Abstract
Prader–Willi syndrome (PWS) is a neurodevelopmental disorder caused by deletion or inactivation of paternally expressed imprinted genes on human chromosome 15q11–q13. In addition to endocrine and developmental issues, PWS presents with behavioural problems including stereotyped behaviour, impulsiveness and cognitive deficits. The PWS genetic interval contains several brain-expressed small nucleolar (sno) RNA species that are subject to genomic imprinting, including snord115 that negatively regulates post-transcriptional modification of the serotonin 2C receptor (5-HT2CR) pre-mRNA potentially leading to a reduction in 5-HT2CR function. Using the imprinting centre deletion mouse model for PWS (PWSICdel) we have previously shown impairments in a number of behaviours, some of which are abnormally sensitive to 5-HT2CR-selective drugs. In the stop-signal reaction time task test of impulsivity, PWSICdel mice showed increased impulsivity relative to wild-type (WT) littermates. Challenge with the selective 5-HT2CR agonist WAY163909 reduced impulsivity in PWSICdel mice but had no effect on WT behaviour. This behavioural dissociation in was also reflected in differential patterns of immunoreactivity of the immediate early gene c-Fos, with a blunted response to the drug in the orbitofrontal cortex of PWSICdel mice, but no difference in c-Fos activation in the nucleus accumbens. These findings suggest specific facets of response inhibition are impaired in PWSICdel mice and that abnormal 5-HT2CR function may mediate this dissociation. These data have implications for our understanding of the aetiology of PWS-related behavioural traits and translational relevance for individuals with PWS who may seek to control appetite with the new obesity treatment 5-HT2CR agonist lorcaserin.
Introduction
Prader–Willi Syndrome (PWS) is a neurodevelopmental disorder caused by deletion or inactivation of paternally expressed imprinted genes on human chromosome 15q11–q13 (1). The core phenotypic features of PWS are severe neonatal hypotonia and a failure to thrive in infancy, and the subsequent development of hyperphagia and reduced satiety responses result in obesity, unless managed carefully (2). Additionally, individuals with PWS also display a number of behavioural and cognitive problems. These are manifest as a mild to moderate intellectual disability with an average IQ of 60–80: impulsive and compulsive behaviours, including increased rates of tantrums, oppositionality and aggression (3) and a particular cognitive deficit in attention switching (4,5). Finally, a significant proportion of individuals with PWS will develop a severe affective psychotic illness (6). It is these behavioural and psychiatric problems that are most difficult to manage and affect quality of life for individuals and carers (7).
In the vast majority of cases of PWS, individuals have a genetic or epigenetic abnormality that leads to complete loss of all paternally expressed genes on 15q11–q13, including MKRN3, MAGEL2, NDN, NPAP1, SNURF-SNRPN, the C/D box small nucleolar (sno) RNAs SNORD109A, SNORD115 and SNORD116 (previously termed HBII-438A, HBII-52 and HBII-85, respectively) and the non-coding RNA IPW. However, rare clinical cases have narrowed the critical PWS genetic interval to a region spanning the SNORD116 repeat and the IPW gene (8–10). The importance of this interval is supported by animal studies showing paternal deletion of Snord116 and Ipw leads to feeding, anxiety, sleep, growth and metabolic phenotypes reminiscent of PWS (11–14). However, although data is currently limited, deletion of Snord116 and Ipw alone does not appear to contribute to other behavioural and cognitive problems (15). In contrast, using the PWSICdel mouse model, in which an imprinting centre (IC) deletion results in complete loss of paternal gene expression at the PWS interval, we have demonstrated a wide range of behavioural abnormalities. In addition to feeding (16) and metabolic (17) phenotypes, PWSICdel mice are hypoactive and show impaired sensory-motor gating (18). Furthermore, PWSICdel mice show a number of cognitive abnormalities, including enhanced appetitive learning and reversal learning (19) and deficits in visuo-spatial attention (18).
By using the PWSICdel mice we have also demonstrated that abnormal post-transcriptional processing of the serotonin 2C receptor (5-HT2CR) pre-RNA contributes to aspects of these behavioural phenotypes (20,21). The 5-HT2CR pre-RNA is subject to adenosine-to-inosine editing at five sites within an alternatively spliced exon, exon Vb. Both of these events result in less functional receptor moieties due to their effects on the amino acid sequence of the critical G-protein binding domain. Specifically, RNA-editing within exon Vb leads to a change in codon-specificity and subsequent amino acid sequence. Alternate splicing of exon Vb results in a truncated protein lacking a functional G-protein binding domain. Although this truncated splice variant cannot act as a receptor, it plays a critical role in overall 5-HT2CR function, by forming a heterodimer and sequestering the full-length splice variant in the endoplasmic reticulum and reducing cell surface expression (22). Processing of Htr2c pre-RNA is mediated, in part, by the actions of the snoRNA Snord115 (23–25), present within the imprinted PWS locus (26). Expression of Snord115 is lost in PWSICdel mice (18), and there are concomitant increases in 5-HT2CR RNA editing (21) and splicing (20). Although loss of SNORD115 alone does not give rise to PWS per se (27), PWSICdel mice demonstrate abnormal 5-HT2CR-mediated feeding (20), suggesting that this snoRNA may contribute to the general PWS phenotype in those cases where all paternal gene expression is lost (28).
The current study investigated the impact of abnormal 5-HT2CR functioning in PWSICdel mice on a key aspect of cognition, namely response control. Specifically, we examined the performance of the PWSICdel mice to inhibit a pre-potent response in a stop-signal reaction time (SSRT) task. The SSRT task (Fig. 1) is based on a ‘race’ between two response tendencies, ‘going’ and ‘stopping’ (29,30), where the subjects are trained to make a rapid go response, between two stimuli locations, but must withhold this response when a stop signal is presented. The closer the stop signal is to the initial movement the easier it is to stop the go response, and if the stop signal is presented towards the end of the go response, stopping is much more difficult. Performance of mice in the SSRTT is sensitive to pharmacological manipulation of 5-HT2CR (31). We found that PWSICdel mice displayed baseline (BL) deficits in the ability to inhibit responding (i.e. increased impulsivity) compared to wild-type (WT) littermate controls. Furthermore, administration of a selective 5-HT2CR agonist had dissociable effects between PWSICdel and WT mice, which was also reflected by specific alterations in the pattern of neuronal activity, as measured by c-Fos activation, in brain regions known to be important for correct response control (32). These data suggest a psychological basis to the impulsive/compulsive endophenotype shown by PWS patients. This abnormal response control may be mediated, in part, by aberrant 5-HT2CR functioning and therefore these findings have translational relevance for individuals with PWS who may seek to control appetite with the new obesity treatment 5-HT2CR agonist lorcaserin.
Results
No differences in shaping and training between WT and PWSICdel mice
PWSICdel and WT mice were generated by crossing males positive for the IC deletion with CD1 females and consistent with previous studies (18,33), the PWSICdel mice were considerably smaller than their WT littermates (31% and 30% lighter in weight, for males and females, respectively), although this size difference did not affect their propensity to consume reward during habituation to the reinforcement (Table 1). The majority of the mice were able to learn the SSRTT (Fig. 1), passing through the different stages of shaping and training (See Supplementary Material, Table S1); however, two WT and two PWSICdel mice failed to reach criteria during training and were therefore removed from the study leaving a final N of 13 and 14 for the groups of WT and PWSICdel mice, respectively (Table 1). There were no gender- or genotype-related differences in the number of sessions taken to reach BL SSRTT performance (Table 1), and there were also no differences in the mean programmed go stimulus duration (main effect of GENOTYPE, F1,23 = 3.64, P = 0.08, η2 = 0.14) or time that the mice were required to withhold a response in a stop trial to earn reward (main effect of GENOTYPE, F1,23 = 1.16, P = 0.29, η2 = 0.05), key parameters which govern performance in the SSRTT (Table 1).
Table 1.
Number of | Shaping and training | Performance at 50% correct stopping | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
subjects | Going | Stopping | |||||||||
Start | At BL |
Sessions to BL | Sessions to stable BL performance | Duration of go stimulus (ms) | Correct responses (%) | Reaction time (ms) | Time to withhold responding (ms) | Correct responses (%) | SSRT (ms) |
||
WT | Male | 4 | 4 | 44.25 ± 1.65 | 13.75 ± 5.17 | 1475.00 ± 137.69 | 78.31 ± 3.23 | 877.78 ± 49.27 | 612.50 ± 42.70 | 52.76 ± 3.24 | 450.15 ± 63.17 |
Female | 11 | 9 | 37.22 ± 2.38 | 13.78 ± 3.22 | 1100.00 ± 27.39 | 84.55 ± 5.59 | 718.58 ± 52.19 | 533.33 ± 37.91 | 46.96 ± 1.22 | 349.50 ± 56.60 | |
All WT | 15 | 13 | 39.38 ± 2.62 | 13.77 ± 2.62 | 1215.38 ± 65.87 | 82.63 ± 3.99 | 798.33 ± 51.35 | 557.69 ± 31.44 | 48.74 ± 1.45 | 380.47 ± 44.33 | |
PWSICdel | Male | 10 | 9 | 43.56 ± 0.80 | 8.11 ± 1.97 | 1134.72 ± 91.52 | 85.58 ± 3.13 | 744.50 ± 54.24 | 583.33 ± 47.14 | 50.22 ± 1.50 | 499.95 ± 39.86 |
Female | 6 | 5 | 41.60 ± 0.80 | 11.80 ± 3.26 | 1120.00 ± 60.42 | 90.75 ± 2.73 | 751.65 ± 36.64 | 670.00 ± 46.37 | 46.31 ± 4.56 | 531.74 ± 64.98 | |
All PWSICdel | 16 | 14 | 42.86 ± 0.65 | 9.43 ± 1.72 | 1129.46 ± 60.98 | 87.43 ± 2.27 | 747.05 ± 36.23 | 614.29 ± 35.33 | 48.82 ± 1.86 | 511.30 ± 33.32 | |
WT: GENDER comparison | P = 0.098 | P = 0.421 | P = 0.069 | P = 0.354 | P = 0.064 | P = 0.212 | P = 0.171 | P = 0.270 | |||
PWSICdel GENDER comparison | P = 0.156 | P = 0.365 | P = 0.895 | P = 0.238 | P = 0.915 | P = 0.217 | P = 0.453 | P = 0.689 |
Notes: Sessions to BL reflect the number of sessions taken to progress through the first stages of training (See Supplementary Material, Table S1), and sessions for stable BL performance the number of sessions for consistent intra-session performance in the full SSRTT with stop-signal presentation coincident with the start of the go response (i.e. a 0% stop-signal delay). Other measures are from when individualized performance was assessed over the range of stop-signal positions used (10%, 20%, 30%, 40%, 50%, 60%, 70%, 80% and 90% of the go response) and 50% stopping was calculated. Separate unpaired two-tailed t-tests were used for the gender comparisons for each genotype. Data shows mean ± SE. SSRT: stop-signal reaction time (see main text for derivation).
PWSICdel mice show more impulsivity than WT littermates in the SSRTT
All of the mice showed the anticipated increase in impulsive responding as the stop-signal was moved closer to the end of the go response, represented by the reduced ability to stop and withhold responding (Fig. 2A, main effect of STOP-SIGNAL POSITION, DELAY, F5,125 = 67.65, P = 0.001, η2 = 0.73); however, PWSICdel mice showed increased impulsivity relative to WT littermate controls indicated by decreased proportion of correctly stopped trials (main effect of GENOTYPE, F1,25 = 4.74, P = 0.04, η2 = 0.16). A significant STOP-SIGNAL POSITION*GENOTYPE interaction (F5,125 = 2.38, P = 0.04, η2 = 0.09) indicated that PWSICdel mice differed from WT, only when the stop-signal was presented between 40% and 70%, or halfway through the individualized go response (post hoc analysis for each stop-signal position: 40% P = 0.002, 50% P = 0.11, 60% P = 0.033, 70% P = 0.001), the point where the stop and go processes are competing at their greatest. There were no differences between PWSICdel and WT mice in the number of correct go responses (Fig. 2B, main effect of GENOTYPE, F1,25 = 1.36, P = 0.25, η2 = 0.05), or in the speed of making the go response (Fig. 2C, main effect of GENOTYPE, F1,25 = 0.23, P = 0.64, η2 = 0.01). Altering the stop-signal position on ‘stopping’ were quite specific, as these manipulations did not affect amount of (main effect of STOP-SIGNAL POSITION, F5,125 = 0.40, P = 0.85, η2 = 0.02), or the latency in these interpolated go trials (main effect of STOP-SIGNAL POSITION, F5,125 = 1.19, P = 0.32, η2 = 0.05).
Examination of these effects further, by looking at the point where the mice were making 50% correct stop responses (see Materials and Methods), showed that for PWSICdel mice, the stop signal was presented 30.4 ± 5.5% into the go response, compared with 46.7 ± 6.6% for their WT littermates. At this point, the SSRT, the virtual time take to decide to stop responding can be calculated, which was significantly slower for PWSICdel mice in comparison to WT (Fig. 2D, t25 = 2.38, P = 0.025), consistent with a pattern of more impulsive responding. There were no differences between PWSICdel and WT mice for ancillary task measures, such as the proportion of trials started (Fig. 2E, main effect of GENOTYPE, F1,25 = 0.14, P = 0.71, η2 = 0.01), time taken to start a trial (Fig. 2E, main effect of GENOTYPE, F1,25 = 2.56, P = 0.12, η2 = 0.09) or for the latency to collect the reward (Fig. 2E, main effect of GENOTYPE, F1,25 = 1.85, P = 0.19, η2 = 0.07), further demonstrating the specificity of the effects on stopping.
The effects of gender on SSRTT performance were also examined, especially in terms of interactions with the genotype of the mice. Although, there were some significant effects of GENDER and interactions between GENOTYPE and GENDER, these were derived from differences between male and female WT mice and there were no differences between male and female PWSICdel mice (See Supplementary Material, Table S1).
Effects of 5-HT2cR agonism on SSRTT performance
With increased alternate splicing (20) and RNA-editing (21) of 5-HT2CR RNA in the brains of PWSICdel mice, and the demonstration that manipulating 5HT2CR can affect stopping in the SSRTT (31) we investigated whether PWSICdel mice would show differential affects to WT in response to WAY163909, a specific 5-HT2CR agonist. The effects of WAY163909 were investigated in sessions were the stop signal was placed at a point in the individualized go responses where each mouse had shown 50% correct stopping, i.e. the point where the go and stop responses were equivalent. For the parameters analysed there were no main effects of GENDER, or interactions between GENOTYPE and GENDER in the initial ANOVA with a planned contrast to vehicle treatment (See Supplementary Material, Table S2), therefore the data were re-analysed without GENDER as a factor.
WAY163909 significantly increased the ability of PWSICdel mice to withhold responding under conditions where stopping was at 50% (Fig. 3A, main effect of GENOTYPE, F1,20 = 6.94, P = 0.016, η2 = 0.26). A significant interaction for the linear contrast between DOSE and GENOTYPE (F1,20 = 6.09, P = 0.023, η2 = 0.23) demonstrated that this effect occurred only at the higher does used, confirmed by post hoc comparisons between WT and PWSICdel mice (t20 = 0.12, t20 = 0.02 and t20 = 0.014 at the 0.1, 1 and 3 mg/kg doses, respectively). WAY 163909 at all doses did not affect stopping behaviour in the WT mice, where performance remained consistent with vehicle treatment. WAY163909 did not significantly alter any other of the task parameters and there were no genotype-related effects of the drug at any dose on the SSRT (Fig. 3B, main effect of GENOTYPE, F1,20 = 0.41, P = 0.53, η2 = 0.02), proportion of go responding (Fig. 3C, main effect of GENOTYPE, F1,20 = 1.65, P = 0.21, η2 = 0.08), speed of the go response (Fig. 3D, main effect of GENOTYPE, F1,20 = 0.66, P = 0.43, η2 = 0.03), proportion of trials started (Fig. 3E, main effect of GENOTYPE, F1,20 = 1.09, P = 0.31, η2 = 0.05), trial initiation latency (Fig. 3F, main effect of GENOTYPE, F1,20 = 0.02, P = 0.88, η2 = 0.01) or the latency to collect the reward (Fig. 3G, main effect of GENOTYPE, F1,20 = 0.13, P = 0.83, η2 = 0.01).
Abnormal c-Fos immunoreactivity in PWSICdel mouse brains
Injection of WAY163909 (1 mg/kg, i.p.) resulted in differential effects on c-Fos reactivity between PWSICdel and WT littermate mice that were specific to the brain regions analysed (Fig. 4A and B). In the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), the 5-HT2c agonist significantly increased the number of c-Fos positive cells (Fig. 4B, main effect of DOSE, F1,12 = 6.85, P = 0.02, η2 = 0.36), in both WT and PWSICdel mice (main effect of GENOTYPE, F1,12 = 1.07, P = 0.32, η2 = 0.08). However, this increase was greater in WT mice (~23% increase relative to ~ 8% increase in PWSICdel mice, t6 = 1.96, P = 0.09), with only a significant increase in c-fos reactivity in WT OFC samples (t6 = 2.84, P = 0.03, cf. PWSICdel: t6 = 0.84, P = 0.44, for comparison between vehicle and WAY163909 treatment). In the nucleus accumbens, there was no effect of WAY163909 on c-Fos reactivity (Fig. 4C, main effect of DOSE, F1,12 = 0.99, P = 0.34, η2 = 0.08) in either PWSICdel mice or their WT littermates (main effect of GENOTYPE, F1,12 = 0.06, P = 0.82, η2 = 0.01).
Discussion
We show that PWSICdel mice have impaired response inhibition, as indicated by a reduced ability to stop in the SSRTT, and an increased correct SSRT. Given previous work indicating increased alternate splicing and RNA-editing of Htr2c in the PWSICdel mice and the role of 5-HT2CRs in response inhibition, we further explored stopping performance by systemic administration of a 5-HT2CR agonist (WAY163909). PWSICdel mice demonstrated an abnormal response relative with WT littermate controls. This behavioural response to 5-HT2CR agonism was also reflected in blunted c-Fos activation in the orbital frontal cortex of the PWSICdel mice following 5-HT2CR agonist. These data suggest impaired response control, mediated in part by aberrant 5-HT2CR functioning, could be a psychological basis to the impulsive/compulsive endophenotype shown by PWS patients.
Consistent with our previous studies (31,34), all mice were able to complete the SSRTT and demonstrated the expected patterns of behaviour, with stopping performance systematically decreasing as the stop-signal moved closer to the end of the go reaction time. There were no effects of stop-signal position on the go correct rate, or correct go reaction time. However, PWSICdel mice showed reduced response inhibition (i.e. increased impulsivity) in the SSRTT as indexed by impaired stopping accuracy and longer SSRT (the time taken after a stop-signal is presented for inhibition to be completed), the standard measure of response inhibition. SSRT cannot be measured directly as there is no observable endpoint to the response inhibition but is estimated using a well-defined and tested mathematical model, the race model (30).
In addition to a BL measure of response control, we also wanted to probe the function of the serotonin system in PWSICdel mouse model while performing the SSRTT. In particular, we focused on the serotonin 2C receptor (5-HT2CR). This is because post-transcriptional modification of Htr2c RNA is altered in the brain of PWSICdel mice, with increased alternate splicing (20) and RNA-editing (21), most probably as a consequence of loss of brain-specific snoRNA snord115 in this model. Furthermore, there is a known role for 5-HT2CR in stopping behaviour, as response control is improved by the 5-HT2CR-specific antagonist SB242084 in the SSRTT (31). Here we employed the use of the 5-HT2CR-specific agonist WAY163909 and saw a divergence in effects on stopping behaviour between WT and PWSICdel mice. Specifically, following WAY163909 administration WT mice showed some reduction in performance but were generally unaffected by the drug, whereas PWSICdel mice displayed a marked improvement in stopping behaviour. Although not as clear-cut, this pattern of effects is very similar to those seen on feeding behaviour, where 3 mg/Kg WAY163909 inhibited feeding in WT mice, but enhanced feeding in PWSICdel mice (20). Moreover, this apparent opposite effect of WAY163909 administration, decreasing impulsivity in the SSRTT and increasing feeding, may be a result of effects of 5-HT2CR agonism in different parts of the brain with distinct dissociable functions on behaviour. However, a recent study (35) has demonstrated that agonism of the 5-HT2CR with WAY163909, affected response control, akin to the improved stopping we have found here, in combination with consumption of a high calorific diet suggesting a relationship between dietary habits and decision-making strategies with 5-HT2CR at the nexus (36,37).
The pattern of findings of impaired response control on the SSRTT contrasts with previous findings in the five-choice serial reaction time task (5-CSRTT), where PWSICdel mice exhibited no differences in premature responding (21). The 5-CSRTT is primarily used to measure visuospatial attention, and indeed PWSICdel mice were found to be impaired in attentional function (18). However, premature responding in the 5-CSRTT is also a measure of response inhibition (38). Why PWSICdel mice show BL deficits in one task and not the other could be due two possible reasons. First, it could be that PWSICdel mice are generally impaired in response control, but that the tasks are not equivalent in terms of difficulty and so BL deficits only emerge on the more demanding SSRTT where the inhibitory load is highest (39). Alternatively, the PWSICdel mice may be more impaired in a specific aspect of response control. It is now established that response control can be dissociated into a number of psychologies that are also neurally distinct (40,41). This is certainly the case for the SSRTT and the 5-CSRTT that measure the ability to cancel an ongoing action (‘stopping’) and the ability to inhibit the initiation of an action (‘waiting’), respectively. Our data would therefore suggest that PWSICdel mice are more impaired at stopping (an ongoing action), something that could, in part, be due to the differential influence of less-functional 5-HT2CRs in this model.
To further explore the idea that 5-HT2CR function varies in the PWSICdel mice, we also examined c-Fos activity in the brain following administration of 1 mg/Kg WAY163909. This dose was selected as it resulted in the biggest divergence in stopping behaviour between PWSICdel and WT mice. Thus, following drug treatment, PWSICdel mice showed reduced c-Fos immunoreactivity in the OFC, an area that is important for ‘stopping’ behaviour (42) but not in the nucleus accumbens, an area that is less critical for inhibitory behaviour in the SSRTT (43,44). This blunted engagement of OFC c-Fos neurons may be a reflection of diminished activation of 5-HT2CR in PWSICdel mice, as a result of a decrease of the most active 5-HT2CR isoforms in this area. Regional variation in the degree of post-transcriptional modification, giving rise to differential sensitivity and activation, has been reported in the rat brain (45), and it may well be that this is reflected in variability of 5-HT2CR isoform expression, and therefore drug sensitivity, in PWSICdel mice. Regional variation in 5-HT2CR functionality, and recruitment of different brain regions implicated in response control, may also account for the dissociation in altered impulsive responding in PWSICdel mice between the current data using the SSRTT, and previous findings in the 5-CSRTT (18).
Taken together with previous findings, this study suggests that response control is compromised in PWSICdel mice. This cognitive deficit may underpin some of the behavioural characteristics seen in PWS, such as temper tantrums, stubbornness and compulsive behaviour (3). The work also adds to the burgeoning literature suggesting that 5-HT2CR function is impaired in the PWSICdel mice (20,21). The most logical explanation for this is alteration in alternate splicing and/or editing of 5-HT2CR pre-mRNA due to loss of expression of the imprinted regulatory snoRNA snord115 (20,21,23). However, it should also be noted that serotonin neurochemistry is also altered in mice that lack the PWS imprinting locus genes Necdin (46) or Magel2 (47) alone. Nevertheless, the specificity of some the effects demonstrated here and previously (21) indicates these are not just generalized abnormalities due to serotonin imbalance but are due, in part, to global impairments in 5-HT2CR function in the PWSICdel mice. With the introduction of 5-HT2CR agonists, such as lorcaserin, to control appetite and act as anti-obesity treatments (48), our results have translational implications for treatment of individuals with PWS. As demonstrated in our current and previous studies (e.g. 21), using 5-HT2CR agonists may produce differential effects in PWS patients to non-affected people, therefore management of PWS-related dietary issues using these drugs may not be less affective and alternative therapeutic pathways may need to be pursued.
Materials and Methods
Subjects
Sixteen PWSICdel and fifteen WT littermates adult mice, aged ~ 4 months old at the start of the experiment were used. Mice were obtained from breeding males positive for the IC deletion with CD1 females, which generated paternally derived mutants and WT littermates; the nature of the epigenetic regulation of imprinted genes means that a paternally inherited IC deletion will result in a lack of gene expression from the PWS interval. As previously described (18,21,33) breeding to a C57BL/6 J*CD1 background reduces the risk of postnatal lethality in PWSICdel mice; however, to increase yield excess WT pups in each litter were culled (identified on the basis of their increased size 48 h after birth) leaving only one or two per litter. However, even with these measures in place, both male and female mice were used to generate a viable amount of PWSICdel mice for the current study, housed in separate sex litters with two to three animals each. All subjects were housed in a vivarium with environmental control (temperature: 21 ± 2°C, humidity 50 ± 10%) and a 12:12 light/dark cycle (lights on at 07:00 hr). Following 2 weeks of habituation and handling, a home cage water and food restriction schedule was introduced systematically for over a further 2 weeks. For the duration of the experiment the mice received access to water and food for 2 and 8 h access/day, respectively. This regime maintained the subjects at ~ 90% of free-feeding body weight and motivated the animals to work in the task. All procedures were conducted in accordance with the requirements of the UK Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act 1986 and with local institute approval from the School of Psychology, Cardiff University.
Apparatus
The SSRTT was performed in mouse nine hole chambers (31,49) (Campden Cognition, UK) with the task under the control of custom written software. Each test chamber (14 × 13 cm) was enclosed in a sound attenuating box equipped with a fan to provide ventilation and also a consistent level of background noise. The test chamber was equipped with nine circular response apertures (10 mm diameter) arranged in an arc on the back wall. Each aperture was configured with a vertically orientated infrared beam and a 40 mA stimulus light at the distal end. For the SSRTT the stimulus array was configured such that only two of the stimuli apertures were open (the others were blocked by black plastic). The open apertures, holes number 3 and 7 (from the left), were equally placed relative to the centre line of the chamber and were designated as the initiation and go responses, respectively. The near wall, including the access door, held the food magazine (2 cm wide) that was enclosed by a clear Perspex door and could also be illuminated by a 60 mA lamp. Opening of the food magazine door was recorded by the triggering of a micro-switch, as panel pushes. Reward was delivered into a small well in the floor of the food magazine via a 21-gauge hypodermic needle and 0.8-mm silicone tubing from a peristaltic pump located outside of the test chamber but within the sound attenuating box. A 60-mA house light and speaker were fitted to both side walls of the test chamber and a pair of infrared beams which spanned the chambers, perpendicular to the stimulus array and 5 mm above the grid floor, were used to record motor activity. An infrared camera (Watac, USA) mounted inside the sound attenuating box permitted observation and recording of behaving mice. The white noise stop signal, 105 db, was provided by a custom built sound generator.
SSRTT: initial shaping and training to BL
After the 2 weeks on the home cage water and food restriction schedule, the mice were habituated to the liquid reward to be used in the experiments, 10% condensed milk (Nestle Ltd, UK) solution, using methods employed previously (31,49). All sessions in the nine-hole boxes were performed with the house light off. The subjects were first habituated to the chambers for 3 days before training to the BL SSRTT (Fig. 1), which involved shaping the mice to respond sequentially at two stimulus locations, using nose pokes, to give rise to a ‘go’ response, and then learning to withhold responding to the second stimulus location when an auditory stop signal was presented, to give rise to a ‘stop’ response (see Supplementary Material, Fig S1). During training, the duration of the go stimulus, and the time that a response had to be withheld in a stop trial, were determined individually for each subject. In an SSRTT session (≤20 min session duration), there was a maximum of 100 trials, where 80% of trials were ‘go’ trials and 20% were interpolated ‘stop’ trials (Fig. 1).
SSRTT; behavioural manipulations
At BL, in ‘stop’ trials, the stop signal was always presented coincident with the beginning of the go response (i.e. 0% of the individual correct go reaction time for each subject), making stopping relatively easy, but in separate probe sessions the position of the stop-signal was varied relative to the individual correct go reaction times for each mouse making stopping more or less difficult i.e. at 10%, 20%, 30%, 40%, 50%, 60%, 70%, 80% and 90% into the individualized go reaction time for each mouse. At 90%, the stop signal was played close to the execution of the response, and hence stopping was most difficult (31,50). The order of presentation of these sessions was randomized between subjects using a Latin square design.
Pharmacological administration for assessing SSRTT performance
The effects of the 5-HT2CR agonist WAY163909 ((7bR,10aR)-1,2,3,4,8,9,10,10a-Octahydro-7bH-cyclopenta-[b][1,4]diazepino[6,7,1hi]indole) (Sigma, UK) on SSRTT performance in WT and PWSICdel mice were investigated using doses shown previously to have a biological effect in rodents (0, 0.1, 1 and 3 m/kg, (20,51)). Following at least 4 days of stable drug-free performance at BL SSRTT criteria (i.e. stop-signal position of 0%), intraperitoneal injections of vehicle or WAY163909 (0.1, 1 and 3 m/kg) made up freshly on the day of use as freebase in physiological saline were administered 30 min before testing following a Latin square design. Drug treatment was given in combination with SSRTT sessions where the stop-signal was presented at a position that had given rise to 50% correct stopping for each subject, thus individual stop-signal positions were employed for each subject. For these studies 10 WT and 12 PWSICdel mice from the original cohort were used.
Pharmacological administration for assessing c-Fos reactivity
On completion of the behavioural study, some of the WT and PWSICdel mice were randomly assigned to receive either vehicle or 1 mg/kg WAY163909 i.p. treatment (four subjects/treatment/genotype, see Table 1) 90 min prior to sacrifice. During this interval, the animals remained uninterrupted in their home cage to permit optimum c-Fos expression. They were then deeply anaesthetized with Euthasol (100 mg/kg, i.p.) and perfused transcardially with 50 mL of ice-cold 0.1 M phosphate-buffered saline (PBS), pH 7.4, followed by 100 mL of 4% paraformaldehyde in 0.1 M PBS, pH 7.4. The brains were then removed, postfixed overnight at 4°C in 4% paraformaldehyde, and then transferred to 30% sucrose for 24 h while continuously being stored at 4°C. Coronal sections (40 μm) were collected using a freezing microtome then stored at −20°C in a cryoprotectant solution until required. Sections were first incubated in rabbit anti-Fos polyclonal antibody (ABE457; 1:3000; Merck) for 24 h at 4°C. Sections were then rinsed in TBST (3 × 10 min) containing 3% NGS, then incubated in Tris buffered saline with Tween-20 (TBST) containing biotinylated goat anti-rabbit IgG (1:100; Vector Laboratories) and 3% NGS for 1 h and then rinsed in 0.1 M TBST (3 × 10 min). After which sections were incubated for 1 h in ABC complex (Vectastain Elite ABC Kit; Vector Laboratories), then washed in 0.1 M TBST (3 × 10 min), then washed in 0.05 M Tris buffer (2 × 10 min). Sections were then incubated in DAB solution (DAB Peroxidase (HRP) Substrate Kit (with Nickel), 3,3′-diaminobenzidine; Vector Labs) for ~30 s. Sections were then mounted onto subbed slides, dried and dehydrated before cover slipping. Regions of interest, the OFC and nucleus accumbens, were selected according to a mouse stereotaxic atlas of the brain (52) (Fig. 4A and B). Three sections per brain region per animal were counted and averaged to give number of Fos positive cells within an area of 0.6 mm2 at 10× magnification using the Image J analysis program (version 1.43q, NIH, USA).
Statistical analysis
The main measures from the SSRTT were the proportions of correct stopping and going, the correct go reaction time, the trial initiation latency, proportion of trials started and the reward collection latency. Ancillary measures of general task performance, including the numbers of food magazine entries, initiation and go nose-pokes and locomotion measured as beam break were also assessed. SSRTs in the task were estimated employing the standard procedure described in Logan et al. (30), using data from where the proportion of correct stop responses is ~ 50%. For each subject, data from the sessions in which the stop-signal positions were varied relative to the individualized go reaction time, were ranked by the proportion of correct stop responses, and data from sessions in which this value was between 40% and 60% (i.e. 50% ± 10%) were averaged. The latency of stopping as defined by the SSRT was derived from the distribution of correct go reaction times and the proportion of correctly stopped trials as previously described (29,53). Hence, for each of the sessions determined above, the correct go reaction times were rank ordered from smallest to largest and the nth value found, where n is the rank order position based on the proportion of failing to stop correctly in stop trials, corrected for the occurrence of omitted go trials (53–55). To determine the SSRT, the time the stop signal was presented (i.e. ‘mean correct go reaction time’ × ‘% mean stop-signal position’) was subtracted from the nth correct go reaction time value. Scores calculated as percentages were arcsine, and latencies square root transformed prior to analysis.
Data were analysed using SPSS (V.20, SPSS Inc, USA), and were first assessed for normality, and then subjected to two-tailed t-test or ANOVA, if appropriate or equivalent non-parametric analyses, and where sphericity assumptions were violated, Greenhouse–Geisser corrections were used. As both male and female mice were used in these experiments, all data was subjected to an initial analysis that included GENDER as a between-subjects factor; however, if this was non-significant then a further analysis was conducted without this factor. The total volume consumed/bodyweight0.75 and reward preference from reward consumption test were assessed by ANOVAs with between subjects factor of GENOTYPE (WT, PSWICdel) and GENDER, and within subject factor of SESSION (Days 1 through 7), and the number of sessions taken to shape and train the mice in the SSRTT by unpaired two-tailed t-tests. SSRTT parameters were analysed by separate ANOVAs with between-subject factors of GENOTYPE and GENDER and within-subjects factor of STOP-SIGNAL POSITION (0 (mean BL), 10%, 20%, 30%, 40%, 50%, 60%, 70%, 80%, 90%) and STOP-PERFORMANCE (mean BL, 50% correct stopping). The effects of WAY163909 were analysed by ANOVA with between subject factors of GENOTYPE and GENDER and within-subjects contrasts factor of DOSE (vehicle, 0.1, 1 and 3 mg/kg), with vehicle as the planned contrasting level. For the analysis of c-Fos reactivity, separate two-way ANOVA were conducted for each brain region investigated, with between subject factors of GENOTYPE and DOSE (vehicle, 1 mg/kg WAY163909) were used. If a significant difference was found, then post hoc pairwise comparisons were performed using t-tests or Bonferroni tests, and adjusted for multiple comparisons. Criterion level of significance was set at the 0.05 level. All data are shown as mean ± standard error of the mean (S.E.M.).
Funding
The Biotechnology and Biological Research Council UK (BB/J016756/1). The authors are members of Medical Research Council Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics and Genomics (G0801418).
Supplementary Material
Author contributions
L.S.W., A.R.I. and T.H. devised the experiments. J.R.D. and T.H. performed the experiments and analysed the data. J.R.D., L.S.W., A.R.I. and T.H. wrote the paper.
References
- 1. Buiting K. (2010) Prader–Willi syndrome and Angelman syndrome. Am. J. Med. Genet. C Semin. Med. Genet., 154C, 365–376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Goldstone A.P. (2004) Prader–Willi syndrome: advances in genetics, pathophysiology and treatment. Trends Endocrinol. Metab., 15, 12–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Sinnema M., Boer H., Collin P., Maaskant M.A., van Roozendaal K.E., Schrander-Stumpel C.T. and Curfs L.M. (2011) Psychiatric illness in a cohort of adults with Prader–Willi syndrome. Res. Dev. Disabil., 32, 1729–1735. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Woodcock K.A., Oliver C. and Humphreys G.W. (2011) The relationship between specific cognitive impairment and behaviour in Prader–Willi syndrome. J. Intellect. Disabil. Res., 55, 152–171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Woodcock K.A., Oliver C. and Humphreys G.W. (2009) Task-switching deficits and repetitive behaviour in genetic neurodevelopmental disorders: data from children with Prader–Willi syndrome chromosome 15 q11–q13 deletion and boys with fragile X syndrome. Cogn. Neuropsychol., 26, 172–194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Soni S., Whittington J., Holland A.J., Webb T., Maina E.N., Boer H. and Clarke D. (2008) The phenomenology and diagnosis of psychiatric illness in people with Prader–Willi syndrome. Psychol. Med., 38, 1505–1514. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Lanfranchi S. and Vianello R. (2012) Stress, locus of control, and family cohesion and adaptability in parents of children with down, Williams, fragile X, and Prader–Willi syndromes. Am. J. Intellect. Dev. Disabil., 117, 207–224. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Bieth E., Eddiry S., Gaston V., Lorenzini F., Buffet A., Conte Auriol F., Molinas C., Cailley D., Rooryck C., Arveiler B. et al. (2014) Highly restricted deletion of the SNORD116 region is implicated in Prader–Willi syndrome. Eur. J. Hum. Gen., 23, 252–255. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Ding F., Li H.H., Li J., Myers R.M. and Francke U. (2010) Neonatal maternal deprivation response and developmental changes in gene expression revealed by hypothalamic gene expression profiling in mice. PLoS One, 5, e9402. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Sahoo T., del Gaudio D., German J.R., Shinawi M., Peters S.U., Person R.E., Garnica A., Cheung S.W. and Beaudet A.L. (2008) Prader-Willi phenotype caused by paternal deficiency for the HBII-85 C/D box small nucleolar RNA cluster. Nat. Genet., 40, 719–721. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Burnett L.C., Hubner G., LeDuc C.A., Morabito M.V., Carli J.F.M. and Leibel R.L. (2017) Loss of the imprinted, non-coding Snord116 gene cluster in the interval deleted in the Prader Willi syndrome results in murine neuronal and endocrine pancreatic developmental phenotypes. Hum. Mol. Genet., 26, 4606–4616. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Lassi G., Priano L., Maggi S., Garcia-Garcia C., Balzani E., El-Assawy N., Pagani M., Tinarelli F., Giardino D., Mauro A. et al. (2016) Deletion of the Snord116/SNORD116 alters sleep in mice and patients with Prader–Willi syndrome. Sleep, 39, 637–644. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Lassi G., Maggi S., Balzani E., Cosentini I., Garcia-Garcia C. and Tucci V. (2016) Working-for-food behaviors: a preclinical study in Prader–Willi mutant mice. Genetics, 204, 1129–1138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Ding F., Li H.H., Zhang S., Solomon N.M., Camper S.A., Cohen P. and Francke U. (2008) SnoRNA Snord116 (Pwcr1/MBII-85) deletion causes growth deficiency and Hyperphagia in mice. PLoS One, 3, e1709. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Zieba J., Low J.K., Purtell L., Qi Y., Campbell L., Herzog H. and Karl T. (2015) Behavioural characteristics of the Prader–Willi syndrome related biallelic Snord116 mouse model. Neuropeptides, 53, 71–77. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Davies J.R., Humby T., Dwyer D.M., Garfield A.S., Furby H., Wilkinson L.S., Wells T. and Isles A. (2015) Calorie seeking, but not hedonic response, contributes to hyperphagia in a mouse model for Prader–Willi syndrome. Eur. J. Neurosci., 42, 2105–2113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Golding D.M., Rees D.J., Davies J.R., Relkovic D., Furby H.V., Guschina I.A., Hopkins A.L., Davies J.S., Resnick J.L., Isles A.R. et al. (2017) Paradoxical leanness in the imprinting-Centre deletion mouse model for Prader–Willi syndrome. J. Endocrinol., 232, 123–135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Relkovic D., Doe C.M., Humby T., Johnstone K.A., Resnick J.L., Holland A.J., Hagan J.J., Wilkinson L.S. and Isles A.R. (2010) Behavioural and cognitive abnormalities in an imprinting Centre deletion mouse model for Prader–Willi syndrome. Eur. J. Neurosci., 31, 156–164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Relkovic D., Humby T., Hagan J.J., Wilkinson L.S. and Isles A.R. (2012) Enhanced appetitive learning and reversal learning in a mouse model for Prader–Willi syndrome. Behav. Neurosci., 126, 488–492. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Garfield A.S., Davies J.R., Burke L.K., Furby H.V., Wilkinson L.S., Heisler L.K. and Isles A.R. (2016) Increased alternate splicing of Htr2c in a mouse model for Prader–Willi syndrome leads disruption of 5HT2C receptor mediated appetite. Mol. Brain, 9, 95. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Doe C.M., Relkovic D., Garfield A.S., Dalley J.W., Theobald D.E., Humby T., Wilkinson L.S. and Isles A.R. (2009) Loss of the imprinted snoRNA mbii-52 leads to increased 5htr2c pre-RNA editing and altered 5HT2CR-mediated behaviour. Hum. Mol. Genet., 18, 2140–2148. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Martin C.B., Ramond F., Farrington D.T., Aguiar A.S. Jr., Chevarin C., Berthiau A.S., Caussanel S., Lanfumey L., Herrick-Davis K., Hamon M. et al. (2013) RNA splicing and editing modulation of 5-HT(2C) receptor function: relevance to anxiety and aggression in VGV mice. Mol. Psychiatry, 18, 656–665. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Kishore S. and Stamm S. (2006) The snoRNA HBII-52 regulates alternative splicing of the serotonin receptor 2C. Science, 311, 230–232. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Vitali P., Basyuk E., Le Meur E., Bertrand E., Muscatelli F., Cavaille J. and Huttenhofer A. (2005) ADAR2-mediated editing of RNA substrates in the nucleolus is inhibited by C/D small nucleolar RNAs. J. Cell Biol., 169, 745–753. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Kishore S., Khanna A., Zhang Z., Hui J., Balwierz P.J., Stefan M., Beach C., Nicholls R.D., Zavolan M. and Stamm S. (2010) The snoRNA MBII-52 (SNORD 115) is processed into smaller RNAs and regulates alternative splicing. Hum. Mol. Genet., 19, 1153–1164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Cavaille J., Buiting K., Kiefmann M., Lalande M., Brannan C.I., Horsthemke B., Bachellerie J.P., Brosius J. and Huttenhofer A. (2000) Identification of brain-specific and imprinted small nucleolar RNA genes exhibiting an unusual genomic organization. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A., 97, 14311–14316. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Runte M., Varon R., Horn D., Horsthemke B. and Buiting K. (2005) Exclusion of the C/D box snoRNA gene cluster HBII-52 from a major role in Prader–Willi syndrome. Hum. Genet., 116, 228–230. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Isles A.R. (2017) Htr2c splice variants and 5HT2CR-mediated appetite. Trends Endocrinol. Metab., 28, 542–544. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Logan G.D. (1994) On the ability to inhibit thought and action: A users' guide to the stop signal paradigm. In Dagenbach D. and Carr T.H. (eds), Inhibitory processes in attention, memory, and language. (pp. 189-239). San Diego: Academic Press.
- 30. Logan G.D., Cowan W.B. and Davis K.A. (1984) On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time responses: a model and a method. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform., 10, 276–291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Humby T., Eddy J.B., Good M.A., Reichelt A.C. and Wilkinson L.S. (2013) A novel translational assay of response inhibition and impulsivity: effects of prefrontal cortex lesions, drugs used in ADHD, and serotonin 2C receptor antagonism. Neuropsychopharmacology, 38, 2150–2159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Bari A. and Robbins T.W. (2013) Inhibition and impulsivity: behavioral and neural basis of response control. Prog. Neurobiol., 108, 44–79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Chamberlain S.J., Johnstone K.A., DuBose A.J., Simon T.A., Bartolomei M.S., Resnick J.L. and Brannan C.I. (2004) Evidence for genetic modifiers of postnatal lethality in PWS-IC deletion mice. Hum. Mol. Genet., 13, 2971–2977. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Davies W., Humby T., Trent S., Eddy J.B., Ojarikre O.A. and Wilkinson L.S. (2014) Genetic and pharmacological modulation of the steroid sulfatase axis improves response control; comparison with drugs used in ADHD. Neuropsychopharmacology, 39, 2622–2632. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Perez Diaz M., Wilson M.E. and Howell L.L. (2019) Effects of long-term high-fat food or methamphetamine intake and serotonin 2C receptors on reversal learning in female rhesus macaques. Neuropsychopharmacology, 44, 478–486. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Higgins G.A., Zeeb F.D. and Fletcher P.J. (2017) Role of impulsivity and reward in the anti-obesity actions of 5-HT2C receptor agonists. J. Psychopharmacol., 31, 1403–1418. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Banks M.L. (2019) Impaired cognitive behavioral flexibility following methamphetamine or high caloric diet consumption: a common 5-HT2C mechanism? Neuropsychopharmacology, 44, 461–462. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Dent C.L., Isles A.R. and Humby T. (2014) Measuring risk-taking in mice: balancing the risk between seeking reward and danger. Eur. J. Neurosci., 39, 520–530. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Schachar R., Logan G.D., Robaey P., Chen S., Ickowicz A. and Barr C. (2007) Restraint and cancellation: multiple inhibition deficits in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol., 35, 229–238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Dent C.L. and Isles A.R. (2014) An overview of measuring impulsive behaviour in mice. Curr. Protoc. Mouse Biol., 4, 35–45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Humby T. and Wilkinson L.S. (2011) Assaying dissociable elements of behavioural inhibition and impulsivity: translational utility of animal models. Curr. Opin. Pharmacol., 11, 534–539. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Eagle D.M., Baunez C., Hutcheson D.M., Lehmann O., Shah A.P. and Robbins T.W. (2008) Stop-signal reaction-time task performance: role of prefrontal cortex and subthalamic nucleus. Cereb. Cortex, 18, 178–188. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Eagle D.M. and Robbins T.W. (2003) Lesions of the medial prefrontal cortex or nucleus accumbens core do not impair inhibitory control in rats performing a stop-signal reaction time task. Behav. Brain Res., 146, 131–144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Robinson E.S., Dalley J.W., Theobald D.E., Glennon J.C., Pezze M.A., Murphy E.R. and Robbins T.W. (2008) Opposing roles for 5-HT2A and 5-HT2C receptors in the nucleus accumbens on inhibitory response control in the 5-choice serial reaction time task. Neuropsychopharmacology, 33, 2398–2406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Dracheva S., Lyddon R., Barley K., Marcus S.M., Hurd Y.L. and Byne W.M. (2009) Editing of serotonin 2C receptor mRNA in the prefrontal cortex characterizes high-novelty locomotor response behavioral trait. Neuropsychopharmacology, 34, 2237–2251. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Matarazzo V., Caccialupi L., Schaller F., Shvarev Y., Kourdougli N., Bertoni A., Menuet C., Voituron N., Deneris E., Gaspar P. et al. (2017) Necdin shapes serotonergic development and SERT activity modulating breathing in a mouse model for Prader–Willi syndrome. Elife, 6:e32640. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Mercer R.E., Kwolek E.M., Bischof J.M., van Eede M., Henkelman R.M. and Wevrick R. (2009) Regionally reduced brain volume, altered serotonin neurochemistry, and abnormal behavior in mice null for the circadian rhythm output gene Magel2. Am. J. Med. Gen. B Neuro. Gen. Int. Soc. Psychiatr. Genet., 150B, 1085–1099. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Hurt R.T., Mundi M.S. and Ebbert J.O. (2018) Challenging obesity, diabetes, and addiction: the potential of lorcaserin extended release. Diabetes Metab. Syndr. Obes., 11, 469–47849. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Humby T., Laird F.M., Davies W. and Wilkinson L.S. (1999) Visuospatial attentional functioning in mice: interactions between cholinergic manipulations and genotype. Eur. J. Neurosci., 11, 2813–2823. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Carter J.D., Farrow M., Silberstein R.B., Stough C., Tucker A. and Pipingas A. (2003) Assessing inhibitory control: a revised approach to the stop signal task. J. Atten. Disord., 6, 153–161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Rosenzweig-Lipson S., Zhang J., Mazandarani H., Harrison B.L., Sabb A., Sabalski J., Stack G., Welmaker G., Barrett J.E. and Dunlop J. (2006) Antiobesity-like effects of the 5-HT2C rece Supplementary Results Table 1: Training and shaping measures ptor agonist WAY-161503. Brain Res., 1073–1074, 240–251. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Paxinos G. and Franklin K.B.J. (2001) The Mouse Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates. Academic Press, San Diego. [Google Scholar]
- 53. Eagle D.M. and Robbins T.W. (2003) Inhibitory control in rats performing a stop-signal reaction-time task: effects of lesions of the medial striatum and d-amphetamine. Behav. Neurosci., 117, 1302–1317. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. Solanto M.V., Abikoff H., Sonuga-Barke E., Schachar R., Logan G.D., Wigal T., Hechtman L., Hinshaw S. and Turkel E. (2001) The ecological validity of delay aversion and response inhibition as measures of impulsivity in AD/HD: a supplement to the NIMH multimodal treatment study of AD/HD. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol., 29, 215–228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Tannock R., Schachar R.J., Carr R.P., Chajczyk D. and Logan G.D. (1989) Effects of methylphenidate on inhibitory control in hyperactive children. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol., 17, 473–491. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.