx)

PubMed search strategy. (DOCX 16 kb)

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank our research librarian Lara Christianson (BIPS, Bremen) for providing feedback on the search strategy and the selection of the databases.

Abbreviations

HEPA PAT

Health-enhancing physical activity (HEPA) policy audit tool (PAT)

OECD

Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development

PA

Physical activity

UN

United Nations

WHO

World Health Organization

Authors’ contributions

Regarding author roles, SF made substantial contributions to the conception, design of the manuscript and search strategy, acquisition of data, analysis, and interpretation of the data and drafted the manuscript. LR, TK and HZ made substantial contributions to the design of the search strategy and provided support in the interpretation of data. SF and TK executed the search strategy and data extraction. All listed authors read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Funding

The scoping review was conducted as part of the “AEQUIPA-Physical activity and health equity: Primary prevention for healthy ageing” project, a regional prevention research project and network funded by the German Federal Ministry of Education and Research (grant number: 01EL1422A). The funding agency had no involvement in the execution of this review and the decision to submit the article for publication.

Availability of data and materials

All data generated or analysed during this study are included in this published article [and its supplementary information files].

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable

Consent for publication

Not applicable

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Contributor Information

S. Forberger, Email: forberger@leibniz-bips.de

L. Reisch, Email: lre.msc@cbs.dk

T. Kampfmann, Email: kampfmann@leibniz-bips.de

H. Zeeb, Email: zeeb@leibniz-bips.de

References

  • 1.Sunstein CR, Thaler R. Nudge. The politics of libertarian paternalism New Haven. 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.OECD . Behavioural insights and public policy: lessons from around the world. Paris: OECD Publishing; 2017. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Stra ßheim H. Die Globalisierung der Verhaltenspolitik. In: Hirschbrunn K, Kubon-Gilke G, Sturn R, editors. Jahrbuch Normative und institutionelle Grundfragen der Ökonomik, Marburg: metropolis; 2017; Bd16:211-242.
  • 4.Lourenço JS, Ciriolo E, Almeida SR, Troussard X. Behavioural insights applied to policy: European Report 2016. Brussels: European Union; 2016.
  • 5.Lunn P. Behavioural insights applied to policy. Paris: OECD; 2014. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Osman M. Nudge: how far have we come? Œconomia History, Methodology, Philosophy. 2017. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Hansen PG, Skov LR, Skov KL. Making healthy choices easier: regulation versus nudging. Annu Rev Public Health. 2016;37:237–251. doi: 10.1146/annurev-publhealth-032315-021537. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Sunstein CR. Nudging: a very short guide. J Consumer Policy. 2014;37(4):583–588. doi: 10.1007/s10603-014-9273-1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Marjanovic Z. The fruitless pursuit of nudging healthy behavior: comment on Lin, Osman, and Ashcroft. Basic Appl Soc Psych. 2017;39(6):309–310. doi: 10.1080/01973533.2017.1369413. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Alemanno A, Amir O, Bovens L, Burgess A, Lobel O, Whyte K, et al. Nudging healthy lifestyles–informing regulatory governance with behavioural research. Eur J Risk Regul. 2012;3(1):16. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Roberto CA, Kawachi I. Behavioral economics and public health. New York: Oxford University Press; 2015. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Lodge M, Wegrich K. The rationality paradox of nudge: rational tools of government in a world of bounded rationality. Law Policy. 2016;38(3):250–267. doi: 10.1111/lapo.12056. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Thorun C, Diels J, Vetter M, Reisch L, Bernauer M, Micklitz H-W, et al. Nudge-Ansätze beim nachhaltigen Konsum: Ermittlung und Entwicklung von Maßnahmen zum „Anstoßen “nachhaltiger Konsummuster. Abschlussbericht für den Umweltforschungsplan des Bundesministeriums für Umwelt, Naturschutz, Bau und Reaktorsicherheit, Forschungskennzahl. 2016;3714(93):303.
  • 14.Sunstein CR, Reisch LA. Automatically green: behavioral economics and environmental protection. Harv Envtl L Rev. 2014;38:127. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Hollands GJ, Shemilt I, Marteau TM, Jebb SA, Kelly MP, Nakamura R, et al. Altering micro-environments to change population health behaviour: towards an evidence base for choice architecture interventions. BMC Public Health. 2013;13(1):1218. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-13-1218. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.REISCH LUCIA A., ZHAO MIN. Behavioural economics, consumer behaviour and consumer policy: state of the art. Behavioural Public Policy. 2017;1(2):190–206. doi: 10.1017/bpp.2017.1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Das P, Horton R. Rethinking our approach to physical activity. Lancet. 2012;380(9838):189–190. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)61024-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.World Health Organization. A conceptual framework for action on the social determinants of health. Geneva: WHO; 2010.
  • 19.Jepperson R, Meyer JW. Multiple levels of analysis and the limitations of methodological individualisms. Sociol Theory. 2011;29(1):54–73. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9558.2010.01387.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Bauman A, Milton K, Kariuki M, Fedel K, Lewicka M. Is there sufficient evidence regarding signage-based stair use interventions? A sequential meta-analysis. BMJ Open. 2017;7(11):e012459. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2016-012459. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Zimmerman FJ. Using behavioral economics to promote physical activity. Prev Med. 2009;49(4):289–291. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2009.07.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Nocon M, Muller-Riemenschneider F, Nitzschke K, Willich SN. Review article: increasing physical activity with point-of-choice prompts--a systematic review. Scand J Public Health. 2010;38(6):633–638. doi: 10.1177/1403494810375865. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Carter P, Bignardi G, Hollands GJ, Marteau TM. Information-based cues at point of choice to change selection and consumption of food, alcohol and tobacco products: a systematic review. BMC Public Health. 2018;18(1):418. doi: 10.1186/s12889-018-5280-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Wright B, Bragge P. Interventions to promote healthy eating choices when dining out: a systematic review of reviews. Br J Health Psychol. 2018;23(2):278–295. doi: 10.1111/bjhp.12285. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Al-Khudairy L, Uthman OA, Walmsley R, Johnson S, Oyebode O. Choice architecture interventions to improve diet and/or dietary behaviour by healthcare staff in high-income countries: a systematic review. BMJ Open. 2019;9(1):e023687. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2018-023687. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Marteau TM, Ogilvie D, Roland M, Suhrcke M, Kelly MP. Judging nudging: can nudging improve population health? BMJ. 2011;342:263–265. doi: 10.1136/bmj.d228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Thaler R, Sunstein CR. Nudge: improving decision about Helath, wealth, and happiness. Yale: Yale University Press; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Blumenthal-Barby JS, Burroughs H. Seeking better health care outcomes: the ethics of using the “nudge”. Am J Bioeth. 2012;12(2):1–10. doi: 10.1080/15265161.2011.634481. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Ploug T, Holm S, Brodersen J. To nudge or not to nudge: cancer screening programmes and the limits of libertarian paternalism. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2012;66(12):1193–1196. doi: 10.1136/jech-2012-201194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Houghtaling B, Serrano EL, Kraak VI, Harden SM, Davis GC, Misyak SA. A systematic review of factors that influence food store owner and manager decision making and ability or willingness to use choice architecture and marketing mix strategies to encourage healthy consumer purchases in the United States, 2005-2017. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2019;16(1):5. doi: 10.1186/s12966-019-0767-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Dolan P, Hallsworth M, Halpern D, King D, Vlaev I. MINDSPACE: influencing behaviour through public policy. London: Cabinet Office; 2010. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Blaga OM, Vasilescu L, Chereches RM. Use and effectiveness of behavioural economics in interventions for lifestyle risk factors of non-communicable diseases: a systematic review with policy implications. Perspect Public Health. 2018;138(2):100–110. doi: 10.1177/1757913917720233. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Cotton V, Patel MS. Gamification use and Design in Popular Health and Fitness Mobile Applications. Am J Health Promot. 2019;33(3):448–451. doi: 10.1177/0890117118790394. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Kraak VI, Englund T, Misyak S, Serrano EL. A novel marketing mix and choice architecture framework to nudge restaurant customers toward healthy food environments to reduce obesity in the United States. Obes Rev. 2017;18(8):852–868. doi: 10.1111/obr.12553. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Broers VJV, De Breucker C, Van den Broucke S, Luminet O. A systematic review and meta-analysis of the effectiveness of nudging to increase fruit and vegetable choice. Eur J Pub Health. 2017;27(5):912–920. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/ckx085. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Nørnberg TR, Houlby L, Skov LR, Peréz-Cueto FJA. Choice architecture interventions for increased vegetable intake and behaviour change in a school setting: a systematic review. Perspect Public Health. 2016;136(3):132–142. doi: 10.1177/1757913915596017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Bucher T, Collins C, Rollo ME, McCaffrey TA, De Vlieger N, Van der Bend D, et al. Nudging consumers towards healthier choices: a systematic review of positional influences on food choice. Br J Nutr. 2016;115(12):2252–2263. doi: 10.1017/S0007114516001653. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Byerly H, Balmford A, Ferraro PJ, Hammond Wagner C, Palchak E, Polasky S, et al. Nudging pro-environmental behavior: evidence and opportunities. Front Ecol Environ. 2018;16(3):159–168. doi: 10.1002/fee.1777. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Szaszi B, Palinkas A, Palfi B, Szollosi A, Aczel B. A systematic scoping review of the choice architecture movement: toward understanding when and why nudges work. J Behav Decis Mak. 2018;31(3):355–366. doi: 10.1002/bdm.2035. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Münscher R, Vetter M, Scheuerle T. A review and taxonomy of choice architecture techniques. J Behav Decis Mak. 2016;29(5):511–524. doi: 10.1002/bdm.1897. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Hollands GJ, Bignardi G, Johnston M, Kelly MP, Ogilvie D, Petticrew M, et al. The TIPPME intervention typology for changing environments to change behaviour. Nat Hum Behav. 2017;1(8):0140. doi: 10.1038/s41562-017-0140. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Rhodes RE, Janssen I, Bredin SSD, Warburton DER, Bauman A. Physical activity: health impact, prevalence, correlates and interventions. Psychol Health. 2017;32(8):942–975. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2017.1325486. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Collaborators GBDRF Global, regional, and national comparative risk assessment of 79 behavioural, environmental and occupational, and metabolic risks or clusters of risks, 1990-2015: a systematic analysis for the global burden of disease study 2015. Lancet. 2016;388(10053):1659–1724. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(16)31679-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Guthold R, Stevens GA, Riley LM, Bull FC. Worldwide trends in insufficient physical activity from 2001 to 2016: a pooled analysis of 358 population-based surveys with 1·9 million participants. Lancet Glob Health. 2018;6(10):e1077–e1e86. doi: 10.1016/S2214-109X(18)30357-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.McDonald SM, Clennin MN, Pate RR. Specific strategies for promotion of physical activity in kids-which ones work? A systematic review of the literature. Am J Lifestyle Med. 2018;12(1):51–82. doi: 10.1177/1559827615616381. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Zubala A, MacGillivray S, Frost H, Kroll T, Skelton DA, Gavine A, et al. Promotion of physical activity interventions for community dwelling older adults: a systematic review of reviews. PLoS One. 2017;12(7):e0180902. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0180902. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Maselli M, Ward PB, Gobbi E, Carraro A. Promoting physical activity Among University students: a systematic review of controlled trials. Am J Health Promot. 2018;32(7):1602–1612. doi: 10.1177/0890117117753798. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Rosenbaum S, Vancampfort D, Steel Z, Newby J, Ward PB, Stubbs B. Physical activity in the treatment of post-traumatic stress disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychiatry Res. 2015;230(2):130–136. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2015.10.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Smith AD, Crippa A, Woodcock J, Brage S. Physical activity and incident type 2 diabetes mellitus: a systematic review and dose–response meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies. Diabetologia. 2016;59(12):2527–2545. doi: 10.1007/s00125-016-4079-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Block VA, Pitsch E, Tahir P, Cree BA, Allen DD, Gelfand JM. Remote physical activity monitoring in neurological disease: a systematic review. PLoS One. 2016;11(4):e0154335. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0154335. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Muellmann S, Forberger S, Mollers T, Broring E, Zeeb H, Pischke CR. Effectiveness of eHealth interventions for the promotion of physical activity in older adults: a systematic review. Prev Med. 2018;108:93–110. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2017.12.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Robbins TD, Lim Choi Keung SN, Arvanitis TN. E-health for active ageing; a systematic review. Maturitas. 2018;114:34–40. doi: 10.1016/j.maturitas.2018.05.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Lau PW, Lau EY, Wong DP, Ransdell L. A systematic review of information and communication technology–based interventions for promoting physical activity behavior change in children and adolescents. J Med Internet Res. 2011;13(3):e48. doi: 10.2196/jmir.1533. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Daugbjerg SB, Kahlmeier S, Racioppi F, Martin-Diener E, Martin B, Oja P, et al. Promotion of physical activity in the European region: content analysis of 27 national policy documents. J Phys Act Health. 2009;6(6):805–817. doi: 10.1123/jpah.6.6.805. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Craig CL. Evolution and devolution of national physical activity policy in Canada. J Phys Act Health. 2011;8(8):1044–1056. doi: 10.1123/jpah.8.8.1044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Pratt M, Perez LG, Goenka S, Brownson RC, Bauman A, Sarmiento OL, et al. Can population levels of physical activity be increased? Global evidence and experience. Prog Cardiovasc Dis. 2015;57(4):356–367. doi: 10.1016/j.pcad.2014.09.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Luong MN, Bennell KL, Hall M, Harris A, Hinman RS. The impact of financial incentives on physical activity in adults: a systematic review protocol. Syst Rev. 2018;7(1):21. doi: 10.1186/s13643-018-0687-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Martin A, Suhrcke M, Ogilvie D. Financial incentives to promote active travel: an evidence review and economic framework. Am J Prev Med. 2012;43(6):e45–e57. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2012.09.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Mitchell MS, Goodman JM, Alter DA, John LK, Oh PI, Pakosh MT, et al. Financial incentives for exercise adherence in adults: systematic review and meta-analysis. Am J Prev Med. 2013;45(5):658–667. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2013.06.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.O'Malley G, Baker PR, Francis D, Perry IJ, Foster C. Incentive-based interventions for increasing physical activity and fitness. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2012;1:1465–1858. [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Strohacker K, Galarraga O, Williams DM. The impact of incentives on exercise behavior: a systematic review of randomized controlled trials. Ann Behav Med. 2013;48(1):92–99. doi: 10.1007/s12160-013-9577-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Adams J, Giles EL, McColl E, Sniehotta FF. Carrots, sticks and health behaviours: a framework for documenting the complexity of financial incentive interventions to change health behaviours. Health Psychol Rev. 2014;8(3):286–295. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2013.848410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Soler RE, Leeks KD, Buchanan LR, Brownson RC, Heath GW, Hopkins DH, et al. Point-of-decision prompts to increase stair use a systematic review update. Am J Prev Med. 2010;38(2):S292–S300. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2009.10.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Joanna Briggs Institute . Methodology for JBI Scoping Reviews. Joanna Briggs Institute Reviewers’ Manual: 2015 Edition/Supplement. South Australia: The Joanna Briggs Institute; 2015. [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Shamseer L, Moher D, Clarke M, Ghersi D, Liberati A, Petticrew M, et al. Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analysis protocols (PRISMA-P) 2015: elaboration and explanation. BMJ. 2015;350:g7647. doi: 10.1136/bmj.g7647. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Higgins JPT, Green S. Guide to the contents of a Cochrane protocol and review. In: JPT H, Green S, editors. Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions The Cochrane Collaboration. 2011. [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Peters MD, Godfrey CM, Khalil H, McInerney P, Parker D, Soares CB. Guidance for conducting systematic scoping reviews. Int J Evid Based Healthc. 2015;13(3):141–146. doi: 10.1097/XEB.0000000000000050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Whitehead M, Jones R, Howell R, Lilley R, Pykett J. Nudging all over the world: assessing the global impact of the behavioural sciences on public policy. UK.: Economic Social & Research Council Report Swindon; 2014. [Google Scholar]
  • 69.United Nations . Behavioural Insigths at the United Nations. Achieving agenda 2030. New York: United Nations Development Program; 2016. [Google Scholar]
  • 70.World Bank . Mind, society and behavior. Washington: World Bank; 2015. [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Wohlin C. Guidelines for snowballing in systematic literature studies and a replication in software engineering. Proceedings of the 18th international conference on evaluation and assessment in software engineering. London: ACM; 2014.
  • 72.Ouzzani M, Hammady H, Fedorowicz Z, Elmagarmid A. Rayyan-a web and mobile app for systematic reviews. Syst Rev. 2016;5(1):210. doi: 10.1186/s13643-016-0384-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Anderson S, Allen P, Peckham S, Goodwin N. Asking the right questions: scoping studies in the commissioning of research on the organisation and delivery of health services. Health Res Policy Syst. 2008;6(1):7. doi: 10.1186/1478-4505-6-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Valaitis R, Martin-Misener R, Wong ST, MacDonald M, Meagher-Stewart D, Austin P, et al. Methods, strategies and technologies used to conduct a scoping literature review of collaboration between primary care and public health. Prim Health Care Res Dev. 2012;13(3):219–236. doi: 10.1017/S1463423611000594. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Eves FF, Masters RS, McManus AM. Effects of point-of-choice stair climbing interventions in Hong Kong. Hong Kong Med J. 2008;14(5 Suppl):36–39. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Andersen RE, Franckowiak SC, Snyder J, Bartlett SJ, Fontaine KR. Can inexpensive signs encourage the use of stairs? Results from a community intervention. Ann Intern Med. 1998;129(5):363–369. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-129-5-199809010-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Blamey A, Mutrie N, Aitchison T. Health promotion by encouraged use of stairs. BMJ. 1995;311(7000):289–290. doi: 10.1136/bmj.311.7000.289. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Brownell KD, Stunkard AJ, Albaum JM. Evaluation and modification of exercise patterns in the natural environment. Am J Psychiatry. 1980;137(12):1540–1545. doi: 10.1176/ajp.137.12.1540. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Coleman KJ, Gonzalez EC. Promoting stair use in a US–Mexico border community. Am J Public Health. 2001;91(12):2007–2009. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.91.12.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Eves FF, Masters RS. An uphill struggle: effects of a point-of-choice stair climbing intervention in a non-English speaking population. Int J Epidemiol. 2006;35(5):1286–1290. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyl141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Eves FF, Olander EK, Nicoll G, Puig-Ribera A, Griffin C. Increasing stair climbing in a train station: the effects of contextual variables and visibility. J Environ Psychol. 2009;29(2):300–303. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2008.10.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Faskunger J, Poortvliet E, Nylund K, Rossen J. Effect of an environmental barrier to physical activity on commuter stair use. Scand J Nutr. 2003;47(1):26–28. doi: 10.1080/11026480310004262. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Iversen MK, Handel MN, Jensen EN, Frederiksen P, Heitmann BL. Effect of health-promoting posters placed on the platforms of two train stations in Copenhagen, Denmark, on the choice between taking the stairs or the escalators: a secondary publication. Int J Obes. 2007;31(6):950–955. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0803497. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Kerr J, Eves F, Carroll D. Posters can prompt less active people to use the stairs. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2000;54(12):942–943. doi: 10.1136/jech.54.12.942. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Kerr J, Eves F, Carroll D. Six-month observational study of prompted stair climbing. Prev Med. 2001;33(5):422–427. doi: 10.1006/pmed.2001.0908. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Kerr J, Eves F, Carroll D. Encouraging stair use: stair-riser banners are better than posters. Am J Public Health. 2001;91(8):1192–1193. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.91.8.1192. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Kerr J, Eves FF, Carroll D. The influence of poster prompts on stair use: the effects of setting, poster size and content. Br J Health Psychol. 2001;6(4):397–405. doi: 10.1348/135910701169296. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Kerr J, Eves FF, Carroll D. Getting more people on the stairs: the impact of a new message format. J Health Psychol. 2001;6(5):495–500. doi: 10.1177/135910530100600503. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Lewis AL, Eves FF. Prompts to increase stair climbing in stations: the effect of message complexity. J Phys Act Health. 2012;9(7):954–961. doi: 10.1123/jpah.9.7.954. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Muller-Riemenschneider F, Nocon M, Reinhold T, Willich SN. Promotion of physical activity using point-of-decision prompts in Berlin underground stations. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2010;7(8):3063–3070. doi: 10.3390/ijerph7083063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Nomura T, Yoshimoto Y, Akezaki Y, Sato A. Changing behavioral patterns to promote physical activity with motivational signs. Environ Health Prev Med. 2009;14(1):20–25. doi: 10.1007/s12199-008-0053-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Olander EK, Eves FF, Puig-Ribera A. Promoting stair climbing: stair-riser banners are better than posters … sometimes. Prev Med. 2008;46(4):308–310. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2007.11.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Pillay JD, Kolbe-Alexander T, Achmat M, Carstene M, Lambert EV. Are point-of-decision prompts in a sports science and medicine centre effective in changing the prevalence of stair usage? A preliminary study. South Afr J Sports Med. 2009;21(2):58–64. doi: 10.17159/2078-516X/2009/v21i2a299. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Puig-Ribera A, Eves FF. Promoting stair climbing in Barcelona: similarities and differences with interventions in English-speaking populations. Eur J Pub Health. 2010;20(1):100–102. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/ckp059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Webb OJ, Eves FF. Effects of environmental changes in a stair climbing intervention: generalization to stair descent. Am J Health Promot. 2007;22(1):38–44. doi: 10.4278/0890-1171-22.1.38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Sloan RA, Haaland BA, Leung C, Muller-Riemenschneider F. The use of point-of-decision prompts to increase stair climbing in Singapore. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2013;10(1):210–218. doi: 10.3390/ijerph10010210. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Fulton JE, Frederick GM, Paul P, Omura JD, Carlson SA, Dorn JM. Increasing walking in the Hartsfield-Jackson Atlanta international airport: the walk to Fly study. Am J Public Health. 2017;107(7):1143–1149. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2017.303766. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Bellettiere J, Liles S, BenPorat Y, Bliss N, Hughes SC, Bishop B, et al. And She’s buying a stairway to health: signs and participant factors influencing stair ascent at a public airport. J Prim Prev. 2017;38(6):597–611. doi: 10.1007/s10935-017-0491-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Allais O, Bazoche P, Teyssier S. Getting more people on the stairs: the impact of point-of-decision prompts. Soc Sci Med. 2017;192:18–27. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2017.09.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Boen F, Maurissen K, Opdenacker J. A simple health sign increases stair use in a shopping mall and two train stations in Flanders, Belgium. Health Promot Int. 2010;25(2):183–191. doi: 10.1093/heapro/daq001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Bhattacharya J, Garber AM, Goldhaber-Fiebert JD. Nudges in exercise commitment contracts: a randomized trial. 2015. [Google Scholar]
  • 102.de Bruijn GJ, Out K, Rhodes RE. Testing the effects of message framing, kernel state, and exercise guideline adherence on exercise intentions and resolve. Br J Health Psychol. 2014;19(4):871–885. doi: 10.1111/bjhp.12086. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Goldhaber-Fiebert JD, Blumenkranz E, Garber AM. Committing to exercise: contract Design for Virtuous Habit Formation. 2010. [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Hsu AS, Vlaev I. Monetary cost for time spent in everyday physical activities. Soc Sci Med. 2014;108:74–80. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.02.043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Patel MS, Benjamin EJ, Volpp KG, Fox CS, Small DS, Massaro JM, et al. Effect of a game-based intervention designed to enhance social incentives to increase physical activity among families: the BE FIT randomized clinical trial. JAMA Intern Med. 2017;177(11):1586–1593. doi: 10.1001/jamainternmed.2017.3458. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.van Mierlo T, Hyatt D, Ching AT, Fournier R, Dembo RS. Behavioral economics, wearable devices, and cooperative games: results from a population-based intervention to increase physical activity. JMIR Serious Games. 2016;4(1):e1. doi: 10.2196/games.5358. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.van't Riet J, Ruiter RA, Werrij MQ, de Vries H. Investigating message-framing effects in the context of a tailored intervention promoting physical activity. Health Educ Res. 2009;25(2):343–354. doi: 10.1093/her/cyp061. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Mitchell M, White L, Lau E, Leahey T, Adams MA, Faulkner G. Evaluating the carrot rewards app, a population-level incentive-based intervention promoting step counts across two Canadian provinces: quasi-experimental study. JMIR Mhealth Uhealth. 2018;6(9):e178. doi: 10.2196/10456. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Russell WD, Hutchinson J. Comparison of health promotion and deterrent prompts in increasing use of stairs over escalators. Percept Mot Skills. 2000;91(1):55–61. doi: 10.2466/pms.2000.91.1.55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Andersen K, Gudnason V. Health policy interventions: the pathway to public health. Laeknabladid. 2013;99(3):129–134. doi: 10.17992/lbl.2013.03.487. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Townshend T, Lake A. Obesogenic environments: current evidence of the built and food environments. Perspect Public Health. 2017;137(1):38–44. doi: 10.1177/1757913916679860. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Ulijaszek S. Physical activity and the human body in the (increasingly smart) built environment. Obes Rev. 2018;19(Suppl 1):84–93. doi: 10.1111/obr.12790. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Westerterp KR. Changes in physical activity over the lifespan: impact on body composition and sarcopenic obesity. Obes Rev. 2018;19(S1):8–13. doi: 10.1111/obr.12781. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Findholt NE, Michael YL, Jerofke LJ, Brogoitti VW. Environmental influences on children's physical activity and eating habits in a rural Oregon County. Am J Health Promot. 2011;26(2):e74–e85. doi: 10.4278/ajhp.100622-QUAL-210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Smith M, Toprakkiran N. Behavioural insights, nudge and the choice environment in obesity policy. Policy Studies. 2019;40(2):173–187. doi: 10.1080/01442872.2018.1554806. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Sunstein CR, Reisch LA, Kaiser M. Trusting nudges? Lessons from an international survey. J Eur Public Policy. 2018;1–27.
  • 117.Sunstein CR, Reisch LA, Rauber J. Behavioral insights all over the world? Public attitudes toward nudging in a multi-country study. 2017. [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Reisch LA, Sunstein CR, Gwozdz W. Better than a whip?: European attitudes toward health nudges. The Harvard John M. Olin Discussion Paper Series. 2016. [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Sunstein Cass R., Reisch Lucia A., Kaiser Micha. Trusting nudges? Lessons from an international survey. Journal of European Public Policy. 2018;26(10):1417–1443. doi: 10.1080/13501763.2018.1531912. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Loibl C, Sunstein CR, Rauber J, Reisch LA. Which Europeans like nudges? Approval and controversy in four European countries. J Consum Aff. 2018;52(3):655–688. doi: 10.1111/joca.12181. [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Additional file 1: (43.4KB, docx)

Characteristics of studies included. (DOCX 43 kb)

Additional file 2: (16.4KB, docx)

PubMed search strategy. (DOCX 16 kb)

Data Availability Statement

All data generated or analysed during this study are included in this published article [and its supplementary information files].


Articles from The International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity are provided here courtesy of BMC

RESOURCES