Abstract
BACKGROUND:
Vitamin D has been suggested as a protective factor for cognitive health, however results of prior studies have been mixed. To examine whether serum 25(OH)D concentration is related to cognition and cognitive decline in a study of Boston Area Puerto Ricans.
METHODS:
We examined the association between serum 25(OH)D, cognitive function and cognitive decline in a longitudinal study of 967 Boston Area Puerto Rican adults.
RESULTS:
In analyses adjusted for potential confounders, participants in the bottom quintile of 25(OH)D had similar cognitive function at baseline, as measured by a global cognitive score (mean difference: 0.09 (95% CI: −0.02, 0.19; p-trend: 0.18), and similar 2-year rates of cognitive decline (mean difference: −0.01 (95% CI: −0.09, 0.07), p-trend: 0.61) as those in the top 25(OH)D quintile. No significant associations were observed between baseline serum 25(OH)D concentration and 2-year change in individual cognitive test scores or change in executive function or memory domains.
CONCLUSIONS:
We observed no significant association between serum 25(OH)D and cognition in this cohort of Boston Area Puerto Ricans.
Keywords: epidemiology, nutrition, minority, aging, 25(OH)D, cognition, cohort, biomarker
INTRODUCTION
Background/rationale:
Vitamin D is a fat-soluble vitamin that has been implicated as a protective factor in a wide range of chronic diseases, including multiple sclerosis (1, 2), cancer (3, 4), and cardiovascular disease (5–7). It has well-known anti-inflammatory and antioxidant properties (8, 9). Vitamin D receptors are located in brain regions that are key for cognitive function (10, 11). It has, thus, been suggested that Vitamin D may play an important role in cognitive aging and the maintenance of healthy cognition(12).
The principal source of Vitamin D is its synthesis in the skin via sunlight exposure (13). Dietary sources of Vitamin D include fatty fish, meats, fish liver oils, eggs, dairy and vitamin-fortified products(14–16). Serum 25(OH)D is the accepted biomarker of Vitamin D status in humans and is commonly used in epidemiological studies of Vitamin D(14). Although debated, generally accepted cutoffs for serum 25(OH)D are: 1) sufficient (≥30 nmol/L), 2) insufficient (<30 nmol/L but >20 nmol/L) and 3) deficient (<20 nmol/L). More than 40% of the US population is estimated to be deficient in Vitamin D, and the deficiency disproportionally impacts minority populations(17).
A number of cross-sectional and longitudinal studies have examined the association between 25(OHD) and cognition. Several cross-sectional studies have reported lower cognition among participants with low 25(OH)D, compared to those with sufficient concentration (18–21). Longitudinal studies of 25(OH)D and cognitive decline have produced mixed results (22–28).
Mainland US Puerto Ricans represent a significant portion of the US Hispanic population. This population suffers from well-documented health disparities (29) and has been shown to have high rates of chronic disease including depression, type 2 diabetes, heart disease and cognitive impairment (30–33). Thus, identification of modifiable risk factors for cognitive function and decline is key to improving the long-term cognitive health of this at-risk population.
Objectives:
In this study, we investigated whether serum 25(OH)D concentration was related to cognitive function and 2-year cognitive decline in a longitudinal study of Boston Area Puerto Ricans.
METHODS
Study Design, Setting and Participants.
The study design and methodology are described in previous publications (34–38). This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board at the university where the study was conducted. All study participants provided written informed consent. Briefly, 1499 participants in the Boston Puerto Rican Study were 45–75 years old at baseline. Participants were interviewed in their home by bilingual interviewers in English and Spanish, and information was collected on their demographic factors, current health conditions and and health behaviors. During the baseline visits, the interviewers also collected anthropometric and blood-pressure measurements, and administered a food-frequency questionnaire. The interviewers provided instructions on fasting and returned to draw fasting blood samples soon after the interview. The interviewers were trained and performed cognitive testing on the participants at study baseline and after two years of follow-up. Our analysis was restricted to 967 study participants with complete cognitive data at baseline and at the 2-year follow-up, as well complete data on serum 25(OH)D.
Variabes, data sources and measurenment:
Serum vitamin D measurement.
Serum vitamin D was measured in fasting blood samples obtained at baseline as 25(OH)D, by extraction followed by 25I radioimmunoassay Packard COBRA II Gamma Counter (DiaSorin Inc., Stillwater, MN 55082). These measurements had intra and inter-assay coefficients of variation of 10.8% and 9.4% respectively. In our study sample, serum vitamin D ranged from 10 nmol/L to 120 nmol/L with a mean of 43.8 nmol/L.
Outcomes.
A trained research assistant administered a cognitive testing battery in either English (2%) or Spanish (98%) to all eligible study participants (36). The battery of cognitive tests consisted of the following: 1) Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE, a test of general cognition) (39); a 16-word list learning test (40) that included 2) word-list learning (sum of words recalled over 5 attempts), 3) recognition 4) percentage retention (number of words recalled after a delay as a proportion of the number of correct responses on the fifth learning trial); 5) digit span forward and backward (a test of working memory) (40); 6) the Stroop test (40); 7) verbal fluency (naming as many words as possible starting with a given letter) (40); 8) clock drawing (41); and 9) figure copying (42) (a test of executive function). The full battery of tests was administered at baseline and at the 2-year follow-up. In our data analysis, we used a baseline global cognitive and 2-year global cognitive change scores as our outcomes. The baseline global cognitive score was calculated as the mean of the z-scores of baseline values of the following cognitive tests: MMSE, word list learning, recognition, percentage retention, Stroop, letter fluency, digit span forward and backward, clock drawing, and weighted figure copying. The 2-year global cognitive change score was calculated as the mean of the z-scores of the changes between baseline and year 2 in each of the aforementioned individual tests. Derivation of both the baseline global cognitive score and the 2-year global cognitive change score are described in detail in previous publications (37).
Furthermore, we used principal-components analysis (PCA) with varimax rotation (37) to quantify the relationship between serum 25(OH)D and two major cognitive function factors: 1) executive function, and 2) memory. We performed PCA on baseline cognitive data, resulting in two principal components with eigenvalues >1. Based on the loadings for the individual baseline test scores on the two principal components, specific individual components were designated as contributing to the cognitive domains of executive function and memory. By definition, each PCA-derived composite score had a mean of 0 and a standard deviation of 1. Composite scores were also computed for year-two follow-up testing data using the means, standard deviations, and scoring coefficients from the baseline PCA. Difference scores between the follow-up and baseline observations were calculated for each domain, and descriptive statistics were obtained for these difference scores. For each cognitive domain, a “decline” was defined as a drop in score ≥ one standard deviation from the group-mean difference score.
Covariates.
Information collected at baseline included age, years of education, physical activity, smoking status, alcohol intake and other demographic variables. Body Mass Index (BMI, in kg/m2) was computed from body weight and height as measured during the interview by trained field workers. A physical activity score, a variation of the Framingham physical activity index, a weighted 24-h score of typical daily activity, based on hours spent doing heavy, moderate, light, or sedentary activity was computed and used as a covariate(43) The field workers also measured blood pressure at three time points during the study. Study participants with mean systolic blood pressure ≥ 140 mm Hg and/or a mean diastolic blood pressure ≥ 90 mm Hg were classified as having hypertension. A certified phlebotomist drew fasting (12-h) blood samples in the home of the study participants on a day following the interview. Participants with fasting plasma glucose > 7.0 nmol/L or those who reported taking medications for diabetes (insulin or oral medicines) were considered as having type 2 diabetes (44). The TaqMan single nucleotide polymorphism genotyping assay (Applied Biosystems) was used to assess APOE genotype, with a success rate of 95%.
Statistical methods:
R version 3.3.1 was used for all statistical analyses. Compared to participants who were excluded due to missing data on exposure and outcome, included participants were older (mean age 58.1 vs 56.4, p<0.001), had higher serum 25(OH)D (mean 43.8 nmol/L vs 35.5 nmol/L; p<0.001), higher baseline MMSE score (mean 23.9 vs 20.01; p<0.001), and a higher proportion of women (73.1% vs. 59.2%; p < 0.001).
Primary analyses were conducted using quintiles of baseline 25(OH)D concentration. In the analyses of executive and memory domains, we used tertiles of baseline 25(OH)D because of the low number of participants who met the criteria for significant (>1 standard deviation) decline in those domains. We performed additional sensitivity analyses with serum 25(OH)D in three categories: sufficient (c25(OH)D >=30 nmol/L), insufficient (20 nmol/L <= c25(OH)D < 30 nmol/L) and deficient (c25(OH)D < 20 nmol/L). The results of these analyses are presented in the supplementary tables. We used multivariable linear regression and calculated the adjusted mean differences and 95% confidence intervals (95% CI) in cognitive decline across quintiles of 25(OH)D. We adjusted our multivariate models for age in years, sex, season of blood collection, physical activity (physical activity (PA) score <30, PA score 30–40, PA score 40–50, PA score >=50), education (<5th grade, 5–8th grade, 9–12 grade, some college or college degree, some graduate school), diabetes (yes/no), smoking (never/past/current), alcohol use (never/past current), hypertension (yes/no), BMI (normal weight/overweight/obese), multivitamin supplement use (yes/no), CRP, and APOE (presence of APOE 4). Linear trends were tested for significance with a linear variable representing the median of the quintiles. We used the missing indicator method to account for missingness in model covariates.
The goal of our primary analyses was to assess the association between serum 25(OH)D and a global cognitive decline score. In addition, we examined associations of baseline serum 25(OH)D with baseline values as well as 2-year change in each individual test score including MMSE, world list learning, recognition, percentage retention, Stroop, letter fluency, digit span forward and backward, clock drawing, and weighted figure copying).
We adjusted all multivariable models for age in years, season of blood collection, sex, education (less than 5th grade/5th-8th grade/9th-12th grade/some college or bachelor’s degree/at least some graduate school), diabetes, smoking (never/past/current), alcohol use (never/past/current), c-reactive protein (CRP, < 3 vs. higher), hypertension (yes/no), body mass index (BMI), multivitamin supplement use, and APOE (presence of APOE 4). Longitudinal models of 2-year cognitive change were additionally adjusted for the appropriate baseline cognitive score.
Because Vitamin D supplements may have been prescribed to participants with severely low 25(OH)D levels, and may confound the association between serum 25(OH)D levels and cognition, we conducted sensitivity analyses adjusting for initiation of supplement use between baseline and year 2. We also conducted sensitivity analyses restricted to participants a) who did not initiate vitamin D supplement use between baseline and year 2 (N = 840) and b) who never reported vitamin D supplement use at baseline or year 2 (N = 744). We performed additional sensitivity analyses adding a measure of average skin melanin to our models, as well as adjusting for depression using baseline values of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale.
Finally, we assessed effect modification due to the association between serum 25(OH)D and cognitive decline by age, sex, smoking or BMI. Effect modification was evaluated by including multiplicative terms of these variables with the cognitive decline variable in the regression models. We performed these interaction analyses for our primary analyses - the global cognitive decline models.
RESULTS
This sample of Puerto Rican adults tended to have low education, and high prevalence of diabetes, hypertension and obesity (Table 1). Participants in the higher quintiles of serum 25(OH)D were older (p-trend 0.03), had lower prevalence of current smoking (p-trend 0.03), higher physical activity (p-trend: 0.02) and had lower prevalence of high CRP (p-trend 0.07). Baseline BMI, MMSE score, sex distribution, % past smoker, education level, current or past alcohol use, and prevalence of diabetes or hypertension did not differ significantly across vitamin D quintiles.
Table 1.
Baseline Serum Vitamin D Quintile | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Q1 | Q2 | Q3 | Q4 | Q5 | p-trend | |
n = 150 | n = 213 | n = 219 | n = 179 | n = 206 | ||
Serum 25(OH)D (range) | 21.2 | 31.5 | 41.5 | 51.1 | 68.4 | |
nmol/L | (10–25) | (27–35) | (38–45) | (48–55) | (58–120) | |
Age (mean), yearsa | 55.6 | 56.0 | 56.4 | 56.8 | 57.1 | 0.03 |
BMI (mean), kg/m2 | 32.6 | 32.2 | 31.8 | 31.4 | 31.0 | 0.63 |
MMSE score (mean) | 24.1 | 24.0 | 23.9 | 23.8 | 23.7 | 0.21 |
Women %b | 74.7 | 74.0 | 73.3 | 72.5 | 71.8 | 0.53 |
Past smoker % | 29.4 | 30.2 | 30.9 | 31.8 | 32.6 | 0.51 |
Current smoker % | 27.4 | 24.8 | 22.3 | 20.0 | 17.9 | 0.03 |
Physical activity score (mean) | 31.4 | 31.6 | 31.8 | 32.0 | 32.2 | 0.02 |
Less then high school education % | 41.7 | 41.0 | 40.3 | 39.5 | 38.8 | 0.37 |
Current alcohol use % | 44.1 | 43.9 | 43.7 | 43.6 | 43.4 | 0.96 |
Past alcohol use % | 28.9 | 28.5 | 28.2 | 27.8 | 27.4 | 0.63 |
Diabetes % | 36.9 | 36.1 | 35.4 | 34.7 | 33.9 | 0.39 |
High CRP (≥3 mg/dL) % | 61.5 | 59.3 | 57.0 | 54.7 | 52.4 | 0.07 |
Hypertension % | 71.7 | 70.3 | 68.9 | 67.5 | 66.1 | 0.72 |
All values adjusted for age and sex.
adjusted for sex
adjusted for age
After adjusting for potential confounders, including age in years, sex, season of blood collection, physical activity, education, diabetes smoking, alcohol use, hypertension, BMI, multivitamin supplement use, CRP, and APOE4, serum 25(OH)D was not associated with global cognitive function at baseline (Table 2, Model 2). The mean difference in global cognition, comparing participants in the bottom to those in the top quintile of 25(OH)D, was 0.09 (95% CI: −0.02, 0.19; p-trend: 0.17) Likewise, in a longitudinal model adjusted for covariates, baseline 25(OH)D was not significantly associated with 2-year cognitive change. The mean difference in 2-year global cognitive decline, comparing participants in the bottom to those in the top quintile of 25(OH)D, was −0.01 (95% CI: −0.09, 0.07), p-trend: 0.64) (Table 3, Model 2). Results of sensitivity analyses treating 25(OH)D in categories of deficient, insufficient and sufficient, were similar to the main analyses (Supplementary Tables S2 and S3).
Table 2.
Baseline Serum 25(OH)D Quintile | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Q1 | Q2 | Q3 | Q4 | Q5 | p-trend | ||
n = 150 | n = 213 | n = 219 | n = 179 | n = 206 | |||
Global Cognitive Score | Model 1b | 0.06 (−0.06, 0.17) |
0.03 (−0.08, 0.13) |
0.001 (−0.10, 0.10) |
0.05 (−0.06, 0.16) |
Ref | 0.26 |
Model 2c | 0.09 (−0.02, 0.19) |
0.06 (−0.03, 0.16) |
0.07 (−0.02, 0.17) |
0.08 (−0.02, 0.18) |
Ref | 0.18 |
The baseline global cognitive score was calculated as the mean of the z-scores of baseline values of the following cognitive tests: MMSE, word list learning, recognition, percentage retention, Stroop, letter fluency, digit span forward and backward, clock drawing, and weighted figure copying.
Multivariate model adjusted for age and sex and season of blood collection.
multivariable model, adjusted for age in years, sex, season of blood collection, physical activity (physical activity (PA) score <30, PA score 30–40, PA score 40–50, PA score >=50), education (<5th grade, 5–8th grade, 9–12 grade, some college or college degree, some graduate school), diabetes (yes/no), smoking (never/past/current), alcohol use (never/past current), hypertension (yes/no), BMI (normal weight/overweight/obese), multivitamin supplement use (yes/no), CRP, and APOE (presence of APOE 4).
Table 3.
Baseline Serum 25(OH)D Quintile | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Global Cognitive Score | Q1 | Q2 | Q3 | Q4 | Q5 | p-trend | |
n = 150 | n = 213 | n = 219 | n = 179 | n = 206 | |||
Model 1b | −0.01 (−0.09, 0.07) |
−0.03 (−0.10, 0.04) |
−0.01 (−0.09, 0.06) |
−0.02 (−0.09, 0.06) |
Ref | 0.66 | |
Model 2c | −0.01 (−0.09, 0.07) |
−0.01 (−0.08, 0.06) |
0.01 (−0.06, 0.08) |
−0.0008 (−0.07, 0.08) |
Ref | 0.61 |
The 2-year global cognitive change score was calculated as the mean of the z-scores of the changes between baseline and year 2 of MMSE, word list learning, recognition, percentage retention, Stroop, letter fluency, digit span forward and backward, clock drawing, and weighted figure copying.
Multivariate model adjusted for age, sex, season of blood collection and baseline score
multivariable model, adjusted for age in years, sex, season of blood collection, physical activity (physical activity (PA) score <30, PA score 30–40, PA score 40–50, PA score >=50), education (<5th grade, 5–8th grade, 9–12 grade, some college or college degree, some graduate school), diabetes (yes/no), smoking (never/past/current), alcohol use (never/past current), hypertension (yes/no), BMI (normal weight/overweight/obese), multivitamin supplement use (yes/no), CRP, and APOE (presence of APOE 4).
In multivariable adjusted cross-sectional analyses of individual cognitive tests, baseline serum 25(OH)D was inversely related to performance on the Stroop test (p-trend: 0.02) but not on any of the other tests. In longitudinal analyses, higher 25(OH)D concentration was related to better performance on the test of figure copying (p-trend: 0.02), but not any other tests. Results of analyses of individual cognitive tests are presented in supplemental tables S4 and S5.
We did not observe significant associations between baseline 25(OH)D and either the executive function or memory domains. Comparing the lowest to the highest tertile of baseline serum 25(OH)D, the odds of decline were 1.16 (95% CI: 0.50, 2.71; p-trend: 0.58) for 2-year change in executive function and 1.09 (95% CI: 0.62, 1.92; p-trend: 0.81) for memory (Table 4). Results for executive and memory domains were similar in linear models (Supplemental Table S1), as well as in cross-setional analyses.
Table 4.
Baseline Serum 25(OH)D Tertile | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Tertile 1 | Tertile 2 | Tertile 3 | p-trend | ||
Executive Function Component Score Decline (>=1SD) | |||||
OR (95% CI) | Crudea | 1.16 (0.50, 2.69) |
1.46 (0.67, 3.25) |
REF | 0.62 |
OR (95% CI) | Multivb | 1.16 (0.50, 2.71) |
1.81 (0.67, 3.25) |
REF | 0.58 |
Memory Component Score Decline (>=1SD) | |||||
OR (95% CI) | Crudea | 1.07 (0.63, 1.80) |
0.87 (0.52, 1.48) |
REF | 0.73 |
OR (95% CI) | Multivb | 1.09 (0.62, 1.92) |
0.83 (0.47, 1.45) |
REF | 0.81 |
adjusted for age, sex and baseline score
multivariable model, adjusted for age in years, sex, season of blood collection, physical activity (physical activity (PA) score <30, PA score 30–40, PA score 40–50, PA score >=50), education (<5th grade, 5–8th grade, 9–12 grade, some college or college degree, some graduate school), diabetes (yes/no), smoking (never/past/current), alcohol use (never/past current), hypertension (yes/no), BMI (normal weight/overweight/obese), multivitamin supplement use (yes/no), CRP, and APOE (presence of APOE 4).
Results of sensitivity analyses adjusting for initiation of vitamin D supplement use between baseline and year 2 of the study, and for average skin melanin, did not differ significantly from our main results. Furthermore, the results of analyses restricted to participants who a) did not initiate vitamin D supplements (N = 840) or b) never reported vitamin D supplement use (N = 744) did not differ from our main results. Results adjusted for baseline depression symptomology (CESD scale) were not materially different from the main results.
We did not observe significant interactions with smoking, BMI, sex, or baseline MMSE score (all p-interaction > 0.1). In analyses restricted to participants 50 years of age or younger, the mean difference in 2-year cognitive declie comparing participants in the top vs. bottom quintile of 25(OH)D was 0.095 (p-trend: 0.25).
DISCUSSION
Key Results:
In the population of Boston Area Puerto Rican adults considered in this study, we observed no significant association between serum 25(OH)D and cognition or cognitive decline over 2 years of follow-up.
Interpretation:
Epidemiological evidence on the association between serum 25(OH)D and cognition has been mixed. A number of cross-sectional studies have shown lower performance on tests of cognition in persons with low 25(OH)D concentration, compared to those with sufficient concentration (18–21, 45–52). However, cross-sectional studies may be subject to reverse causation. Longitudinal studies and randomized control trials are more suited to support a potential causal association between 25(OH)D and coginitive impairment. Several longitudinal studies have observed an association between lower serum 25(OH)D and faster rates of cognitive decline(21, 28, 53, 54). Llewellyn et al. (53) found that elderly participants with low 25(OH)D had an increased risk of cognitive decline over 6 years. In a longitudinal cohort of 6247 community-dwelling older women, Slinin et al. (54) reported higher odds of global cognitive decline over 4 years (OR = 1.31 (95% CI: 1.04–1.64) among women with deficient 25(OH)D (54). Matchar et al. (28) followed a community-based longitudinal cohort of 1202 cognitively intact elderly adults in China for 2 years and found a doubling in the odds of cognitive decline and a tripling in the odds of incident cognitive impairment (MMSE score <18) among participants in the lowest compared to the highest quartile of serum 25(OH)D. In a study of 1639 participants who underwent cognitive testing via telephone, Breitling et al. (27) reported twice the odds of 2-year decline in COGTEL scores in women, and a weaker association in men. In the Progetto Veneto Anziani (Pro.V.A.) cohort that followed 1,927 elderly cognitively intact participants at baseline, deficient baseline 25(OH)D was associated with a 30% increase in the risk of cognitive dysfunction over 4.4 years (55).
Despite the above findings of a significant association between serum 25(OH)D and cognition, a number of other studies detected no such association. Chan et al. (56), found no significant association between baseline 25(OH)D and cognition in cross-sectional analyses among 939 community-dwelling Chinese men aged over 65. Slinin et al. (26) reported no association between serum 25(OH)D and either baseline cognitive function or incident cognitive decline in a study of 1604 elderly men enrolled in the Osteoporotic Fractures in Men Study. In another cohort study of community-living elderly men, Olsson et al. (25) also showed no significant association between baseline Vitamin D status, including plasma 25(OH)D, and dementia or cognitive impairment over 18 years.
Prior studies of 25(OH)D and cognition have varied with regards to exposure (dietary vs serum 25(OH)D, outcome (cognitive tests used) assessment, as well as study design and duration. Cross-sectional studies are particularly subject to reverse causation, because poor cognitive status could lead to poor nutrition, lower physical activity and lower sunlight exposure, and thus low serum 25(OH)D.
Strengths and limitations:
Our study has several strengths, particularly a longitudinal design, thorough assessment of exposure, covariates and outcome. The study benefited from extensive and repeated assessments of cognition, including a variety of validated instruments. The study also benefited from careful assessment of potential confounders, including dietary factors, and comorbidities, at baseline. This allowed us to adjust our analyses for these potential confounders and also to examine the potential for effect modification. The use of serum 25(OH)D, a widely used and accepted biomarker of Vitamin D status is also a strength of our study, as 25(OH)D provides a reliable estimate of a person’s Vitamin D status and has been used extensively is studies of vitamin D and chronic disease (14).
Generalizability:
This study focused on a unique and underserved population of community dwelling American Puerto Ricans, which allowed us to contribute much-needed evidence of the potential association between serum 25(OH)D and cognition relevant to this group, which suffers from many health disparities(29). However, because of the unique population used, our findings might not be entirely generalizable to other populations. The significance of the finding is relevant to Hispanics and Puerto Ricans residing within the US. According to the US census, Hispanics are the fastest-growing population in the US, and are expected to become the majority in the US by 2045. Puerto Ricans are the 2nd most prevalent Hispanic group in the US, after Mexican Americans.
The relatively short two-year follow-up interval between cognitive testing may have limited our ability to detect an association between 25(OH)D and cognitive change. Because vitamin D status is dependent on sun exposure, it may vary across the year. We did not have a measure for sun exposure, but adjusted our results for season of blood collection. Furthermore, although we adjusted for a wide range of potential confounders, we cannot exclude confounding by other non-measured factors and residual confounding.
In summary, the results of our study do not support the hypothesis that higher serum 25(OH)D may improve cognition and impact 2-year cognitive change in Puerto Ricans living in Massachusetts. However, larger studies with longer follow-up in a variety of populations is needed to better understand the role of Vitamin D in cognition and cognition change.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
The authors thank Esther Carver for assistance with this manuscript.
Funding
This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health under Grant P01 AG023394 and Grant P50 HL105185. Natalia Palacios receives funding from the National Institutes of Health under Grant R01 NS097723.
Biographical Notes:
Natalia Palacios is an Assistant Professor in the Department of Public Health at the University of Massachussets, Lowell. Her research focuses on the epidemiology of neurolgoical disease, including Parkinson’s Disease, ALS and cognitive decline. She is interested in the role of diet, lifestyle factors and biomarkers in these conditions, particularly the gut microbiome. Is currently PI of an NIND R01 investigating the assocaition between the gut microbiome and Parkinson Disease within the Nurses Health Study and Health Professionals Follow-up Study cohorts.
Tammy Scott is an Assistant Professor at the Friedman School of Nutrition Science and Policy, as well as the Tufts University School of Medicine Department of Psychiatry. Her research is focused on the impact of nutrition on aging and cognition, mood and quality of life. She has extensive experience in conducting human studies, and is the neurocognition expert in multiple NIH- and foundation-funded clinical trials and observational studies. She is affiliated with the Tufts Clinical and Translational Science Institute (CTSI) as part of the Biostatistics, Epidemiology, and Research Design (BERD) Center. Scott has served for over twelve years on the Tufts Health Sciences Campus Institutional Review Board, and is on the editorial advisory board for the Tufts Health and Nutrition Letter.
Neha Sahasrabudhe is a doctoral student in Public Health at the Univeristy of Massacnussets, Lowell. Her doctoral thesis focuses on dietary and lifestyle factors and risk of chronic disease within the Boston Area Puerto Rican Study. She holds a Masters degree in mathematics from the University of Massachussets, Lowell.
Xiang Gao is an Associate Professor in the Department of Nutritional Sciences, College of Health and Human Development, Pennsylvania State University. Dr Gao’s research interests include nutritional Epidemiology, sleep disorders, neurological diseases, aging, global health and stress. Data from several large international ongoing prospective cohorts have been used, in which approximately ~500,000 men and women have been followed regarding their lifestyles and health conditions. These cohorts include the Nurses’ Health Study, the Health Professionals Follow-up Study, the Cancer Prevention Study II Nutrition Cohort, and the Kailuan Cohort.
Katherine Tucker is Professor of Nutritional Epidemiology and Director of the Center for Propulation Health at UMass Lowell. She is Editor-in Chief of Advanced in Nutrition, an international review journal. Dr. Tucker has contributed to more than 3000 articles in scientific journals. Her research focuses on dietary intake and risk of chronic diseae, including osteoporosis, cognitive decline, obesity, metabolic syndrome, and heart disease, and on dietary methodology. She is PI of the Boston Puerto Rican Health Study, a longitudinal study on the roles and interactions of stress, social support, diet, and health behavior and genetic predisposition in relation to health disparities in Puerto Rican adults.
Footnotes
Disclosure of Interest
The authors report no conflicts of interst.
Contributor Information
Natalia Palacios, Department of Public Health, University of Massachusetts Lowell, Southwick Hall, 326C, Lowell, MA 01854, Phone: 978-934-5373, natalia_palacios@uml.edu.
Tammy Scott, Neuroscience and Aging Laboratory, JM USDA Human Nutrition Research Center on Aging, Tufts University, 711 Washington St., Boston, MA 02111, Phone: 617-556-3356, Tammy.scott@tufts.edu.
Neha Sahasrabudhe, Department of Public Health, University of Massachusetts Lowell, Southwick Hall, 326C, Lowell, MA 01854, Phone: 978-934-5373, Neha_Sahasrabudhe@student.uml.edu
Xiang Gao, Department of Nutritional Sciences, College of Health and Human Development, Pennsylvania State University, 110 Chandlee Laboratory, University Park, PA 16802, Phone: 814-867-5959, xxg14@psu.edu.
Katherine L. Tucker, Department of Biomedical and Nutritional Sciences, University of Massachusetts Lowell, 3 Solomont Way, Suite 4, Lowell, MA 01854, Phone: 978-934-4066, katherine_tucker@uml.edu.
REFERENCES
- 1.Ascherio A, Munger KL, Simon KC. Vitamin D and multiple sclerosis. Lancet Neurol. 2010;9(6):599–612. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(10)70086-7. PubMed PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Raghuwanshi A, Joshi SS, Christakos S. Vitamin D and multiple sclerosis. J Cell Biochem. 2008;105(2):338–43. doi: 10.1002/jcb.21858. PubMed PMID: ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC2859312. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Mohr SB. A Brief History of Vitamin D and Cancer Prevention. Annals of Epidemiology. 2009;19(2):79–83. doi: 10.1016/j.annepidem.2008.10.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Garland CF, Gorham ED, Mohr SB, Garland FC. Vitamin D for cancer prevention: global perspective. Ann Epidemiol. 2009;19(7):468–83. doi: 10.1016/j.annepidem.2009.03.021. PubMed PMID: . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Grandi NC, Breitling LP, Brenner H. Vitamin D and cardiovascular disease: systematic review and meta-analysis of prospective studies. Prev Med. 2010;51(3–4):228–33. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2010.06.013. PubMed PMID: . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Parker J, Hashmi O, Dutton D, Mavrodaris A, Stranges S, Kandala NB, et al. Levels of vitamin D and cardiometabolic disorders: systematic review and meta-analysis. Maturitas. 2010;65(3):225–36. doi: 10.1016/j.maturitas.2009.12.013. PubMed PMID: . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Lavie CJ, Lee JH, Milani RV. Vitamin D and cardiovascular disease will it live up to its hype? J Am Coll Cardiol. 2011;58(15):1547–56. doi: 10.1016/j.jacc.2011.07.008. PubMed PMID: . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Chei CL, Raman P, Yin ZX, Shi XM, Zeng Y, Matchar DB. Vitamin D levels and cognition in elderly adults in China. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2014;62(11):2125–9. doi: 10.1111/jgs.13082. PubMed PMID: ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC4241162. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Yin K, Agrawal DK. Vitamin D and inflammatory diseases. J Inflamm Res. 2014;7:69–87. doi: 10.2147/JIR.S63898. PubMed PMID: ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC4070857. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Annweiler C, Schott AM, Berrut G, Chauvire V, Le Gall D, Inzitari M, et al. Vitamin D and ageing: neurological issues. Neuropsychobiology. 2010;62(3):139–50. doi: 10.1159/000318570. PubMed PMID: . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Buell JS, Dawson-Hughes B. Vitamin D and neurocognitive dysfunction: preventing “D”ecline? Mol Aspects Med. 2008;29(6):415–22. doi: 10.1016/j.mam.2008.05.001. PubMed PMID: ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC2829975. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Annweiler C, Dursun E, Feron F, Gezen-Ak D, Kalueff AV, Littlejohns T, et al. ‘Vitamin D and cognition in older adults’: updated international recommendations. J Intern Med. 2015;277(1):45–57. doi: 10.1111/joim.12279. PubMed PMID: 24995480. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Palacios C, Gil K, Pérez CM, Joshipura K. Determinants of Vitamin D Status among Overweight and Obese Puerto Rican Adults. Annals of Nutrition and Metabolism. 2012;60(1):35–43. doi: 10.1159/000335282. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Spiro A, Buttriss JL. Vitamin D: An overview of vitamin D status and intake in Europe. Nutr Bull. 2014;39(4):322–50. doi: 10.1111/nbu.12108. PubMed PMID: ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC4288313. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Jungert A, Spinneker A, Nagel A, Neuhauser-Berthold M. Dietary intake and main food sources of vitamin D as a function of age, sex, vitamin D status, body composition, and income in an elderly German cohort. Food Nutr Res. 2014;58. doi: 10.3402/fnr.v58.23632. PubMed PMID: ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC4168313. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Gonzalez-Rodriguez LG, Estaire P, Penas-Ruiz C, Ortega RM, VALORNUT UCMRG. Vitamin D intake and dietary sources in a representative sample of Spanish adults. J Hum Nutr Diet. 2013;26 Suppl 1: 64–72. doi: 10.1111/jhn.12061. PubMed PMID: . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Forrest KY, Stuhldreher WL. Prevalence and correlates of vitamin D deficiency in US adults. Nutr Res. 2011;31(1):48–54. doi: 10.1016/j.nutres.2010.12.001. PubMed PMID: . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Llewellyn DJ, Lang IA, Langa KM, Melzer D. Vitamin D and cognitive impairment in the elderly U.S. population. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2011;66(1):59–65. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glq185. PubMed PMID: ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC3011960. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Wilkins CH, Sheline YI, Roe CM, Birge SJ, Morris JC. Vitamin D Deficiency Is Associated With Low Mood and Worse Cognitive Performance in Older Adults. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2006;14(12):1032–40. doi: 10.1097/01.JGP.0000240986.74642.7c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Annweiler C, Fantino B, Schott AM, Krolak-Salmon P, Allali G, Beauchet O. Vitamin D insufficiency and mild cognitive impairment: cross-sectional association. Eur J Neurol. 2012;19(7):1023–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-1331.2012.03675.x. PubMed PMID: . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Jorde R, Mathiesen EB, Rogne S, Wilsgaard T, Kjaergaard M, Grimnes G, et al. Vitamin D and cognitive function: The Tromso Study. J Neurol Sci. 2015;355(1–2):155–61. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2015.06.009. PubMed PMID: . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Annweiler C, Allali G, Allain P, Bridenbaugh S, Schott AM, Kressig RW, et al. Vitamin D and cognitive performance in adults: a systematic review. European journal of neurology. 2009;16(10):1083–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-1331.2009.02755.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.van der Schaft J, Koek HL, Dijkstra E, Verhaar HJJ, van der Schouw YT, Emmelot-Vonk MH. The association between vitamin D and cognition: a systematic review. Ageing research reviews. 2013;12(4):1013–23. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2013.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Etgen T, Sander D, Bickel H, Sander K, Forstl H. Vitamin D deficiency, cognitive impairment and dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2012;33(5):297–305. doi: 10.1159/000339702. PubMed PMID: . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Olsson E, Byberg L, Karlstrom B, Cederholm T, Melhus H, Sjogren P, et al. Vitamin D is not associated with incident dementia or cognitive impairment: an 18-y follow-up study in community-living old men. Am J Clin Nutr. 2017;105(4):936–43. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.116.141531. PubMed PMID: . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Slinin Y, Paudel ML, Taylor BC, Fink HA, Ishani A, Canales MT, et al. 25-Hydroxyvitamin D levels and cognitive performance and decline in elderly men. Neurology. 2010;74(1):33–41. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181c7197b. PubMed PMID: ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC2809025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Breitling LP, Perna L, Muller H, Raum E, Kliegel M, Brenner H. Vitamin D and cognitive functioning in the elderly population in Germany. Exp Gerontol. 2012;47(1):122–7. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2011.11.004. PubMed PMID: 22123431. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Matchar DB, Chei CL, Yin ZX, Koh V, Chakraborty B, Shi XM, et al. Vitamin D Levels and the Risk of Cognitive Decline in Chinese Elderly People: the Chinese Longitudinal Healthy Longevity Survey. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2016;71(10):1363–8. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glw128. PubMed PMID: ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC5018565. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Tucker KL, Mattei J, Noel SE, Collado BM, Mendez J, Nelson J, et al. The Boston Puerto Rican Health Study, a longitudinal cohort study on health disparities in Puerto Rican adults: challenges and opportunities. BMC Public Health. 2010;10(1):107. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-10-107. PubMed PMID: ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC2848197. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Tucker KL, Bermudez OI, Castaneda C. Type 2 diabetes is prevalent and poorly controlled among Hispanic elders of Caribbean origin. Am J Public Health. 2000;90(8):1288–93. PubMed PMID: ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC1446320. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Tucker KL. Stress and nutrition in relation to excess development of chronic disease in Puerto Rican adults living in the Northeastern USA. J Med Invest. 2005;52 Suppl:252–8 PubMed PMID: . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Bermudez OI, Tucker KL. Total and central obesity among elderly Hispanics and the association with Type 2 diabetes. Obes Res. 2001;9(8):443–51. doi: 10.1038/oby.2001.58. PubMed PMID: . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Junyent M, Tucker KL, Shen J, Lee YC, Smith CE, Mattei J, et al. A composite scoring of genotypes discriminates coronary heart disease risk beyond conventional risk factors in the Boston Puerto Rican Health Study. Nutr Metab Cardiovasc Dis. 2010;20(3):157–64. doi: 10.1016/j.numecd.2009.03.016. PubMed PMID: ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC4031647. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Ye X, Scott T, Gao X, Maras JE, Bakun PJ, Tucker KL. Mediterranean diet, healthy eating index 2005, and cognitive function in middle-aged and older Puerto Rican adults. J Acad Nutr Diet. 2013;113(2):276–81 e1–3. doi: 10.1016/j.jand.2012.10.014. PubMed PMID: ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC3662236. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Wong JC, Scott T, Wilde P, Li YG, Tucker KL, Gao X. Food Insecurity Is Associated with Subsequent Cognitive Decline in the Boston Puerto Rican Health Study. J Nutr. 2016;146(9):1740–5. doi: 10.3945/jn.115.228700. PubMed PMID: ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC4997276. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Gao X, Scott T, Falcon LM, Wilde PE, Tucker KL. Food insecurity and cognitive function in Puerto Rican adults. Am J Clin Nutr. 2009;89(4):1197–203. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.2008.26941. PubMed PMID: ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC2667463. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Gao X, Lai CQ, Scott T, Shen J, Cai T, Ordovas JM, et al. Urinary 8-hydroxy-2-deoxyguanosine and cognitive function in Puerto Rican adults. Am J Epidemiol. 2010;172(3):271–8. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwq136. PubMed PMID: ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC3026653. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Shen J, Lai CQ, Mattei J, Ordovas JM, Tucker KL. Association of vitamin B-6 status with inflammation, oxidative stress, and chronic inflammatory conditions: the Boston Puerto Rican Health Study. Am J Clin Nutr. 2010;91(2):337–42. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.2009.28571. PubMed PMID: ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC2806890. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12(3):189–98. PubMed PMID: . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Artiola Frotuny L HRD, Heaton RK, Pardee RE III, editors. Manual de normas y procedimientos para la bateria neuropsicologica en espanol. [Manual of norms and procedures for the Spanish neuropsychological battery.]. Tucson (AZ): 2000. [Google Scholar]
- 41.Wolf-Klein GP, Silverstone FA, Levy AP, Brod MS. Screening for Alzheimer’s disease by clock drawing. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1989;37(8):730–4. PubMed PMID: . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Beery K The development test of visual-motor intergration manual. Modern Curriculum Press; 1989. [Google Scholar]
- 43.Kannel WB, Sorlie P. Some health benefits of physical activity. The Framingham Study. Arch Intern Med. 1979;139(8):857–61. Epub 1979/08/01. PubMed PMID: . [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Report of the expert committee on the diagnosis and classificaiton of diabetes mellitus. Diabetes Care. 2003;26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Annweiler C, Maby E, Meyerber M, Beauchet O. Hypovitaminosis D and executive dysfunction in older adults with memory complaint: a memory clinic-based study. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2014;37(5–6):286–93. Epub 2013/12/21. doi: 10.1159/000356483. PubMed PMID: . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Brouwer-Brolsma EM, van de Rest O, Tieland M, van der Zwaluw NL, Steegenga WT, Adam JJ, et al. Serum 25-hydroxyvitamin D is associated with cognitive executive function in Dutch prefrail and frail elderly: a cross-sectional study exploring the associations of 25-hydroxyvitamin D with glucose metabolism, cognitive performance and depression. J Am Med Dir Assoc. 2013;14(11):852 e9–17. Epub 2013/08/08. doi: 10.1016/j.jamda.2013.06.010. PubMed PMID: . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Brouwer-Brolsma EM, Feskens EJ, Steegenga WT, de Groot LC. Associations of 25-hydroxyvitamin D with fasting glucose, fasting insulin, dementia and depression in European elderly: the SENECA study. Eur J Nutr. 2013;52(3):917–25. Epub 2012/06/26. doi: 10.1007/s00394-012-0399-0. PubMed PMID: ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC3611027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Brouwer-Brolsma EM, Dhonukshe-Rutten RA, van Wijngaarden JP, van de Zwaluw NL, in ‘t Veld PH, Wins S, et al. Cognitive Performance: A Cross-Sectional Study on Serum Vitamin D and Its Interplay With Glucose Homeostasis in Dutch Older Adults. J Am Med Dir Assoc. 2015;16(7):621–7. Epub 2015/04/04. doi: 10.1016/j.jamda.2015.02.013. PubMed PMID: . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Buell JS, Scott TM, Dawson-Hughes B, Dallal GE, Rosenberg IH, Folstein MF, et al. Vitamin D is associated with cognitive function in elders receiving home health services. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2009;64(8):888–95. Epub 2009/04/21. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glp032. PubMed PMID: 19377013; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC2981461. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Darwish H, Zeinoun P, Ghusn H, Khoury B, Tamim H, Khoury SJ. Serum 25-hydroxyvitamin D predicts cognitive performance in adults. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2015;11:2217–23. Epub 2015/09/09. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S87014. PubMed PMID: ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC4556248. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Seamans KM, Hill TR, Scully L, Meunier N, Andrillo-Sanchez M, Polito A, et al. Vitamin D status and measures of cognitive function in healthy older European adults. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2010;64(10):1172–8. Epub 2010/08/12. doi: 10.1038/ejcn.2010.117. PubMed PMID: . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Wilkins CH, Birge SJ, Sheline YI, Morris JC. Vitamin D deficiency is associated with worse cognitive performance and lower bone density in older African Americans. J Natl Med Assoc. 2009;101(4):349–54. Epub 2009/04/29. PubMed PMID: ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC2801439. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Llewellyn DJ, Lang IA, Langa KM, Muniz-Terrera G, Phillips CL, Cherubini A, et al. Vitamin D and risk of cognitive decline in elderly persons. Arch Intern Med. 2010;170(13):1135–41. doi: 10.1001/archinternmed.2010.173. PubMed PMID: ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC4053858. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Slinin Y, Paudel M, Taylor BC, Ishani A, Rossom R, Yaffe K, et al. Association between serum 25(OH) vitamin D and the risk of cognitive decline in older women. The journals of gerontology Series A, Biological sciences and medical sciences. 2012;67(10):1092–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Toffanello ED, Coin A, Perissinotto E, Zambon S, Sarti S, Veronese N, et al. Vitamin D deficiency predicts cognitive decline in older men and women: The Pro.V.A. Study. Neurology. 2014;83(24):2292–8. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000001080. PubMed PMID: . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Chan R, Chan D, Woo J, Ohlsson C, Mellström D, Kwok T, et al. Association between serum 25-hydroxyvitamin D and psychological health in older Chinese men in a cohort study. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2011;130(1):251–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2010.10.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.