Abstract
The neocortex is thought to be organized into functional columns of neurons, each of which processes an element of a larger representation. In the barrel cortex, the thalamic input to the column preferentially terminates in a barrel. To study the extent and nature of functional connections between columns, we measured the degree to which whisker responses are relayed between columns in the barrel cortex. Inactivating a single barrel by iontophoresis of the GABAA agonist muscimol abolished the representation of that barrel's whisker in neighboring barrels. Reactivating a single barrel by iontophoresis of the GABAA antagonist bicuculline while the rest of the cortex was blocked by muscimol led to single whisker receptive fields. Under the same conditions, septal cells tended to exhibit multiwhisker receptive fields. These studies demonstrate that the surround receptive fields of barrel cells are generated by intracortical transmission and that many septal cells derive a component of their surround receptive field from the thalamus.
Keywords: barrels, whiskers, GABA, rat, sensory processing, muscimol
Introduction
The cortical column is thought to be one of the basic elements of neocortical organization. A column is composed of a vertical array of cortical neurons that primarily processes one component of a larger representation (Mountcastle, 1957; Hubel and Wiesel, 1962; Szentagothai, 1975). The column is therefore primarily defined as a functional entity, but in the barrel cortex, the anatomical correlate of the column can also be seen in layer IV and is known as a barrel (Woolsey and Van der Loos, 1970). Thalamic axons project into layer IV, where they synapse mainly on the dendrites of stellate cells that orient their dendrites into the middle of the barrel (Simons and Woolsey, 1984). Vertically directed axons then project excitation from layer IV cells up into layers II and III of the column (Lubke et al., 2000), where neurons respond to whisker stimulation 2-3 msec after their layer IV counterparts (Armstrong-James et al., 1992). The neurons of individual barrel columns respond primarily to a single whisker on the whisker pad (Simons, 1985). However, neurons in a single barrel column respond to inputs from adjacent whiskers as well, measured in a variety of ways (Armstrong-James and Fox, 1987; Moore and Nelson, 1998; Zhu and Connors, 1999; Petersen and Diamond, 2000). Responses to the adjacent whisker inputs form the surround receptive fields of the cells. A key question for understanding how cortical processing occurs is to ask whether surround receptive fields arise from intracortical processing between neighboring cortical columns or whether, instead, they reflect convergence of information at subcortical sites.
The issue is important for a number of reasons. First, it concerns the way in which basic sensory processing occurs in the somatosensory cortex. Second, surround receptive fields exhibit substantial plasticity in mature somatosensory cortex, and it is necessary to know whether this is attributable to plasticity in cortical or subcortical pathways to discover the underlying plasticity mechanisms (Fox, 1994; Glazewski and Fox, 1996; Buonomano and Merzenich, 1998). Third, clues about cortical processing in a functionally relatively simple columnar structure like the somatosensory cortex could help uncover principles of cortical organization and thereby inform experimentation on cortical areas with more complex columnar processing (Fujita et al., 1992; Tsunoda et al., 2001).
Here we used a novel approach to examine the degree of intracortical processing that occurs in the barrel cortex. In the first series of experiments, we reversibly inactivated a single barrel by iontophoresis of muscimol (a GABAA agonist) to see whether this reversibly inactivated the corresponding whisker representation in surrounding cortical barrels. In the second series of experiments, we initially inactivated a large number of barrels simultaneously with muscimol diffused from the cortical surface to abolish activity in intracortical pathways. We then reactivated neurons in a single barrel with bicuculline (a competitive GABAA antagonist) to see whether surround receptive fields could be detected in the absence of intracortical activity. The results of both the muscimol and bicuculline iontophoresis studies imply that intercolumnar transmission forms a major feature of cortical receptive field processing, including that in layer IV.
Materials and Methods
Subjects. Subjects were mature Long-Evans rats of 6-10 weeks of age. Twenty-three animals were used in the studies in which a single barrel was inactivated by muscimol iontophoresis. In addition, 24 animals were used in the studies in which the cortex was inhibited with muscimol and locally reactivated with bicuculline. In addition, nine animals were studied to characterize the effect of bicuculline on normal (untreated) cortex. Thalamic responses were studied during cortical muscimol application in an additional four animals.
Surgery, anesthesia, electrodes, and recording. The methods used were identical to those described in previous studies (Fox et al., 1996; Wallace et al., 2001), except that sodium pentobarbitone anesthesia (65 mg/kg) was used in some animals for comparison with urethane anesthesia (1.5 gm/kg). Briefly, a small craniotomy was made between 4 and 7 mm lateral to the midline and 1-4 mm caudal to bregma. Surgery relating to muscimol application is described below. Electrodes were single- or triple-barreled carbon fiber microelectrodes. Recordings were bandpassed between 600 Hz and 6 KHz, and spikes were discriminated using a voltage window discriminator.
Levels of anesthesia were monitored by recording focal EEG activity, spontaneous cortical spike activity, breathing rate, and reflexes. On the basis of these measures, we were able to categorize the anesthetic level according to the clinical stages of Guedal (1920) as demonstrated by Friedberg et al. (1999). Under pentobarbitone anesthesia, the anesthetic level was termed Guedal stage III-3 if a hindlimb pinch withdrawal reflex and corneal blink reflex were present and the respiratory rate was 60-86 breaths/min. Anesthesia was termed stage III-4 if both the hindlimb and corneal reflexes were absent and the respiratory rate was between 46 and 68 breaths/min. The anesthetic level was maintained at stage III-3 during administration of pentobarbitone and urethane anesthesia. Under urethane, this also corresponded to a state in which δ waves occurred at between 1 and 2 Hz with occasional spindle waves (Fox and Armstrong-James, 1986). The breathing rate was slightly depressed under pentobarbitone (60-86 breaths/min) compared with urethane anesthesia (100-120 breaths/min) at equivalent levels of anesthesia based on reflexes and focal EEG (stage III-3).
Sensory responses, stimulation, and quantification. The stimulus was a 200 μm vertical deflection of a single vibrissa ∼10 mm from the face, delivered at 1 Hz (i.e., a 1° deflection). The stimulator was a fast piezoelectric bimorph wafer attached to a lightweight glass capillary touching the vibrissa. Spikes were discriminated on the basis of amplitude and time course. All data were analyzed using poststimulus time histograms (PSTHs) and latency histograms. The response magnitude to stimulation of a particular vibrissa was defined as the number of spikes per stimulus occurring between 5 and 50 msec after the stimulus minus the spontaneous activity occurring during an identical period before the stimulus. The modal latency was used to describe the response latency of the neuron. For a complete description, see the article by Armstrong-James and Fox (1987).
Histology. At the end of recording from each penetration, a small focal lesion (1.5 μA, DC, 10 sec, tip-negative) was made at a site of known depth in layer IV. Electrodes were located at depths of between 550 and 750 μm. The cortex was flattened and processed for cytochrome oxidase histology as described before (Wong-Riley, 1979; Fox, 1994), and the location of each recording penetration was identified within the barrel field. In this way, we could identify the principal vibrissa for each recorded cell and, in the case of the dual-electrode recordings, the distance between the two electrodes.
Diffusion of muscimol. A small well was made above the cortex by first protecting the surface of the cortex with a small plug of Gelfoam and then covering the exposed area with agar. Once the agar had set, the Gelfoam plug was removed, leaving a small agar well above the cortex. The dura was retracted over several square millimeters, and muscimol dissolved in PBS was applied to the surface of the brain at known concentrations (100, 200, and 500 μm, pH 7.4). The well of muscimol was replenished several times during the recording session to keep the concentration constant. The advance of muscimol into the cortex was measured by plotting PSTHs for the principal whisker response for cells at known depths. The depth was verified by making two lesions, one at the top and one at the bottom of the penetration at the end of the experiment and reconstructing the penetration from the histology.
It has already been shown that the time at which muscimol extinguished the principal whisker response as a function of depth can be described by diffusion from an interface of constant concentration (Wallace et al., 2001) as follows:
where Cx,t is the concentration at a given depth and time; C0 is the concentration at the surface; erfc is the error function complementary; x is the depth within the cortex; t is time; and D* is the modified diffusion coefficient for muscimol. The diffusion coefficient for muscimol was taken to be ∼8.7 χ 10-6 cm2/sec and was modified by values for tortuosity and volume fraction according to the following equation:
where α is the volume fraction (∼0.21), and λ is tortuosity (∼1.55) (Nicholson and Phillips, 1981). As muscimol reaches an effective concentration at ∼50 μm, we were able to block activity to a depth of ∼1mm after 4 hr with a surface concentration of 200 μm.
Iontophoresis of muscimol and bicuculline. Iontophoretic electrodes were either filled with muscimol or bicuculline metachloride (BMC, 10 mm, pH 5.5; Tocris Cookson, Avonmouth, UK). A retaining current of 5-10 nA was usually applied depending on the electrode. The effective retain current was determined at the start of the experiment at a superficial location before moving the electrode to layer IV. Ejecting currents of 10 nA were typically used in these studies for muscimol, although we often started with a higher current to offset the effect of previous retain charge, particularly if a retain current had been applied for a long period beforehand.
The period of current ejection was timed accurately. This precaution enabled us to keep the sphere of disinhibition localized to the tip of the electrode and to prevent epileptiform discharges. During ejection of muscimol, we generally maintained the ejecting current close to 10 nA and monitored the evoked field potential for the principal whisker of the barrel concerned to judge when the barrel had been silenced.
The behavior of the muscimol diffusion with time was modeled by the diffusion equations solved for diffusion in an infinite medium from a point source (Carslaw and Jaeger, 1956) as follows:
where r is the radial distance from the point source; Q is the flux of drug; and the other parameters have the same meanings as denoted above. The flux can be derived from the ejecting current as:
where i is the ejecting current; F is the Faraday number; and n is the transport number, which we have measured previously to average 0.242 with these electrodes (Armstrong-James et al., 1982).
The time at which principal whisker responses in neighboring barrels were first affected by muscimol diffusion gave a measure of how quickly muscimol diffused between the iontophoretic and recording electrodes. Using the same values as above for the modified diffusion coefficient for muscimol, it was found that the time at which cells were affected was predicted by the theoretical time course for diffusion. The concentration at which muscimol was effective was again found to be ∼50 μm (see Fig. 2 A). Because the behavior of muscimol followed simple diffusion laws, we were able to predict that 10 nA would produce inhibition over no more than 300 μm when applied for 33 min (see Fig. 1 B). Given that a barrel in the posterior medial subfield is at least 350 μm in diameter in the rat, this allowed us to inhibit one barrel without affecting its neighbor when the electrodes were appropriately positioned. The best configuration occurred when the iontophoretic electrode was located on the far side of one barrel and the recording electrode was in the middle of the adjacent barrel. For 10 nA ejecting current, the concentration reaches an asymptote of ∼50 μm at 400 μm (see Fig. 1 B) as time tends to infinity. This makes it practically impossible for the neighboring barrel to be inhibited directly from the muscimol, whereas points within a 300 μm radius are affected by the time ∼500 sec have elapsed (i.e., 8.33 min).
Definition of nomenclature for whiskers. The “principal whisker” is the topographically related whisker for the barrel in question; i.e., the D1 whisker is the principal whisker for cells in the D1 barrel. For septal cells, the closest barrel defined the principal whisker. The “surround whisker” refers to any whisker that forms part of the receptive field other than the principal whisker. The “secondary whisker” is one of the surround whiskers and is defined, for the first experiment, as the whisker related to the inactivated barrel; i.e., it is the D2 whisker if the muscimol iontophoresis electrode is located in the D2 barrel (see Fig. 1 A). Typically in this study, we might record the D2 whisker responses in the D1 barrel while inactivating the D2 barrel with muscimol.
For the second experiment, in which the cortex was inactivated by muscimol and locally reactivated with BMC, secondary, tertiary, and so forth (2′, 3′, and so forth) mean the whiskers giving the second, third, and so forth largest responses.
Analysis. Sensory responses were quantified by calculating the average response level across cells subject to the same treatment. To judge the effect of muscimol iontophoresis in the neighboring barrel on the surround receptive field, we compared a period of control responses (usually 20-30 min) with the response after the neighboring barrel was inactivated. The principal and secondary whisker responses from this period were averaged across cells and compared with average control values for the same cells.
To quantify the size and focus of receptive fields, surround receptive field responses were categorized according to how large a response they generated in each cell. Average response levels were then calculated for the principal whisker and corresponding secondary, tertiary, and so forth surround whiskers. To judge the effect of the various treatments on the composition of the surround receptive fields, average surround receptive field responses were normalized to the average principal whisker response for particular cases and compared between control and muscimol-treated cases using a t test.
Results
Cortical receptive fields in the absence of activity in a single barrel
To inactivate a single barrel, muscimol was iontophoresed in layer IV. A second electrode was inserted into an adjacent barrel to record the receptive field properties in layer IV during inactivation of the neighboring barrel (Fig. 1A). We had previously found that cells lose their sensory responses at a muscimol concentration of ∼50 μm in barrel cortex (Wallace et al., 2001). Iontophoresis using 10 nA ejecting current would be expected to raise the muscimol concentration to 50 μm at 300 μm from the tip of the electrode within ∼7 min (420 sec), whereas at 400 μm, it would take at least 33 min (2000 sec) to raise the concentration to the same level (Fig. 1B). Therefore, we attempted to place the electrodes at >300 μm separation in the horizontal plane to give sufficient time between inactivating one barrel and its neighbor to accurately record the effect on the receptive field.
In practice, the electrodes were found to have been located at a variety of separations when the histology was examined (see Materials and Methods). The point at which cells located at the recording electrode were affected directly by iontophoresis of muscimol was estimated by measuring the time taken to cause a significant decrease in the principal whisker response (for nomenclature, see Materials and Methods). The farther the cell was located from the iontophoretic source, the longer it took for the principal whisker response to be affected. The experimental time points for first affecting the principal whisker response all lay close to the theoretical line for the time at which muscimol reached 50 μm by diffusion from the iontophoresis electrode (Fig. 2A, dashed line). Furthermore, the best straight line fit through the data points (solid line; R2 = 0.78) almost superimposes on the theoretical line representing the 50 μm isoconcentration contour. This both confirms that the muscimol behaved approximately as predicted by diffusion from a point source (see Materials and Methods) and that the effect on the principal whisker response could be explained by diffusion of the muscimol to the site of the recorded cell.
In contrast, responses were lost far more rapidly for the whisker related to the inactivated barrel (which we define here as the secondary whisker). The time at which the first sign of a reduction in the secondary whisker response occurred (Fig. 2A, black squares) was far earlier than that for the principal whisker response and too early to be explained by direct diffusion of muscimol except in cases in which the iontophoretic electrode was located very close to the recorded cell. Consequently, the difference in time between affecting principal and secondary whisker responses was similar for cells located close to the iontophoretic electrode and very different for cells located farther away (Fig. 2B). Furthermore, the best straight line fit fitted the data poorly (R2 = 0.45; Fig. 2A; note that the data points are shown but not the linear fit). This indicates that affecting the secondary whisker response depended on inhibiting something closer to the iontophoretic electrode than the recorded cell when the iontophoretic electrode was distant from the recorded cell.
Useful distances between the recorded cell and the iontophoretic electrode occurred for separations of at least 300 μm. It took at least 10 min to first affect the principal whisker response after abolishing the secondary whisker response for seven of eight cells where the electrodes were located >300 μm apart (Fig. 2B). Note that negative times indicate that the secondary whisker response was lost before the principal whisker response was affected at all. Figure 3 shows two examples of recordings in which the response to the secondary whisker was abolished for a long period with a minimal effect on the principal whisker response. In all cases, cells showed recovery of the surround whisker response when the iontophoretic current was switched to retain.
The average response to the secondary whisker decreased to 5% of control values during inactivation of the adjacent barrel [control response, 35.8 ± 5.4 spikes per 50 stimuli (sp/50 st); test response, 1.9 ± 1.2 sp/50 st; n = 7), a reduction that was statistically significant (p < 0.01; t = 9.9; df = 6). During the same period, the principal whisker response fell to 88% of control values (control response, 98.3 ± 18.2 sp/50 st; test response, 86.4 ± 10.6 sp/50 st), which was not significantly different from control (p > 0.05; t = 0.98; df = 6; Fig. 4).
Cortical receptive fields in the absence of intracortical activity
To inactivate synaptic transmission between barrel columns, we applied muscimol to the surface of the cortex and allowed it to diffuse throughout the depth of the cortex (Fig. 5A). Within ∼4 hr of the first application of muscimol (200 μm), all sensory responses were abolished to a depth below layer V. Low-voltage activity (30 μV) remained in layer IV after blocking postsynaptic responses and was considered to originate from presynaptic thalamic fibers because it did not respond to iontophoresis of glutamate (Wallace et al., 2001). Putative thalamocortical fiber activity was recorded on 12 occasions and was always found to respond within 5-8 msec of whisker stimulation. The postsynaptic responses to the principal whisker at the same location could be measured before the block had occurred or by iontophoresis of bicuculline after the muscimol had blocked responses. In all cases, postsynaptic responses were found to be 1-3 msec later than that of the low-voltage activity.
Sensory responses were recorded before blocking intracortical transmission to compare them with responses after drug treatment. Sensory responses were recorded during blockade of intracortical transmission by low-level iontophoresis of BMC. The IC50 for displacement of muscimol from GABAA receptors by bicuculline is ∼20 μm (Huang and Johnston, 1990). This implies that to maintain very local diffusion when ejecting BMC, we require less than half the iontophoretic current needed to eject muscimol (<5 nA; Fig. 5B). This corresponded to using currents of 1-5 nA for <10 min. In four cases, sensory responses were recorded after the effect of muscimol had worn off. Most of the cells were located within the boundaries of the barrel itself, but some were found in septal locations (Table 1).
Table 1.
Condition |
Barrel |
Septal |
Total |
---|---|---|---|
Control, urethane | 17 | 4 | 21 |
Control, barbiturate | 18 | 17 | 35 |
Muscimol + BMC, urethane | 27 | 7 | 34 |
Muscimol + BMC, barbiturate | 14 | 4 | 18 |
Total
|
76
|
32
|
108
|
The numbers of cells located in septal and barrel locations are shown for control conditions and for cases in which the cortex was blocked with muscimol and subsequently locally disinhibited with BMC. The numbers of cells in different locations are broken down into those recorded under urethane and those under barbiturate anesthesia.
Neurons located within barrels
In normal untreated cortex, most barrel neurons exhibited a surround receptive field (71%), whereas 29% only responded to the principal whisker. Most surround receptive fields comprised one other whisker (47% of all cells recorded) but could exhibit receptive fields of up to five whiskers. After inactivating intracortical activity with muscimol and reinstating local activity with bicuculline, almost none of the neurons exhibited a surround receptive field (3.6%), but they showed a powerful response to the principal whisker. The decrease in the proportion of cells exhibiting a surround receptive field in the absence of intracortical activity was statistically significant (p < 0.01; χ2 = 14; df = 1).
Figure 6a shows an example of a cell that responded to five whiskers before blocking cortical activity (only three whisker responses are shown here for clarity). Applying muscimol blocked all responses, but after local disinhibition with BMC, only the principal whisker response was reinstated. On average, the surround receptive field whisker that gave the greatest response was found to evoke ∼42% of the principal whisker response in untreated animals. However, after applying muscimol to the cortex and locally disinhibiting with bicuculline, the surround receptive field whisker generating the greatest response was just 2.2% of the principal whisker response, which was significantly different from control (p << 0.01; df = 42).
The average response of the principal whisker could be increased greatly above control levels without revealing a surround receptive field component (Fig. 7). On average, the principal whisker response reactivated by BMC was 213% greater than the average control principal whisker response (test, 163.04 sp/50 st; control, 76.64 sp/50 st). In contrast, in the few cases in which any surround whisker response was detected, the 2′ whisker response intensity was just 13% of the control level (test, 4.44 sp/50 st; control, 34.29 sp/50 st; t = 6.99; p << 0.01; df = 42). The 3′-5′ components of the receptive field were completely abolished in muscimol-treated cortex despite the local reactivation with BMC and were therefore also highly significantly different from control receptive field responses (Fig. 8).
To compare the effect of local disinhibition in the absence of transbarrel communication with local disinhibition in the presence of transbarrel communication, we also applied BMC in otherwise untreated cortex. BMC was iontophoresed at the same current (1-5 nA) for the same time limit (< 10 min) as it had been in muscimol-blocked cortex. We found that BMC increased the principal whisker response to 201% of control levels (average value for 38 cells), which was very similar to its effect in muscimol-blocked cortex (213% of control). However, BMC also had a profound effect on the surround receptive field components in unblocked cortex, unlike its action in muscimol-treated cortex, where it had little or no effect (Fig. 7). All whiskers contributing to the surround receptive field increased in the presence of BMC (Fig. 7C), whereas they were absent in the presence of BMC in muscimol-treated cortex (Fig. 7B). The secondary whisker responses increased to 317% of control in unblocked cortex versus 13% in blocked cortex, and this difference was significant (t = 8.56; p << 0.01; df = 37). Similar observations were made for the other surround receptive field whiskers (3′ whisker, 297 vs 0%; 4′ whisker, 281 vs 0%; and 5′ whisker, 222 vs 0%; p << 0.01; df = 37 for all cases; Fig. 7B,C, compare gray bars).
Neurons located in septa
The effect of blocking intracortical transmission was less pronounced for septal cells. As shown in Figure 6B, some cells retained a minor response from a surround receptive field whisker when treated locally with BMC while the surrounding cortex was blocked with muscimol. Although 42% of the septal cells lost all surround receptive field responses during blockade of intracortical transmission, just like the barrel cells, the rest retained input from at least one other whisker (68%; Fig. 8). The number of cells recorded in septal regions in muscimol-treated cortex was relatively low in this study (Table 1), so our estimate of the proportion of septal cells retaining input in the absence of cortical activity is only a first approximation. However, similar behavior was seen under barbiturate anesthesia (see below), which suggests that the properties of the cells we recorded may be representative of many septal cells.
The second strongest whisker response was 65% of the most powerful whisker response in control cases and 25% after treatment (note that a principal whisker is difficult to attribute in some of these cases). This value is significantly lower than that of the control, indicating some intracortical component to the surround receptive fields of these septal cells (t = 2.17; p < 0.05; df = 9), but greater than zero, indicating some thalamic component.
In contrast to the effect on the barrel cells, the application of BMC in muscimol-blocked cortex tended to increase the average response to both the principal and the 2′ surround whiskers. The principal whisker response increased 333% (from 108 to 360 sp/50 st), and the 2′ surround response increased by 139% (from 67.4 to 94 sp/50 st). Occasionally, the temporal response to the principal and surround whiskers was quicker in onset and briefer in duration after application of BMC compared with before muscimol application (see Fig. 6B). This is presumably because only the thalamic component of the response is available to drive the cell in this case, which it does in an exaggerated manner because of the BMC, whereas the longer-latency intracortical components are absent, making the response shorter in duration.
Neurons located in the ventroposterior medial thalamic nucleus
To check whether blocking intracortical activity affected the receptive field properties of thalamic neurons, one electrode was placed in the ventroposterior medial thalamic nucleus (VPm), and a second was placed in the corresponding receptive field area in the barrel cortex. Thalamic receptive fields were measured before and after application of muscimol to the cortex (Fig. 8). The cortical electrode was used to monitor the cortical activity blockade during thalamic recordings. Blocking intracortical transmission had no effect on VPm receptive field properties. The principal whisker responses were the same before and after muscimol application, as were the surround receptive field responses (principal whisker response: before cortical muscimol, 60.5 ± 6.0 sp/50 st; after cortical muscimol, 74.7 ± 6.1 sp/50 st; secondary whisker response: before, 14.5 ± 3.6 sp/50 st; after, 20.4 ± 5.8 sp/50 st), and these values are not statistically different (p > 0.05, t test). Figure 8 shows that the kurtosis of the surround receptive field profile is not changed, either. The main surround receptive field whisker response is 24% of the principal whisker response before cortical muscimol application and 27% after. These results demonstrate that the thalamic input to the cortex was probably constant during application of muscimol to the cortex.
Effect of anesthetic on cortical receptive fields in the absence of intracortical activity
Although it is unlikely that the organization of receptive fields described above can be ascribed to the anesthetic, we wanted to check whether the same results were evident when using an anesthetic with a different mode of action. Therefore, we repeated the experiment in animals anesthetized with sodium pentobarbitone. This anesthetic principally affects GABAergic receptors (Dilger, 2002) and therefore gives the advantage that our manipulations with the anesthetic, muscimol, and BMC were affecting the same main target. The level of anesthesia was maintained constant for each animal (see Materials and Methods). Therefore, once the muscimol had diffused into the cortex the only variable involved was BMC.
In barrel neurons, as found with urethane anesthesia, the average response of the principal whisker under muscimol blockade could be increased greatly above control levels without, in most cases, revealing any surround receptive field component (Fig. 9). On average, the principal whisker response reactivated by BMC was 230% greater than the average control response (test mean, 100.6 sp/50 st; n = 14; control, 43.75 sp/50 st; n = 18). The surround receptive fields when present were limited to at most one other whisker. In muscimol-treated cortex, neurons “responded” to stimulation of the 2′-5′ whiskers at 7, 0, 0, and 0% of the principal whisker response, respectively. In contrast, neurons in untreated cortex responded to surround whisker stimulation of the 2′-5′ whiskers at 18.5, 12.3, 11.8, and 10% of the principal whisker response, respectively. The 3′-5′ surround receptive field values for muscimol-treated cortex are all significantly different from control (Fig. 9).
In septal neurons, consistent with the result in urethane-anesthetized animals, the effect of blocking intracortical transmission in pentobarbitone-anesthetized animals was less pronounced than for barrel cells. Neurons retained responses from a surround receptive field whisker when treated locally with BMC (Fig. 9). The second strongest whisker response was 46.7% of the most powerful whisker response in control cases (n = 17) and 44% after muscimol and local BMC treatment (n = 4). Similarly, the 3′-5′ whiskers generated smaller but significant responses of 8, 5, and 3% of the principal whisker response, respectively, in locally activated cortex compared with 32, 25, and 15%, respectively, in untreated cortex. The sample of septal cells in barbiturate-anesthetized animals was too low to achieve significance with statistical tests (Table 1) but showed a tendency similar to those recorded under urethane anesthesia.
Discussion
We found that when intracortical activity is limited to the immediate surrounds of the layer IV barrel neuron the receptive field shrinks to a single whisker with occasional evidence of a weak second whisker input. In contrast, septal cells often showed multiwhisker responses in the absence of intracortical transmission. These findings were consistent independent of whether the experiments were conducted in urethane- or barbiturate-anesthetized animals. Neither could these findings be explained by a reduction in thalamic receptive field size attributable to treatment of the cortex with muscimol because thalamic receptive fields remained constant in the thalamus during this procedure. The most likely explanation for these findings is that the surround receptive fields of barrel neurons are generated intracortically. This view is consistent with experiments in which we blocked activity in a single barrel. Inactivating a single barrel with muscimol selectively removed the corresponding whisker from the surround receptive field of a neuron located in the neighboring barrel.
Receptive field size measurements
Previous measurements of receptive field size have shown that as many as 15 whiskers can generate suprathreshold extracellular responses in cortical cells, and, on average, approximately eight whiskers can produce an output in a particular neuron (Armstrong-James and Fox, 1987). Similar findings have been reported after intracellular recordings in which 7-16 whiskers have been shown to generate EPSPs, the majority of which were subthreshold for spiking under the anesthetic conditions used (Moore and Nelson, 1998; Zhu and Connors, 1999). The degree to which the inputs are translated into outputs depends partly on general levels of excitability, which under experimental conditions depend on the level of anesthesia. In our studies, it was possible to look at subthreshold responses by using bicuculline to disinhibit neurons.
For barrel neurons, we found that local disinhibition produced a large increase in the surround receptive field response provided that intracortical transmission was intact, but that disinhibition did not reveal any surround receptive field response when intracortical activity was blocked. Some exposure of the surround receptive field input would be expected if it had been present because (1) the surround receptive field was suprathreshold under the conditions in which we recorded before blocking intracortical transmission; (2) the surround receptive field is sensitive to treatment by bicuculline; (3) the principal whisker response was easily brought from subthreshold to generate double its normal level of response by local disinhibition; and (4) the surround receptive fields were easily brought above threshold by the same means in septal cells. This implies that the suprathreshold and subthreshold multivibrissae receptive field components reported in previous studies originate in the cortex.
Receptive field size is known to be affected by anesthetic level (Armstrong-James and George, 1988; Friedberg et al., 1999); therefore, it was carefully monitored and controlled at levels equivalent to those used in other studies (see Materials and Methods). It has been suggested that receptive fields might differ under urethane anesthesia (Simons et al., 1992); therefore, we repeated the experiments under barbiturate anesthesia. Mechanistically, urethane anesthesia decreases NMDA and non-NMDA receptor function approximately equally and enhances GABAergic receptor function (Hara and Harris, 2002), whereas barbiturates appear to have a simpler action, acting principally as a GABAA agonist (Steinbach and Akk, 2001). In practice, the findings were similar under urethane and barbiturate anesthesia in that surround receptive fields were present when intracortical transmission was intact but absent when it was blocked. Other studies have also found receptive fields to be similar under urethane and barbiturate anesthesia (cf. Moore and Nelson, 1998; Zhu and Connors, 1999). In our hands, the response levels were lower in barbiturate anesthesia but nevertheless showed just as many multiwhisker receptive fields at an equivalent anesthetic level provided intracortical transmission was present. In any case, because barbiturate acts principally on GABA receptors, any differences in anesthetic level should have been reversed locally by bicuculline.
Intracortical transmission
In these studies, we created conditions in which the local neuronal circuit in the barrel was strongly disinhibited by bicuculline, yet under these conditions, practically all the barrel neurons showed single whisker receptive fields in the absence of intracortical transmission. The principal whisker response could be elevated from zero to twice its control value with bicuculline without revealing a surround receptive field component in almost all cases. Because the thalamic afferents are unaffected by manipulation of the postsynaptic GABAergic receptors and are not restricted by inhibition from depolarizing the postsynaptic cells, this implies that they cannot be responsible for generating layer IV surround receptive fields. This leaves the possibility that the surround receptive fields of layer IV barrel neurons are generated intracortically; in which case, from what source?
Recent studies performed on living slices of barrel cortex imaged using voltage-sensitive dyes have shown that there is no spread of excitation between columns at the level of layer IV when the thalamic inputs are stimulated electrically (Petersen and Sakmann, 2001; Laaris and Keller, 2002). Similarly, studies using natural stimuli have indicated that single barrel lesions do not affect surround receptive fields in layer IV, although they do abolish the representation of the corresponding whisker in layers II and III (Goldreich et al., 1999).
Nevertheless, there are at least two pathways by which excitation could be transferred between barrel columns. Anatomical tracer experiments show that horizontal connections link barrels at the supragranular and infragranular levels (Bernardo et al., 1990). One possibility is that these connect with the layer IV pyramidal and star pyramidal cells located in the barrels and receive neighboring whisker information from the neighboring barrel column. A second possibility is that the relevant connections run directly between the barrels. Although there are fewer and shorter-range direct transbarrel connections in layer IV than in superficial or infragranular layers, they nevertheless exist (Bernardo et al., 1990). Furthermore, recent slice studies using release of caged glutamate do show evidence of transbarrel communication (Schubert et al., 2003). These studies show that transbarrel pathways project primarily to pyramidal cells in layer IV, whereas spiny stellate neurons primarily process intrabarrel information (Schubert et al., 2003). In addition, transbarrel projections evoke EPSPs and IPSPs in cells located in neighboring barrels.
The lack of evidence for transbarrel communication in some slice studies may therefore be attributable to the difficulty in cutting the slice to preserve all the required connections. Therefore, it is possible that layer IV surround receptive fields are derived directly from neighboring barrels. The lack of evidence for transbarrel communication in some in vivo experiments may be because it is often difficult to ablate a barrel completely in these studies (Goldreich et al., 1999). Even small surviving fragments of the ablated barrel are sufficient to support surround receptive field inputs to neighboring barrels (Fox, 1994), and these are not discovered until after the recording has finished. An advantage of the iontophoretic “lesion” reported here is that it allows the degree of the block to be varied and increased during the experiment if necessary. The present results show that a “reversible lesion” created by iontophoresis of muscimol can selectively abolish responses to the whisker related to the inhibited barrel.
Differences between barrel and septal subdivisions
A distinction was found between the behavior of septal and barrel cells to abolition of intracortical activity. Although barrel cells tended to lose their surround receptive fields, septal receptive fields decreased in size and intensity but were not completely abolished. This suggests that unlike barrel cells, septal cells do derive some surround receptive field input from the thalamus. One explanation for this finding is based on the fact that septal and barrel innervations arise from thalamic neurons with different receptive field properties. It has been shown that the somatosensory part of the posterior thalamic nucleus (POm) projects to the septal region (Koralek et al., 1988), but it is unlikely to have influenced septal receptive fields in these studies because POm relies on cortical activity itself for full expression of its receptive fields (Diamond et al., 1992). Because we had inhibited cortex with muscimol, one would expect POm to be silenced, too. A more likely explanation is that the subset of thalamic cells in the tail portion of the barreloids (VPMvl), which project to the septal regions of the barrel field (as well as S2), are responsible for the multiwhisker responses of the septal cells in the absence of intracortical activity (Pierret et al., 2000). The VPMvl neurons receive input from the interpolaris nucleus of the brainstem trigeminal nuclei, which itself exhibits multiwhisker receptive fields, and might therefore project this information via VPMvl to the cortex (Woolston et al., 1982; Jacquin et al., 1989; Pierret et al., 2000).
Implications for information processing
The present results imply that information from a single whisker is relayed within the cortex to the adjacent columns either from the principal barrel or to some extent from mixed whisker information in septal regions. Despite transmission through several synapses, the unmixed information from individual whiskers arrives in the cortex intact. This arrangement gives greater flexibility for the way individual whisker information is combined or integrated by the cortical circuitry than if it had arrived premixed. It therefore seems likely that the cortical surround receptive field structure is synthesized by the connectivity of intracortical connections. For example, layer II and III cells receive a strong projection from layer IV cells in their principal barrel, thereby providing their principal whisker input; on the other hand, connections from neighboring barrels provide their surround receptive field information. Future studies will be required to track the information flow from individual barrels to see how receptive fields are generated in different layers of the cortex. Finally, it has been noted that subcortical structures in the somatosensory system show little if any experience-dependent plasticity, whereas cortical pathways show plasticity into adulthood (Fox et al., 2002). Therefore, one of the advantages of generating surround receptive field information in the cortex rather than subcortically is that it allows the system to remain more adaptable to changes in the periphery throughout life.
Footnotes
This work was supported by grants from the Medical Research Council (United Kingdom) and the Human Frontiers Science Program Organization (K.F.). We thank Paul Chapman and Frank Sengpiel for critical reading of this manuscript and Danielle Green and Mervyn McKenna for undertaking the histology.
Correspondence should be addressed to Prof. Kevin Fox, School of Biosciences, Cardiff University, Museum Avenue, Cardiff CF10 3US, UK. E-mail: foxkd@cf.ac.uk.
H. Wallace's present address: Institute of Child Health, Alder Hey Children's Hospital, Eaton Road, Liverpool L12 2AP, UK.
S. Glazewski's present address: McKay Institute of Communication and Neuroscience, Keele University, Staffordshire ST5 5BG, UK.
Copyright © 2003 Society for Neuroscience 0270-6474/03/238380-12$15.00/0
References
- Armstrong-James M, Fox K ( 1987) Spatiotemporal convergence and divergence in the rat S1 “barrel” cortex. J Comp Neurol 263 : 265-281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Armstrong-James M, George MJ ( 1988) Influence of anesthesia on spontaneous activity and receptive field size of single units in rat SmI neocortex. Exp Neurol 99 : 369-387. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Armstrong-James M, Fox K, Kruk ZL, Millar J ( 1982) Quantitative iontophoresis of catecholamines using multibarrel carbon fibre microelectrodes. J Neurosci Methods 4 : 385-407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Armstrong-James M, Fox K, Das-Gupta A ( 1992) Flow of excitation within rat barrel cortex on striking a single vibrissa. J Neurophysiol 68 : 1345-1358. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bernardo KL, McCasland JS, Woolsey TA ( 1990) Local axonal trajectories in mouse barrel cortex. Exp Brain Res 82 : 247-253. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buonomano DV, Merzenich MM ( 1998) Cortical plasticity: from synapses to maps. Annu Rev Neurosci 21 : 149-186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carslaw HS, Jaeger JC ( 1956) The conduction of heat in solids. London: Oxford UP.
- Diamond ME, Armstrong-James M, Budway MJ, Ebner FF ( 1992) Somatic sensory responses in the rostral sector of the posterior group (POm) and in the ventral posterior medial nucleus (VPM) of the rat thalamus: dependence on the barrel field cortex. J Comp Neurol 319 : 66-84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dilger JP ( 2002) The effects of anesthetics on ligand-gated ion channels. Br J Anaesth 89 : 41-51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox K ( 1994) The cortical component of experience-dependent synaptic plasticity in the rat barrel cortex. J Neurosci 14 : 7665-7679. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox K, Armstrong-James M ( 1986) The role of the anterior intralaminar nuclei and N-methyl d-aspartate receptors in the generation of spontaneous bursts in rat neocortical neurones. Exp Brain Res 63 : 505-518. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox K, Schlaggar BL, Glazewski S, O'Leary DD ( 1996) Glutamate receptor blockade at cortical synapses disrupts development of thalamocortical and columnar organization in somatosensory cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 93 : 5584-5589. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox K, Glazewski S, Wallace H ( 2002) Is there a thalamic component to experience-dependent cortical plasticity? Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 357 : 1709-1715. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friedberg MH, Lee SM, Ebner FF ( 1999) Modulation of receptive field properties of thalamic somatosensory neurons by the depth of anesthesia. J Neurophysiol 81 : 2243-2252. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fujita I, Tanaka K, Ito M, Cheng K ( 1992) Columns for visual features of objects in monkey inferotemporal cortex. Nature 360 : 343-346. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glazewski S, Fox K ( 1996) Time course of experience-dependent synaptic potentiation and depression in barrel cortex of adolescent rats. J Neurophysiol 75 : 1714-1729. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldreich D, Kyriazi HT, Simons DJ ( 1999) Functional independence of layer IV barrels in rodent somatosensory cortex. J Neurophysiol 82 : 1311-1316. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guedal AE ( 1920) Signs of inhalation anesthesia: a fundamental guide. In: Inhalation anesthesia (Guedal AE, ed), pp 10-52. New York: Macmillian.
- Hara K, Harris RA ( 2002) The anesthetic mechanism of urethane: the effects on neurotransmitter-gated ion channels. Anesth Analg 94 : 313-318. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huang JH, Johnston GA ( 1990) (+)-Hydrastine, a potent competitive antagonist at mammalian GABAA receptors. Br J Pharmacol 99 : 727-730. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hubel DH, Wiesel TN ( 1962) Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex. J Physiol 160 : 106-154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jacquin MF, Barcia M, Rhoades RW ( 1989) Structure-function relationships in rat brainstem subnucleus interpolaris: IV. Projection neurons. J Comp Neurol 282 : 45-62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koralek KA, Jensen KF, Killackey HP ( 1988) Evidence for two complementary patterns of thalamic input to the rat somatosensory cortex. Brain Res 463 : 346-351. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laaris N, Keller A ( 2002) Functional independence of layer IV barrels. J Neurophysiol 87 : 1028-1034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lubke J, Egger V, Sakmann B, Feldmeyer D ( 2000) Columnar organization of dendrites and axons of single and synaptically coupled excitatory spiny neurons in layer 4 of the rat barrel cortex. J Neurosci 20 : 5300-5311. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mountcastle VB ( 1957) Modality and topographic properties of single neurons of cat's somatic sensory cortex. J Neurophysiol 20 : 408-434. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moore CI, Nelson SB ( 1998) Spatio-temporal subthreshold receptive fields in the vibrissa representation of rat primary somatosensory cortex. J Neurophysiol 80 : 2882-2892. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nicholson C, Phillips JM ( 1981) Ion diffusion modified by tortuosity and volume fraction in the extracellular microenvironment of the rat cerebellum. J Physiol (Lond) 321 : 225-257. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petersen RS, Diamond ME ( 2000) Spatiotemporal distribution of whisker evoked activity in rat somatosensory cortex and the coding of stimulus location. J Neurosci 20 : 6135-6143. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petersen CC, Sakmann B ( 2001) Functionally independent columns of rat somatosensory barrel cortex revealed with voltage-sensitive dye imaging. J Neurosci 21 : 8435-8446. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pierret T, Lavallee P, Deschenes M ( 2000) Parallel streams for the relay of vibrissal information through thalamic barreloids. J Neurosci 20 : 7455-7462. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schubert D, Kotter R, Zilles K, Luhmann HJ, Staiger JF ( 2003) Cell-type specific circuitry of layer IV spiny neurons. J Neurosci 23 : 2961-2970. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simons DJ ( 1985) Temporal and spatial integration in the rat SI vibrissa cortex. J Neurophysiol 54 : 615-635. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simons DJ, Woolsey TA ( 1984) Morphology of Golgi-Cox-impregnated barrel neurons in rat SmI cortex. J Comp Neurol 230 : 119-132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simons DJ, Carvell GE, Hershey AE, Bryant DP ( 1992) Responses of barrel cortex neurons in awake rats and effects of urethane anesthesia. Exp Brain Res 91 : 259-272. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steinbach JH, Akk G ( 2001) Modulation of GABA(A) receptor channel gating by pentobarbital. J Physiol (Lond) 537 : 715-733. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Szentagothai J ( 1975) The “module-concept” in cerebral cortex architecture. Brain Res 95 : 475-496. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tsunoda K, Yamane Y, Nishizaki M, Tanifuji M ( 2001) Complex objects are represented in macaque inferotemporal cortex by the combination of feature columns. Nat Neurosci 4 : 832-838. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wallace H, Glazewski S, Liming K, Fox K ( 2001) The role of cortical activity in experience-dependent potentiation and depression of sensory responses in rat barrel cortex. J Neurosci 21 : 3881-3894. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wong-Riley M ( 1979) Changes in the visual system of monocularly sutured or enucleated cats demonstrable with cytochrome oxidase histochemistry. Brain Res 171 : 11-28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woolsey TA, Van der Loos H ( 1970) The structural organization of layer IV in the somatosensory region (SI) of mouse cerebral cortex. The description of a cortical field composed of discrete cytoarchitectonic units. Brain Res 17 : 205-242. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woolston DC, La Londe JR, Gibson JM ( 1982) Comparison of response properties of cerebellar- and thalamic-projecting interpolaris neurons. J Neurophysiol 48 : 160-173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhu JJ, Connors BW ( 1999) Intrinsic firing patterns and whisker-evoked synaptic responses of neurons in the rat barrel cortex. J Neurophysiol 81 : 1171-1183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]