Abstract
Borderline personality pathology is a serious mental illness characterized by pervasive interpersonal deficits that onsets during adolescence. Risk factors for borderline personality pathology include maladaptive interpersonal dynamics within attachment relationships. Given the shift toward emphasizing romantic relationships during adolescence as an important attachment relationship with implications for healthy development, the current study aimed to evaluate the longitudinal and reciprocal relations between victimization in dating relationships and borderline pathology in the transition from late adolescence into early adulthood. A large sample of high school daters (N = 818; 58% female; Mage = 16.10 years, SDage = .78) were recruited to complete annual assessments of borderline personality features and dating violence victimization (DVV) across five years. Results of a cross-lagged panel model revealed that primarily among females, borderline features predicted increased levels of relational, psychological, and physical violence whereas psychological and sexual violence predicted greater borderline features. The current findings provide the first evidence of a longitudinal association between victimization and borderline pathology in adolescence and suggest, particularly among females, that interventions for borderline features have important implications for reducing dating violence victimization among adolescents and young adults.
Keywords: Borderline personality disorder, dating violence victimization, adolescence, longitudinal, risk factor
Borderline personality pathology is a serious mental illness characterized by affective, behavioral, and relational instability (Linehan, 1993). Though discrete “causes” of borderline pathology have not been identified, a number of risk factors have been suggested to contribute to its development. Major theories of the etiology of borderline pathology agree that an inborn, constitutional vulnerability interacts with what is referred to as a chronically invalidating environment. In a systematic review of studies, Stepp and colleagues (2016) found that family adversity and low SES, maternal psychopathology, affective parenting, and maltreatment are the most robust environmental predictors of borderline symptomology based on existing research. Due to the association between borderline pathology and attachment insecurity (Agrawal, Gunderson, Holmes, & Lyons-Ruth, 2004), it is not surprising that research into risk factors has focused on the child’s early caregiver relationships. However, attachment evolves further after childhood, and risk factors beyond the immediate family environment must be considered. Specifically, dating relationships in adolescence may represent an attachment relationship that is formative in the development, maintenance, or exacerbation of borderline pathology.
Up to 95% of US teens report ever having dated by the age of 18 (Manning, Longmore, Copp, & Giordano, 2014), corresponding to the age that adults in the US report having experienced their first episode of intimate partner violence (Breiding et al., 2014). A recent meta-analysis spanning multiple countries found the prevalence of physical violence victimization to be 21% among adolescents and sexual violence victimization to be 14% and 8% for females and males, respectively (Wincentak, Connolly, & Card, 2017). In the US specifically, a 2013 CDC study of high schoolers revealed that around 1 in 5 female and 1 and 10 male students were victimized by physical and/or sexual violence in the past year (Vagi, Olsen, Basile, & Vivolo-Kantor, 2015) with higher rates reported for psychological victimization according to the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health (Halpern, Oslak, Young, Martin, & Kupper, 2001). While research has clearly demonstrated that dating violence victimization (DVV) is a risk factor for psychopathology during adolescence, research related to borderline pathology is less prevalent.
To date there are only two studies conducted in adolescence examining the role of DVV in relation to borderline pathology, both of which were conducted in the US. One study found a positive association between borderline features and DVV in a large sample of high schoolers, with stronger relations among females (Reuter, Sharp, Temple, & Babcock, 2015). Another study replicated the concurrent relations between borderline features and DVV in a sample of adolescent inpatients; additionally, inpatients with high levels of borderline features exhibited similar rates of self-harm regardless of their experiences of DVV, whereas among those with low levels of borderline features, the presence of victimization related to increased self-harm (Hatkevich, Mellick, Reuter, Temple, & Sharp, 2017). These findings suggests that the co-occurrence of even low levels of borderline features and DVV puts adolescents at risk for self-damaging behaviors. While these studies provide an important first step in investigating the role of dating violence in borderline pathology, their cross-sectional nature prevents inferences about whether dating violence is truly a risk factor for borderline pathology. Further, they limit the investigation of more complex reciprocal relations between borderline pathology and dating violence. There has been much greater attention to the relations between borderline pathology and peer victimization and bullying, with findings from longitudinal studies generally converging on the fact that bullying increases risk for subsequent borderline pathology (Winsper, Hall, Strauss, & Wolke, 2017). A recent study conducted in Europe replicated this association; however, they found that only among females was bullying associated with subsequent personality pathology, suggesting potential gender differences in the association between victimization and borderline pathology (Antila et al., 2017).
One of the limitations of cross-sectional designs to evaluate risk factors is the inability to determine temporal precedence. Various well-established correlates of borderline pathology are both longitudinal predictors and consequences of DVV. For instance, low self-esteem and maladaptive parent-child dynamics, correlates of borderline pathology, are robust predictors of victimization (Foshee, Benefield, Ennett, Bauman, & Suchindran, 2004). Additionally, common comorbidities of borderline pathology including substance use, suicidal ideation, and depressive symptoms, are predicted by DVV (Exner-Cortens, Eckenrode, & Rothman, 2013). Therefore, while not tested previously, existing evidence suggests that the relation between borderline pathology and DVV may be reciprocal in nature. Specifically, adolescents with borderline features are likely to be experiencing maladaptive parent-child dynamics and low self-esteem, which is related to risk for being victimized by dating partners. Further, if victimization is present, existing borderline features are likely to be exacerbated. However, these relations must be parsed from stability of borderline pathology (Bornovalova, Hicks, Iacono, & McGue, 2009) and dating victimization (Foshee et al., 2004), which requires the use of longitudinal designs.
Despite limitations in design among adolescent studies, research conducted with adults provide some clues regarding the role of victimization in the development of borderline pathology in adolescents, especially given that interaction patterns within relationships are often established in adolescence (Bouchey & Furman, 2006). While research is more prevalent that demonstrates general relationship dysfunction associated with borderline pathology (Daley, Burge, & Hammen, 2000), there is evidence that borderline features are overrepresented among individuals who have been the victim of intimate partner violence (Pico-Alfonso, Echebúma, & Martinez, 2008). Further, in a study by Maneta and colleagues (2013), both males’ and females’ borderline features were related to romantic partners’ perpetration of violence against them. These authors suggested that individuals with borderline features may be more likely to choose partners prone to violence, or that reactive and dysregulated behaviors may elicit aggressive responses from others. To be clear, victims are never to be blamed for their victimization; nevertheless, it is important to understand factors that contribute to violence.
Understanding the dynamics between borderline pathology and DVV during adolescence has potential value in improving our understanding of the development and maintenance of this disorder. Given that adolescent romantic relationships influence developmental tasks of adolescence such as identity and sexual development (Exner-Cortens, 2014), there may be a feedback loop in which dating victimization exacerbates the presence of borderline features. To this end, the current study examined temporal associations and lagged effects between borderline personality features and dating violence across late adolescence through young adulthood. As adolescents reduce their dependence on parents as exclusive attachment figures, they become more reliant on non-familial relations, especially peers and romantic partners (Scharf & Mayseless, 2007). Romantic relationships, in particular, have more distinct intensity than peers and therefore may be more salient (Collins, Welsh, & Furman, 2009). We expected to find reciprocal associations between borderline features and DVV such that greater borderline features would predict greater victimization and vice versa; however, we had no a priori hypotheses about the types of violence that may be related to borderline pathology. We also expected to find at least moderate autoregressive associations within each construct. Given that consequences of victimization differ depending on the form and severity of violence (Mechanic, Weaver, & Resick, 2008) and that the two previous studies conducted in adolescence utilized an overarching measure of DVV (Hatkevich et al., 2017; Reuter et al., 2015), we evaluated different forms of dating violence, although in the same model. Therefore, this is the first study to evaluate differential effects of various forms of dating violence on borderline pathology.
Methods
Participants and Procedures
Participants were recruited from seven public schools representing five major school districts in a large and diverse metropolitan city in the United States as part of a longitudinal study (Temple, Shorey, Tortolero, Wolfe, & Stuart, 2013). The current sample is the same that was utilized in the previous study by Reuter and colleagues (2015). Study recruitment and assessment occurred during school hours in classes with mandated attendance. Assessments continued annually for five years. Participants completed post-graduation assessments via a web-based platform. All students present in the selected classes were eligible to participate and there was a response rate of 62%, with a final recruitment of N = 1,042. 34 subjects did not participate at any wave of the study. Other patterns of missing data described below.
For the purpose of including the same individuals across waves in the current study, only participants who reported having dated at each wave they participated in were included (excluding n = 224). This was determined based responses to the question “Please check the statement that best applies to you” as either “I have begun dating, going out with someone, or had a boyfriend/girlfriend” or “I have not yet begun dating or going out with someone” for waves 1-3. From waves 4-5, participants responded to the question “How many boyfriends/ girlfriends or dating partners have you had since the last survey?”. Those who selected a response “I have not yet begun dating or going out with someone” or “I have never dated” were excluded. This rate of dating (78% of the 1,008 who participated in at least one wave) is consistent with previous reports of high schoolers in the US; in 2013, the CDC found that 75% of female and 72.8% of male high schoolers reported dating in the past year (Vagi et al., 2015).
At the initial time point, the sample had a mean age of 16.10 years (SD = .78), was 58% female, and identified their race/ethnicity as Hispanic (32%), White/not Hispanic (31.3%), African American (27.1%), Asian/Pacific Islander (1.8%), and Other/Mixed Race (7.7%). Analyses were conducted to determine whether the participants who were included in this study differed from the overall sample. Those who were excluded due to incomplete data (at all waves) were significantly older (Mage = 16.44, SDage = 0.86) than those who were included (Mage = 16.08, SDage = 0.79; t(1040) = −2.64, p= .008); however, they did not differ in regard to gender (χ2(1) = 0.13, p = .719) or race (χ2(4) = 5.54, p = .236). Those who were excluded due to dating status did not differ in gender (χ2(1) = 3.51, p = .061), age (t(1040) = −.71, p = .477), or borderline personality features at all five waves (t’s(656-879) = −1.84 - .53, p’s = .066 - .685). However, those who were excluded based on dating status differed on race/ethnicity (χ2(4) = 40.48, p < .001) with the biggest discrepancy between rate of dating and expected rate being among those who identified as Asian or Pacific Islander (more likely to report not having dated across waves) and White/not Hispanic (less likely to report not having dated across waves).
The current study was approved by the appropriate institutional review board. Research staff presented the study to students and answered any questions, and take-home packets with study information and parental consent forms were sent home. Students who returned with parental consent provided assent and completed assessments during school hours. Participants were compensated with $10 (years 1-3) and $20 (year 4-5) gift cards for participating.
Measures
Borderline Personality Features.
The 24-item Borderline Personality Feature Scale for Children (BPFS-C; Crick, Murray–Close, & Woods, 2005) was used. The BPFS-C includes indicators of childhood borderline features such as affective instability, identity problems, negative relationships, and self-harm. Item responses are on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from “not true at all” to “always true”. Examples of items include “I get into trouble because I do things without thinking” and “I feel that there is something important missing about me, but I don’t know what it is.” Research supports the criterion and concurrent validity of both parent and child reports of the BPFS (Chang, Sharp, & Ha, 2011; Sharp, Mosko, Chang, & Ha, 2011). In the current sample among females and males, respectively, Cronbach’s alpha was .87 and .90 in year 1; .88 and .89 in year 2; .88 and .90 in year 3; and .91 for both genders in years 4 and 5.
Dating Violence Victimization.
The Conflict in Adolescent Dating and Relationship Inventory (CADRI; Wolfe et al., 2001) is a 50-item self-report measure assessing both dating violence perpetration and victimization across five domains: physical abuse, psychological and emotional verbal abuse, sexual abuse, threatening behavior, and relational aggression. Each question is divided into two parts, one that indicates perpetration (e.g., “I threw something at him/her”) and one that indicates victimization (“He or she threw something at me”). Using binary responses, participants reported whether or not they perpetrated and/or were victimized by an act during a conflict or argument with their boyfriend/girlfriend (or ex-boyfriend/ex-girlfriend) in the past year, which were summed to create a total score for each form of violence. Due to the low prevalence of DVV, many previous studies have dichotomized measure of violence; however, there is evidence, based on other measures of dating violence (e.g., Conflict Tactics Scale 2; Straus, Hamby, Boney-McCoy, & Sugarman, 1996), that using total scores created by summing the total number of acts within a specific scale is a close indication of a latent variable of violence severity. Additionally, using total scores gives equal weight to all abusive acts, with endorsement of more acts indicating greater severity because the most severe acts are those least frequently endorsed (Goncy, Farrell, Sullivan, & Taylor, 2016). Furthermore, constraining the variability of any measure into a dichotomous variable is problematic due to the loss of statistical power.
Four subscales were used for the current study. First, a scale evaluating relational aggression (3 items) evaluated the extent to which a romantic partner intruded and affected an individual’s peer relationships (“He/She tried to turn my friends against me”). Among females and males, respectively, internal consistency was .67 and .63 in year 1; .64 and .75 in year 2; .69 and .75 in year 3; .78 and .75 in year 4; and .69 and .65 in year 5. A scale evaluating psychological violence (10 items) included indices of emotional and verbal abuse (“He/She did something to try to make me jealous”). Among females and males, respectively, internal consistency was .81 and .77 in year 1; .83 and .80 in year 2; .85 and .85 in year 3; .86 and .86 in year 4; and .83 and .89 in year 5. A scale evaluating physical violence (4 items) assessed deliberate physical harm (“He/She threw something at me”). Among females and males, respectively, internal consistency was .79 and .58 in year 1; .80 and .79 in year 2; .82 and .73 in year 3; .82 and .73 in year 4; and .88 and .75 in year 5. Finally, a scale consisting of 4 items assessed sexual abuse (He/She touched me sexually when I didn’t want them to”). Among females and males, respectively, internal consistency was .51 and .41 in year 1; .44 and .41 in year 2; .58 and .63 in year 3; .69 and .44 in year 4; and .69 and .66 in year 5, which was notably less than the other scales.
Parent-Child Relationship Quality.
Four items assessed quality of mother and father relationships: “Do you feel close to your mother/father” and “Do you share your thoughts and feelings with your mother/father” were rated on a 4-point Likert scale ranging from “very true” to “very false”. Responses were averaged for an index of relationship quality with each parent.
Data Analysis
Descriptive statistics were run using SPSS version 25.0 (IBM Corp, 2016) to evaluate prevalence of DVV and borderline features at each year of the study. Further, we examined the gender breakdown of these rates using independent sample t-tests and Cohen’s D as a measure of effect size. Bivariate correlations were run to evaluate associations between these variables across all years of the study. As is common in longitudinal data, missing data was prevalent, which is described in more detail in an online supplement table. There were 1,042 students enrolled into the study one year prior to the first wave of data collection. Only 531 (51%) of these students participated in all waves of the study while 34 (3.3%) did not participate in any of the waves of data collection. Next to attrition, there was wave non-response (i.e., respondents in some but not all waves), which characterized 477 subjects (45.8%). We first evaluated missing data within each wave for assumptions of missing completely at random using Little’s MCAR test, which demonstrated that this assumption could be rejected at each wave. Next, we evaluated wave non-completion and attrition across time by evaluating differences between the group of individuals who participated at all waves and those who had any wave missingness. Logistic regressions were used to evaluate whether key variables at each wave (entered simultaneously) predicted missingness across the whole study with variables predicting participation at a significance level < .05 interpreted. Additionally, we examined group differences in age, race, and gender to determine whether they distinguished between those who participated at all waves, those who did not participate at any waves, and those who did not complete some waves. Those who reported higher physical victimization at Wave 2 were .74 times more likely to participate in all waves, but those reporting higher sexual victimization at Wave 2 were 1.44 times more likely to participate in all waves. Those who participated at all waves were significantly younger than those who either missed all waves or participated in some waves (F(2,1041) = 8.43, P < .001). There were a greater than expected proportion of females participating in some waves, but less than expected proportion of females participating in all waves (χ2(2)= 37.90, P < .001) and significant differences for completion based on race (χ2(8) = 20.44, p = .009).
Next, we utilized path analysis to examine relations between DVV and borderline features across five years. These analyses were conducted in MPlus version 8.1 (Muthén & Muthén, n.d.). With the exception of the measure of psychological violence, all measures of DVV were positively skewed with high kurtosis. Therefore, maximum likelihood estimation with robust standard errors (MLR) was used to account for missing data due to attrition, which is based on the assumptions of data missing at random and enables use of all available data. Across all models, fit was evaluated based on values of the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA; with values of less than .08 indicating reasonable fit and values above .10 suggesting poor fit; Browne & Cudeck, 1993), comparative fit index (CFI; with values between 0.95 and 1.00 indicating excellent fit and values between .90 and .95 indicating acceptable fit), and the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR; with values less than .08 indicating good fit, but with a large sample size and number of parameters, values less than .10 were considered acceptable; Kline, 2011).
The main path model evaluated the transactional relations between borderline features and all forms of dating violence across five years with concurrent correlations between all constructs estimated at each time point, autoregressive relations modeled within each construct over time, and one-year cross-lagged relations between each form of dating violence and borderline features (Figure 1). Additionally, given the inclusion of all forms of dating violence in the same model, we included cross-lagged paths between certain types of dating violence that were justified given previous research. Specifically, within relationships, sexual violence is likely to be preceded by physical violence (Howard & Wang, 2005), which is likely preceded by verbal abuse or psychological violence (Giordano, Soto, Manning, & Longmore, 2010). Further, given the complexity of the model, autoregressive paths within constructs were held equal when doing so did not result in significantly worse model fit. We used a multi-group analysis for the model; in order to determine differential fit of models across genders, chi-square difference test based on loglikelihood values and scaling correction factors obtained with the MLR estimator was used to compare nested models. Gender differences in model fit were tested due to the fact that in previous research, adolescent females have reported experiencing greater rates of sexual dating violence than males (Howard & Wang, 2005) and are more likely to experience negative consequences of dating violence (Hamby, Sugarman, & Boney-McCoy, 2006). It has also been found that reporting of borderline features differs across genders (Sharp et al., 2014), with females more easily endorsing self-harm and suicidality and affective instability and males more easily endorsing anger and impulsivity (Aggen, Neale, Røysamb, Reichborn-Kjennerud, & Kendler, 2009; Hoertel, Peyre, Wall, Limosin, & Blanco, 2014; Sharp et al., 2014), although when examined in community samples, there are little to no mean level differences in reports of borderline features (Johnson et al., 2003). We also controlled for the effects of SES on DVV and quality of parental relationships on borderline features in the analyses. Previous research has found higher rates of DVV among samples from disadvantaged neighborhoods (Wincentak et al., 2017) and harsh parenting has been found to not only predict dating violence (Jouriles, McDonald, Mueller, & Grych, 2012) but also BPD features in adolescence (Stepp et al., 2014). However, given that borderline pathology is related to negative perceptions of parenting quality, there is the possibility that controlling for these variables may remove valid variance of borderline features. Therefore, we also tested the final model while removing this covariate and examined differences in results.
Results
Table 1 displays descriptive statistics for and correlations between main study variables at each time point of the study. Examining correlations across time within single constructs revealed that correlations across time for BPD were medium to strong (.41-.58 across one time lag and .38 for the longest time lag). A similar pattern was seen for psychological violence (.47-.52 for one time lag and .31 for the longest time lag). Sexual violence showed medium sized correlations between scores one year apart. Interestingly, relational violence had the lowest magnitude of correlations across time (.13 - .27 for one time lag and .02 for the longest time lag). Across constructs at the same wave, correlations were mostly small to medium in magnitude with correlations between scores of relational violence and scores of physical and sexual violence being mostly small in magnitude. Similarly, correlations between borderline features scores and all forms of dating victimization were mostly small in magnitude.
Table 1.
1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 | 11 | 12 | 13 | 14 | 15 | 16 | 17 | 18 | 19 | 20 | 21 | 22 | 23 | 24 | 25 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. | BPF-1 | |||||||||||||||||||||||||
2. | BPF-2 | .58 | ||||||||||||||||||||||||
3. | BPF-3 | .50 | .55 | |||||||||||||||||||||||
4. | BPF-4 | .30 | .38 | .41 | ||||||||||||||||||||||
5. | BPF-5 | .38 | .39 | .45 | .43 | |||||||||||||||||||||
6. | Rel-1 | .14 | .09 | .06 | .01 | .04 | ||||||||||||||||||||
7. | Rel-2 | .15 | .13 | .08 | .03 | .05 | .13 | |||||||||||||||||||
8. | Rel-3 | .03 | .04 | .05 | .03 | .05 | .07 | .24 | ||||||||||||||||||
9. | Rel-4 | .12 | .11 | .11 | .10 | .10 | .10 | .30 | .27 | |||||||||||||||||
10. | Rel-5 | .06 | .05 | .09 | .10 | .15 | .02 | .19 | .05 | .24 | ||||||||||||||||
11. | Psy-1 | .28 | .23 | .15 | .03 | .14 | .33 | .17 | .05 | .11 | .06 | |||||||||||||||
12. | Psy-2 | .22 | .31 | .22 | .09 | .12 | .13 | .32 | .24 | .22 | .05 | .49 | ||||||||||||||
13. | Psy-3 | .22 | .19 | .25 | .13 | .18 | .12 | .15 | .35 | .14 | .14 | .38 | .52 | |||||||||||||
14. | Psy-4 | .18 | .18 | .21 | .21 | .22 | .04 | .16 | .15 | .40 | .20 | .31 | .41 | .47 | ||||||||||||
15. | Psy-5 | .12 | .22 | .19 | .21 | .27 | .06 | .10 | .11 | .13 | .36 | .31 | .34 | .43 | .48 | |||||||||||
16. | Phy-1 | .18 | .14 | .13 | −.05 | .10 | .26 | .09 | .05 | .05 | .05 | .50 | .27 | .22 | .15 | .16 | ||||||||||
17. | Phy-2 | .05 | .15 | .12 | .05 | .07 | .06 | .09 | .15 | .09 | .03 | .25 | .45 | .27 | .19 | .21 | .36 | |||||||||
18. | Phy-3 | .13 | .15 | .18 | .02 | .05 | .05 | .04 | .29 | .05 | .09 | .23 | .30 | .49 | .23 | .27 | .27 | .40 | ||||||||
19. | Phy-4 | .16 | .11 | .15 | .16 | .14 | .02 | .14 | .14 | .41 | .16 | .19 | .25 | .26 | .55 | .27 | .14 | .29 | .41 | |||||||
20. | Phy-5 | .08 | .08 | .10 | .11 | .15 | .04 | .05 | .06 | .13 | .34 | .15 | .14 | .23 | .30 | .55 | .13 | .22 | .27 | .44 | ||||||
21. | Sex-1 | .17 | .10 | .05 | −.00 | .10 | .29 | .14 | .14 | .08 | .06 | .41 | .24 | .19 | .14 | .21 | .43 | .20 | .21 | .15 | .16 | |||||
22. | Sex-2 | .13 | .12 | .07 | .00 | .08 | .11 | .18 | .11 | .19 | .11 | .19 | .34 | .19 | .21 | .20 | .16 | .26 | .19 | .17 | .07 | .34 | ||||
23. | Sex-3 | .11 | .09 | .07 | −.03 | .12 | .08 | .05 | .24 | .11 | .10 | .20 | .22 | .35 | .20 | .25 | .26 | .15 | .42 | .25 | .21 | .37 | .29 | |||
24. | Sex-4 | .16 | .12 | .09 | .08 | .15 | .09 | .06 | .14 | .24 | .09 | .23 | .21 | .24 | .37 | .21 | .28 | .13 | .29 | .38 | .19 | .40 | .36 | .46 | ||
25. | Sex-5 | .13 | .10 | .08 | .07 | .13 | .07 | −.03 | .07 | .04 | .27 | .16 | .12 | .16 | .18 | .39 | .21 | .10 | .12 | .11 | .42 | .25 | .15 | .29 | .37 | |
Fem.-M | 63.4 | 61.9 | 61.0 | 57.4 | 57.6 | 0.21 | 0.18 | 0.15 | 0.15 | 0.11 | 3.66 | 3.70 | 3.61 | 3.36 | 3.19 | 0.44 | 0.43 | 0.41 | 0.38 | 0.36 | 0.29 | 0.22 | 0.26 | 0.30 | 0.27 | |
SD | 12.7 | 12.7 | 12.6 | 15.5 | 14.8 | 0.60 | 0.54 | 0.52 | 0.55 | 0.41 | 2.82 | 2.96 | 3.10 | 3.10 | 2.85 | 0.98 | 0.98 | 0.98 | 1.01 | 0.89 | 0.64 | 0.55 | 0.65 | 0.75 | 0.72 | |
Male-M | 58.9 | 58.1 | 58.1 | 56.6 | 57.1 | 0.21 | 0.22 | 0.20 | 0.24 | 0.18 | 2.63 | 3.06 | 3.02 | 2.81 | 3.01 | 0.27 | 0.35 | 0.30 | 0.34 | 0.52 | 0.12 | 0.12 | 0.15 | 0.17 | 0.25 | |
SD | 14.1 | 12.8 | 13.7 | 13.6 | 15.5 | 0.58 | 0.63 | 0.61 | 0.67 | 0.55 | 2.42 | 2.63 | 2.87 | 2.95 | 3.18 | 0.67 | 0.88 | 0.79 | 0.87 | 1.11 | 0.41 | 0.41 | 0.54 | 0.50 | 0.68 | |
t | 4.5** | 3.8** | 2.6* | 0.6 | 0.4 | 0.2 | −1.0 | −1.1 | −1.6 | −1.8 | 5.3** | 3.0** | 2.3* | 2.0* | 0.7 | 2.7** | 1.2 | 1.5 | 0.4 | −1.9 | 4.0** | 2.5* | 2.1* | 2.2* | 0.3 | |
Cohen’s D | .34 | .30 | .22 | .05 | .03 | .01 | −.07 | −.09 | −.14 | −.14 | .39 | .23 | .20 | .18 | .06 | .21 | .09 | .12 | .04 | −.16 | .30 | .19 | .18 | .21 | .02 |
Note. Correlations > ∣.08∣ were significant at a .05 level, correlations > ∣ .11∣ were significant at a .01 level;
p<.01,
p<.05;
BPF: Borderline Personality Features Scale for Children; all DVV scales measured with the Conflicts in Adolescent Dating Relationships: Rel: Relational Violence, Psy: Psychological Violence, Phy: Physical Violence, Sex: Sexual Violence. Values from independent samples t-tests displayed examining mean differences between genders.
Results from independent samples t-tests demonstrated that females reported higher levels of borderline features, at least for the first three waves of the study (Cohen’s D ranging from .22 - .34). While statistically significant, differences were small in magnitude. Females, relative to males, reported higher levels of psychological (Waves 1-4), physical (Wave 1), and sexual violence (Waves 1-4) perpetrated against them (Cohen’s D ranging from .18 - .39), although again, these differences were small in magnitude. Given the bidirectional nature of violent dating relationships, we tested whether levels of victimization matched levels of perpetration as measured with the CADRI. In comparing means, we found small differences on annual reports of relational and sexual violence, with minimal differences across other scales (Cohen’s D for relational violence ranging from .18-.31 for Waves 1-4 and from .20-.23 for Waves 1, 2, and 4). These results are available from the first author upon request.
The majority of current or most recent dating relationships were reported to be heterosexual. Among females, rates of reported same sex relationships were 3.8% at Wave 1, 5.4% in Wave 2, 5.6% in Wave 3, 4.5% in Wave 4, and 4.8% in Wave 5. Among males, rates of reported same sex relationships were 4.3% in Wave 1, 6% in Wave 2, 8.7% in Wave 3, 8.8% in Wave 4, and 9.1% in Wave 5. Independent samples t-tests were conducted to evaluate whether those who reported being in a same sex relationship differed in their levels of borderline features and DVV to determine whether this would be a potential confound. Among females at Wave 1, those in a same sex relationship reported higher level of borderline features (t(401) = 2.31, p = .021, D = 0.64) but were not different in reports of any form of DVV (t’s = 0.55 – 0.87, ps > .05). Similarly, among males, those in a same sex relationship reported higher levels of borderline features (t(299) = 2.62, p = .009, D = 0.85) but did not differ in the amount of victimization reported (ts = −0.29 – −1.35, p’s > .05). Because there were no differences between those in same sex versus heterosexual relationships on rates of dating violence victimization, further analyses were conducted within the full sample.
To examine the longitudinal dynamics between borderline features and dating violence, we evaluated a cross-lagged panel model in which concurrent relations between all constructs were modeled as well as cross-lagged paths between borderline features and all forms of violence, from psychological to physical violence, and from physical violence to sexual violence (Figure 1). First, we tested whether constraining all autoregressive paths to be equal within each construct would result in significant changes in fit, which was not the case (χ2(30) = 37.78, p = .155). Next, in a model in which all paths were set to be equal across gender, fit was good according to the RMSEA estimate, but poor according to other indicators (χ2(628) = 1066,935, p < .001; RMSEA = .041 [90% CI: .037, .046]; SRMR = .101; CFI = .883). Next, all paths were freed between genders, leading to adequate model fit across all indicators (χ2(500) = 875.74, p > .001; RMSEA = .042 [90% CI: .028, .048]; SRMR = .090; CFI = .900). Model comparison test revealed that model fit improvement was statistically significant when allowing paths to differ between genders (χ2(128) = 194.45, p < .001). Cross-lagged path estimates for the non-constrained model are displayed in Table 2.
Table 2.
Girls n=470 | Boys n=348 | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Path | Unstd (SE) | Std (95% CI) | Unstd (SE) | Std (95% CI) |
W1 BPF → W2 Relational | 0.01 (.00)* | 0.15 (0.01, 0.29) | 0.00 (.00) | 0.08 (−0.05, 0.21) |
→ W2 Psych. | 0.03 (.01)* | 0.12 (0.01, 0.21) | 0.01 (.01) | 0.07 (−0.02 0.17) |
→ W2 Physical | 0.01 (.00) | 0.06 (−0.05, 0.17) | −0.01 (.00)* | −0.14 (−0.26, −0.03) |
→ W2 Sexual | 0.01 (.00) | 0.11 (−0.01, 0.22) | 0.00 (.00) | 0.01 (−0.07, 0.09) |
W1 Psych. → W2 Phys. | 0.03 (.02)* | 0.10 (0.01, 0.19) | 0.02 (.02) | 0.05 (−0.06, 0.16) |
W1 Phys. → W2 Sex. | 0.01 (.04) | 0.01 (−0.11, 0.14) | −0.04 (.04) | −0.06 (−0.20, 0.08) |
W2 BPF → W3 Relational | 0.00 (.00) | 0.00 (−0.10, 0.10) | 0.00 (.00) | 0.05 (−0.08, 0.18) |
→ W3 Psych. | 0.01 (.01) | 0.02 (−0.07, 0.12) | 0.02 (.01) | 0.08 (−0.04, 0.19) |
→ W3 Physical | 0.01 (.00)* | 0.10(0.01, 0.19) | 0.00 (.00) | 0.04 (−0.08, 0.15) |
→ W3 Sexual | 0.00 (.00) | 0.04 (−0.04, 0.11) | 0.00 (.00) | 0.04 (−0.06, 0.14) |
W2 Psych. → W3 Phys. | 0.02 (.02) | 0.05 (−0.04, 0.14) | 0.03 (.02) | 0.10 (−0.02, 0.21) |
W2 Phys. → W3 Sex. | 0.06 (.04) | 0.09 (−0.01, 0.19) | −0.02 (.04) | −0.04 (−0.17, 0.09) |
W3 BPF → W4 Relational | 0.01 (.00)** | 0.16 (0.06, 0.26) | 0.01 (.00) | 0.11 (−0.05, 0.25) |
→ W4 Psych. | 0.03 (.01)* | 0.13 (0.03, 0.23) | 0.02 (.02) | 0.09 (−0.06, 0.24) |
→ W4 Physical | 0.01 (.01)* | 0.14 (0.03, 0.26) | 0.00 (.01) | 0.07 (−0.10, 0.23) |
→ W4 Sexual | 0.01 (.00) | 0.09 (−0.02, 0.19) | 0.00 (.00) | 0.01 (−0.07, 0.19) |
W3 Psych. → W4 Phys. | 0.02 (.02) | 0.04 (−0.05, 0.13) | 0.04 (.02) | 0.12 (−0.02, 0.27) |
W3 Phys. → W4 Sex. | 0.15 (.06)* | 0.21 (−0.12, 0.13) | 0.04 (.04) | 0.06 (−0.07, 0.19) |
W4 BPF → W5 Relational | 0.00 (.00) | 0.07 (−0.04, 0.18) | 0.01 (.01) | 0.18 (−0.04, 0.40) |
→ W5 Psych. | 0.02 (.01)* | 0.12 (0.02, 0.21) | 0.04 (.02) | 0.16 (−0.00, 0.33) |
→ W5 Physical | 0.00 (.00) | −0.01 (−0.11, 0.10) | 0.01 (.01) | 0.15 (−0.02, 0.32) |
→ W5 Sexual | 0.00 (.00) | 0.01 (−0.09, 0.12) | 0.01 (.01) | 0.14 (−0.04, 0.32) |
W4 Psych. → W5 Phys. | 0.01 (.02) | 0.03 (−0.07, 0.14) | 0.07 (.03)* | 0.20 (0.05, 0.35) |
W4 Phys. → W5 Sex. | 0.00 (.05) | 0.00 (−0.12, 0.13) | −0.02 (.04) | −0.03 (−0.14, 0.08) |
W1 Relational → W2 BPF | −0.15 (.96) | −0.01 (−0.10, 0.09) | −0.58 (1.13) | −0.03 (−0.12, 0.07) |
W1 Psych. → | 0.50 (.24)* | 0.12 (0.01, 0.22) | 0.36 (.30) | 0.07 (−0.04, 0.17) |
W1 Physical → | 0.24 (.61) | 0.02 (−0.08, 0.11) | 0.33 (1.09) | 0.02 (−0.09, 0.12) |
W1 Sexual → | −1.19 (.87) | −0.06 (−0.15, 0.03) | −0.65 (1.47) | −0.02 (−0.11, 0.07) |
W2 Relational → W3 BPF | 0.23 (1.21) | 0.01 (−0.09, 0.11) | 0.12 (1.42) | 0.01 (−0.13, 0.14) |
W2 Psych. → | −0.22 (.23) | −0.05 (−0.15, 0.05) | 0.71 (.36)* | 0.14(0.00, 0.28) |
W2 Physical → | 1.08 (.79) | 0.08 (−0.03, 0.19) | −0.85 (.83) | −0.06 (−0.17, 0.05) |
W2 Sexual → | −0.25 (1.55) | −0.01 (−0.14, 0.12) | 0.32 (1.73) | 0.01 (−0.10, 0.12) |
W3 Relational → W4 BPF | 2.57 (2.12) | 0.09 (−0.05, 0.22) | −0.38 (1.74) | −0.02 (−0.17, (0.14) |
W3 Psych. | 0.26 (.29) | 0.05 (−0.06, 0.16) | −0.14 (.37) | −0.03 (−0.18, 0.12) |
W3 Physical → | −0.66 (1.58) | −0.04 (−0.24, 0.16) | 0.10 (1.25) | 0.01 (−0.13, 0.14) |
W3 Sexual → | −2.08 (1.50) | −0.09 (−0.21, 0.03) | −3.11 (1.94) | −0.13 (−0.28, 0.03) |
W4 Relational → W5 BPF | 1.64 (1.30) | 0.06 (−0.03, 0.15) | −1.22 (1.69) | −0.05 (−0.19, 0.09) |
W4 Psych. → | −0.05 (.33) | −0.01 (−0.14, 0.12) | 1.28 (0.50)* | 0.24 (0.06, 0.42) |
W4 Physical → | −0.21 (.72) | −0.01 (−0.10, 0.08) | −1.33 (1.17) | −0.07 (−0.20, 0.05) |
W4 Sexual → | 2.52 (.96)** | 0.12 (0.03, 0.21) | −1.48 (2.12) | −0.05 (−0.17, 0.08) |
Note: **p<.01, *p<.05.
First, in examining cross-lagged paths among females, borderline features predicted higher reports of psychological violence at nearly every wave, while standardized effects were small (ranging from .12 to .14), they seemed to increase over time. This hypothesis was tested by conducting a nested model comparison between a model with these three cross-lagged paths set to equivalence (H0) compared to a model in which they were freed (H1). Constraining these three cross-lagged paths to equivalence did not significantly change model fit (χ2(2) = 0.41, p = .816) and when examining the difference in estimates from Waves 1 to 2 with paths from Waves 3 to 4/Waves 4 to 5, these differences were not significantly different from zero. Borderline features at previous wave also predicted relational violence at Waves 2 and 4 and physical violence at Waves 2 and 3; while these effects also increased over time, differences were not significantly different from zero. When looking at cross-lagged paths predicting borderline features at subsequent waves, there were less significant findings. Wave 1 psychological violence and Wave 4 sexual violence predicted increases in borderline features at subsequent waves, with small magnitude of effects.
The pattern of results regarding prediction of dating violence by previous levels of borderline features was not mirrored among males. In fact, borderline features at Wave 1 predicted less physical violence at the subsequent wave. However, psychological violence was a significant predictor of subsequent borderline features at Waves 3 (standardized effect of .14) and 5 (standardized effect of .24). The difference in magnitude between these two effects was not significant from zero; however, they were significantly greater than the same effects among females (psych W2 predicting borderline W3: unstandardized effect difference = 0.92, SE = 0.42, p = .030; psych W4 predicting borderline W5: unstandardized effect difference = 1.33, SE = 0.60, p = .025).
Next, looking at autoregressive relations, paths largely mirrored what was found in correlational analysis; autoregressive paths for borderline features were moderate to strong (standardized effects of .44 to .54 in females, .44 to .53 in males), but for the most part, were small to moderate for measures of dating violence victimization, a pattern that was consistent across genders. The exception to this were the autoregressive paths for sexual violence victimization which ranged from .24 to .32 among females and .39 to .63 among males. Differences in these parameters between genders were statistically significant (unstandardized difference = −0.26, SE = 0.09, p = .004) suggesting that among males, sexual violence demonstrates significantly stronger stability over time than it does among females. Relational violence had the lowest magnitude of autoregressive paths across both genders (suggesting the lowest level of stability from mid-adolescence to early adulthood).
As an ancillary test, we fit the same model without controlling for the effects of parental relationship quality. Model fit was adequate across all indicators (χ2(420 = 713.19, p) < .001; RMSEA = .048 [90% CI: .042, .054]; SRMR = .092; CFI = .905). Path estimates are available from the first author upon request. On the whole, results were largely unchanged, with the exception of the cross-lagged paths from borderline features to subsequent levels of dating violence. Specifically, while removing parental relationship quality from the model led to several paths to reduce in magnitude and no longer be significant among females (borderline predicting relational violence at Wave 2, physical violence at Wave 3 and 4, and psychological violence at Wave 5). Parental relationship quality seemed to have a suppression effect for males such that removing it from the model led to an increase in magnitude of some of these cross-lagged effects and the effect from borderline features to subsequent sexual dating violence at Wave 5 became statistically significant.
Discussion
In the first study to evaluate the concurrent associations and bidirectional lagged effects across time between DVV and borderline pathology from late adolescence to early adulthood, we emphasize three findings. First, from mid-adolescence into young adulthood, higher borderline features predicted increased likelihood of being victimized in a dating relationship among females; however, this was not the case for males. Second, for males, we found that psychological violence predicted increases in subsequent borderline features, which was stronger than the parallel effect among females. Finally, of all forms of violence, psychological violence had the most robust associations with borderline personality features across genders. Altogether, these findings suggest that at least among females, borderline features prospectively is linked to victimization in dating relationships, whereas DVV (particularly psychological violence) is a more robust risk factor for borderline pathology among males. Together, although demonstrating small effect sizes, it appears that DVV is an important factor in the maintenance and exacerbation of borderline symptomatology for individuals transitioning from adolescence into young adulthood. This is particularly meaningful given that these effects were found even when controlling for stability of these constructs and potential confounds of parental relationship quality and SES.
Regarding our first finding, it is notable that the direction of prediction was largely characterized by borderline features predicting DVV among women, with only psychological violence at age 16 and sexual violence at age 19 predicting borderline features one year later. Overall, it appears that during middle to late adolescence, higher levels of borderline features put females at risk for being victimized by psychological, relational and physical aggression. This finding is interesting when evaluated alongside research demonstrating that individuals with BPD display greater hostility and aggression toward romantic partners when experiencing anxiety and avoidance (Critchfield, Levy, Clarkin, & Kernberg, 2008). It is possible that individuals with borderline features elicit aggressive and hostile behavior from romantic partners, consistent with the suggestions made by Maneta and colleagues (2013). In turn, although not as robust, victimization predicts increases in borderline symptomatology.
Direction of predictive relations are in line with a developmental cascade model that is best understood in the context of typical development. Developmental cascades refer to the dynamic interplay of multiple factors across development in which functioning in one domain impacts functioning in other areas. Further, timing of various processes within developmental cascades can provide important information regarding critical periods for intervention (Masten & Cicchetti, 2010). Adolescent intimate relationships contribute to psychological well-being by satisfying needs for identity and intimacy (Collins & Sroufe, 1999; Shulman & Knafo, 1997) during a developmental phase characterized by changes to the attachment systems (Scharf & Mayseless, 2007) and identity development (Kroger, 2006). Previous findings have shown that among females, changes in borderline features across adolescence correspond to worsening social skills, increased sexual activity, and poor self-perception (Wright, Zalewski, Hallquist, Hipwell, & Stepp, 2016), thereby demonstrating that early borderline features are linked to psychosocial domains that may disrupt the process of healthy identity and personality development via instability in close relationships. Complementing these findings, the current study demonstrates directionality in these influences such that increased victimization may be a result of impaired interpersonal functioning in adolescents with borderline features. Further, it is possible that over time, persistence in victimization would lead to exacerbation of existing borderline personality pathology, as seen in the final waves of the study with sexual violence predicting increases in borderline personality features. Therefore, intervening in early disturbed interpersonal processes among adolescents with borderline features may be crucial to prevent escalation of disturbed relationship functioning.
Another surprising finding was the gender differences that emerged within the model. While borderline features among females largely predicted DVV in subsequent years, borderline features in males predicted decreases in physical dating violence. Previous research has found that men with borderline personality pathology are more likely to demonstrate an explosive temperament and impulsive aggressiveness (Mancke, Bertsch, & Herpertz, 2015; Sansone & Sansone, 2011), which in the context of romantic discord, may actually lead partners to withdraw rather than retaliate. Future research is needed to elaborate on these findings as they may not necessarily apply to same-sex relationships.
In terms of DVV acting as a risk factor for subsequent borderline features, effects were significantly stronger for psychological violence predicting increases in later borderline personality features in males compared to females. Previous research with cortisol data among patients with borderline personality disorder (BPD) has found that in response to psychosocial stress, adult males with BPD show increases in cortisol levels compared to females with BPD and male controls (Inoue et al., 2015). Together, these results suggest possible etiological differences in the borderline personality pathology development based on gender. Alternatively, differences may be due to developmental timing (with interpersonal factors carrying stronger risk for borderline features among women earlier in development; Roeder et al., 2014). In fact, previous research has found that while both DVV and borderline features are equally distributed across males and females (for borderline pathology, specifically in community samples; Johnson et al., 2003; Kimmel, 2002), prevalence of victimization is higher among female patients with BPD compared to male patients (Bohle & Vogel, 2017). Given that previous research on risk factors for borderline personality pathology tends to not explicitly model gender differences or includes female-only samples, future research should focus on understanding potential sex-specific trajectories of borderline personality pathology.
No a priori hypotheses were made regarding the forms of violence that may be associated with borderline personality pathology. Results suggested that psychological violence had the most robust associations with borderline personality features, both as a predictor and as a consequence. This was not surprising given that theories of BPD emphasize the centrality of emotional invalidation in perpetuating aspects of the disorder (Linehan, 1993). Additionally, it has been found that borderline features in adolescence is related to psychological control and guilt induction by parents (Vanwoerden, Kalpakci, & Sharp, 2017). However, it has been suggested that psychological violence such as yelling or swearing at a partner may represent a less severe dimension of dating violence when potential for harm is not expected (Cascardi, Blank, & Dodani, 2016). Therefore, the lack of findings for the more severe forms of violence may be due to overall lower prevalence of sexual and physical violence in the current sample.
The current study has several strengths that contribute to research regarding risk for the development and maintenance of borderline personality pathology. First, using a longitudinal design with several assessment points allow us to evaluate more dynamic associations across a critical developmental period. Additionally, given noted gender differences in developmental mechanisms of borderline personality pathology (Johnson et al., 2003), testing gender differences in the overall model allows for greater specificity in our understanding of these associations. Finally, the use of a large, ethnically and geographically diverse community sample increases external validity of the findings.
Despite these implications, several limitations must be noted. First, our study relied solely on self-report, which limits the generalizability of findings. It is a well-established finding that borderline personality pathology is associated with distorted interpersonal perception characterized by hypersensitivity to rejection (Lazarus, Cheavens, Festa, & Zachary Rosenthal, 2014); therefore, dyadic reports of conflicts in dating relationships may allow for greater confidence in findings. Additionally, we utilized manifest variables, rather than using SEM to estimate models using latent variables. Future research should utilize these methods to account for measurement error and to demonstrate measurement invariance of these constructs over time. Second, while we controlled for quality of the parental relationship, we did not consider child abuse or neglect. Previous research has found that victimization by parents predicts greater victimization by intimate partners in adolescence (Foshee et al., 2004). Further, when removing parental relationship quality as a covariate in our model, results were somewhat altered, but altered differently by gender. Future research is needed to unpack the complex dynamics between maladaptive parent-child dynamics and subsequent child-peer/romantic partner dynamics that are related to borderline pathology, and how this may differ by gender. Finally, it is unclear whether victimization as a risk factor for borderline pathology is exclusive within dating relationships. There has been research finding that victimization by peers represents risk for borderline pathology in adolescence (Kawabata, Youngblood, & Hamaguchi, 2014). Therefore, future research should investigate whether these effects are unique to dating relationships or are representative of close interpersonal relationships.
The current study strengthens previous suggestions that DVV is a risk factor for borderline pathology. Further, it provides evidence that borderline features in adolescence may increase the likelihood of being victimized. This has important health policy implications; there are several programs that have been developed to eliminate dating violence in adolescents including school-based programs promoting healthy relationships (Fourth R Program; Wolfe et al., 2009), primary and secondary dating violence prevention programs that address beliefs and norms of dating violence as well as behavioral strategies for those engaged in dating violence (Safe Dates; Foshee et al., 2005), as well as interventions that combine classroom- and school-level programs (Shifting Boundaries; Taylor, Mumford, & Stein, 2015). These interventions may benefit from incorporating interventions for borderline personality pathology. Fostering interpersonal skills early in adolescence may not only assist in decreasing rates of dating violence, but can assist in preventing the development of borderline personality pathology.
Supplementary Material
References
- Aggen SH, Neale MC, Røysamb E, Reichborn-Kjennerud T, & Kendler KS (2009). A psychometric evaluation of the DSM-IV borderline personality disorder criteria: age and sex moderation of criterion functioning. Psychological Medicine, 39(12), 1967–1978. 10.1017/S0033291709005807 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Agrawal HR, Gunderson J, Holmes BM, & Lyons-Ruth K (2004). Attachment studies with borderline patients: A review. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 12(2), 94–104. 10.1080/10673220490447218 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Antila H, Arola R, Hakko H, Riala K, Riipinen P, & Kantojarvi L (2017). Bullying involvement in relation to personality disorders: A prospective follow-up of 508 inpatient adolescents. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 26, 779–789. 10.1007/s00787-017-0946-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bohle A, & Vogel V. de. (2017). Gender Differences in Victimization and the Relation to Personality Disorders in Forensic Psychiatry. Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment & Trauma, 26(4), 411–429. 10.1080/10926771.2017.1284170 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bornovalova MA, Hicks BM, Iacono WG, & McGue M (2009). Stability, Change, and Heritability of Borderline Personality Disorder Traits from Adolescence to Adulthood: A Longitudinal Twin Study. Development and Psychopathology, 21(4), 1335–1353. 10.1017/S0954579409990186 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bouchey HA, & Furman W (2006). Dating and Romantic Experiences in Adolescence In Adams GR & Berzonsky MD (Eds.), Blackwell Handbook of Adolescence (pp. 312–329). Blackwell Publishing Ltd; 10.1002/9780470756607.ch15 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Breiding MJ, Smith SG, Basile KC, Walters ML, Chen J, & Merrick MT (2014). Prevalence and characteristics of sexual violence, stalking, and intimate partner violence victimization--national intimate partner and sexual violence survey, United States, 2011. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report. Surveillance Summaries (Washington, D.C.: 2002), 63(8), 1–18. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Browne MW, & Cudeck R (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit In Bollen KA & Long JS, Testing Structural Equation Models (pp. 136–162). Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications. [Google Scholar]
- Cascardi M, Blank S, & Dodani V (2016). Comparison of the CADRI and CTS2 for Measuring Psychological and Physical Dating Violence Perpetration and Victimization. Journal of Interpersonal Violence. 10.1177/0886260516670182 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chang B, Sharp C, & Ha C (2011). The criterion validity of the borderline personality features scale for children in an adolescent inpatient setting. Journal of Personality Disorders, 25(4), 492–503. 10.1521/pedi.2011.25.4.492 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Collins WA, & Sroufe LA (1999). Capacity for intimate relationships In Furman Wyndol, Brown BB, & Feiring C (Eds.), The development of romantic relationships in adolescence (pp. 125–147). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Collins WA, Welsh DP, & Furman W (2009). Adolescent Romantic Relationships. Annual Review of Psychology, 60(1), 631–652. 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163459 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crick NR, Murray–Close D, & Woods K (2005). Borderline personality features in childhood: A short-term longitudinal study. Development and Psychopathology, null(04), 1051–1070. 10.1017/S0954579405050492 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Critchfield KL, Levy KN, Clarkin JF, & Kernberg OF (2008). The relational context of aggression in borderline personality disorder: using adult attachment style to predict forms of hostility. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 64(1), 67–82. 10.1002/jclp.20434 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daley SE, Burge D, & Hammen C (2000). Borderline personality disorder symptoms as predictors of 4-year romantic relationship dysfunction in young women: Addresing issues of specificity. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109(3), 451–460. 10.1037/0021-843X.109.3.451 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Exner-Cortens D (2014). Theory and teen dating violence victimization: Considering adolescent development. Developmental Review, 34(2), 168–188. 10.1016/j.dr.2014.03.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Exner-Cortens D, Eckenrode J, & Rothman E (2013). Longitudinal Associations Between Teen Dating Violence Victimization and Adverse Health Outcomes. Pediatrics, 131(1), 71–78. 10.1542/peds.2012-1029 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foshee VA, Bauman KE, Ennett ST, Suchindran C, Benefield T, & Linder GF (2005). Assessing the Effects of the Dating Violence Prevention Program “Safe Dates” Using Random CoefficientRegression Modeling. Prevention Science, 6(3), 245 10.1007/s11121-005-0007-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foshee VA, Benefield TS, Ennett ST, Bauman KE, & Suchindran C (2004). Longitudinal predictors of serious physical and sexual dating violence victimization during adolescence. Preventive Medicine, 39(5), 1007–1016. 10.1016/j.ypmed.2004.04.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fossati A, Sharp C, Borroni S, & Somma A (2016). Psychometric Properties of the Borderline Personality Features Scale for Children-11 (BPFSC-11) in a Sample of Community Dwelling Italian Adolescents. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 1–8. 10.1027/1015-5759/a000377 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Giordano PC, Soto DA, Manning WD, & Longmore MA (2010). The Characteristics of Romantic Relationships Associated with Teen Dating Violence. Social Science Research, 39(6), 863–874. 10.1016/j.ssresearch.2010.03.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goncy EA, Farrell AD, Sullivan TN, & Taylor KA (2016). Measurement of Dating Aggression During Middle School: Structure, Measurement Invariance, and Distinction From General Aggression. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 26(3), 509–523. 10.1111/jora.12208 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Halpern CT, Oslak SG, Young ML, Martin SL, & Kupper LL (2001). Partner Violence Among Adolescents in Opposite-Sex Romantic Relationships: Findings From the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health. American Journal of Public Health, 91(10), 1679–1685. 10.2105/AJPH.91.10.1679 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haltigan JD, & Vaillancourt T (2016). The Borderline Personality Features Scale for Children (BPFS-C): Factor Structure and Measurement Invariance across Time and Sex in a Community-Based Sample. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 38(4), 600–614. 10.1007/s10862-016-9550-1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hamby S, Sugarman DB, & Boney-McCoy S (2006). Does questionnaire format impact reported partner violence rates?: An experimental study. Violence and Victims, 21(4), 507–518. 10.1891/0886-6708.21.4.507 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hatkevich C, Mellick W, Reuter T, Temple JR, & Sharp C (2017). Dating Violence Victimization, Nonsuicidal Self-Injury, and the Moderating Effect of Borderline Personality Disorder Features in Adolescent Inpatients. Journal of Interpersonal Violence. 10.1177/0886260517708402 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoertel N, Peyre H, Wall MM, Limosin F, & Blanco C (2014). Examining sex differences in DSM-IV borderline personality disorder symptom expression using Item Response Theory (IRT). Journal of Psychiatric Research, 59, 213–219. 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.08.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Howard DE, & Wang MQ (2005). Psychosocial correlates of U.S. adolescents who report a history of forced sexual intercourse. The Journal of Adolescent Health : Official Publication of the Society for Adolescent Medicine, 36(5), 372–379. 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2004.07.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- IBM Corp. (2016). IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 24.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp. [Google Scholar]
- Inoue A, Oshita H, Maruyama Y, Tanaka Y, Ishitobi Y, Kawano A, … Akiyoshi J (2015). Gender determines cortisol and alpha-amylase responses to acute physical and psychosocial stress in patients with borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Research, 228(1), 46–52. 10.1016/j.psychres.2015.04.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson DM, Shea MT, Yen S, Battle CL, Zlotnick C, Sanislow CA, … Zanarini MC (2003). Gender differences in borderline personality disorder: findings from the collaborative longitudinal personality disorders study. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 44(4), 284–292. 10.1016/S0010-440X(03)00090-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jouriles EN, McDonald R, Mueller V, & Grych JH (2012). Youth Experiences of Family Violence and Teen Dating Violence Perpetration: Cognitive and Emotional Mediators. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 75(1), 58–68. 10.1007/s10567-011-0102-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kawabata Y, Youngblood J, & Hamaguchi Y (2014). Preadolescents’ borderline personality features in a non-Western urban context: Concurrent and longitudinal associations with physical and relational aggression, friendship exclusivity and peer victimization. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 17(3), 219–228. 10.1111/ajsp.12067 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kimmel MS (2002). “Gender Symmetry” in Domestic Violence: A Substantive and Methodological Research Review. Violence Against Women, 8(11), 1332–1363. 10.1177/107780102237407 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kline RB (2011). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. 2011 (3rd ed.). New York: Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
- Kroger J (2006). Identity development: Adolescence through adulthood. Sage publications; Retrieved from, https://books.google.com/books?hl=en&lr=&id=jz91AwAAQBAJ&oi=fnd&pg=PP1&dq=adolescence+key+phase+for+identity+consolidation&ots=AdkzrOwaAo&sig=YdZarGBRgl_6X5ryEQfAl2mVQdk [Google Scholar]
- Lazarus SA, Cheavens JS, Festa F, & Zachary Rosenthal M (2014). Interpersonal functioning in borderline personality disorder: a systematic review of behavioral and laboratory-based assessments. Clinical Psychology Review, 34(3), 193–205. 10.1016/j.cpr.2014.01.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Linehan MM (1993). Cognitive-behavioral treatment of borderline personality disorder (Vol. xvii). New York, NY, US: Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
- Mancke F, Bertsch K, & Herpertz SC (2015). Gender differences in aggression of borderline personality disorder. Borderline Personality Disorder and Emotion Dysregulation, 2(1), 7 10.1186/s40479-015-0028-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maneta EK, Cohen S, Schulz MS, & Waldinger RJ (2013). Two to tango: a dyadic analysis of links between borderline personality traits and intimate partner violence. Journal of Personality Disorders, 27(2), 233–243. 10.1521/pedi_2013_27_082 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manning WD, Longmore MA, Copp J, & Giordano PC (2014). The Complexities of Adolescent Dating and Sexual Relationships: Fluidity, Meaning(s), and Implications for Young Adults’ Well-Being: The Complexities of Adolescent Dating and Sexual Relationships. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 2014(144), 53–69. 10.1002/cad.20060 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Masten AS, & Cicchetti D (2010). Developmental cascades. Development and Psychopathology, 22(3), 491–495. 10.1017/S0954579410000222 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mechanic MB, Weaver TL, & Resick PA (2008). Mental health consequences of intimate partner abuse: A multidimensional assessment of four different forms of abuse. Violence Against Women, 14(6), 634–654. 10.1177/1077801208319283 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muthén LK, & Muthén BO (n.d.). MPlus User’s Guide. Eighth Edition. Muthen & Muthen. [Google Scholar]
- Nocentini A, Menesini E, Pastorelli C, Connolly J, Pepler D, & Craig W (2011). Physical dating aggression in adolescence: Cultural and gender invariance. European Psychologist, 16(4), 278–287. 10.1027/1016-9040/a000045 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Pico-Alfonso MA, Echeburúa E, & Martinez M (2008). Personality Disorder Symptoms in Women as a Result of Chronic Intimate Male Partner Violence. Journal of Family Violence, 23(7), 577–588. 10.1007/s10896-008-9180-9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Reuter TR, Sharp C, Temple JR, & Babcock JC (2015). The relation between borderline personality disorder features and teen dating violence. Psychology of Violence, 5(2), 163–173. 10.1037/a0037891 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Sansone RA, & Sansone LA (2011). Gender Patterns in Borderline Personality Disorder. Innovations in Clinical Neuroscience, 5(5), 16–20. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scharf M, & Mayseless O (2007). Putting eggs in more than one basket: A new look at developmental processes of attachment in adolescence. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 2007(117), 1–22. 10.1002/cd.191 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sharp C, Michonski J, Steinberg L, Christopher J, Christopher B, & Oldham JM (2014). An investigation of differential item functioning across gender of BPD criteria. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 123(1), 231–236. 10.1037/a0035637 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sharp C, Mosko O, Chang B, & Ha C (2011). The cross-informant concordance and concurrent validity of the Borderline Personality Features Scale for Children in a community sample of boys. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 16(3), 335–349. 10.1177/1359104510366279 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shulman S, & Knafo D (1997). Balancing Closeness and Individuality in Adolescent Close Relationships. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 21(4), 687–702. 10.1080/016502597384622 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Stepp SD, Lazarus SA, & Byrd AL (2016). A systematic review of risk factors prospectively associated with borderline personality disorder: Taking stock and moving forward. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 7(4), 316–323. 10.1037/per0000186 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stepp SD, Whalen DJ, Scott LN, Zalewski M, Loeber R, & Hipwell AE (2014, May). Reciprocal effects of parenting and borderline personality disorder symptoms in adolescent girls. 10.1017/S0954579413001041 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Straus MA, Hamby SL, Boney-McCoy S, & Sugarman DB (1996). The Revised Conflict Tactics Scales (CTS2): Development and Preliminary Psychometric Data. Journal of Family Issues, 17(3), 283–316. 10.1177/019251396017003001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor BG, Mumford EA, & Stein ND (2015). Effectiveness of “Shifting Boundaries” Teen Dating Violence Prevention Program for Subgroups of Middle School Students. Journal of Adolescent Health, 56(2), S20–S26. 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2014.07.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Temple JR, Shorey RC, Tortolero SR, Wolfe DA, & Stuart GL (2013). Importance of gender and attitudes about violence in the relationship between exposure to interparental violence and the perpetration of teen dating violence. Child Abuse & Neglect, 37(5), 343–352. 10.1016/j.chiabu.2013.02.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vagi KJ, Olsen EO, Basile KC, & Vivolo-Kantor AM (2015). Teen Dating Violence (Physical and Sexual) Among US High School Students: Findings From the 2013 National Youth Risk Behavior Survey. JAMA Pediatrics, 169(5), 474–482. 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2014.3577 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vanwoerden S, Kalpakci AH, & Sharp C (2017). The relations between inadequate parent-child boundaries and borderline personality disorder in adolescence. Psychiatry Research, 257, 462–471. 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.08.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vanwoerden S, Steinberg L, Coffman AD, Paulus DJ, Morey LC, & Sharp C (2018). Evaluation of the PAI-A Anxiety and Depression Scales: Evidence of Construct Validity. Journal of Personality Assessment, 100(3), 313–320. 10.1080/00223891.2017.1347569 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wincentak K, Connolly J, & Card N (2017). Teen dating violence: A meta-analytic review of prevalence rates. Psychology of Violence, 7(2), 224–241. 10.1037/a0040194 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Winsper C, Hall J, Strauss VY, & Wolke D (2017). Aetiological pathways to borderline personality disorder symptoms in early adolescence: childhood dysregulated behaviour, maladaptive parenting and bully victimisation. Borderline Personality Disorder and Emotion Dysregulation, 4(10), 1–10. 10.1186/s40479-017-0060-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolfe DA, Crooks C, Jaffe P, Chiodo D, Hughes R, Ellis W, … Donner A (2009). A School-Based Program to Prevent Adolescent Dating Violence: A Cluster Randomized Trial. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 163(8), 692–699. 10.1001/archpediatrics.2009.69 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolfe DA, Scott K, Reitzel-Jaffe D, Wekerle C, Grasley C, & Straatman A-L (2001). Development and validation of the Conflict in Adolescent Dating Relationships Inventory. Psychological Assessment, 13(2), 277–293. 10.1037/1040-3590.13.2.277 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wright AGC, Zalewski M, Hallquist MN, Hipwell AE, & Stepp SD (2016). Developmental Trajectories of Borderline Personality Disorder Symptoms and Psychosocial Functioning in Adolescence. Journal Of Personality Disorders, 30(3), 351–372. 10.1521/pedi_2015_29_200 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.