Abstract
Genetic variation in CACNA1C, which encodes the alpha-1 subunit of Cav1.2 L-type voltage-gated calcium channels (VGCCs), has been strongly linked to risk for psychiatric disorders including schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. How genetic variation in CACNA1C contributes to risk for these disorders is however not fully known. Both schizophrenia and bipolar disorder are associated with impairments in reversal learning (RL), which may contribute to symptoms seen in these conditions. We used a translational RL paradigm to investigate whether genetic variation in CACNA1C affects RL in both humans and transgenic rats. Associated changes in gene expression were explored using in situ hybridization and quantitative PCR in rats and the BRAINEAC online human database. Risk-associated genetic variation in CACNA1C in healthy human participants was associated with impairments in RL. Consistent with this finding, rats bearing a heterozygous deletion of Cacna1c were impaired in an analogous touchscreen RL task. We investigated the possible molecular mechanism underlying this impairment and found that Cacna1c +/− rats show decreased expression of Bdnf in prefrontal cortex. Examination of BRAINEAC data showed that human risk-associated genetic variation in CACNA1C is also associated with altered expression of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) in the prefrontal cortex in humans. These results indicate that genetic variation in CACNA1C may contribute to risk for schizophrenia and bipolar disorder by impacting behavioral flexibility, potentially through altered regulation of BDNF expression in the prefrontal cortex. Tests of RL may be useful for translational studies and in the development of therapies targeting VGCCs.
Keywords: reversal learning, rat, behavior, BDNF, translational, calcium
Introduction
Genetic variation in CACNA1C, which encodes the alpha-1 subunit of Cav1.2 L-type voltage-gated calcium channels (VGCCs), has been strongly linked to risk for psychiatric disorders including schizophrenia and bipolar disorder.1,2 The association between common genetic variation in intron 3 of the CACNA1C gene and schizophrenia and bipolar disorder has been confirmed by multiple studies across different populations.1,3–7 Risk mediated through common variants in CACNA1C has also been found to be shared across disorders, with Smoller et al2 finding similar associations with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), autism spectrum disorder, bipolar disorder, major depressive disorder, and schizophrenia. Expression studies in cell lines and postmortem tissue have suggested that the intron 3 risk variants may act to decrease expression of CACNA1C, although the exact direction of effect has varied between studies and tissue types.8–12 Rare deleterious mutations in VGCCs have also been found to be associated with risk for schizophrenia.13 Notably, this enrichment was primarily driven by pore-forming alpha-1 subunits and alpha-2-delta auxiliary subunits of VGCCs, including deleterious mutation loss of function variants in CACNA1C.13
The robust association of CACNA1C with multiple psychiatric disorders has led to the investigation of potential endophenotypes and downstream biological mechanisms that may link genetic risk to disease-relevant behavior. Previous studies have identified a link between risk-associated variation in CACNA1C and a range of tasks requiring cognitive and behavioral flexibility. Specifically, genetic variation in CACNA1C and related VGCCs has been strongly associated with working memory performance as assessed by the N-back test, which is known to depend on prefrontal cortex (PFC) function.14–16 Genetic variation in CACNA1C has similarly been shown to affect logical memory in patients17 and verbal working memory in healthy controls.18CACNA1C genotype has also been found to affect reward learning.19,20 Genetic imaging studies have been conducted to identify neural circuits affected by genetic variation in CACNA1C and have shown altered activation of fronto-limbic brain circuitry in risk allele carriers.12,15,19,21
Reversal learning (RL) tasks represent a powerful method for investigating cognitive flexibility with considerable potential for cross-species translational studies.22 RL involves the inhibition of a previously learnt association between a stimulus and reward and the acquisition of a new opposite contingency or rule. It is a necessary form of behavioral flexibility that allows new experience to influence behavior.23 Deficits in this form of learning can lead to inappropriate perseverative behavior that is not supported by, or beneficial in, the current environment. Impairments in RL have been reliably observed in patients with a range of psychiatric conditions including schizophrenia and bipolar disorder.24–29 Furthermore, deficits in RL have also been observed in first-episode psychosis patients30 and in the context of high polygenic risk for schizophrenia,31 suggesting that RL deficits may reflect a causal mechanism linking common genetic schizophrenia risk to symptomology. Homologous fronto-limbic networks have been found to be involved in RL in both rodents and humans,22,23,32 and the development of a touchscreen-based RL paradigm for rodents makes the task highly translatable.33,34
At a molecular level, the neurotrophin brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) has been shown to play a critical role in PFC during RL.35,36 Mice lacking the main activity-regulated form of Bdnf show impaired plasticity in the PFC and behavioral deficits in RL tasks.35,36 Furthermore, manipulations in animal models that decrease prefrontal Bdnf, including maternal separation, have been shown to result in impairments in RL.37,38 VGCCs, including CACNA1C, have been shown to be central to the activity-dependent regulation of BDNF, suggesting the possibility that genetic variation in CACNA1C may affect cognitive flexibility in part through altering Bdnf expression in the PFC.39–43
On the basis of these findings, we sought to investigate the effect on RL of CACNA1C risk variants in humans and altered Cacna1c dosage in rats. We additionally assessed the impact of genetic variation in CACNA1C on prefrontal BDNF levels in both humans and rats. By conducting these behavioral, genetic, and molecular studies in parallel, we aimed to increase confidence in our findings and develop translational models for the future development of novel therapies for psychiatric disorders targeting VGCCs.
Methods and Materials
Human Participants
One hundred right-handed Caucasian healthy participants (aged 19–47 years) derived from the local community took part in the study. Ethical approval was given by the ethics committee of the School of Psychology, Cardiff University, and informed consent was obtained from each participant prior to the study. Participants had no history of psychiatric illness (themselves or first-degree relative) and did not report taking any psychotropic medication or illegal substances. All participants were right-handed, university graduates (with 17+ years’ education). A total of 84 participants (mean age 23.95 ± 3.64 SD, 49 women) were used for analysis, after exclusion for quality control of genetic data (N = 10) or incomplete/missing behavioral data (N = 6). The data used in this study have previously been used to observe associations between genome-wide polygenic risk for schizophrenia and RL behavior.31
Probabilistic RL Behavioral Task
Participants were asked to make a choice between a blue and a green stimulus presented simultaneously (figure 1). A correct response was followed by a white “smiley” face and rewarded with a +1 p monetary reward and a red “frowny” face and a −1 p monetary punishment followed an incorrect response. Participants were told to maximize their “earnings” throughout the task. After 7–11 trials, the contingencies were reversed and the previously rewarded stimulus was now punished. In total there were 12 reversal sessions (total of 108 choice trials). Each session included 1 or 2 “probabilistic errors” in which the correct response was punished to ensure sufficient difficulty in the task. “Total Earnings” were calculated for each participant for the full task (in pence) based on the amount of rewards and punishments they received throughout. Performance following the first contingency reversal (“Reversal Accuracy”) was calculated as percentage correct for that session block.
DNA Extraction and Participant Genotyping
Samples of saliva were collected from participants for genotyping using Oragene DNA (OG-500) kits (DNA Genotek Inc). Genotyping was conducted using a custom HumanCoreExome BeadChip Kit (Illumina), which included 570 038 variants. Data were quality-controlled in PLINK.44 Participants were removed if data suggested non-European ancestry, relation to other participants, or incomplete genotyping <97%. Genotypes were then imputed by estimating haplotypes using SHAPEIT and imputing genotypes from the reference set 1000 genomes (December 2013, release 1000 genomes haplotypes phase 1 integrated variant set) using IMPUTE245,46. For the current analysis, genotypes were extracted from the full single-nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) dataset for rs1006737 and rs2007044 only. These SNPs were selected because rs1006737 is the most consistently reported disease-associated SNP in CACNA1C across disorders,47 and the most recent schizophrenia genome-wide association study found rs2007044 to have the strongest association with schizophrenia (as well as confirming association with rs1006737).48 These 2 SNPs are in high linkage disequilibrium (r2 = .8). Allelic association analyses were conducted to determine whether incidence of each individual allele at each SNP location was associated with performance. Student’s t tests were used to compare performance for each allele.
Animals
Eighty-eight male Sprague‑Dawley Cacna1c heterozygous (HET) knockout rats were housed with wild-type (WT) littermates in groups of 1–4. Heterozygous Cacna1c Sprague–Dawley rats were obtained from cryopreserved embryos, created with zinc-finger nuclease (ZFN) technology (Sage Research Labs). A pair of 5-finger ZFNs, recognizing a total of 30 base pairs, was used to target exon 6 (location 460649–460652 bp in genomic sequence), resulting in a frameshift and an early stop codon. Manipulation of founders was confirmed by PCR (Fwd: 5′- GCTGCTGAGCCTTTTATTGG-3′; Rev: 5′- CCTCCTGGATAGCTGCTGAC-3′) and sequencing. Details of basic molecular and behavioral characterization of Cacna1c HET rats are given in the Supplementary Material, including supplementary figure S1. Given the limitations of individual Cav1.2 alpha-1 subunit antibodies, we used 2 separate antibodies in western blots and an ELISA assay to confirm protein levels are reduced in our heterozygous line (see Supplementary Material). All procedures were conducted in accordance with the guidelines published in the Institute of Laboratory Animals Resources Commission on Life Sciences 1996 Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals.
Animal Touchscreen RL Paradigm
In this study, 13 WT and 12 HET rats were tested using a touchscreen RL task.33 Animals were placed on water restriction on testing days to maximize motivation for reward, in accordance with Home Office regulations. Animals were run in sets of 4 using Bussey–Saksida rat touchscreen chambers (model 80604; Campden Instruments) with accompanying Animal Behavior Environment Test (ABET) II software (model 89505; Campden Instruments). The overall stages and criteria for moving on to the next phase of the experiment are shown in supplementary table S1. Animals were pretrained to collect the reward (10% sucrose solution) from the magazine (Habituation), and to nose poke a stimulus to receive reward (Must Touch). Following completion of pretraining, animals were presented with a pair of stimuli (S+ and S−, side of presentation was allocated pseudorandomly). Touches to the S+ resulted in delivery of the reward and the termination of the trial. Touches to the S− resulted in the termination of the trial and a 10 s time-out period before the next trial could be initiated (figure 2). Visual discrimination training continued until an animal was performing at 80% correct within a session on 2 consecutive days (supplementary table S1), following this the contingencies were reversed (S+ becomes the S−, and vice versa). Early phase reversal was characterized by overcoming the previously learnt association and obtaining at least 50% correct responding to the newly rewarded stimulus. Late phase reversal was defined as obtaining 80% correct responding to the newly rewarded stimulus on 2 consecutive days (supplementary table S1).
Correct responses and number of errors (incorrect responses) for each session were recorded to establish performance across phases and perseveration. For each session of discrimination and RL, % correct response [(correct responses/total responses) × 100] was calculated. The number of sessions and trials required to reach criterion were recorded for each condition. Animals were only included if they had reached criterion for that training phase. The proportion of animals of each genotype to complete each phase was compared to establish success rate. Latencies to respond to stimuli were recorded to indicate speed of decision and response. Latencies were averaged across the first 3 sessions and the last 3 sessions to give an indication of change within phase. Student’s t tests were used to compare the number of sessions taken to complete and error number per session and chi-squared tests were used to compare the frequency of completion for experimental condition by genotype. Repeated measures ANOVAs with time and genotype were conducted to analyze response latencies across sessions.
Quantitative In Situ Hybridization
In situ hybridization was used to quantify the expression of Bdnf and Cacna1c in rodent tissue using established techniques.49,50 Full details of the in situ hybridization methods and analysis are given in the Supplementary Material.
Quantitative PCR
Quantitative PCR (qPCR) was conducted with SensiMix SYBR Green (Bioline) as described in Supplementary Material.
Human Gene Expression Analysis by Risk SNP
We examined the effects of genetic variation in CACNA1C at rs1006737 and rs2007044 on postmortem gene expression using the BRAINEAC online database (www.braineac.org).51 Using the BRAINEAC online general user interface, the expression of BDNF was stratified by rs1006737 and rs2007044 for each individual probe set available. Allele frequencies for the two SNPS were rs1006737: A = 29.1%, G = 70.9%; rs2007044: G = 36.9%, A = 63.1%.
Results
Healthy Human CACNA1C Risk Allele Carriers Show Altered RL
The risk allele at both rs1006737 and rs2007044 was associated with fewer correct responses during RL (figure 3). Average performance associated with the risk allele (A) at rs1006737 was 64.08% (SEM = 2.66%) compared with 71.27% (SEM = 1.90) associated with the non-risk allele (G); and at rs2007044 average performance associated with the risk allele (G) was 64.66% (SEM = 2.41) compared with 71.69% for the non-risk allele (SEM = 2.04%). Student’s t tests between revealed a significant effect of risk allele at rs1006737 and rs2007044 with first “Reversal Accuracy” [t(166) = −2.242, P = .026 and t(165) = −2.230, P = .027, respectively]. There were no significant effects of risk allele at rs1006737 (t(166) = −1.562, P = .120) or rs2007044 on “Total Earnings” [t(165) = −1.122, P = .264] indicating comparative levels of task motivation. Further analysis confirmed that the association between CACNA1C risk allele and performance was specific to trials following reversal and was not affected by correction for relevant covariates including age and gender (see Supplementary Material including supplementary figure S2 and supplementary table S3).
Cacna1c Heterozygote Rats Show Altered RL
There were no differences between rat genotypes in number of sessions to reach criteria for Habituation or Must Touch [t(21) = −0.249, P = .806 and t(18.206) = 1.323, P = .202, respectively]. Twelve of the 13 WT animals and 9 out of 12 HETs animals reached criteria for “Visual Discrimination” with no difference in the proportion of completion [χ(1) = 1.391, P = .238] or number of sessions taken to reach criteria [t(19) = −0.833, P = .415]. Analyses were conducted to compare the proportion of WT and HET animals that completed the “Early” reversal stage, indicating a successful inhibition of response to prior contingency. Fewer HETs completed “Early Reversal,” with 12 WT animals successfully completing early reversal (92%), compared with 7 HET animals (58%) [χ(1) = 3.949, P = .047] (figure 4). Mean number of sessions for the completion of each training session by HET and WT animals are shown in supplementary table S4. This pattern was reflected in the total number of errors made at each stage. Specifically, there was no difference between genotypes in total errors during the Visual Discrimination phase [t(21) = −0.001, P = 1], but HET animals made significantly more errors compared to WT during Early Reversal [t(17) = 2.176, P = .044].
Comparison of response latencies further indicates altered behavior in HET animals following reversal of contingencies. There was no evidence of a difference in time to respond to stimuli during Visual Discrimination [F(1,19) = 1.134, P = .300]; however, HET animals responded significantly faster than WT during early reversal [F(1,16) = 10.921, P = .004] (supplementary figure S3).
Reduced Prefrontal Cortex Bdnf Expression in Cacna1c Heterozygote Rats
qPCR analysis revealed a significant decrease in the expression of Bdnf in the PFC of Cacna1c heterozygous rats [t(19) = −2.223, P = .039] (figure 5), with no differences observed in the CA1 region of the hippocampus [t(19) = 0.741, P = .408]. In situ hybridization was used to further compare levels of Bdnf mRNA expression in the PFC of a separate cohort of Cacna1c HET (n = 5) and WT (n = 5) rats. Analysis confirmed that HET animals showed significantly reduced expression of total Bdnf mRNA levels in the PFC compared with WT animals [t(8) = 2.316, P = .049] (figure 5), with no difference in expression in the hippocampal CA1 field [t(8) = −1.418, P = .194].
Risk SNPs in CACNA1C are associated with decreased expression of BDNF in human frontal cortex
We investigated whether genetic variation at the two CACNA1C risk loci is associated with expression changes of BDNF in the human PFC. Examination of the BRAINEAC database revealed a significant association between genotype at rs1006737 and rs2007044 and the expression of BDNF (P = .007 and = .042, respectively) as assessed by probe set 3367273 (supplementary figure S4). Risk allele carriers at both SNPs showed significantly reduced expression of BDNF, with homozygous carriers having a more pronounced reduction.
Discussion
Summary
We report that healthy human subjects bearing alleles in the CACNA1C gene associated with risk for psychiatric disorders show poorer reversal performance on a probabilistic RL task than non-risk allele carriers. We also report convergent evidence that rats heterozygous for the Cacna1c gene are impaired in the same phase of RL. We show that Cacna1c hemizygosity in rats is associated with decreased Bdnf expression in the PFC, and that human risk variants in CACNA1C are also associated with decreased BDNF expression as indexed by selective markers in postmortem tissue. Overall, these results suggest that genetic variation in CACNA1C may contribute to risk for psychiatric disorders through impacting cognitive flexibility, and that at a molecular level, this may be in part caused by alterations in BDNF expression.
Human Risk Alleles in CACNA1C Impact RL
We found that risk-associated common genetic variants in CACNA1C were found to be associated with deficits in the reversal stage of a probabilistic RL task. Specifically, in healthy human participants the risk-associated alleles of CACNA1C were associated with impaired performance in a probabilistic RL task, indicating an impact of genetic variation in CACNA1C on cognitive flexibility. These results suggest that the risk variant may be affecting neural circuitry involved in the comparison and updating of existing information to inform subsequent behavior and forming optimal choice contingencies. Prior studies in human participants suggest a specific role for the PFC, especially orbito-frontal regions, and the striatum in RL.22 Furthermore, human genetic imaging studies have indicated an impact of genetic variation of CACNA1C genotype on fronto-limbic brain activation and connectivity, although the impact of CACNA1C genotype on brain activation during RL has not been specifically investigated.12,15,21,52 Polygenic risk scores for schizophrenia, to which genetic variants in CACNA1C contribute, have however been shown to influence fronto-striatal brain activation during RL.31
Cognitive flexibility is known to be impaired in a range of psychiatric disorders that have been associated with genetic variation in CACNA1C. Deficits in RL have, for example, been demonstrated in patients with psychotic disorders including schizophrenia and bipolar disorder.23,25–27,29,30 In schizophrenia, impairments in RL have been shown to be related to clinical ratings of symptoms including disorganization and thought disorder, findings that were not explained by impairments in general intelligence.26 Furthermore, imaging studies of patients with schizophrenia performing RL tasks have demonstrated altered activation of fronto-striatal brain networks.29,53 The present results suggest that genetic variation in the psychiatric risk gene CACNA1C may contribute to the impairments in cognitive flexibility seen in these disorders.
Alterations in Cacna1c in Rats Impact RL
In a touchscreen reward-based RL task, we found that Cacna1c heterozygous knockout rats demonstrated a selective impairment in the early stage of RL. Specifically, fewer Cacna1c heterozygous animals successfully reached criteria for reversal. Furthermore, heterozygous animals responded more quickly and with more errors, indicative of behavioral inflexibility and inappropriate responding. This pattern of responses is consistent with a lack of inhibition of previous responding. There was no difference between Cacna1c heterozygous animals and controls during the initial visual discrimination component of the task, indicating that these animals could successfully acquire novel stimulus–reward associations. These results therefore suggest that Cacna1c heterozygous animals have a selective impairment in inhibition and the switching of reinforced contingencies.
These results in rats show a striking degree of convergence with the effects seen on RL in human risk allele carriers, suggesting that the low-dosage Cacna1c rat may represent a valuable model of the effects of genetic variation in CACNA1C on cognition in humans. It is notable that some, although not all, reports of the effects of CACNA1C genotype on expression suggest that the common variants in humans may be associated with decreased CACNA1C expression.9,10 Furthermore deleterious mutations in L-type VGCCs have also been associated with risk for schizophrenia in exome-sequencing studies, further implicating low dosage of L-type VGCCs in risk for this disorder.13
Our results are broadly consistent with previous studies on mice with forebrain-specific total knockout of Cacna1c.54,55 Koppe et al55 found forebrain-specific conditional Cacna1c knockout mice adopt different behavioral strategies during an operant reward-based learning task compared to controls. Cacna1c knockout mice did not show an overall behavioral deficit; however, detailed behavioral analysis showed that these animals gained reward based on an outcome-based strategy, rather than learning cue–reward associations, basing their responses more on the previous location of the reward.55 Similarly, Temme et al54 found that mice lacking forebrain Cacna1c could learn contextual and spatial associations normally but were impaired on more subtle tests of context discrimination and pattern completion. A recent study also demonstrated impacts of Cacna1c hemizygosity on social behavior and communication.56 Our results extend these previous findings to show an impact of genetic manipulation of Cacna1c on behavioral flexibility as assessed by RL. In addition, we show a significant behavioral effect of reduced dosage, rather than total ablation, of Cacna1c. This is important as genetic variants associated with increased risk for psychiatric disorders in humans are anticipated to affect Cacna1c dosage and not to produce a total ablation of the gene (which is not compatible with viability).9,10,13
Impact of Genetic Variation in CACNA1C on Prefrontal BDNF Across Species
L-type VGCCs are known to play a critical role in the regulation of BDNF expression.39,57 Previous studies in mice completely lacking forebrain Cacna1c have also demonstrated a reduction in central BDNF levels.58 Furthermore, activity-dependent regulation of BDNF has been shown in rodent models to be critical for both plasticity in the PFC and RL.35,36 These findings suggest that altered regulation of BDNF in the PFC may contribute to deficits in RL.
We found that Cacna1c heterozygous rats have decreased expression of Bdnf in the PFC, a result we confirmed with both qPCR and in situ hybridization. These findings demonstrate that decreased dosage of Cacna1c is sufficient to have a significant impact on prefrontal Bdnf expression. In addition, we found that CACNA1C risk-associated genetic variation in humans is associated with decreased BDNF expression in human PFC, as assessed by selected markers of BDNF in postmortem tissue in the BRAINEAC database. Notably, this effect was not seen for all markers across the BDNF gene, suggesting that genetic variation in CACNA1C may affect specific BDNF transcripts in the PFC, an area that will be important for future investigation. These results suggest a mechanism through which genetic variation in CACNA1C may affect cognitive flexibility and RL, because previous studies have shown that mice lacking activity-regulated expression of Bdnf show impaired RL and altered spike-timing-dependent plasticity in the PFC.35,36 These results also suggest that therapeutic approaches aimed at enhancing prefrontal BDNF levels may be efficacious in alleviating some of the cognitive impairments seen in neuropsychiatric disorders.
Supplementary Material
Supplementary material is available at Schizophrenia Bulletin online.
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by a Wellcome Trust PhD studentship to Lucy Sykes, a Wellcome Trust Strategic Award “DEFINE” (100202/Z/12/Z), and a grant from the Life Sciences Research Network Wales. We thank Caroline Best for expert technical support.
Conflict of interest
The authors have no relevant biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest to declare.
References
- 1. Ripke S, Neale BM, Corvin A, et al. . Biological insights from 108 schizophrenia-associated genetic loci. Nature. 2014;511:421–427. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Smoller JW, Craddock N, Kendler K, et al. . Identification of risk loci with shared effects on five major psychiatric disorders: a genome-wide analysis. Lancet. 2013;381(9875):1371–1379. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Ferreira MA, O’Donovan MC, Meng YA, et al. ; Wellcome Trust Case Control Consortium Collaborative genome-wide association analysis supports a role for ANK3 and CACNA1C in bipolar disorder. Nat Genet. 2008;40:1056–1058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Sklar P, Smoller JW, Fan J, et al. . Whole-genome association study of bipolar disorder. Mol Psychiatry. 2008;13:558–569. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Green EK, Grozeva D, Jones I, et al. . The bipolar disorder risk allele at CACNA1C also confers risk of recurrent major depression and of schizophrenia. Mol Psychiatry. 2010;15:1016–1022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. He K, An Z, Wang Q, et al. . CACNA1C, schizophrenia and major depressive disorder in the Han Chinese population. Br J Psychiatry. 2014;204:36–39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Porcelli S, Lee SJ, Han C, Patkar AA, Serretti A, Pae CU. CACNA1C gene and schizophrenia: a case–control and pharmacogenetic study. Psychiatr Genet. 2015;25:163–167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Roussos P, Mitchell AC, Voloudakis G, et al. . A role for noncoding variation in schizophrenia. Cell Rep. 2014;9:1417–1429. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Eckart N, Song Q, Yang R, et al. . Functional Characterization of Schizophrenia- Associated Variation in CACNA1C. PLoS One. 2016;11:e0157086. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Gershon ES, Grennan K, Busnello J, et al. . A rare mutation of CACNA1C in a patient with bipolar disorder, and decreased gene expression associated with a bipolar-associated common SNP of CACNA1C in brain. Mol Psychiatry. 2014;19:890–894. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Yoshimizu T, Pan JQ, Mungenast AE, et al. . Functional implications of a psychiatric risk variant within CACNA1C in induced human neurons. Mol Psychiatry. 2015;20:162–169. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Bigos KL, Mattay VS, Callicott JH, et al. . Genetic variation in CACNA1C affects brain circuitries related to mental illness. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67:939–945. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Purcell SM, Moran JL, Fromer M, et al. . A polygenic burden of rare disruptive mutations in schizophrenia. Nature. 2014;506:185–190. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Zhang Q, Shen Q, Xu Z, et al. . The effects of CACNA1C gene polymorphism on spatial working memory in both healthy controls and patients with schizophrenia or bipolar disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012;37:677–684. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Cosgrove D, Mothersill O, Kendall K, et al. ; Wellcome Trust Case Control Consortium Cognitive characterization of schizophrenia risk variants involved in synaptic transmission: evidence of CACNA1C’s role in working memory. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2017;42:2612–2622. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Heck A, Fastenrath M, Ackermann S, et al. . Converging genetic and functional brain imaging evidence links neuronal excitability to working memory, psychiatric disease, and brain activity. Neuron. 2014;81:1203–1213. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Hori H, Yamamoto N, Fujii T, et al. . Effects of the CACNA1C risk allele on neurocognition in patients with schizophrenia and healthy individuals. Sci Rep. 2012;2:634. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Dietsche B, Backes H, Laneri D, et al. . The impact of a CACNA1C gene polymorphism on learning and hippocampal formation in healthy individuals: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Neuroimage. 2014;89:256–261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Wessa M, Linke J, Witt SH, et al. . The CACNA1C risk variant for bipolar disorder influences limbic activity. Mol Psychiatry. 2010;15:1126–1127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Lancaster TM, Heerey EA, Mantripragada K, Linden DE. CACNA1C risk variant affects reward responsiveness in healthy individuals. Transl Psychiatry. 2014;4:e461. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Erk S, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Linden DEJ, et al. . Replication of brain function effects of a genome-wide supported psychiatric risk variant in the CACNA1C gene and new multi-locus effects. Neuroimage. 2014;94:147–154. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Gilmour G, Arguello A, Bari A, et al. . Measuring the construct of executive control in schizophrenia: defining and validating translational animal paradigms for discovery research. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2013;37:2125–2140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Floresco SB, Zhang Y, Enomoto T. Neural circuits subserving behavioral flexibility and their relevance to schizophrenia. Behav Brain Res. 2009;204:396–409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Elliott R, McKenna PJ, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ. Neuropsychological evidence for frontostriatal dysfunction in schizophrenia. Psychol Med. 1995;25:619–630. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Waltz JA, Gold JM. Probabilistic reversal learning impairments in schizophrenia: further evidence of orbitofrontal dysfunction. Schizophr Res. 2007;93:296–303. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Leeson VC, Robbins TW, Matheson E, et al. . Discrimination learning, reversal, and set-shifting in first-episode schizophrenia: stability over six years and specific associations with medication type and disorganization syndrome. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;66:586–593. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. McKirdy J, Sussmann JE, Hall J, Lawrie SM, Johnstone EC, McIntosh AM. Set shifting and reversal learning in patients with bipolar disorder or schizophrenia. Psychol Med. 2009;39:1289–1293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Adleman NE, Kayser R, Dickstein D, Blair RJ, Pine D, Leibenluft E. Neural correlates of reversal learning in severe mood dysregulation and pediatric bipolar disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2011;50:1173–1185.e2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Culbreth AJ, Gold JM, Cools R, Barch DM. Impaired activation in cognitive control regions predicts reversal learning in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2016;42:484–493. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Murray GK, Cheng F, Clark L, et al. . Reinforcement and reversal learning in first-episode psychosis. Schizophr Bull. 2008;34:848–855. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Lancaster TM, Ihssen N, Brindley LM, et al. . Associations between polygenic risk for schizophrenia and brain function during probabilistic learning in healthy individuals. Hum Brain Mapp. 2016;37:491–500. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Balleine BW, O’Doherty JP. Human and rodent homologies in action control: corticostriatal determinants of goal-directed and habitual action. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:48–69. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Bussey TJ, Holmes A, Lyon L, et al. . New translational assays for preclinical modelling of cognition in schizophrenia: the touchscreen testing method for mice and rats. Neuropharmacology. 2012;62:1191–1203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Hvoslef-Eide M, Mar AC, Nilsson SR, et al. . The NEWMEDS rodent touchscreen test battery for cognition relevant to schizophrenia. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2015;232:3853–3872. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Sakata K, Woo NH, Martinowich K, et al. . Critical role of promoter IV-driven BDNF transcription in GABAergic transmission and synaptic plasticity in the prefrontal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106:5942–5947. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Sakata K, Martinowich K, Woo NH, et al. . Role of activity-dependent BDNF expression in hippocampal-prefrontal cortical regulation of behavioral perseverance. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013;110:15103–15108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Xue X, Shao S, Wang W, Shao F. Maternal separation induces alterations in reversal learning and brain-derived neurotrophic factor expression in adult rats. Neuropsychobiology. 2013;68:243–249. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Amodeo DA, Grospe G, Zang H, Dwivedi Y, Ragozzino ME. Cognitive flexibility impairment and reduced frontal cortex BDNF expression in the ouabain model of mania. Neuroscience. 2017;345:229–242. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Tao X, Finkbeiner S, Arnold DB, Shaywitz AJ, Greenberg ME. Ca2+ influx regulates BDNF transcription by a CREB family transcription factor-dependent mechanism. Neuron. 1998;20:709–726. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Tao X, West AE, Chen WG, Corfas G, Greenberg ME. A calcium-responsive transcription factor, CaRF, that regulates neuronal activity-dependent expression of BDNF. Neuron. 2002;33:383–395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Ghosh A, Carnahan J, Greenberg ME. Requirement for BDNF in activity-dependent survival of cortical neurons. Science. 1994;263:1618–1623. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. West AE, Chen WG, Dalva MB, et al. . Calcium regulation of neuronal gene expression. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2001;98:11024–11031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Tabuchi A, Nakaoka R, Amano K, et al. . Differential activation of brain-derived neurotrophic factor gene promoters I and III by Ca2+ signals evoked via L-type voltage-dependent and N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor Ca2+ channels. J Biol Chem. 2000;275(23):17269–17275. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Purcell S, Neale B, Todd-Brown K, et al. . PLINK: a tool set for whole-genome association and population-based linkage analyses. Am J Hum Genet. 2007;81:559–575. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Howie BN, Donnelly P, Marchini J. A flexible and accurate genotype imputation method for the next generation of genome-wide association studies. PLoS Genet. 2009;5:e1000529. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Delaneau O, Marchini J, Zagury JF. A linear complexity phasing method for thousands of genomes. Nat Methods. 2011;9:179–181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Bhat S, Dao DT, Terrillion CE, et al. . CACNA1C (Cav1.2) in the pathophysiology of psychiatric disease. Prog Neurobiol. 2012;99:1–14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Ripke S, O’Dushlaine C, Chambert K, et al. ; Multicenter Genetic Studies of Schizophrenia Consortium; Psychosis Endophenotypes International Consortium; Wellcome Trust Case Control Consortium 2 Genome-wide association analysis identifies 13 new risk loci for schizophrenia. Nat Genet. 2013;45:1150–1159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Wisden W, Morris BJ,eds. In Situ Hybridization Protocols for the Brain (Biological Techniques). San Diego, CA: Academic Press; 1994. [Google Scholar]
- 50. Thomas KL, Laroche S, Errington ML, Bliss TV, Hunt SP. Spatial and temporal changes in signal transduction pathways during LTP. Neuron. 1994;13(3):737–745. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Trabzuni D, Ryten M, Walker R, et al. . Quality control parameters on a large dataset of regionally dissected human control brains for whole genome expression studies. J Neurochem. 2011;119:275–282. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Erk S, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Schnell K, et al. . Brain function in carriers of a genome-wide supported bipolar disorder variant. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67:803–811. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Schlagenhauf F, Huys QJ, Deserno L, et al. . Striatal dysfunction during reversal learning in unmedicated schizophrenia patients. Neuroimage. 2014;89:171–180. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. Temme SJ, Bell RZ, Fisher GL, Murphy GG. Deletion of the mouse homolog of CACNA1C disrupts discrete forms of hippocampal-dependent memory and neurogenesis within the dentate gyrus. eNeuro. 2016;3:1–14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Koppe G, Mallien AS, Berger S, et al. . CACNA1C gene regulates behavioral strategies in operant rule learning. PLoS Biol. 2017;15:e2000936. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56. Kisko TM, Braun MD, Michels S, et al. . Cacna1c haploinsufficiency leads to pro-social 50-kHz ultrasonic communication deficits in rats. Dis Model Mech. 2018;11(6):dmm034116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57. Zheng F, Zhou X, Luo Y, Xiao H, Wayman G, Wang H. Regulation of brain-derived neurotrophic factor exon IV transcription through calcium responsive elements in cortical neurons. PLoS One. 2011;6:e28441. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Lee AS, De Jesus-Cortes H, Kabir ZD, et al. . The neuropsychiatric disease-associated gene cacna1c mediates survival of young hippocampal neurons. eNeuro. 2016;3(2):0006-16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.