Abstract
Mutant mice lacking the glutamate receptor subunit δ2 exhibit changes in the structure and function of the cerebellar cortex. The most prominent functional feature is a deficiency in the long-term depression (LTD) at parallel fiber–Purkinje cell synapses. These mutant mice exhibit severe impairment during delay eyeblink conditioning but learn normally during trace eyeblink conditioning without the cerebellar LTD, even with a 0 trace interval. We investigated the hippocampal contribution to this cerebellar LTD-independent “0 trace interval” learning. The mutant mice whose dorsal hippocampi were aspirated exhibited severe impairment in learning, whereas those that received post-training hippocampal lesions retained the memory. The wild-type mice showed no impairment in either case. These results suggest that the hippocampal component of the eyeblink conditioning task becomes dominant when cerebellar LTD is impaired.
Keywords: classical conditioning, eyeblink, hippocampus, cerebellum, long-term depression, mouse
Introduction
The hippocampus is involved in various kinds of learning (Morris et al., 1982; Barnes, 1988). Although its role could be interpreted in terms of behavioral function, the exact mechanism to execute that function remains to be elucidated. To achieve this understanding, knowledge about the neural network, with which the hippocampus interacts, is critical. Classical eyeblink conditioning is one of candidates from which such knowledge could be obtained. In this learning, the essential neural circuit has been studied extensively with a delay paradigm, in which the unconditioned stimulus (US) is delayed and coterminates with the conditioned stimulus (CS). Thompson and colleagues have indicated that the locus of the memory trace is in the cerebellum; they stress the importance for memory function of long-term depression (LTD) at parallel fiber (PF)–Purkinje cell (PC) synapses, as well as the presumptive plasticity in the interpositus nucleus (Kim and Thompson, 1997). The contribution of the hippocampus to eyeblink conditioning has been shown to differ across conditioning paradigms. For example, an intact hippocampus is required for successful learning in trace conditioning (Solomon et al., 1986; Moyer et al., 1990) and discrimination reversal conditioning (Berger and Orr, 1983) but not in delay conditioning (Solomon et al., 1986).
We investigated previously classical eyeblink conditioning in mutant mice lacking the glutamate receptor subunit δ2 (GluRδ2). These mutant mice exhibit a selective dysfunction of the cerebellar cortex, impaired cerebellar LTD (Kashiwabuchi et al., 1995), a decrease in the number of PF–PC synapses (Kurihara et al., 1997), and multiple innervation of PCs by climbing fibers (Kashiwabuchi et al., 1995). Consistent with the hypothesis that cerebellar LTD plays a critical role in eyeblink conditioning (Ito, 1989; Thompson and Kim, 1996), GluRδ2−/− mice exhibited severe impairment in learning when a delay paradigm was used (Kishimoto et al., 2001a). However, GluRδ2−/− mice learned normally in the trace paradigm, in which a stimulus-free trace interval intervenes between the CS and US (Kishimoto et al., 2001a), and even in a “trace 0 paradigm,” in which the US starts just after the CS ends (Kishimoto et al., 2001c). These results suggest that another learning mechanism, one that does not require cerebellar LTD, underlies the ability of GluRδ2−/−mice to learn in eyeblink conditioning paradigms in which the US does not overlap with the CS. Results in another cerebellar LTD-deficient mouse lacking the phospholipase Cβ4 subunit also support this hypothesis (Kishimoto et al., 2001b). Furthermore, we found recently that scopolamine impairs cerebellar LTD-independent learning in GluRδ2−/−mice (Takatsuki et al., 2002). This suggests that the hippocampus contributes to LTD-independent learning, because scopolamine slows delay eyeblink conditioning primarily through its effect on the hippocampus (Solomon et al., 1983) via the septum (Solomon and Gottfried, 1981); scopolamine also impairs trace eyeblink conditioning, which we know requires an intact hippocampus (Kaneko and Thompson, 1997). To examine the hippocampal contribution to cerebellar LTD-independent learning, we investigated the effect of hippocampal lesions on learning using a trace 0 paradigm with GluRδ2−/− mice.
Materials and Methods
Subjects and surgery. We used GluRδ2−/− mice with a 99.99% C57BL/6 genetic background (Kishimoto et al., 2001a) and wild-type C57BL/6 mice (Japan SLC, Hamamatsu, Shizuoka, Japan). The animals were kept on a 12 hr light/dark cycle with ad libitum access to food and water. All experiments were performed in accordance with the guidelines established by the Institutional Animal Investigation Committee at the University of Tokyo and the National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals. Before surgery, the animals were anesthetized with ketamine (80 mg/kg, i.p.; Sankyo, Tokyo, Japan) and xylazine (20 mg/kg, i.p.; Bayer, Tokyo, Japan). A bilateral aspiration of the dorsal hippocampus and overlying neocortex was performed on the hippocampal lesion group. The control group received only bilateral aspiration of the neocortex. Four Teflon-coated stainless steel wires (#7910; A-M Systems, Carlsborg, WA) were implanted to record electromyographic (EMG) activity and to deliver a periorbital shock, as described in our previous report (Kishimoto et al., 2001a).
Behavioral training. At least 3 d after implanting the electrodes, the animals were given 2 d to adapt to the experimental apparatus, during which EMGs were recorded to calculate spontaneous eyeblink frequency. The conditioning began the next day. Daily conditioning consisted of 90 CS–US paired trials and 10 CS-alone trials on every 10th trial, with a pseudorandomized intertrial interval of 20–40 sec. In the paired trials, a 352 msec tone CS (1 kHz, 90 dB) was followed by a 100 msec periorbital shock US (100 Hz square pulses). The US intensity was carefully calibrated to elicit an eyeblink–head-turn response and adjusted daily for each animal; it ranged from 8 to 17 V (Kishimoto et al., 2001a). The interstimulus interval was set at 352 msec so that the trace interval was 0 msec.
Data analysis. The conditioned response (CR) was monitored through eyelid EMG activity, and the analysis of that activity was performed as described previously (Kishimoto et al., 2001a). In brief, mean + SD of the amplitudes of the EMG activity for 300 msec before CS onset in 100 trials was defined as the threshold, which was then used in the analysis below. In each trial, average values of EMG amplitude above the threshold were calculated for 300 msec before CS onset (pre-value), 30 msec after CS onset (startle-value), and 202 msec before US onset (CR-value). If both the pre-value and startle-value were <10% of threshold, the trial was regarded as a valid trial. Among the valid trials, a trial was assumed to contain the CR if the CR value was larger than 1% of threshold and it exceeded two times the pre-value. For the CS-alone trials, the period for CR-value calculation was extended to the CS end. To evaluate the effect on the startle response, we calculated the frequency of trials in which the startle-value exceeded 10% of threshold. The frequency of CRs in the valid trials (CR%) was expressed as mean ± SEM. Statistical significance was determined by a two-way ANOVA and a three-way repeated-measures ANOVA using SPSS (Chicago, IL) statistical software. p < 0.05 was considered significant in this study.
Experimental protocols. In acquisition experiments, mice first received the lesion, electrodes were implanted after a recovery period of 10 d, and then conditioning occurred for 7 d for CR acquisition. In the retention experiments, the order was electrode implanting, acquisition sessions, and then lesion. Mice for which CR% reached the criteria described below were divided randomly into two groups and received a cortical lesion or a hippocampal lesion on the next day. After a recovery period of 2 weeks, all mice underwent 7 d of retention testing (CS–US paired session). In retention experiment 1, mice underwent 7 d of acquisition session. We used only those mice whose CR% exceeded 50% on the seventh day of the acquisition session. In retention experiment 2, mice were conditioned until their CR% exceeded 70%. The mice that did not reach this criterion after 14 d of acquisition session were excluded from additional study.
Histology. After completing all behavioral experiments, the mice were overdosed with sodium pentobarbital and perfused transcardially with 0.9% saline, followed by 10% Formalin. Brains were removed and placed in 10% Formalin. Before sectioning, brains were placed in a 30% sucrose solution overnight. Frozen sections (40 μm thick) were made using a cryostat and stained with cresyl violet. The largest and smallest lesioned areas were reconstructed from the sections according to the stereotaxic atlas of the mouse brain (Paxinos and Franklin, 2001).
Results
Histology
To confirm the extent of each lesion, coronal sections (40 μm thick) were made through the length of the hippocampus after conclusion of the behavioral experiments. We completely removed the dorsal hippocampus in all mice in the hippocampal-lesion group (Fig.1B). Although some of them retained the posteroventral portion of the hippocampus, the fimbria was bilaterally transected so that the remaining ventral hippocampus was isolated from the fornix. There was no damage to surrounding structures, such as the amygdala, thalamus, or entorhinal cortex, except the overlying neocortex. The results of the histological analysis are summarized in Figure 1, C and D.
Acquisition of the CR depends primarily on the hippocampus in GluRδ2 mutant mice
When conditioned using a trace 0 paradigm, the hippocampal-lesioned GluRδ2−/− mice exhibited severe learning impairment compared with the cortical-lesioned GluRδ2−/− mice (Fig.2B). In contrast, the hippocampal-lesioned wild-type mice learned as normally as the control wild-type mice did (Fig. 2A). A three-way repeated-measures ANOVA revealed that there was a significant effect for lesion type (F(1,203) = 14.645;p < 0.001) and for genotype (F(1,203) = 4.891; p< 0.05), indicating that the learning impairment attributable to a hippocampal lesion differed between wild-type and GluRδ2−/− mice. There was no significant effect of interactions between sessions and lesion type (F(6,203) = 0.089; p> 0.9) or between sessions and genotype (F(6,203) = 0.708; p> 0.6).
Although most of the hippocampal-lesioned GluRδ2−/− mice exhibited severe learning impairment, two of them learned well, resulting in a smaller increase of CR% on average. The CR% reached ∼90% in one mouse and 50% in another mouse by the end of training and stayed almost at spontaneous levels in the remaining six mice (Fig. 2C). The extent of the hippocampal lesion in these two learned mice was intermediate, showing no correlation with their CR%. To investigate the temporal pattern of the EMG activities of these mice, the amplitude data of the valid trials in the last session were averaged in each mouse and then over the mice. Therefore, the pattern does not depend on the criterion used for CR detection. Consistent with their CR%, the amplitude of the average EMG activities of the six nonlearned hippocampal-lesioned GluRδ2−/− mice was small, whereas that of the two learned mice was similar to that of the control mice (Fig. 2D). The temporal pattern of the EMG activities was not noticeably changed in either groups, which is consistent with the previous results with this line of mutant mice (Kishimoto et al., 2001a,c).
Retention experiment 1: the hippocampus is not essential for retention of the CR in GluRδ2 mutant mice
Next, we tested whether the hippocampus was required for retention of the CR. In this experiment, mice received the lesion after 7 d of acquisition sessions. Eighteen of 28 wild-type mice and 20 of 31 GluRδ2−/− mice reached the criterion (see Materials and Methods). Their CR% at the last acquisition session was 73.4 ± 3.9 and 73.6 ± 4.2%, respectively. Even after removing the hippocampus and the overlying cortex, the CR remained at the same level as before, in both wild-type mice (Fig.3A) and GluRδ2−/− mice (Fig. 3B). A three-way repeated-measures ANOVA revealed that the CR% of the postlesion sessions was not significantly influenced by lesion type (F(1,196) = 3.103; p> 0.05) or by genotype (F(1,196) = 0.131; p > 0.7), indicating that the hippocampus is not necessary for retention of the CR in either GluRδ2−/− mice or the wild-type mice. Figure 3C shows the retention index of the CR, calculated by dividing the CR% in the first postlesion session by the CR% in the last prelesion session. A two-way ANOVA revealed that there was no significant effect for either lesion type (F(1,27) = 0.477; p > 0.4) or genotype (F(1,27) = 0.793;p > 0.3). The average temporal pattern of the EMG activities in the first postlesion session of the hippocampal-lesioned GluRδ2−/− mice was also almost the same as that of the control mutant mice (Fig. 3D).
Retention experiment 2: the hippocampus is partially involved for retention or expression of the CR right after reaching asymptotic level
The retention experiment 1 indicated that the hippocampus is not essential for retention of the CR a few days after reaching asymptotic level. In this experiment, we examined the contribution of the hippocampus in an early phase of the asymptotic state of memory. Mice received the lesion on the next day after their CR% exceeded 70%. Seventeen of 20 wild-type mice and 18 of 22 GluRδ2−/− mice reached the criterion. Their CR% in the last session and the number of days conditioned were 84.9 ± 2.2% and 3.4 ± 0.5 d in the wild-type mice and 85.4 ± 2.5% and 5.1 ± 0.9 d in the GluRδ2−/− mice. They were assigned randomly to the lesion groups.
In the wild-type mice, the hippocampal-lesion group retained the memory as well as the control group (Fig.4A). In GluRδ2−/− mice, the hippocampal-lesion group exhibited moderate impairment in retention of the CR compared with the cortical-lesion group (Fig. 4B). Among the eight hippocampal-lesioned GluRδ2−/−mice, five mice retained the CR well, whereas the other three mice showed severe impairment, although two of the latter three mice showed an increase in CR% during the subsequent conditioning (Fig.4D). Among seven cortical-lesioned GluRδ2−/− mice, five mice retained the CR well, whereas the other two mice showed impairment (Fig.4C). A two-way ANOVA on the retention index (Fig.4E) revealed that there was no significant effect for lesion type (F(1,28) = 0.772;p > 0.3) or genotype (F(1,28) = 1.335; p > 0.2), suggesting that the effect of the hippocampal lesion in GluRδ2−/− mice was limited. The average temporal pattern of the EMG activity in the first postlesion session of the hippocampal-lesioned GluRδ2−/− mice also was similar to that of the control mutant mice (Fig. 4F). During the postlesion sessions, the CR% of the hippocampal-lesioned GluRδ2−/− mice (Fig.4D) was unstable compared with the cortical-lesioned mice (Fig. 4C). A three-way repeated-measures ANOVA revealed that there was significant effect for both lesion type (F(1,203) = 9.007; p< 0.01) and genotype (F(1,203) = 23.539; p < 0.001).
The basic sensory and motor performances are normal
To check for any possible effects of these lesions on the basic sensory or motor performance involved in eyeblink conditioning, we also looked at spontaneous eyeblink and startle responses to a tone. Spontaneous eyeblink frequency was not influenced by hippocampal destruction in either wild-type mice or GluRδ2−/− mice (Fig. 2). A two-way ANOVA indicated no significant interaction effects between genotype and lesion type (F(1,28) = 0.005;p > 0.9) and no significant effects for genotype (F(1,28) = 0.856; p > 0.3) or lesion type (F(1,28) = 2.885;p = 0.1). The frequency of the startle responses during conditioning was 5.5 ± 0.6% (n = 8) and 5.7 ± 0.6% (n = 7) for the cortical- and hippocampal-lesioned wild-type mice, respectively, and 5.7 ± 0.5% (n = 9) and 5.0 ± 0.6% (n= 8) for the cortical- and hippocampal-lesioned GluRδ2−/− mice, respectively. The mean startle response of the hippocampal-lesion group was almost the same as that of the control group and did not increase during conditioning. There were no significant effects for genotype (F(1,203) = 0.108; p> 0.7), lesion type (F(1,203) = 0.240; p > 0.6), or session (F(6,203) = 0.525; p> 0.7).
Discussion
In the present study, we demonstrated that ablation of the dorsal hippocampus severely impairs the acquisition of a CR in GluRδ2−/− mice. This result clearly indicates that eyeblink conditioning with a short trace interval in GluRδ2−/− mice depends primarily on the hippocampus and mirrors the effect of scopolamine in GluRδ2−/− mice (Takatsuki et al., 2002), thus supporting the idea that the functional hippocampus is important to cerebellar LTD-independent learning to occur.
Previous studies using rabbits and rats have indicated that the hippocampus is required for eyeblink conditioning with a long trace interval but not with a shorter trace interval (Solomon et al., 1986;Moyer et al., 1990; Weiss et al., 1999) and therefore have paid no attention to the role of the hippocampus in short trace-interval conditioning. The present study has revealed that the hippocampal-lesioned GluRδ2−/− mice have severe impairment in learning the trace 0 paradigm, but the hippocampal-lesioned wild-type mice do not. Thus, the contribution of the hippocampus to short trace interval conditioning becomes greater in GluRδ2−/− mice than that in the wild-type mice, suggesting that cerebellar LTD-independent learning occurs efficiently with a contribution of the hippocampus. Because GluRδ2−/− mice exhibit severe learning impairment with a delay paradigm (Kishimoto et al., 2001a,c), this kind of hippocampal function does not work well during delay conditioning.
When considering the role of the hippocampus in GluRδ2−/− mice, it is important to ask whether it is required for retention of the acquired memory. Because the acquired CR was retained fairly well after a hippocampal lesion, we conclude that the hippocampus is not required for retention and recall of the memory. In eyeblink conditioning with a long trace interval, the contribution of the hippocampus appears to be time limited: hippocampal ablation 1 d after learning impairs retention of a trace eyeblink CR, but ablation has no effect if it occurs 4 weeks after learning in rabbits (Kim et al., 1995) and mice (Takehara et al., 2002). Consistent with these lesion studies, excitability increases in hippocampal pyramidal neurons remain for a limited period of time after learning (Moyer et al., 1996; Thompson et al., 1996). In the present study, the hippocampal lesion affected the preacquired CR in GluRδ2−/− mice in a time-dependent manner; it did not impair the retention at all if made a few days after asymptotic level of learning but moderately impaired the acquired CR if made right after asymptotic level of learning. This is consistent with the previous reports (Kim et al., 1995; Takehara et al., 2002) in the sense that the role of hippocampus in retention or expression of the memory diminishes as time elapses. However, because the CR remained to some extent even if the lesion were made right after learning, there is a possibility that the role of the hippocampus in eyeblink conditioning with a short trace interval is a little different from that in long trace interval conditioning. Additional experiments using wild-type animals will be needed to test this hypothesis.
In conclusion, the present study has demonstrated that the hippocampus is important for the acquisition, but not for the retention, of a motor memory in GluRδ2−/− mice, which have deficiencies selective to the cerebellar cortex, including cerebellar LTD. These results suggest that the hippocampal component of the eyeblink conditioning task becomes dominant in the cerebellar LTD-independent learning. It remains to be clarified where that memory trace is formed and stored and what the role of the hippocampus is in cerebellar LTD-independent learning. Answering these questions should lead to a better understanding of the role of the hippocampus in eyeblink conditioning and may provide valuable information concerning the general mechanism of hippocampus-dependent learning.
Footnotes
This work was supported by Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology of Japan Grants-in-Aid 13210036 and 13680734 and a grant from the Core Research for Evolution Science and Technology of Japan Science and Technology Corporation.
Correspondence should be addressed to Shigenori Kawahara, Laboratory of Neurobiophysics, School of Pharmaceutical Sciences, The University of Tokyo, 7-3-1 Hongo, Bunkyo-ku, Tokyo 113-0033, Japan. E-mail:kawahara@mol.f.u-tokyo.ac.jp.
References
- 1.Barnes CA. Spatial learning and memory process: the search for their neurobiological mechanisms in the rat. Trends Neurosci. 1988;11:163–169. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(88)90143-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Berger TW, Orr WB. Hippocampectomy selectively disrupts discrimination reversal conditioning of the rabbit nictitating membrane response. Behav Brain Res. 1983;8:49–68. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(83)90171-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Ito M. Long-term depression. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1989;12:85–102. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.12.030189.000505. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Kaneko T, Thompson RF. Disruption of trace conditioning of the nictitating membrane response in rabbits by central cholinergic blockade. Psychopharmacology. 1997;131:161–166. doi: 10.1007/s002130050279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Kashiwabuchi N, Ikeda K, Araki K, Hirano T, Shibuki K, Takayama C, Inoue Y, Kutsuwada T, Yagi T, Kang Y, Aizawa S, Mishina M. Impairment of motor coordination, Purkinje cell synapse formation, and cerebellar long-term depression in GluRδ2 mutant mice. Cell. 1995;81:245–252. doi: 10.1016/0092-8674(95)90334-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Kim JJ, Thompson RF. Cerebellar circuits and synaptic mechanisms involved in classical eyeblink conditioning. Trends Neurosci. 1997;20:177–181. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(96)10081-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Kim JJ, Clark RE, Thompson RF. Hippocampectomy impairs the memory of recently, but not remotely, acquired trace eyeblink conditioned responses. Behav Neurosci. 1995;109:195–203. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.109.2.195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Kishimoto Y, Kawahara S, Suzuki M, Mori H, Mishina M, Kirino Y. Classical eyeblink conditioning in glutamate receptor subunit δ2 mutant mice is impaired in the delay paradigm but not in the trace paradigm. Eur J Neurosci. 2001a;13:1249–1253. doi: 10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01488.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Kishimoto Y, Hirono M, Sugiyama T, Kawahara S, Nakao K, Kishio M, Katsuki M, Yoshioka T, Kirino Y. Impaired delay but normal trace eyeblink conditioning in PLCβ4 mutant mice. NeuroReport. 2001b;12:2919–2922. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200109170-00033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Kishimoto Y, Kawahara S, Fujimichi R, Mori H, Mishina M, Kirino Y. Impairment of eyeblink conditioning in GluRδ2 mutant mice depends on the temporal overlap between conditioned and unconditioned stimuli. Eur J Neurosci. 2001c;14:1515–1521. doi: 10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01772.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Kurihara H, Hashimoto K, Kano M, Takayama C, Sakimura K, Mishina M, Inoue Y, Watanabe M. Impaired parallel fiber → Purkinje cell synapse stabilization during cerebellar development of mutant mice lacking the glutamate receptor δ2 subunit. J Neurosci. 1997;17:9613–9623. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-24-09613.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Morris RG, Garrud P, Rawlins JN, O'Keefe J. Place navigation impaired in rats with hippocampal lesion. Nature. 1982;297:681–683. doi: 10.1038/297681a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Moyer JR, Jr, Deyo RA, Disterhoft JF. Hippocampectomy disrupts trace eye-blink conditioning in rabbits. Behav Neurosci. 1990;104:243–252. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.104.2.243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Moyer JR, Jr, Thompson LT, Disterhoft JF. Trace eyeblink conditioning increases CA1 excitability in a transient and learning-specific manner. J Neurosci. 1996;16:5536–5546. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-17-05536.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Paxinos G, Franklin KBJ. The mouse brain in stereotaxic coordinates. Academic; San Diego: 2001. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Solomon PR, Gottfried KE. The septohippocampal cholinergic system and classical conditioning of the rabbit's nictitating membrane response. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1981;95:322–330. doi: 10.1037/h0077779. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Solomon PR, Solomon SD, Vander Schaaf E, Perry HE. Altered activity in the hippocampus is more detrimental to classical conditioning than removing the structure. Science. 1983;220:329–331. doi: 10.1126/science.6836277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Solomon PR, Vander Schaaf ER, Thompson RF, Weisz DJ. Hippocampus and trace conditioning of the rabbit's classically conditioned nictitating membrane response. Behav Neurosci. 1986;100:729–744. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.100.5.729. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Takatsuki K, Kawahara S, Mori H, Mishina M, Kirino Y. Scopolamine impairs eyeblink conditioning in cerebellar LTD-deficient mice. NeuroReport. 2002;13:159–162. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200201210-00036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Takehara K, Kawahara S, Takatsuki K, Kirino Y. Time-limited role of the hippocampus in the memory for trace eyeblink conditioning in mice. Brain Res. 2002;951:183–190. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(02)03159-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Thompson LT, Moyer JR, Jr, Disterhoft JF. Transient changes in excitability of rabbit CA3 neurons with a time course appropriate to support memory consolidation. J Neurophysiol. 1996;76:1836–1849. doi: 10.1152/jn.1996.76.3.1836. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Thompson RF, Kim JJ. Memory systems in the brain and localization of a memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1996;93:13438–13444. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.24.13438. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Weiss C, Bouwmeester H, Power JM, Disterhoft JF. Hippocampal lesions prevent trace eyeblink conditioning in the freely moving rat. Behav Brain Res. 1999;99:123–132. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(98)00096-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]