Skip to main content
BMJ Open logoLink to BMJ Open
. 2019 Sep 8;9(9):e028982. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2019-028982

Impact of gestational age on child intelligence, attention and executive function at age 5: a cohort study

Emilie Pi Fogtmann Sejer 1,, Frederik Jager Bruun 1, Julie Anna Slavensky 1, Erik Lykke Mortensen 2, Ulrik Schiøler Kesmodel 1,3
PMCID: PMC6738700  PMID: 31501108

Abstract

Objectives

Preterm birth can affect cognition, but other factors including parental education and intelligence may also play a role, but few studies have adjusted for these potential confounders. We aimed to assess the impact of gestational age (GA), late preterm birth (34 to <37 weeks GA) and very to moderately preterm birth (<34 weeks GA) on intelligence, attention and executive function in a population of Danish children aged 5 years.

Design

Population-based prospective cohort study.

Setting

Denmark 2003–2008.

Participants

A cohort of 1776 children and their mothers sampled from the Danish National Birth Cohort with information on GA, family and background factors and completed neuropsychological assessment at age 5.

Primary outcome measures

Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence-Revised, Test of Everyday Attention for Children at Five and Behaviour Rating Inventory of Executive Function scores.

Results

For preterm birth <34 weeks GA (n=8), the mean difference in full-scale intelligence quotient(IQ) was −10.6 points (95% CI −19.4 to −1.8) when compared with the term group ≥37 weeks GA (n=1728), and adjusted for potential confounders. For the teacher-assessed Global Executive Composite, the mean difference was 5.3 points (95% CI 2.4 to 8.3) in the adjusted analysis, indicating more executive function difficulties in the preterm group <34 weeks GA compared with the term group. Maternal intelligence and parental education were weak confounders. No associations between late preterm birth 34 to <37 weeks GA (n=40) and poor cognition were shown.

Conclusions

This study showed substantially lower intelligence and poorer executive function in children born <34 weeks GA compared with children born at term. GA may play an important role in determining cognitive abilities independent of maternal intelligence and parental education. Studies with larger sample sizes are needed to confirm these findings, as the proportion of children born preterm in this study population was small.

Keywords: attention, child development, executive function, gestational age, intelligence, preterm birth


Strengths and limitations of this study.

  • In this study population, thorough information on family and background factors that may influence the cognitive outcome of a child was obtained.

  • Directed acyclic graphs were composed to identify potential confounders prior to data analysis, and it was possible to adjust for an extensive set of confounders.

  • The study population was sampled based on average alcohol consumption and binge drinking during pregnancy and may not be representative for the entire population, however, sample weights were applied in analyses to accommodate this.

  • Robust standard errors (SEs) were used to account for the sample design, possible deviations from normality and variance homogeneity.

  • The proportion of children born preterm in this study population was small (48 out of 1776), which limited our power to detect any true differences.

Introduction

In the past decades, there has been an increase in the number of children being born preterm.1 Advances in treatment have led to lower mortality rates, but morbidity rates have not been reduced to the same degree.2 Many organs are vulnerable to preterm birth, and the preterm brain in particular can suffer long-term neurological impairments.3 A dose-response relationship has been proposed, suggesting that the lower the gestational age (GA), the higher the risk of cognitive impairment.4

A study showed that at age 5, 10% of children born preterm still received care in centres specialised for children with disabilities compared with 2% of children born at term (OR 7.9, 95% CI 3.5 to 18.0).5 Hence, it is important to determine the association between preterm birth and cognitive outcomes in order to advise women at risk of preterm delivery and to give informed predictions about the future. Also, the knowledge can be of value to the obstetrician and paediatrician when making decisions about time and mode of delivery and on whether or not resuscitation should be offered at a GA as low as 22–24 weeks.

Previous studies have shown associations between preterm birth and low intelligence, attention deficits and impaired executive function.4 6 These negative outcomes may in part be a consequence of low GA, but other biological and social factors including parental education and intelligence may also affect the cognitive outcome of a child. In our dataset, parental education and maternal intelligence quotient (IQ) have proven to be strong predictors of child IQ,7 and a recent study has shown that maternal IQ predicts IQ in very preterm children at age 5.8 Thus, it is important to adjust for these potential confounders when investigating an association between preterm birth and cognitive outcomes. Previous studies have adjusted for parental education,9 10 but to our knowledge, only one study11 has adjusted for maternal intelligence. In that study, children born before 34 weeks GA were excluded, and the sample size was small (n=336).

The aim of our study was to investigate the influence of GA, late preterm birth (34 to <37 weeks GA) and very to moderately preterm birth (<34 weeks GA) on intelligence, attention and executive function in a population of Danish children aged 5 years adjusted for relevant confounders including parental educational level and maternal intelligence.

Materials and methods

Study sample

We used data from the Lifestyle During Pregnancy Study (LDPS),12 which is a sample from the Danish National Birth Cohort (DNBC). The DNBC contains information on 101 042 Danish women and their children recruited from 1997 to 2003. Of the invited women, 60% chose to participate and 30% of all pregnant women at that time were included.

A total of 3478 women with singleton pregnancies were sampled from the DNBC and invited to participate in the LDPS from 2003 to 2008. Participants were sampled in strata defined by the prenatal maternal average alcohol intake with oversampling of women reporting a relatively high alcohol intake or binge drinking episodes during pregnancy.12 13 Out of the sampled mother and child pairs, 1776 children had neuropsychological tests performed at age 5 and had information on GA available, and thus were included in our analyses. There were no considerable differences between the participants and non-participants with regard to maternal age, body mass index, parity, marital status, prenatal smoking and alcohol consumption, child sex, birth weight and GA at birth.13 Exclusion criteria were multiple pregnancies and congenital diseases with a large risk of mental retardation (the diagnostic term used at the time of data collection), as they represent a fundamentally different group of individuals that may not be representative of the norm. Other exclusion criteria were inability to speak Danish, and impaired vision or hearing abilities preventing the child from completing the tests.12

Data collection

Exposure variables

Information on GA was obtained from the Danish Medical Birth Register and determined by ultrasound, while date of last menses was only used to determine GA in very few cases where an ultrasound estimate was not available. We used GA as 1) a continuous variable (days) and 2) a categorical variable, comparing late preterm birth (34 to <37 completed weeks of gestation) and very to moderately preterm birth (GA <34 weeks) with birth at term (GA ≥37 weeks), respectively.

Outcome measures

At child age 5 (age span: 60–64 months chronological age), a neuropsychological test battery was administered by specially trained psychologists.

Intelligence

The child’s IQ was assessed using the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scales of Intelligence-Revised (WPPSI-R).14 WPPSI-R includes five verbal and five performance subtests that are used to calculate an overall verbal IQ (VIQ), overall performance IQ (PIQ) and full-scale IQ (FIQ). In this test battery, only three of the verbal (arithmetic, information and vocabulary) and three of the performance (block design, geometric design and object assembly) subtests were carried out to ensure the child’s cooperation throughout the testing. Standard procedures were used to prorate scores from the shortened test. Swedish norms were applied to derive the IQ scores, since no Danish norms exist. This should not affect any comparisons made internally within the sample with respect to GA differences.

Attention

Attention measures were assessed with the Test of Everyday Attention for Children at Five (TEACh-5).15 For this study, two subtests assessing selective attention (‘Great Balloon Hunt’ and ‘Hide and Seek II’) and two subtests assessing sustained attention (‘Barking’ and ‘Draw a line’) were used. Each subtest score was standardised to a mean of 0 and a SD of 1. To calculate composite scores for overall, selective and sustained attention, the means of the respective standardised subtest scores for each individual were calculated and re-standardised to a mean of 0 and SD of 1.

Executive function

Executive function was assessed using the Behaviour Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) questionnaire.16 The questionnaire consists of two versions, one for parents and one for teachers. Each questionnaire evaluates eight domains of executive functioning and form the Global Executive Composite (GEC). Three of the eight domains form the Behavioural Regulation Index (BRI), and five of the domains form the Metacognition Index (MI). Since the eight domains do not follow a normal distribution, we performed a normalising t-score transformation to standardise each domain to a mean of 50 and SD of 10. To compute the GEC, BRI and MI, the means of the respective domains for each individual were calculated and re-standardised to a mean of 50 and SD of 10. For all BRIEF scores, a higher score indicates more executive function difficulties.

Covariates

To identify relevant covariates, we constructed directed acyclic graphs (DAGs)17 using the graphical tool DAGitty.18

Important covariates were obtained from prenatal and postnatal telephone interviews, a parent-administered questionnaire at follow-up, the Danish social security number and the Danish Medical Birth Register. In addition, the mother’s intelligence was assessed at follow-up with Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices19 and two subtests (vocabulary and information) of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale.20 The three test results were weighted equally and combined to derive an IQ score.

Prior to analysis, we evaluated the five lowest and five highest observations for all outcomes and covariates to detect unrealistic values (±4 SD for the normally distributed data). This resulted in removal of three birth weight observations (one from the term group and two from the late preterm group) that exceeded our threshold when evaluated according to Danish standards.21 Moreover, we removed one unrealistic body mass index of 13.9 kg/m2 and one observation of average alcohol intake of 36 drinks/week during pregnancy (from the term group).

Statistical analyses

We performed multivariable linear regression using SAS V.9.4 (SAS Institute, Cary, North Carolina, USA).

We assessed term versus late preterm birth and term versus very or moderately preterm birth. We adjusted for a set of a priori defined variables. This included maternal age at birth (continuous), maternal IQ (continuous), average alcohol consumption in pregnancy (0, 1–4, 5+ drinks per week), smoking in pregnancy (yes/no), parity (0, 1, 2+), maternal marital status (single/cohabitating), parental educational level (total duration in years averaged for both parents, if information on the father was missing, maternal only (continuous)) and child sex (male/female). Moreover, we adjusted for the psychologist administering the tests (eight categories) and age at testing (continuous). We created dummy variables from the categorical variables before inserting them in the regression models.

In the study sample, maternal IQ and parental educational level are important predictors of child intelligence,7 and in order to evaluate the importance of adjusting for these factors, sensitivity analyses were conducted removing these two factors separately and simultaneously from the regression models. Moreover, to investigate how much of the effect that could be attributed to birth weight, we inserted this variable in the regression models.

Since the women in our population were sampled based on alcohol intake during pregnancy,22 we used sample weights in our analyses to account for the oversampling of women with relatively high alcohol intake or binge drinking episodes.12 13 To account for the complex stratified sampling design and possible deviations from normality and variance homogeneity, we applied robust SEs.23 All statistical tests were two-sided and with a significance level at 0.05.

We performed complete-case analyses, as multiple imputation strategies to handle missing data in this cohort have produced essentially the same results when compared with complete-case analyses.22

We investigated the possibility for collinearity between covariates and found no evidence of this, as the variance inflation factor never exceeded a value of 2 for any of the covariates in the regression models.

Complete information on child IQ scores was available for 99.3% of the sample, for attention scores 84.7%, and for executive function, 99.8% of the parents and 86.6% of the teachers had completed the questionnaire. All covariates were available for 98.6% of the sample. No statistically significant differences between the term and the two preterm groups were evident with regard to the proportion of missing outcome and covariate data.

Patient and public involvement

For this study, there was no direct patient or public involvement. However, all study results within the DNBC population are available to the study participants, and a participants’ panel is ensuring that as many participants as possible wish to continue being part of the cohort.

Results

The characteristics of the 1776 mother and child pairs are presented in table 1. There were no statistically significant group differences with respect to health, lifestyle and socioeconomic characteristics. Although not statistically significant, the mothers of the children born very or moderately preterm were more likely to be younger, first-time mothers, without a partner, having smoked during pregnancy and had slightly higher IQ and longer education. The mothers of the late preterm children were less likely to have consumed alcohol in pregnancy, but more likely to have male births and lower IQ when compared with the other groups.

Table 1.

Family characteristics among singletons born at term or preterm (Denmark 2003–2008 (n=1776))

Characteristics Born at term
(≥37 weeks)
Late preterm birth
(34 to <37 weeks)
Moderately or very preterm birth
(<34 weeks)*
Number of infants (n) 1728 40 8
Maternal age (years, mean (SD)) 30.8 (4.4) 30.4 (4.5) 28.8 (3.4)
Maternal prepregnancy BMI (kg/m2, median (10/90 percentile))† 22.6 (19.6/28.7) 22.7 (18.4/33.0) 22.8 (16.5/28.2)
Maternal smoking in pregnancy (%) 31.7 30.0 37.5
Maternal alcohol consumption in pregnancy (%)‡
 0 drinks/week 47.6 55.0 50.0
 1–4 drinks/week 41.3 37.5 37.5
 5+ drinks/week§ 11.1 7.5 12.5
Maternal marital status (%)¶
 Single** 12.4 10.3 25.0
 Cohabitating 87.6 89.7 75.0
Maternal IQ (mean (SD))†† 100.0 (14.9) 97.7 (16.9) 104.3 (17.9)
Parental educational level (years, mean (SD))‡‡ 13.2 (1.9) 13.0 (1.6) 14.2 (1.8)
Parity (%)
 0 50.7 60.0 87.5
 1 32.1 30.0 12.5
 2+ 17.2 10.0 0.0
Child sex (%)
 Males 51.7 60.0 50.0
 Females 48.3 40.0 50.0
Gestational age (days, median (10/90 percentile)) 282.0 (269.0/293.0) 251.5 (241.0/257.5) 227.5 (206.0/236.0)
Birth weight (g, mean (SD))§§ 3627.3 (483.4) 2740.8 (482.6) 2040.9 (458.4)
Child age at testing (years, median (10/90 percentile)) 5.23 (5.12/5.30) 5.26 (5.13/5.31) 5.23 (5.10/5.29)

*Lowest observation 29 weeks.

†Information missing for 35 term and 1 late preterm birth.

‡Information missing for one term birth.

§Range 5–14 drinks/week.

¶Information missing for 13 term and 1 late preterm birth.

**If reported being single either during pregnancy or at follow-up at 60–64 months post partum.

††Information missing for nine term births.

‡‡Information missing for five term and one late preterm birth.

§§Information missing for 12 term and 2 late preterm births.

BMI, body mass index; IQ, intelligence quotient; N, number; SD, standard deviation.

With children born at term as the reference, the mean difference in FIQ, VIQ and PIQ for the very or moderately preterm group was −10.6 points (95% CI −19.4 to −1.8), −7.4 points (95% CI −13.4 to −1.5) and −11.7 points (95% CI −21.9 to −1.5), respectively, when adjusting for potential confounders. Among the late preterm children, a tendency towards lower IQs was evident in the unadjusted analyses, but we found no statistically significant differences after adjusting for potential confounders.

For the attention measures, the mean differences were small, and we did not find evidence of statistically significant associations.

With regard to executive function, no statistically significant findings were evident in the parents’ assessment. However, analyses of the teachers’ assessment showed a mean difference in GEC, BRI and MI in the very or moderately preterm group of 5.3 points (95% CI 2.4 to 8.3), 4.2 points (95% CI −0.6 to 9.0) and 5.5 points (95% CI 2.0 to 9.0), respectively, when compared with the term group and adjusting for potential confounders. For the late preterm group, the results were similar but did not reach statistical significance (table 2).

Table 2.

Mean differences in intelligence, attention and executive function between children aged 5 years born at term (reference group) and children born preterm (Denmark 2003–2008 (n=1776))*

Born at term ≥37 weeks (n=1728) Late preterm birth 34 to <37 weeks (n=40) Moderately or very preterm birth <34 weeks (n=8)
Mean (SD) Mean difference 95% CI Mean difference 95% CI
Intelligence (WPPSI-R)
Full-scale IQ
 Unadjusted 105.64 (12.86) −2.09 −6.91 to 2.74 −9.22 −20.25 to 1.81
 Adjusted† −0.05 −4.62 to 4.53 −10.56 -19.37, to 1.75
Verbal IQ
 Unadjusted 104.81 (10.80) −1.73 −5.23 to 1.76 −7.11 −15.64 to 1.41
 Adjusted −0.40 −4.84 to 4.05 −7.41 −13.37, to 1.45
Performance IQ
 Unadjusted 105.14 (16.22) −2.00 −9.02 to 5.03 −9.51 −20.46 to 1.45
 Adjusted 0.38 −5.39 to 6.15 −11.71 −21.89 to 1.52
Attention (TEACh-5)
Overall attention
 Unadjusted 0.01 (1.00) −0.21 −0.70 to 0.28 −0.10 −0.72 to 0.52
 Adjusted −0.16 −0.59 to 0.26 −0.25 −1.00 to 0.50
Sustained attention
 Unadjusted 0.01 (1.00) −0.39 −0.76, to 0.01 −0.09 −0.62 to 0.44
 Adjusted −0.23 −0.64 to 0.19 −0.16 −0.83 to 0.52
Selective attention
 Unadjusted 0.00 (1.00) 0.09 −0.46 to 0.63 0.02 −0.41 to 0.45
 Adjusted 0.06 −0.42 to 0.53 −0.19 −0.65 to 0.27
Executive function (BRIEF)‡
Parent version
Global Executive Composite
 Unadjusted 49.97 (9.98) 1.44 −4.03 to 6.90 −0.39 −16.52 to 15.74
 Adjusted 2.26 −2.01 to 6.53 −0.20 −14.27 to 13.87
Behavioural Regulation Index
 Unadjusted 50.01 (9.98) −0.35 −5.50 to 4.79 −1.18 −16.17 to 13.81
 Adjusted 0.40 −3.97 to 4.76 −1.95 −14.87 to 10.97
Metacognition Index
 Unadjusted 49.95 (9.98) 2.41 −3.10 to 7.92 0.13 −15.24 to 15.51
 Adjusted 3.19 −1.11 to 7.49 0.90 −12.60 to 14.41
Teacher version
Global Executive Composite
 Unadjusted 49.94 (10.03) 4.47 −0.77 to 9.70 5.47 2.57 to 8.36
 Adjusted 3.99 −0.82 to 8.81 5.33 2.39 to 8.27
Behavioural Regulation Index
 Unadjusted 49.95 (10.01) 4.42 −0.38 to 9.21 5.29 2.08 to 8.50
 Adjusted 3.79 −0.89 to 8.48 4.24 −0.56 to 9.03
Metacognition Index
 Unadjusted 49.95 (10.03) 4.09 −1.55 to 9.73 5.07 0.61 to 9.54
 Adjusted 3.77 −1.32 to 8.85 5.46 1.97 to 8.95

*Overall number of participants. Due to complete-case analyses, for the adjusted analyses, the actual number of participants for each outcome was: full-scale IQ (n=1748 (missing data for one late preterm birth)), verbal IQ (n=1749 (missing data for one late preterm birth)), performance IQ (n=1749 (missing data for one late preterm birth)), overall attention (n=1493 (missing data for seven late preterm births)), sustained attention (n=1586 (missing data for four late preterm births)), selective attention (n=1612 (missing data for four late preterm births)), Global Executive Composite (parents, n=1748 (missing data for one late preterm birth); teachers, n=1525 (missing data for seven late preterm births and one very to moderate preterm birth)), Behavioural Regulation Index (parents, n=1748 (missing data for one late preterm birth); teachers, n=1530 (missing data for seven late preterm births and one very to moderate preterm birth)), Metacognition Index (parents, n=1748 (missing data for one late preterm birth); teachers, n=1525 (missing data for seven late preterm births and one very to moderate preterm birth)).

†All adjusted analyses adjusted for maternal age, maternal IQ, average alcohol consumption in pregnancy, smoking in pregnancy, parity, maternal marital status, parental educational level, child sex, testing psychologist and age at testing.

‡A higher BRIEF score indicates more executive function difficulties (opposite than the other outcome measures).

BRIEF, Behaviour Rating Inventory of Executive Function; CI, confidence interval; IQ, intelligence quotient; N, number; SD, standard deviation; TEACh-5, Test of Everyday Attention for Children at Five; WPPSI-R, Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scales of Intelligence-Revised.

In analyses with GA as a continuous variable (table 3), we found a statistically significant increase in FIQ of 0.08 points (95% CI 0.01 to 0.15) per increase in GA (in days) in the adjusted analysis. Similar estimates were seen in the analyses of VIQ and PIQ, however, we found no statistically significant associations in the adjusted analyses. For teacher-assessed executive function, we found a statistically significant decrease in GEC and MI of −0.07 points (95% CI −0.14 to −0.01) per increase in GA (in days) indicating better executive function with increasing GA, however these estimates also became statistically non-significant when adjusting for potential confounders.

Table 3.

Regression coefficients for the association between gestational age (in days) and intelligence, attention and executive function in children aged 5 years (Denmark 2003–2008 (n=1776))*

Intelligence (WPPSI-R) Beta (95% CI) Attention (TEACh-5) Beta (95% CI) Executive function (BRIEF)†
- Parent version
Beta (95% CI) Executive function (BRIEF)
- Teacher version
Beta (95% CI)
Full-scale IQ Overall attention Global Executive Composite Global Executive Composite
Unadjusted 0.10 (0.02 to 0.19) Unadjusted 0.00 (0.00 to 0.01) Unadjusted −0.03 (-0.10 to 0.05) Unadjusted −0.07 (-0.14 to −0.01)
Adjusted‡ 0.08 (0.01 to 0.15) Adjusted 0.00 (0.00 to 0.01) Adjusted −0.02 (-0.08 to 0.05) Adjusted −0.06 (−0.12 to 0.01)
Verbal IQ Sustained attention Behavioural Regulation Index Behavioural Regulation Index
Unadjusted 0.07 (0.00 to 0.14) Unadjusted 0.00 (−0.01 to 0.01) Unadjusted −0.02 (−0.09 to 0.05) Unadjusted −0.06 (−0.13 to 0.01)
Adjusted 0.06 (0.00 to 0.13) Adjusted 0.00 (−0.01 to 0.01) Adjusted −0.01 (−0.08 to 0.05) Adjusted −0.04 (−0.11 to 0.02)
Performance IQ Selective attention Metacognition Index Metacognition Index
Unadjusted 0.12 (0.01 to 0.22) Unadjusted 0.00 (0.00 to 0.01) Unadjusted −0.03 (−0.10 to 0.05) Unadjusted −0.07 (−0.14 to −0.01)
Adjusted 0.08 (−0.01 to 0.18) Adjusted 0.00 (0.00 to 0.01) Adjusted −0.02 (−0.08 to 0.04) Adjusted −0.06 (−0.12 to 0.00)

*Overall number of participants. Due to complete-case analyses, for the adjusted analyses, the actual number of participants for each outcome was: full-scale IQ (n=1748 (missing data for one late preterm birth)), verbal IQ (n=1749 (missing data for one late preterm birth)), performance IQ (n=1749 (missing data for one late preterm birth)), overall attention (n=1493 (missing data for seven late preterm births)), sustained attention (n=1586 (missing data for four late preterm births)), selective attention (n=1612 (missing data for four late preterm births)), Global Executive Composite (parents, n=1748 (missing data for one late preterm birth); teachers, n=1525 (missing data for seven late preterm births and one very to moderate preterm birth)), Behavioural Regulation Index (parents, n=1748 (missing data for one late preterm birth); teachers, n=1530 (missing data for seven late preterm births and one very to moderate preterm birth)), Metacognition Index (parents, n=1748 (missing data for one late preterm birth); teachers, n=1525 (missing data for seven late preterm births and one very to moderate preterm birth)).

†A higher BRIEF score indicates more executive function difficulties (opposite than the other outcome measures).

‡All adjusted analyses adjusted for maternal age, maternal IQ, average alcohol consumption in pregnancy, smoking in pregnancy, parity, maternal marital status, parental educational level, child sex, testing psychologist and age at testing.

BRIEF, Behaviour Rating Inventory of Executive Function; CI, confidence interval; IQ, intelligence quotient; N, number; SD, standard deviation; TEACh-5, Test of Everyday Attention for Children at Five; WPPSI-R, Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scales of Intelligence-Revised.

When maternal IQ and parental education were removed from the regression analyses separately or simultaneously (see online supplementary table 1), the estimates of association did not change notably. However, when these variables were removed simultaneously from the regression, most estimates became statistically non-significant due to wider CIs.

Supplementary data

bmjopen-2019-028982supp001.pdf (117.6KB, pdf)

When introducing birth weight in the regression analyses (see online supplementary table 1), the association between GA and all IQ outcomes became considerably weaker and were no longer statistically significant. However, a trend towards lower IQ in the very or moderately preterm group was still evident, as the mean differences in FIQ, VIQ and PIQ were reduced to −7.0 points (95% CI −15.7 to 1.6), −5.9 points (95% CI −12.2 to 0.3) and −6.8 points (95% CI −16.6 to 3.0), respectively, when compared with the term group. When birth weight was introduced in the analyses of attention and executive function outcomes, the results did not change substantially.

In a post hoc analysis, we excluded the early term births (GA 37–38) and made a direct comparison between the very or moderately preterm group and the term group with GA ≥39 weeks (n=1443), and the late preterm group and the term group (GA ≥39 weeks), respectively (see online supplementary table 2). In these analyses, the results did not change notably for any of the outcomes.

Supplementary data

bmjopen-2019-028982supp002.pdf (77.4KB, pdf)

Discussion

Main findings

We found a statistically significant effect of very or moderately preterm birth on IQ and teacher-assessed executive function when adjusting for potential confounders. Although maternal IQ and parental education accounted for much of the variance in child IQ in this dataset,7 these two factors should only be considered weak confounders with no significant association with GA, as removing these variables from our analyses did not alter the associations notably. However, removal of the variables produced wider CIs confirming that they explain substantial parts of the variance.

The inclusion of birth weight in the regression analyses for IQ outcomes attenuated the associations for the very or moderately preterm group, and the results were no longer statistically significant. For the late preterm group, the associations completely vanished. This could be suggestive of mediation and underlines the importance of looking at GA relatively to birth weight when investigating effects of preterm birth, although our results for the very to moderately preterm children indicate that there may be cognitive effects of GA which are independent of birth weight, perhaps reflecting effects of very low GA on brain development.

Strengths and limitations

One of the strengths of this study is the relatively large sample size with thorough information on family and background factors that may influence the cognitive outcome of a child. Specially trained psychologists, unaware of the GA, conducted neuropsychological tests with a high inter-rater reliability of 97%–97.5%.12 To minimise bias in our analyses, we composed DAGs to identify potential confounders prior to data analysis. Due to our large sample size, we were able to adjust for an exhaustive set of confounders. Other strengths of our study were a predefined protocoled methodology, and use of robust SEs to account for the sample design and shortcomings in the data.

Our study has some limitations. The study population was sampled based on average alcohol consumption and binge drinking during pregnancy,12 13 and therefore, the sample is not representative of the entire DNBC population. We applied sample weights in the analyses to accommodate this. However, the use of weights may be problematic for small subgroups, and together with the use of robust SEs, this approach may have reduced the power to obtain statistically significant results and widened the CIs. Generally, the effect estimates are subject to some uncertainty illustrated by the wide CIs.24

Another weakness of this study is the relatively small proportion of children born preterm, especially children born very preterm (<32 weeks GA). According to Danish Medical Birth Register records from 2000 (our recruitment period was from 1997 to 2003), we would expect 6.3% of all newborns to be born preterm.25 In our population it was only 2.7%, which is equal to an under-representation of 57%. Only 0.2% of our sample was born very preterm, although we would expect 1.0%.25 This can be a result of various factors that prevent parents with children born preterm from participating in a clinical study, in particular if the children are born very preterm and need special care.

However, studies have shown that the influence of selection bias on several exposure-outcome associations in the DNBC is limited.26 We adjusted for a large number of covariates associated with selection, still we cannot rule out that the low prevalence of preterm births in our cohort may have limited our power to detect any true differences as statistically significant. A post hoc power analysis showed that analyses comparing very or moderately preterm birth (n=8) with birth at term (n=1728) had a power of 0.48, 0.28 and 0.59 for FIQ, VIQ and PIQ outcomes, respectively.

The low prevalence of preterm births also prevented us from performing analyses investigating the impact of very or extremely preterm birth.

Despite the limitations, especially the low number of preterm births, we believe that this study contributes with important knowledge that together with existing evidence in the literature may improve the clinicians’ ability to advise women at risk of preterm delivery and give informed predictions about the future.

Interpretation

For the IQ outcomes, the findings in our study are generally in line with previous findings with an IQ reduction of approximately 10 points in children born preterm.4 27 However, in our study, this clinically very relevant difference was only seen among the very or moderately preterm children, and not in the late preterm group. A meta-analysis by Chan et al 28 showed a statistically significant impact of late preterm birth on general cognitive ability and non-verbal intelligence. Our study in part contradicts these findings, as no associations between late preterm birth and IQ (full-scale, verbal and non-verbal) were found. In our unadjusted analyses, we saw a trend towards lower IQ among late preterm children, but the trend disappeared when adjusting for confounders. This discrepancy may reflect insufficient adjustments in other studies and the limited power of our study.

When assessing attention measures, we only found one statistically significant result, which might be because of chance alone. This is not in line with previous findings suggesting that preterm infants are at increased risk of developing, for example, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder with a relative risk of 2.64 (95% CI 1.85 to 3.78).27 However, TEACh-5 has not been validated as a diagnostic test, and given the unambiguous findings in the present study, it is possible that GA does not have an impact on test of basic attention function.

In the field of executive function, it has been suggested that extremely preterm infants (<28 weeks GA) are at increased risk of developing executive function difficulties.29 Studies investigating the association between very, moderately or late preterm birth and poor BRIEF scores have not detected any convincing deficits when evaluating the parents’ questionnaire,30 31 and to our knowledge, the teachers’ questionnaire has not previously been used for this purpose. Hodel et al detected deficits in a population of moderately to late preterm infants at the age of 9 months and at 4 years,32 but in these studies, other executive function measures than BRIEF were applied.

In extremely low birth weight children, teachers have proven to report significantly more difficulties on the BRI subscale compared with the parents.33 In our study, we found that teachers reported more difficulties in all areas (GEC, BRI and MI) when compared with the parents. This can be due to teachers having a more objective viewpoint and being more experienced in working with children with and without difficulties.

Conclusion

This study showed significantly lower IQ and poorer executive function in children born very or moderately preterm (<34 weeks GA) compared with children born at term (≥37 weeks GA), but only the differences in IQ were considered clinically relevant. No associations between late preterm birth (34 to <37 weeks GA) and poor cognitive outcomes were shown.

Maternal IQ and parental education are strong predictors of child IQ in our dataset but were only weak confounders of the association between GA and cognitive outcomes. Therefore, GA may play an important role in determining cognitive abilities independent of maternal IQ and parental educational level. Further studies with larger sample sizes to confirm these findings are needed.

Supplementary Material

Reviewer comments
Author's manuscript

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the participants for their participation in the study.

Footnotes

Contributors: EPFS contributed to the conception and design of the study, performed data management and statistical analyses, analysed and interpreted the data, drafted the initial manuscript and wrote the final manuscript with contributions from the coauthors. JAS and FJB contributed to the conception and design of the study, assisted with statistical analyses and interpretation of the data and critically reviewed and revised the manuscript. ELM and USK conceptualised and designed the study, analysed and interpreted the data and critically reviewed and revised the manuscript. All authors approved the final manuscript as submitted and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

Funding: This work was supported by a scholarship granted by the Novo Nordisk Foundation. The LDPS was supported primarily by Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, Atlanta, Georgia, USA, and also from the Danish National Board of Health, the Lundbeck Foundation, Ludvig & Sara Elsass’ Foundation, the Augustinus Foundation and Aase & Ejnar Danielsen’s Foundation. The Danish National Research Foundation established the Danish Epidemiology Science Center that initiated and created the DNBC. Besides a major grant from this Foundation, additional support for the DNBC was obtained from the Pharmacy Foundation, the Egmont Foundation, the March of Dimes Birth Defects Foundation, the Augustinus Foundation and the Health Foundation.

Competing interests: None declared.

Patient consent for publication: Not required.

Ethics approval: The data collection for the LDPS was approved by the DNBC Board of Directors, the DNBC Steering committee, the regional Ethics Committee, the Danish Data Protection Agency and the Institutional Review Board at the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Signed informed consent was obtained for the LDPS. The current study was further approved by the Danish Data Protection Agency (file number 2012-58-0004).

Provenance and peer review: Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.

Data availability statement: No data are available.

References

  • 1. Langhoff-Roos J, Kesmodel U, Jacobsson B, et al. . Spontaneous preterm delivery in primiparous women at low risk in Denmark: population based study. BMJ 2006;332:937–9. 10.1136/bmj.38751.524132.2F [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Wilson-Costello D, Friedman H, Minich N, et al. . Improved survival rates with increased neurodevelopmental disability for extremely low birth weight infants in the 1990s. Pediatrics 2005;115:997–1003. 10.1542/peds.2004-0221 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Saigal S, Doyle LW. An overview of mortality and sequelae of preterm birth from infancy to adulthood. Lancet 2008;371:261–9. 10.1016/S0140-6736(08)60136-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Kerr-Wilson CO, Mackay DF, Smith GCS, et al. . Meta-analysis of the association between preterm delivery and intelligence. J Public Health 2012;34:209–16. 10.1093/pubmed/fdr024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Larroque B, Ancel P-Y, Marret S, et al. . Neurodevelopmental disabilities and special care of 5-year-old children born before 33 weeks of gestation (the EPIPAGE study): a longitudinal cohort study. Lancet 2008;371:813–20. 10.1016/S0140-6736(08)60380-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Aarnoudse-Moens CSH, Weisglas-Kuperus N, van Goudoever JB, et al. . Meta-analysis of neurobehavioral outcomes in very preterm and/or very low birth weight children. Pediatrics 2009;124:717–28. 10.1542/peds.2008-2816 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Eriksen H-LF, Kesmodel US, Underbjerg M, et al. . Predictors of intelligence at the age of 5: family, pregnancy and birth characteristics, postnatal influences, and postnatal growth. PLoS One 2013;8:e79200 10.1371/journal.pone.0079200 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Lean RE, Paul RA, Smyser CD, et al. . Maternal intelligence quotient (IQ) predicts IQ and language in very preterm children at age 5 years. J Child Psychol Psychiatr 2018;59:150–9. 10.1111/jcpp.12810 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Kirkegaard I, Obel C, Hedegaard M, et al. . Gestational age and birth weight in relation to school performance of 10-year-old children: a follow-up study of children born after 32 completed weeks. Pediatrics 2006;118:1600–6. 10.1542/peds.2005-2700 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Larroque B, Ancel P-Y, Marchand-Martin L, et al. . Special care and school difficulties in 8-year-old very preterm children: the Epipage cohort study. PLoS One 2011;6:e21361 10.1371/journal.pone.0021361 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Talge NM, Holzman C, Wang J, et al. . Late-preterm birth and its association with cognitive and socioemotional outcomes at 6 years of age. Pediatrics 2010;126:1124–31. 10.1542/peds.2010-1536 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Kesmodel US, Underbjerg M, Kilburn TR, et al. . Lifestyle during pregnancy: neurodevelopmental effects at 5 years of age. The design and implementation of a prospective follow-up study. Scand J Public Health 2010;38:208–19. 10.1177/1403494809357093 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Kesmodel US, Eriksen H-LF, Underbjerg M, et al. . The effect of alcohol binge drinking in early pregnancy on general intelligence in children. BJOG 2012;119:1222–31. 10.1111/j.1471-0528.2012.03395.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Wechsler D. Manual for the Wechsler preschool and primary scale of Intelligence-Revised. Swedish version. Stockholm: Psykologiförlaget AB, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Underbjerg M, Kesmodel US, Landrø NI, et al. . The effects of low to moderate alcohol consumption and binge drinking in early pregnancy on selective and sustained attention in 5-year-old children. BJOG 2012;119:1211–21. 10.1111/j.1471-0528.2012.03396.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Gioia GA, Isquith PK, Guy SC, et al. . Test review behavior rating inventory of executive function. Child Neuropsychology 2000;6:235–8. 10.1076/chin.6.3.235.3152 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Greenland S, Pearl J, Robins JM. Causal diagrams for epidemiologic research. Epidemiology 1999;10:37–48. 10.1097/00001648-199901000-00008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Textor J, Hardt J, Knuppel S. DAGitty: a graphical tool for analyzing causal diagrams. Epidemiology 2011;22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Raven J, Court J, Raven J. Manual for raven's progressive matrices and vocabulary scales. Oxford Psychologists Press: Oxford, 1998. [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Wechsler D. Manual for the Wechsler adult intelligence scale. New York, NY: The Psychological Corporation, 1955. [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Maršál K, Persson P-H, Larsen T, et al. . Intrauterine growth curves based on ultrasonically estimated foetal weights. Acta Paediatr 1996;85:843–8. 10.1111/j.1651-2227.1996.tb14164.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Kesmodel US, Bertrand J, Støvring H, et al. . The effect of different alcohol drinking patterns in early to mid pregnancy on the child’s intelligence, attention, and executive function. BJOG 2012;119:1180–90. 10.1111/j.1471-0528.2012.03393.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Huber PJ. The behavior of maximum likelihood estimates under nonstandard conditions. Proceedings of the Fifth Berkeley Symposium on Mathematical Statistics and Probability 1967;1:221–33. [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Greenland S, Senn SJ, Rothman KJ, et al. . Statistical tests, P values, confidence intervals, and power: a guide to misinterpretations. Eur J Epidemiol 2016;31:337–50. 10.1007/s10654-016-0149-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Sundhedsstyrelsen Fødselsregisteret 1997-2001. Nye tal fra Sundhedsstyrelsen. Årgang 7. Nr. 12, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Nohr EA, Liew Z. How to investigate and adjust for selection bias in cohort studies. Acta Obstet Gynecol Scand 2018;97:407–16. 10.1111/aogs.13319 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Bhutta AT, Cleves MA, Casey PH, et al. . Cognitive and behavioral outcomes of school-aged children who were born preterm. JAMA 2002;288:728–37. 10.1001/jama.288.6.728 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Chan E, Leong P, Malouf R, et al. . Long-Term cognitive and school outcomes of late-preterm and early-term births: a systematic review. Child Care Health Dev 2016;42:297–312. 10.1111/cch.12320 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Joseph RM, OShea TM, Allred EN, et al. . Neurocognitive and academic outcomes at age 10 years of extremely preterm newborns. Pediatrics 2016;137:e20154343 10.1542/peds.2015-4343 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Brumbaugh JE, Hodel AS, Thomas KM. The impact of late preterm birth on executive function at preschool age. Am J Perinatol 2014;31:305–14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Ritter BC, Perrig W, Steinlin M, et al. . Cognitive and behavioral aspects of executive functions in children born very preterm. Child Neuropsychol 2014;20:129–44. 10.1080/09297049.2013.773968 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Hodel AS, Brumbaugh JE, Morris AR, et al. . Hot executive function following moderate-to-late preterm birth: altered delay discounting at 4 years of age. Dev Sci 2016;19:221–34. 10.1111/desc.12307 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. McCann JP, Rider GN, Weiss BA, et al. . Latent mean comparisons on the brief in preterm children: parent and teacher differences. Child Neuropsychol 2014;20:737–51. 10.1080/09297049.2013.859663 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary data

bmjopen-2019-028982supp001.pdf (117.6KB, pdf)

Supplementary data

bmjopen-2019-028982supp002.pdf (77.4KB, pdf)

Reviewer comments
Author's manuscript

Articles from BMJ Open are provided here courtesy of BMJ Publishing Group

RESOURCES