Abstract
The temporal pattern of drug use (pharmacokinetics) has a profound effect on the ability of self-administered cocaine to produce addiction-like behavior in rodents, and to change the brain. To further address this issue, we compared the effects of Long Access (LgA) cocaine self-administration, which is widely used to model the transition to addiction, with Intermittent Access (IntA), which is thought to better reflect the pattern of drug use in humans, on the ability of a single, self-administered injection of cocaine to increase dopamine (DA) overflow in the core of the nucleus accumbens (using in vivo microdialysis), and to produce addiction-like behavior. IntA experience was more effective than LgA in producing addiction-like behavior – a drug experience-dependent increase in motivation for cocaine assessed using behavioral economic procedures, and cue-induced reinstatement – despite much less total drug consumption. There were no group differences in basal levels of DA in dialysate [DA], but a single self-administered IV injection of cocaine increased [DA] in the core of the nucleus accumbens to a greater extent in rats with prior IntA experience than those with LgA or Limited Access (LimA) experience, and the latter two groups did not differ. Furthermore, high motivation for cocaine was associated with a high [DA] response. Thus, IntA, but not LgA, produced both incentive and DA sensitization. This is consistent with the notion that a hyper-responsive dopaminergic system may contribute to the transition from casual patterns of drug use to the problematic patterns that define addiction.
Keywords: addiction, intermittent access, cocaine, dopamine, sensitization
Graphical Abstract
We compared the effects of Long Access cocaine self-administration with Intermittent Access on the ability of self-administered cocaine to increase dopamine overflow in the core of the nucleus accumbens and to produce addiction-like behavior. IntA was more effective than LgA in producing addiction-like behavior despite much less total drug consumption and cocaine increased dopamine to a greater extent in rats with prior IntA experience than those with LgA.
INTRODUCTION
Drug self-administration procedures have proved invaluable for isolating how drug use can change the brain in ways that promote the development of addiction-like behavior (Weeks, 1962; Ahmed, 2018). However, mere drug-taking does not necessarily produce the loss of control and aberrant motivation that defines addiction. For this reason there has been considerable interest in the development of self-administration procedures and conditions (here we focus on cocaine) that most reliably produce addiction-like behavior (Ahmed & Koob, 1998; Deroche-Gamonet et al., 2004; Vanderschuren & Everitt, 2004; Ahmed, 2012; Zimmer et al., 2012). To this end, Ahmed and Koob (1998) first introduced what has become known as the Long Access (LgA) self-administration procedure, which is arguably the most widely used rodent model of addiction. During LgA, rats are typically allowed to self-administer cocaine on a Fixed Ratio schedule for 6+ hours/daily session; and relative to rats allowed short, or more limited access (LimA) to cocaine (1–2 hours/session), LgA experience is reported to result in the emergence of a number of addiction-like behaviors, including escalation of intake, increased motivation for drug, and continued drug-seeking in the face of an adverse consequence (for reviews see, Ahmed, 2012; Edwards & Koob, 2013; Kawa et al., 2019).
Studies on the neurobiological consequences of LgA cocaine experience suggest that LgA decreases dopamine (DA) neurotransmission in the nucleus accumbens core (relative to rats with more limited experience), at least when tested soon after the discontinuation of self-administration. For example, LgA experience decreases cocaine-induced inhibition of DA uptake, electrically evoked DA release when measured ex vivo, and cocaine-induced DA overflow in vivo measured with microdialysis (Ferris et al., 2011; Calipari et al., 2013, 2014). In addition, LgA cocaine self-administration experience has been associated with a progressive decrease in nucleus accumbens DA release accompanying operant responding measured using voltammetry in vivo (Willuhn et al., 2014). Although there are exceptions (e.g., Ahmed et al., 2003), these findings have been interpreted as support for the idea that cocaine addiction is due, in part, to a cocaine-induced hypo-dopaminergic state, and continued drug-seeking is motivated by a desire to overcome this DA deficiency (e.g., Caprioli et al., 2014; Koob & Volkow, 2016; U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 2016; Volkow et al., 2016).
A fundamental assumption of the LgA model is that the amount of drug exposure is the critical factor in the emergence of addiction-like behavior. As stated by Ahmed (2012), “addiction-causing neuropathological processes could be set in motion only when rats can expose themselves sufficiently to cocaine to cross the ‘threshold of addiction’ … below this critical level of cocaine exposure, there would be no drug-induced neuropathological changes, and drug use would remain under control, at least in the majority of drug-exposed individuals” (p. 110). However, studies using a more recently introduced model of the transition to cocaine addiction have challenged this assumption (Zimmer et al., 2011, 2012; Allain et al., 2015; Kawa et al., 2019).
During a LgA session rats first ‘load up’ and then maintain relatively high brain concentrations of cocaine for the duration of the 6+ hour session. However, Zimmer et al. (2012) noted that this may not reflect the pattern of cocaine use in humans, which is much more intermittent both between and within bouts of use. Thus, Zimmer et al. (2012) developed what they called the Intermittent Access (IntA) cocaine self-administration procedure to better model the repeated spikes in brain cocaine concentrations thought to reflect the pattern of use in humans within a bout of use, especially during the transition to addiction. To this end, rats were allowed 5 min of unlimited access to cocaine followed by a 25 min period when drug was not available, and this repeated throughout the session, resulting in successive spikes in brain cocaine concentrations. The important finding was that IntA experience produced greater motivation for cocaine than LgA, despite much less total drug consumption. There are now a number of reports that IntA not only increases motivation for cocaine to a greater extent than LgA, but is more effective in producing a number of other addiction-like behaviors (Kawa et al., 2016; Allain et al., 2017, 2018; Allain & Samaha, 2018; James et al., 2018; Kawa & Robinson, 2019).
Furthermore, unlike LgA, which blunts DA neurotransmission, several studies have suggested that IntA does exactly the opposite; it sensitizes DA neurotransmission. For example, IntA experience is reported to increase the ability of cocaine to inhibit DA uptake and to increase electrically-evoked DA release in brain slices (Calipari et al., 2013). Of course, this raises questions concerning the role of a hypo- vs hyper-dopaminergic state in addiction, especially given that IntA is more effective than LgA in producing addiction-like behavior. However, to date, the only studies on the effects of IntA on DA neurotransmission have been in brain slices, ex vivo. Therefore, given the theoretical implications, it is important to determine whether IntA cocaine experience produces similar effects in freely moving rats, in vivo. That was the purpose of the present experiment, in which we used in vivo microdialysis in freely moving rats to quantify changes in [DA] (and other analytes) in response to a single self-administered injection of cocaine, following LimA, LgA or IntA cocaine self-administration experience.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
A total of 50 male Sprague-Dawley rats (Envigo, Haslett, MI) weighing 250–275 g upon arrival were housed individually in a climate-controlled colony room, on a reverse 12-h light/12-h dark cycle (lights on at 20:00). Rats were housed individually to prevent one rat from irritating the surgical site and catheter port of another rat. Individually housing rats is considered a stressor and may have affected their motivation for cocaine, although the effect of social isolation in male rats on cocaine self-administration largely depends on experimental conditions (Schenk et al., 1987; Bozarth et al., 1989; Boyle et al., 1991; Phillips et al., 1994; for review see, Lu et al., 2003) and in this study it is unlikely to have differentially affected the groups. All testing was conducted during the 12-hour lights off period. After arrival, rats were given 1 week to acclimate to the colony room before surgery. Water and food were available ad libitum until 2 days before the first day of self-administration, at which point the rats were mildly food restricted to maintain a stable body weight for the remainder of the experiment (20–24 grams/day). There is evidence that maintaining adult, male rats at a stable body weight is more healthy than ad libitum feeding (Rowland, 2007). Although food restriction affects cocaine self-administration (Carroll et al., 1981; De Vry et al., 1989) it is unlikely to have differentially affected the groups in this study. Furthermore, the procedure used here did not reduce body weight, but maintained a stable body weight. All procedures were approved by the University of Michigan Committee on the Use and Care of Animals (UCUCA).
Apparatus
Behavioral testing was conducted in standard (22×18×13 cm) test chambers (Med Associates, St Albans, VT, USA) located inside sound-attenuating cabinets. A ventilating fan masked background noise. Within the test chambers, two nose poke ports were located 3 cm above the floor on the left and right side of the front wall, and one port was designated active and the other inactive (side counter-balanced across chambers). A red house light was located at the top, center of the back wall opposite the nose ports. During self-administration, intravenous cocaine infusions were delivered by a pump mounted outside the sound attenuating cabinet, through a tube connected to the rat’s catheter back port. The infusion tube was suspended into the chamber via a swivel mechanism, allowing the rat free movement. All measures were recorded using Med Associates software.
Microdialysis test sessions were conducted in separate but identical chambers to those described above. The only difference was that the swivel system in these boxes allowed for the microdialysis inlet tubing, outlet tubing, and the drug delivery tubing to be connected simultaneously.
Intravenous catheter surgery
Rats underwent intravenous catheter surgery as described previously (Crombag et al., 2000). Briefly, rats were anesthetized using ketamine hydrochloride (90 mg/kg i.p.) and xylazine (10mg/kg i.p.) and a catheter was inserted into the right jugular vein and tubing was run subcutaneously to a port located on the rat’s back. Rats were administered the analgesic carprofen (5 mg/kg s.c.) once immediately prior to surgery and once each day on the first and second days post-surgery. Following surgery, catheters were flushed daily with 0.2 ml sterile saline containing 5 mg/ml gentamicin sulfate (Vedco, MO). Catheter patency was tested periodically with intravenous injection of 0.1 ml methohexital sodium (10 mg/ml in sterile water, JHP Pharmaceuticals). If a rat did not become ataxic within 10 seconds of the injection, the catheter was considered not patent and the rat was removed from the study. No rat was tested with methohexital more than three times and the tests for patency were always done at least 16 hours before the next self-administration session.
Self-administration: acquisition
Rats were given ~7 days to recover from the catheter surgery, and then self-administration training commenced (Fig. 1a). The rats were placed into the chamber with the house light illuminated. After 2 minutes the house light was extinguished and this signaled the beginning of each session. At that time a nose poke into the active port, detected by an infrared photo beam, resulted in an intravenous infusion of cocaine hydrochloride (NIDA) dissolved in 0.9% sterile saline (0.4 mg/kg/infusion, weight of the salt, in 50 µl delivered over 2.6 seconds) on a Fixed Ratio-1 (FR-1) schedule. Each infusion was paired with the illumination of a cue light in the active nose port for 20 seconds. Nose pokes during this 20 sec period were recorded but had no consequences. An inactive nose port was also present at all times and pokes there were recorded but had no consequences. To ensure that during initial training all rats received the same amount of cocaine and cue exposure an infusion criteria (IC) procedure was used, as described previously (Saunders & Robinson, 2010). During these acquisition sessions, session length was determined by how long it took each rat to reach the predetermined number of infusions, not by an explicit time limit. Each rat had 2 sessions at IC10 (10 infusions), 3 sessions at IC20, and 4 sessions at IC40. If a rat did not reach the infusion criteria within 6 hours the session was terminated. If a rat failed to self-administer all of the infusions available in 6 hours on two consecutive days that rat was excluded from further testing. In addition, if a rat failed to make twice as many active nose pokes as inactive nose pokes on each of the last 3 acquisition sessions that rat was given up to 3 additional sessions, and if the rat did not reach criteria in those 3 sessions it was excluded from further testing. A total of 5 rats were excluded during acquisition training because they failed to reach the infusion criteria or failed to discriminate between the active nose port and the inactive nose port.
Self-administration: within-session threshold procedure
The day after the final acquisition session, all rats were trained on a within-session threshold procedure and demand curves were generated, as described previously (Bentzley et al., 2013; Kawa et al., 2016). Briefly, each session (one per day) was 110 minutes in length, FR-1 throughout, and every 10 minutes the dose of drug was decreased on a quarter logarithmic scale (1.28, 0.72, 0.40, 0.23, 0.13, 0.072, 0.040, 0.023, 0.013, 0.007, 0.004 mg/kg/infusion). During the threshold procedure, the nose port cue light was illuminated for the duration of each infusion. Rats could take another infusion immediately after the preceding one (i.e., the only time out period was during the duration of the infusion).
The threshold procedure yields a number of behavioral economic metrics. Q0 is a theoretical measure of consumption when no effort is required; that is, an inherent extrapolation of the individual’s consumption at very low prices (Hursh & Silberberg, 2008; Oleson et al., 2011; Bentzley et al., 2013). PMax is defined as the price that elicits maximum responding; i.e., the maximum price (in effort) an individual is willing to pay to maintain Q0 (Hursh, 1991; Bentzley et al., 2013). Consumption remains relatively stable at prices lower than PMax but falls rapidly at prices higher than PMax. Finally, α is a measure of demand elasticity and is equivalent to the slope of the demand curve (Hursh & Silberberg, 2008; Bentzley et al., 2013). Demand elasticity describes how susceptible the consumption of a commodity is to changes in cost. Motivation is inversely proportional to α, meaning a larger α value corresponds to lower motivation. Both PMax and α measure the extent to which an animal is willing to work to defend their preferred level of cocaine intake (Q0) that is achieved at a very low cost (in effort). Each rat was tested daily for at least four sessions and until it produced three consecutive sessions with less than 25% variation in α. For baseline data analysis α, PMax, and Q0 values (see below) were averaged over these last 3 sessions for each rat. A total of 3 rats were excluded for failure to stabilize or loss of catheter patency.
Following the baseline threshold procedure, all rats were assigned to one of three groups: Limited Access (LimA; n=10), prolonged Intermittent Access (IntA; n=20), or prolonged Long Access (LgA; n=12). Groups were assigned following the baseline threshold test so that there were no group differences in baseline measures of α, PMax, or Q0. Following the baseline threshold procedure rats in the LimA group underwent surgery to implant a microdialysis guide cannula (described below). After three days of recovery each rat in the LimA group was habituated to the microdialysis chamber in which they would be tested for ~1 hour. Then these rats were given three more IC40 self-administration sessions and at least one of these sessions was conducted in the microdialysis chamber in which they would be tested, so each rat had at least one habituation session and one self-administration session in the microdialysis chamber. Thus, the LimA group had the same amount of training during acquisition and the threshold test, plus 3 additional days at IC40, but unlike the IntA and LgA groups, had no further cocaine self-administration experience prior to the dialysis test session – their cocaine self-administration was relatively limited. Due to complications on the microdialysis test day one LimA rat had to be excluded.
Self-Administration: intermittent access or long access procedures
After their behavior stabilized on the threshold procedure, the rats in the Prolonged Access groups were given 30 self-administration sessions (Fig. 1a). These rats were trained using either an intermittent access (IntA) procedure similar to that described previously (Zimmer et al., 2012; Kawa et al., 2016) or a long access (LgA) procedure, also similar to that described previously (Ahmed & Koob, 1998). During the IntA procedure, the rats were placed into the chamber with the house light illuminated. The beginning of the first 5-min Drug-Available period started two minutes after the rats were placed into the chamber and was signaled by extinguishing the house light. During the Drug-Available period a nose poke into the active port resulted in an intravenous infusion of cocaine hydrochloride (NIDA) as during acquisition. Each infusion was paired with the illumination of a cue light in the nose port for the duration of the infusion. However, unlike during acquisition, rats could take another infusion immediately after the preceding one (i.e., the only time out period was during the 2.6 second infusion). After the 5-min Drug-Available period, the house light turned on and signaled a 25-min No-Drug Available period. Nose pokes during the No-Drug Available period were recorded but had no consequence. After 25-min, the house light was extinguished and another 5-min Drug-Available period began. Each IntA session lasted 4 hours (8 cycles of Drug-Availability).
During the LgA procedure, the rats were placed into the chamber with the house light illuminated. After two minutes the house light was extinguished and the rats were able to self-administer cocaine. Each LgA session lasted 6 hours and throughout the session a nose poke into the active port resulted in an intravenous infusion of cocaine hydrochloride (NIDA) on a FR-1 schedule, as during acquisition. Each infusion was paired with the illumination of a cue light in the nose port for 20 seconds. Nose pokes during this time were recorded but had no consequences. With the exception of this timeout period, cocaine was available to the rats throughout the 6-hour session. An inactive port was also present at all times and pokes there had no consequences.
Rats in both groups underwent one self-administration session/day for an average of 5 days/week. We varied the number and pattern of days off each week to model the intermittency between bouts of drug use seen in human cocaine users (Cohen & Sas, 1994; Simon et al., 2002) - for example, one week the rats may have had only 1 day off and then the next week the rats may have had 3 days off. However, rats were never given the day directly before a probe test off. The rats were given 25 self-administration sessions and then were tested again using the within-session threshold procedure. This probe test was identical to the baseline threshold test except that the rats were only tested for two days, and the respective values were averaged across the two test days. Two rats in the IntA group were removed from testing prior to this threshold test due to loss of catheter patency. After two threshold test sessions, the rats underwent surgery to implant a microdialysis guide cannula above the nucleus accumbens core, as described below. After three days of recovery the rats were given five additional IntA or LgA self-administration sessions. Also, each rat was first habituated to the microdialysis chamber in which they would be tested for ~1 hour and then at least one of the five additional self-administration sessions was conducted in the microdialysis chamber. Thus each rat had at least one habituation session and one self-administration session in the microdialysis chamber. Then 1–3 days after the last self-administration session these rats underwent a microdialysis test and collection session identical to the LimA rats and described below. Due to complications during surgery or the microdialysis test session 2 IntA rats and 3 LgA rats were excluded, leaving 16 and 9 successful collections, respectively.
Extinction and cue-induced reinstatement test
Following the microdialysis test session, a subset of the Prolonged Access rats was tested for the ability of the cocaine-paired discrete cue (light in the nose port) to reinstate drug-seeking (n=10 IntA; 9 LgA). The rats underwent two-hour extinction sessions (1/day) for at least 5 days and until they made less than 20 active nose pokes for two consecutive sessions. The extinction sessions consisted of placing the rats into a chamber with the house light on and the session started two minutes later. Upon the session starting, the house light turned off and remained off for the duration of the session. Responses into the nose ports during these sessions were recorded but had no consequences. The day after a rat met the extinction criterion it underwent a day of testing identical to extinction except on this day pokes in the active port were reinforced by the illumination of the cue light for 2.6-sec. After the cue-induced reinstatement test these rats were perfused to verify cannula placement.
Microdialysis
Intracranial placement of cannula for microdialysis
After their behavior stabilized on the baseline threshold procedure (LimA group) or after 25 additional self-administration sessions (IntA and LgA groups), the rats underwent surgery to implant a microdialysis guide cannula above the nucleus accumbens core. Rats were anesthetized with a ketamine xylazine cocktail as described for the catheter implantation and placed in a stereotaxic instrument (David Kopf Instruments, Tujunga, CA). A guide cannula (CMA, CMA12 Guide Cannula) was lowered such that its tip was just above the nucleus accumbens core. The coordinates were +1.6mm anterior, +/−1.6mm lateral, and −6.2mm ventral, relative to bregma (Paxinos & Watson, 2007). Hemisphere (right/left) was counter-balanced across all groups. To prevent clogging, a stainless steel stylet was inserted into the cannula. The guide cannula was secured to the skull using three metallic screws and acrylic dental cement. The rats were administered the analgesic carprofen (5 mg/kg s.c.) once immediately prior to surgery and once each day on the first and second days post-surgery. Rats were allowed at least three days of recovery before any subsequent testing.
Probe construction and test session
The microdialysis probes were custom made similar to those described previously (Pitchers et al., 2017). Briefly, two silica capillaries (75µm inner diameter; 150 µm outer diameter; TSP075150; Polymicro Technologies) were glued together and inserted into a 24-gauge stainless steel tube that served as the shaft to be inserted into the guide cannula. The portion of each capillary tube that was not inserted into the shaft was sheathed in 22-gauge stainless steel tubing and one was used for the inlet capillary and one for the outlet capillary. The capillary tip extending from the shaft was sheathed in an 18 kDa molecular weight cutoff regenerated cellulose membrane (Spectrum Labs). The tip and base of the membrane was sealed with an epoxy resin. The membrane extended 2 mm in length beyond the shaft, such that a 2 mm probe would extend into the center of the accumbens core.
In all groups, the microdialysis test session was conducted 1–3 days after the last self-administration session. Approximately 12–16 hours before the microdialysis test session, the stylet was removed from the guide cannula and a probe was inserted. The rat was placed into the microdialysis test chamber with the house light on. The probe was perfused at a rate of 0.5 µL/min with artificial cerebrospinal fluid (aCSF) overnight and the rate was increased to 1 µL/min approximately four hours prior to collection. The aCSF was comprised of 145 mM NaCl, 2.7 mM KCl, 1.0 mM MgSO4, 1.2 mM CaCl2, 1.55 mM NaHPO4, and 0.45 mM NaH2PO4. In addition, 100 nM 13C6-DA was added to the aCSF which allowed for in vivo calibration of the probes (Hershey & Kennedy, 2013) in all but a small subset of the rats (3 LimA, 4 IntA). In this latter subset of rats, recovery rates for each probe were calculated ex vivo by placing the membrane in known concentrations of DA and correcting for the concentration collected in the dialysate sample. These rats did not differ on any measures from rats tested with 13C6-DA added to the aCSF and thus were combined for analysis. Approximately four hours prior to collection the rats were also attached to the drug delivery tubing. Dialysate samples were collected every 3 minutes.
The test session and collection started with ten baseline samples (30 minutes). After 30 minutes, the house light turned off which signaled that cocaine was available, as it had during all of the previous self-administration sessions. No samples were collected until the rat had made one response in the active nose port and self-administered a single 1.25 mg/kg cocaine infusion. The light cue that had been paired with cocaine infusions during self-administration testing was not presented, to better isolate the effect of cocaine alone. As soon as the rat made a response and the infusion ended the house light turned back on and no additional cocaine was available to the rat. After accounting for the time it took for the dialysate to travel from the brain to the outlet collection, dialysis samples were collected for 1 hour (20 samples) that corresponded to the onset of the cocaine infusion. One hour after the cocaine infusion, the house light again turned off and the light in the active nose port that had previously been a conditioned stimulus paired with cocaine delivery during self-administration was flashed 14 times for 2.6 seconds per flash over a three minute period. We collected a single sample that corresponded to this three-minute cue-presentation and five more samples following the cue presentation. Nose pokes in both the active and inactive nose ports were recorded throughout the test session. In summary, 3-min samples were collected over a 30-minute baseline period, for 1-hour following a cocaine infusion, for three minutes during which the cue was presented non-contingently, and then for 15 minutes following cue presentation.
Analysis of neurochemical levels in dialysate samples using high performance liquid chromatography coupled with mass spectrometry (HPLC-MS)
All reagents, drugs, and chemicals were purchased from Sigma-Aldrich (St. Louis, MO) unless otherwise noted. Samples were analyzed using benzoyl chloride derivatization and a modified LC-MS method previously described (Song et al., 2012). Briefly, the 3 µL samples were derivatized by adding 1.5 µL of 100 mM sodium carbonate monohydrate buffer, 1.5 µL of 2% benzoyl chloride in acetonitrile, and 1.5 µL of an internal standard mixture (to improve quantification), in order, briefly vortexing between each addition. A Thermo Fisher (Waltham, MA) Vanquish UHPLC system automatically injected 5 µL of the sample onto a Phenomenex Kinetex C18 HPLC column (2.1mm X 100mm, 1.7 µm). Mobile phase A consisted of 10 mM ammonium formate and 0.15% formic acid. Mobile phase B was pure acetonitrile. Analytes were detected using positive electrospray ionization with a Thermo Fisher TSQ Quantum Ultra triple-quadrupole mass spectrometer operating in multiple-reaction monitoring (MRM) mode.
Inclusion of 13C6 dopamine in the aCSF perfusate allowed us to calculate an extraction fraction (Ed) for each sample (Hershey & Kennedy, 2013). The Ed (Ed=1-(Cin/Cout) is the ratio of the amount of the isotope that exits the probe to the amount retained in the dialysate sample. Absolute extracellular concentrations of dopamine were determined by dividing the dialysate concentration of dopamine by the Ed value. Finally, in a subset of rats we switched the perfusate to aCSF that lacked 13C6 dopamine at the conclusion of the test session. After allowing this aCSF to run for õne hour we collected several samples in the same manner as the samples collected during the test. We analyzed these samples and compared them to samples from the baseline portion of the test session. This analysis, along with comparisons between rats that did not have 13C6-DA added to the aCSF at any point, confirmed that there were no effects of infusing 100 nM 13C6-DA on endogenous dopamine levels (Fig. 3a).
Histological Analysis
After the microdialysis test session, rats were anesthetized using sodium pentobarbital (270 mg/kg; i.p.) and perfused intracardially with 50 mL of 0.9% saline, followed by 500 mL of 4% paraformaldehyde in 0.1 M phosphate buffer (PB). After being perfused, brains were removed, post-fixed in the same paraformaldehyde solution for 2 hours, then immersed in 20% sucrose and 0.01% sodium azide in 0.1 M PB for 48 hours at 4° C. Coronal sections (40 µm) were cut with a freezing microtome (SM 2000R; Leica), collected in PB, and mounted on to a slide immediately. Sections were imaged at 4x magnification using a Leica DM400B digital microscope to verify cannula placement.
Statistical analysis
Linear mixed-models (LMM) analyses were used for all behavioral repeated measures data. The best-fitting model of repeated measures covariance was determined by the lowest Akaike information criterion score (West et al., 2007). Depending on the model selected, the degrees of freedom may have been adjusted to a non-integer value. Data for the α measure was not normally distributed and therefore all statistical tests involving α were run on log transformed data, consistent with previous reports (Bentzley et al., 2014). Planned post-hoc contrasts (and Bonferroni corrections) were done to compare between the different self-administration procedures and within the Prolonged Access groups across the two test periods. Active and inactive nose pokes from the cue-induced reinstatement test were compared to the last day of extinction and between the IntA and LgA groups. Similar LMM analysis and planned post-hoc analysis were used to analyze neurochemical levels from the microdialysis test session. Statistical significance was set at p<0.05.
RESULTS
LgA resulted in much greater cocaine consumption than IntA, but rats in both groups escalated their intake to a similar extent
The overall timeline of the experiment is shown in Fig. 1a. Fig. 1b shows the acquisition of cocaine self-administration as a function of Infusion Criterion for all rats, prior to group assignment. Across sessions the number of responses in the active nose port increased (effect of IC, F(2,40.4)=38.7, p<0.001), and the number of responses in the inactive nose port decreased (effect of IC, F(2,75.1)=3.5, p=0.04). After group assignments there were no group differences on any measure of acquisition.
A subset of rats was given 30 self-administration sessions using either the IntA or LgA procedure. Rats in the IntA group quickly learned to discriminate between the Drug-Available and No-Drug periods, as previously reported (data not shown; see Kawa et al., 2016). When cocaine intake per session from the IntA and LgA rats was analyzed together there was a main effect of test session (F(29,70.7)=4.8, p<0.001). Further analysis confirmed that when each group was analyzed separately both IntA (p=0.01, Fig. 1c) and LgA (p=0.02; Fig. 1d) rats escalated their intake as a function of increasing self-administration experience. Relatedly, the rate at which consumption increased did not differ between the groups (group X session interaction, F(29,70.7)=1.2, p=0.24). There was, however, a large group difference in the total amount of cocaine consumed (effect of group, F(1,92.3)=823, p<0.001; Fig. 1e). On average, LgA rats consumed ~2.8x more cocaine than IntA rats.
Prolonged IntA and LgA experience had different effects on motivated behavior
All rats underwent a baseline threshold test to quantify their motivation to self-administer cocaine following only limited self-administration experience (Fig. 2). Values for each behavioral economic metric were averaged from the last three days of the baseline threshold test. Within each measure, there were no differences across the three days nor did the variability differ across days. Group assignments (LimA, IntA, LgA) were made following this test, such that there were no differences between the groups on any of the measures derived from this test. The rats that were placed into the IntA and LgA groups then underwent 25 self-administration sessions before another threshold probe test to determine how motivation changed as a function of IntA or LgA experience, relative to baseline. For this second threshold test, values for each behavioral economic metric were averaged from the two test days. Within each measure, there were no differences across the two days nor did the variability differ across days. All statistical comparisons to baseline were within-subject.
The metric α is a measure of the elasticity of the demand curve generated during the within-session threshold procedure, and is inversely proportional to motivation (Bentzley et al., 2013; Fig. 2a). Prolonged IntA and LgA experience had different effects on α, as indicated by a significant group X probe test interaction (F(1,69)=7.2, p=0.009). Post-hoc within-subject analysis revealed that IntA experience decreased α (increased motivation) (p=0.02) whereas LgA experience did not significantly change α (p=0.12). On the second probe test, IntA rats had a lower α than LgA rats (effect of group, p=0.04), indicating that rats with IntA experience were more motivated to obtain cocaine.
The metric PMax is a measure of motivation that reflects the maximum price an individual is willing to pay (in effort) to obtain a reinforcer (Bentzley et al., 2013; Fig. 2b). Again, prolonged IntA and LgA had different effects on PMax, as indicated by a significant group X probe test interaction (F(1,33.1)=10.4, p=0.003). Post-hoc within-subject analysis revealed that IntA experience increased PMax (p=0.001), and LgA experience did not significantly change PMax (p=0.27). On the second probe test IntA rats had a higher PMax than LgA rats (effect of group, p=0.005), indicating that they were more motivated to obtain cocaine, consistent with α.
Another metric derived from the threshold procedure is Q0, which measures the preferred level of cocaine consumption when cost is nil (Bentzley et al., 2013; Fig. 2c). Again there was a significant group X probe test interaction (F(1,28.7)=7.7, p=0.009) indicating that IntA and LgA had different effects on Q0. Q0 differed from α and PMax as post-hoc within-subject analysis revealed that LgA experience increased Q0 (p=0.01) but IntA experience had no effect on Q0 (p=0.25). Finally, LgA rats had higher Q0 than IntA rats after prolonged self-administration (effect of group, p=0.03). These differential effects of LgA and IntA on Q0 are consistent with several reports (Oleson & Roberts, 2009; Bentzley et al., 2014; Kawa et al., 2016; James et al., 2018; Singer et al., 2018).
Representative demand curves generated from the threshold procedure are shown from a LimA rat on the baseline threshold test (Fig. 2d), and from a LgA rat (Fig. 2e) and an IntA rat (Fig. 2f) on the second threshold test.
Infusion of stable-isotope labeled dopamine did not affect endogenous dopamine levels
Use of extraction fraction (Ed) to calculate in vivo concentration accounts for possible differences in recovery associated with the probe and brain environment in different rats. In vivo calibration therefore improves the accuracy of such measures and allows comparison between rats. 100 nM stable-isotope labeled dopamine (13C6-DA) was included in the aCSF that was perfused during the microdialysis test session to calculate an Ed for each sample (see methods; Hershey & Kennedy, 2013). As a control, we determined if the infusion of this concentration of 13C6-DA affected DA levels in dialysate [DA]. For this control measurement, the input solution was switched to aCSF that lacked 13C6-DA at the end of each dialysis test session and after allowing the lines to clear of any 13C6-DA aCSF three more dialysate samples were collected to compare to the last samples collected with 13C6-DA aCSF. As we have reported previously (Hershey & Kennedy, 2013) the presence of 13C6-DA did not affect [DA] (Fig. 3a). In addition, the calculated Ed did not differ between groups nor did they change across the dialysis test session (all p-values>0.1; Fig. 3b). These results provide confidence that the calibrated [DA] concentrations reported can be compared between rats and groups.
Prolonged IntA (but not LgA) sensitized cocaine-induced dopamine overflow
Following the collection of baseline dialysate for 30 min, rats with LimA, prolonged IntA, or prolonged LgA experience were allowed to self-administer a single IV injection of 1.25 mg/kg cocaine (see Fig. 4a for timeline), and dialysate was then collected for an additional 60 min. There were no group differences in the latency to self-administer the cocaine infusion as most rats nose poked as soon as the house light turned off signaling drug availability. We first analyzed the average concentration of [DA] in the ten baseline samples and the first ten post-cocaine samples in all 3 groups (Fig. 4b). When analyzing all 20 samples, there was a main effect of group (F(2,326)=10.2, p<0.001) and the cocaine infusion increased [DA], relative to baseline, in all groups (main effect of cocaine, F(1,331)=8.93, p=0.003; Fig. 4b). Planned post-hoc analysis revealed that the main effect of group was due to a differential response to the cocaine infusion. That is, there were no group differences in baseline [DA] (all p-values>0.1) but following the cocaine infusion, [DA] increased to a greater extent in IntA rats, relative to LgA rats (p<0.001) and LimA rats (p=0.004). The latter two groups did not differ (p=0.21). The largest [DA] response to cocaine reliably occurred within the first three samples (9 minutes) after the infusion, but there was individual variation in the time that elapsed from infusion to peak response. To better visualize the peak response to cocaine in all individuals Figure 4c shows [DA] in all three groups when each rat’s peak response was aligned.
Given that there were no group differences in baseline [DA], we averaged the baseline values together for each group to determine the percent change from baseline produced by cocaine (Fig. 4d). For this analysis we aligned each rat’s peak [DA] response. There was a main effect of group (F(2,672)=6.04, p=0.003) and the cocaine infusion increased [DA] in all groups (F(1, 673)=40, p<0.001). Further, the cocaine infusion increased [DA] to a different extent in the three groups evidenced by a group X cocaine interaction (F(2,672)=6.04, p=0.003). Planned post-hoc analysis revealed that the cocaine infusion increased [DA] to a greater extent in IntA rats than LgA rats (p<0.001) or LimA rats (p=0.016), and the latter two groups did not differ (p=0.1), consistent with the analysis of [DA] that was not normalized to baseline.
Active nose pokes that were made in the hour following the single self-administered cocaine infusion during the microdialysis test session were recorded and analyzed as a measure of cocaine-induced drug seeking. A one-way Anova comparing the three groups revealed no effect of group on nose pokes, although there was a trend towards IntA rats making more responses than LimA or LgA rats (effect of group, F(2)=2.7, p=0.08; Fig. 4e).
IntA rats showed greater cue-induced reinstatement than LgA rats
The day after the microdialysis test session, a subset of the rats in the IntA group and the LgA groups underwent extinction training followed by a test for cue-induced reinstatement of cocaine-seeking (conditioned reinforcement). There were no group differences during extinction training in the number of responses made or the number of sessions required to reach extinction criteria (all p-values>0.1; Fig. 5a). On the test day, when responding was reinforced by the presentation of the cue that had previously been paired with cocaine, both IntA and LgA rats reinstated their drug-seeking relative to extinction levels (F(1,20.1)=100, p<0.001; Fig. 5b), specifically at the active nose port (effect of inactive nose port X session, F(1,18.1)=2.3, p=0.15). However, rats with IntA experience showed more robust cue-induced reinstatement than rats with LgA experience (effect of group, F(1,20.1)=22, p<0.001).
Cocaine-seeking on multiple tests of addiction-like behavior predicted cocaine-evoked dopamine overflow
Given the role of DA in motivated behavior we asked whether [DA] in response to self-administered cocaine correlated with cocaine-seeking behavior and/or motivation for cocaine. Fig. 6 shows that the magnitude of the increase in [DA], as a percent of baseline, on the microdialysis test day was positively correlated with cocaine-seeking on the dialysis test day, assessed by the number of nose pokes into the active port (R2=0.14, p=0.03; Fig. 6a). There was a similar correlation between cocaine-induced [DA] and motivation for cocaine (PMax) assessed on the most recent threshold test (baseline test for LimA; second test for IntA/LgA) (R2=0.32, p<0.001; Fig. 6b). This correlation was evident even when the analysis was restricted to the IntA group (R2=0.29, p=0.03). There was a similar correlation between [DA] and α (R2=0.13, p=0.04; data not shown). In contrast, Q0 did not correlate with [DA] when all groups were combined (R2=0.07, p=0.17; Fig. 6c) or within any individual group (all p-values>0.1).
IntA experience reliably produces multiple addiction-like behaviors and this has been shown to be particularly robust in susceptible individuals (Kawa et al., 2016; Singer et al., 2018). In an effort to quantify this variation in behavior, we developed an ‘IntA Addiction Score’, similar to the oft-used addiction-criteria analysis (Deroche-Gamonet et al., 2004; Kawa et al., 2016; Singer et al., 2018), but differing in the behaviors that were analyzed. Rats that were trained with the IntA procedure were separated into those that had a score of 2/3 (“High”) and those that had a score of 0/1 (“Low”). Briefly, a rat was given an ‘Addiction Score’ point if it was within the top third of the population on a given measure. The measures used here were α, active nose pokes during the No-Drug period of IntA, and active cocaine-induced nose pokes during the microdialysis test session (Fig. 7; see discussion for rationale of using these measures). There were 9 rats that scored 0/1 points (“Low”) and 7 rats that scored 2/3 points (“High”). Not surprisingly, the Low and High groups differed in their performance on the behavioral measures for which they were classified (Fig. 7 a, b & c). In addition, the Low and High groups differed in PMax but did not differ in Q0 (data not shown). Importantly, High rats showed greater cocaine-evoked [DA] than Low rats (p=0.05; Fig. 7d). Finally, when High IntA rats (the most “addict-like”) were compared to LimA and LgA rats (that largely did not show addiction-like behavior), the cocaine infusion increased [DA] to a greater extent in High IntA rats (group X cocaine interaction, F(2,484)=19.5, p<0.001; Fig. 7e).
Self-administered cocaine increased extracellular glutamate and 3-MT levels
The HPLC-MS technique used here allows for the analysis of a number of analytes, in addition to DA (Table 1). There were no group differences in the baseline levels of any of the analytes quantified, although there was a trend towards group differences in baseline glutamate levels (effect of group, F(2,328)=2.7, p=0.08; post-hoc analysis revealed this to be driven primarily by elevated baseline levels in LimA rats). Therefore, all values were normalized to baseline and cocaine-induced changes relative to baseline were calculated. Self-administered cocaine increased extracellular glutamate levels relative to baseline in all groups (effect of cocaine, F(1,671)=3.97, p=0.04), but there were no group differences (p>0.1). In addition, self-administered cocaine increased 3-MT, a DA metabolite formed extracellularly, in all groups (effect of cocaine, F(1,581)=10.5, p<0.001). Further, cocaine increased 3-MT to a greater extent in IntA rats than LgA or LimA rats (group X cocaine interaction, F(2,580)=4.93, p=0.008; post-hoc analysis revealed this to be driven by IntA rats). We also analyzed extracellular GABA, ACh, DOPAC, and HVA levels but none of these differed between groups or changed significantly following cocaine.
Table 1.
Analytes | Group Differences | Peak cocaine-induced change from baseline | Std. Error |
---|---|---|---|
Glutamate | No | +32%* | +/− 10.3% |
GABA | No | +1.4% | +/−4.6% |
ACh | No | −10% | +/−33% |
3-MT | Yes | IntA: +98%* Lim&LgA: +25%* |
+/−26% +/−9.1% |
DOPAC | No | −9.9% | +/− 4.1% |
HVA | No | +0.5% | +/−5.2% |
indicates a significant (p<0.05) change in analyte concentration following the cocaine infusion relative to baseline.
Presentation of the cocaine-paired cue did not affect extracellular neurochemical levels
One hour after the self-administered cocaine infusion, the conditioned stimulus that had been paired with cocaine infusions during self-administration training was presented non-contingently for three minutes. Presentation of the conditioned stimulus did not produce any detectable changes in any of the analytes that we measured (data not shown; see discussion).
DISCUSSION
The purpose of this study was to compare the ability of prolonged LgA and IntA cocaine self-administration experience to produce addiction-like behavior (relative to LimA), and how this influenced the ability of self-administered cocaine to change the concentration of DA in dialysate [DA] in the nucleus accumbens core in vivo. The main findings were: 1. As expected, LgA resulted in much greater total cocaine consumption than IntA. 2. Both IntA and LgA produced escalation of intake with increasing self-administration experience. 3. IntA (but not LgA) experience increased motivation for cocaine, as indicated by a decrease in α and increase in PMax. 4. IntA rats showed greater cue-induced reinstatement of cocaine-seeking than LgA rats. 5. LgA (but not IntA) experience increased the preferred level of cocaine intake when no effort was required (Q0). 6. There were no group differences in the basal levels of [DA], but a single self-administered IV injection of cocaine increased [DA] in the core of the nucleus accumbens to a greater extent in rats with prior IntA experience than those with LgA or LimA experience, and the latter two groups did not differ. 7. Across all groups high motivation for cocaine was associated with a greater [DA] response. 8. There were no group differences in dialysate concentrations of glutamate, GABA, ACh, DOPAC or HVA, although cocaine increased 3-MT to a greater extent in IntA than LimA or LgA rats, consistent with the effects on DA.
IntA experience was more effective in producing addiction-like behavior than LgA experience
Since its introduction in 1998 (Ahmed & Koob, 1998) the LgA procedure has been widely adopted to model the transition to cocaine addiction in rats, because it was thought to be especially effective in producing a number of addiction-like behaviors, relative to short or more limited access procedures (for reviews see, Ahmed, 2012; Edwards and Koob 2013). In their 1998 paper Ahmed and Koob reported that LgA, but not shorter access, resulted in escalation of intake. Since that time it has also been reported that, relative to shorter access, rats with LgA experience are more motivated to seek cocaine (Paterson & Markou, 2003; Wee et al., 2008), take more cocaine in the face of adverse consequences (Xue et al., 2012; Bentzley et al., 2014; also see Vanderschuren & Everitt, 2004), and show greater reinstatement of cocaine-seeking behavior following extinction (Mantsch et al., 2004, 2008; Ahmed & Cador, 2006; Kippin et al., 2006). As indicated by the excerpt cited in the Introduction from Ahmed (2012), it has been suggested that the critical factor necessary for the emergence of escalation and other addiction-like behavior is the amount of drug consumed. As put by Edwards and Koob (2013), “excessive drug exposure likely remains an indispensable element driving the development of addiction”. However, the findings presented here add to a growing literature indicating that this is not the case.
IntA self-administration results in much less total cocaine consumption than LgA. Yet as reported here, IntA also produced escalation of intake and was more effective than LgA in increasing motivation for cocaine and in producing cue-induced reinstatement of cocaine-seeking behavior. These findings are consistent with a number of recent studies that also report IntA produces escalation of intake, heightened motivation for cocaine, continued cocaine-seeking in the face of an adverse consequence, continued cocaine-seeking when it is not available, and greater cue-induced reinstatement (Zimmer et al., 2012; Kawa et al., 2016; Allain & Samaha, 2018; Allain et al., 2018; James et al., 2018; Singer et al., 2018; Kawa & Robinson, 2019). Collectively, these studies have established that the consumption of the large amount of cocaine associated with LgA is not necessary for the development of addiction-like behavior and other pharmacokinetic factors appear to be more important (for reviews see Allain et al., 2015; Kawa et al., 2019).
The failure of LgA experience to increase motivation for cocaine in the present study is inconsistent with several previous studies using either the same behavioral economic indicators used here (Zimmer et al., 2012; Bentzley et al., 2014) or Progressive Ratio (PR) tests (Paterson & Markou, 2003; Wee et al., 2008). However, the effects of LgA reported in these studies were often only assessed at a single time point and compared to shorter access, and did not involve within-subject comparisons. In the studies that measured how motivation changed with increasing LgA experience (Bentzley et al., 2014) the effects were modest relative to the changes that occur following IntA. Our findings are consistent with other reports that LgA experience does not increase motivation for cocaine, as assessed with either behavioral economic metrics (Oleson & Roberts, 2009) or PR tests (Liu et al., 2005; Quadros & Miczek, 2009; Willuhn et al., 2014 supplementary). In addition, it has been reported that changes in motivation produced by LgA experience are very transient, lasting for only a few days after the last self-administration session (Bentzley et al., 2014; James et al., 2018), whereas the increased motivation produced by IntA experience is long-lasting - still evident after 50 days of abstinence (James et al., 2018). In summary, evidence that LgA increases motivation for cocaine is somewhat mixed, whereas IntA has been consistently reported to do so.
When allowed to self-administer cocaine under low Fixed Ratio (FR) schedules of reinforcement, rats generally titrate their responding to achieve a preferred brain concentration of cocaine, which they defend within a wide range of doses (Gerber & Wise, 1989; Ahmed & Koob, 1999; Lynch & Carroll, 2001). This preferred level of consumption was quantified here by the metric Q0 - the preferred level of consumption when cost is nil. Q0 presumably represents the brain level of cocaine that produces some optimal desired effect, such that neither more nor less cocaine is better. Some have referred to Q0 as a “hedonic set-point” (Bentzley et al., 2013), although “settling-point” may be more appropriate (see Berridge, 2004). Of course, it is not possible to know if Q0 actually reflects subjective hedonic effects in rodents. Nevertheless, LgA experience does increase the preferred level of cocaine consumption, as reported here and by others (Oleson & Roberts, 2009; Bentzley et al., 2014; James et al., 2018). The present results suggest, therefore, that LgA experience produces tolerance to whatever desired effect of cocaine is defended as price increases, without any change in motivation for cocaine. In contrast, IntA increases motivation for cocaine without any concomitent change in cocaine’s desired effects. Although speculative, this may reflect a dissociation between cocaine “wanting” and “liking” (Robinson & Berridge, 1993; Berridge & Robinson, 2016). It also suggests that cocaine intake may escalate with both LgA and IntA, but for very different reasons – because of tolerance to cocaine’s desired effect in the case of LgA and incentive-sensitization in the case of IntA (Kawa et al., 2016; Kawa & Robinson, 2019). The idea that consummatory and motivational aspects of behavior are psychologically (and neurobiologically) dissociable has been suggested frequently (e.g., Nicola & Deadwyler, 2000; Sharpe & Samson, 2001; Oleson et al., 2011; Guillem et al., 2014).
Neither LgA nor IntA experience altered basal dopamine
A decrease in basal DA levels has been reported when testing occurred soon after the discontinuation of high-dose and/or high-intake cocaine self-administration procedures (Mateo et al., 2005; Ferris et al., 2011). However, in the present study neither LgA nor IntA experience had any effect on basal [DA]. Also, we included 13C6 dopamine in the aCSF, which allowed us to calculate an extraction fraction for each sample, and thus more accurately estimate basal [DA]. There were no group differences in the extraction fraction, thus bolstering our conclusion that neither LgA nor IntA changed basal [DA] (relative to LimA). This result is consistent with other reports that LgA experience does not alter baseline [DA], relative to rats with shorter access to cocaine (Ahmed et al., 2003) or drug-naïve rats (Calipari et al., 2014). In addition, baseline [DA] did not correlate with any of our measures of addiction-like behavior, consistent with other studies (Hurd et al., 1989; Ahmed et al., 2003).
IntA, but not LgA, sensitizes cocaine-evoked dopamine overflow
There have been very few studies on the neurobiological consequences of IntA experience, and those available have all involved ex vivo measures. Most relevant to the present study are reports by Calipari et al. (2013, 2015) that IntA experience sensitizes stimulated DA release from the nucleus accumbens core in tissue slices, relative to naïve rats or rats with a history of shorter access self-administration, and also increases the ability of cocaine to inhibit DA uptake. The main purpose of the present experiment was to determine if a similar sensitization of DA neurotransmission is present in awake, behaving rats. Following prolonged IntA experience a single self-administered cocaine infusion, given in the absence of the cocaine cue, produced a greater increase in extracellular [DA] in the accumbens core than following either LgA or LimA experience, and these latter two groups did not differ.
Furthermore, the magnitude of the [DA] response predicted motivation for cocaine, as assessed by a number of measures, including PMax, α, and cocaine-seeking on the microdialysis test day. In addition, the [DA] response to cocaine was greatest in rats with IntA experience that scored the highest on an ‘IntA Addiction Score’. The ability of cocaine-evoked [DA] to predict multiple addiction-like behaviors across various behavioral tests, both across all groups and within the IntA group, demonstrates that the magnitude of DA efflux in response to cocaine is an important factor in driving the expression of addiction-like behavior across all groups and not simply a function of self-administration history. One alternative interpretation of the microdialysis DA data is that the increased [DA] efflux in IntA rats, relative to LimA and LgA rats, was the result of increased locomotion following the cocaine infusion. However, we do not believe this is the case as the magnitude of [DA] efflux following the cocaine infusion was capable of predicting a number of addiction-like behaviors on a variety of behavioral procedures and changes in locomotion do not necessarily result in changes in [DA] levels in the nucleus accumbens (Paulson & Robinson, 1994).
Active nose pokes during the no-drug period of IntA, α, and cocaine-induced active nose pokes during the microdialysis test were used for the ‘IntA Addiction Score’ analysis as they represent a diverse array of addiction-like behaviors, taken from different test procedures, that are relevant to the DSM V criteria for diagnosing substance use disorder in humans. Thus they represent a good index of general addiction-like behavior. There was similar variation in behavior between α and PMax within IntA rats and replacing α with PMax in the analysis yielded very similar results to those presented in figure 7. We chose to show α for this analysis, as it is normalized with respect to Q0 and thus slightly more informative in regard to differences in motivation than PMax alone (Bentzley et al., 2013, 2014).
These findings establish that IntA, a cocaine self-administration procedure that is especially effective in producing incentive-sensitization and addiction-like behavior, also sensitizes the dopaminergic response to cocaine. IntA also has been reported to be especially effective in producing a number of other neurobiological effects related to the development of addiction-like behavior, including dysregulation of mGluR2/3 receptor function (Allain et al., 2017), elevated BDNF levels (Gueye et al., 2018), and increased activity in orexin/hypocretin neurons (James et al., 2018).
In contrast with the dopaminergic sensitization produced by IntA experience, there are a number of reports that LgA does the opposite – decreases DA function, relative to shorter access self-administration procedures. For example, following LgA, or other high dose cocaine procedures, the ability of cocaine to inhibit DA uptake, or for electrical stimulation to evoke DA release from the accumbens core, is decreased in tissue slices, as is cocaine-evoked [DA] overflow measured with microdialysis in vivo (Ferris et al., 2011; Calipari et al., 2013, 2014; Siciliano et al., 2016). It may seem surprising, therefore, that in the present study a single, self-administered IV injection of cocaine increased [DA] to the same extent in rats with LgA or LimA experience – that is, there was no evidence for tolerance. It is not clear what accounts for the discrepancy – e.g., ex vivo vs. in vivo measures, experimenter-administered IP cocaine challenge vs. self-administered IV injection, measurement technique, or other methodological differences. However, the present results are consistent with one other study on the effects of LgA experience on [DA] measured with microdialysis in vivo. Ahmed (2003) reported that, relative to a shorter access self-administration procedure, LgA did not decrease the [DA] response in the nucleus accumbens to either experimenter-administered IV injections of cocaine, or cocaine self-administration. Thus, it seems that LgA experience does not consistently decrease DA activity. It should also be noted that effects may vary considerably as a function of how long after the discontinuation of self-administration rats are tested (e.g., Ferrario et al., 2005; Siciliano et al., 2016). In addition, Willuhn et al. (2014) reported that the magnitude of a phasic DA response in the NAc following a nosepoke that delivered cocaine progressively decreased with increasing LgA experience and was associated with the emergence of a DA signal in dorsal striatum, as measured with fast scan cyclic voltammetry. However, this phasic DA response peaked approximately 5 sec after a nosepoke, which is too soon to reflect the pharmacological effects of cocaine (Stuber et al., 2005; Aragona et al., 2008), and therefore, may not be relevant to the studies discussed above.
In addition to increasing extracellular [DA] levels, a single, self-administered cocaine infusion increased glutamate levels. Cocaine increases extracellular glutamate levels in the nucleus accumbens (Smith et al., 1995; Reid et al., 1997). Interestingly, prolonged self-adminsitration experience may decrease baseline glutamate levels, indicated by a trend towards elevated baseline levels in LimA rats compared to LgA and IntA rats, which did not differ. This is consistent with at least one previous study (Pierce et al., 1996), although in this study cocaine-evoked glutatmate was greater in rats that had developed behavioral sensitization (Pierce et al., 1996) and we did not see any group differences in cocaine-evoked glutamate here. Nevertheless, this may be an interesting finding for future investigation.
Here we also reported that a single, self-administered cocaine infusion increased 3-MT levels in all groups and 3-MT was increased to a greater extent in rats with IntA experience. DA is catalyzed to 3-MT by the enzyme cathechol-o-methyl transferase which is primarily localized extracellularly (Axelrod, 1966; Marsden et al., 1972). Therefore manipulations that increase extracellular DA also increase 3-MT levels (Wood et al., 1987). That 3-MT levels were highest in rats with IntA experience supports the finidng that cocaine increased extracellular DA to the greatest extent in these rats, compared to rats with LimA or LgA experience.
One hour after the cocaine injection the cue that had been associated with cocaine was presented and we expected to see a conditioned DA response. But the cocaine cue had no effect in any group, on any neurochemical measure. It is not clear why this was the case, because the cue certainly had motivational properties, as indicated by the cue reinstatement test. However, if there were only a very brief (seconds) and relatively small response it may not have been detectable over the 3 min sampling period used here, and other techniques may be required to study the effects of IntA on such conditioned responses. This interpretation is supported by data showing that cues presented non-contingently elicit far less drug-seeking than cues presented contingently upon a drug-seeking response (Kruzich et al., 2001). Indeed we did not see a behavioral response to the non-contingent cue presentation during the microdialysis test session.
Conclusions
It has been suggested that addiction is characterized by a hypodopaminergic, anhedonic state, and compulsive motivation to seek and take cocaine derives from a desire to overcome this DA deficiency (Dackis & Gold, 1985; Koob & Le Moal, 1997, 2001; Blum et al., 2015; Volkow et al., 2016). Reports that LgA cocaine self-administration experience reduces DA function have been interpreted as support for this view, especially given that LgA was thought to best model changes in brain and behavior that lead to a transition from casual patterns of drug use to the escalated use that characterizes addiction. However, as reviewed above, the evidence that LgA produces a hypodopaminergic state is equivocal, as is evidence that it increases motivation for cocaine. In addition, studies using the more recently developed IntA self-administration procedure support a different theory. The IntA procedure was initially developed because it is thought to better model intermittent patterns of cocaine use in humans, especially during the transition to addiction (Zimmer et al., 2012; Allain et al., 2015). There is now considerable evidence that IntA produces incentive-sensitization, and is more effective than LgA in producing addiction-like behavior (Kawa et al., 2016; Allain et al., 2017, 2018; Allain & Samaha, 2018; James et al., 2018; Kawa & Robinson, 2019). Although the evidence is limited, and more work is required, the available evidence indicates that IntA experience also sensitizes DA function (Calipari et al., 2013, 2015), including the ability of cocaine to increase extracellular DA in vivo, as reported here.
In conclusion, studies utilizing the IntA procedure are more consistent with the view that the pathological motivation to seek and take cocaine in addiction is due, at least in part, to a hyper-responsive dopaminergic state, consistent with an incentive-sensitization view of addiction (Robinson & Berridge, 1993; Berridge & Robinson, 2016). Of course, a syndrome as complex as addiction is not going to be reducible to changes in a single neurotransmitter system, or even a single psychological process, and it remains to be seen what other neuropsychological functions are altered by IntA experience (e.g., Allain et al., 2017; Gueye et al., 2018; James et al., 2018). Nevertheless, the growing evidence concerning the importance of pharmacokinetic factors in promoting the development of addiction suggests these need to be given greater consideration in preclinical models of addiction (Allain et al., 2015; Kawa et al., 2019).
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
This research was supported by grants PO1 DA031656, RO1 DA044204 and T32 DA007281 from NIDA to TER and RO1 EB003320 to RTK.
Abbreviations:
- IntA
Intermittent Access
- LgA
Long Access
- LimA
Limited Access
- DA
dopamine
- IC
infusion criteria
Footnotes
CONFLICT OF INTEREST
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
DATA ACCESSIBILITY
Raw data generated by these experiments have been stored on a University of Michigan Server. Data and statistical analyses will be made available upon request.
REFERENCES
- Ahmed SH (2012) The science of making drug-addicted animals. Neuroscience, 211, 107–125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ahmed SH (2018) “A walk on the wild side” of addiction: the history and significance of animal models. In Pickard H & Ahmed SH (eds), Routledge Handbook on Philosophy and Science of Addiction Routledge New York, pp. 192–204. [Google Scholar]
- Ahmed SH & Cador M (2006) Dissociation of Psychomotor Sensitization from Compulsive Cocaine Consumption. Neuropsychopharmacology, 31, 563–571. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ahmed SH & Koob GF (1998) Transition from moderate to excessive drug intake: change in hedonic set point. Science, 282, 298–300. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ahmed SH & Koob GF (1999) Long-lasting increase in the set point for cocaine self-administration after escalation in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 146, 303–312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ahmed SH, Lin D, Koob GF, & Parsons LH (2003) Escalation of cocaine self-administration does not depend on altered cocaine-induced nucleus accumbens dopamine levels. J. Neurochem, 86, 102–113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Allain F, Bouayad-Gervais K, & Samaha A-N (2018) High and escalating levels of cocaine intake are dissociable from subsequent incentive motivation for the drug in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 235, 317–328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Allain F, Minogianis E-A, Roberts DCS, & Samaha A-N (2015) How fast and how often: The pharmacokinetics of drug use are decisive in addiction. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev, 56, 166–179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Allain F, Roberts DCS, Lévesque D, & Samaha A-N (2017) Intermittent intake of rapid cocaine injections promotes robust psychomotor sensitization, increased incentive motivation for the drug and mGlu2/3 receptor dysregulation. Neuropharmacology, 117, 227–237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Allain F & Samaha A-N (2018) Revisiting long-access versus short-access cocaine self-administration in rats: intermittent intake promotes addiction symptoms independent of session length. Addict. Biol, in press DOI: 10.1111/adb.12629 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aragona BJ, Cleaveland NA, Stuber GD, Day JJ, Carelli RM, & Wightman RM (2008) Preferential Enhancement of Dopamine Transmission within the Nucleus Accumbens Shell by Cocaine Is Attributable to a Direct Increase in Phasic Dopamine Release Events. J. Neurosci, 28, 8821–8831. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Axelrod J (1966) Methylation reactions in the formation and metabolism of catecholamines and other biogenic amines. Pharmacol. Rev, 18, 95–113. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bentzley BS, Fender KM, & Aston-Jones G (2013) The behavioral economics of drug self-administration: A review and new analytical approach for within-session procedures. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 226, 113–125. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bentzley BS, Jhou TC, & Aston-Jones G (2014) Economic demand predicts addiction-like behavior and therapeutic efficacy of oxytocin in the rat. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci, 111, 11822–11827. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berridge KC (2004) Motivation concepts in behavioral neuroscience. Physiol. Behav, 81, 179–209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berridge KC & Robinson TE (2016) Liking, wanting, and the incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Am. Psychol, 71, 670–679. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blum K, Thanos PK, Oscar-Berman M, Febo M, Baron D, Badgaiyan RD, Gardner E, Demetrovics Z, Fahlke C, Haberstick BC, Dushaj K, & Gold MS (2015) Dopamine in the Brain: Hypothesizing Surfeit or Deficit Links to Reward and Addiction. J. Reward Defic. Syndr, 1, 95–104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boyle AE, Gill K, Smith BR, & Amit Z (1991) Differential effects of an early housing manipulation on cocaine-induced activity and self-administration in laboratory rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav, 39, 269–274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bozarth MA, Murray A, & Wise RA (1989) Influence of housing conditions on the acquisition of intravenous heroin and cocaine self-administration in rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav, 33, 903–907. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calipari ES, Ferris MJ, & Jones SR (2014) Extended access of cocaine self-administration results in tolerance to the dopamine-elevating and locomotor-stimulating effects of cocaine. J. Neurochem, 128, 224–232. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calipari ES, Ferris MJ, Zimmer BA, Roberts DCS, & Jones SR (2013) Temporal Pattern of Cocaine Intake Determines Tolerance vs Sensitization of Cocaine Effects at the Dopamine Transporter. Neuropsychopharmacology, 38, 2385–2392. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calipari ES, Siciliano CA, Zimmer BA, & Jones SR (2015) Brief intermittent cocaine self-administration and abstinence sensitizes cocaine effects on the dopamine transporter and increases drug seeking. Neuropsychopharmacology, 40, 728–735. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caprioli D, Calu D, & Shaham Y (2014) Loss of phasic dopamine: a new addiction marker? Nat. Neurosci, 17, 644–646. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carroll ME, France CP, & Meisch RA (1981) Intravenous self-administration of etonitazene, cocaine and phencyclidine in rats during food deprivation and satiation. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther, 217, 241–247. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen P & Sas A (1994) Cocaine use in Amsterdam in non Deviant Subcultures. Addict. Res. Theory, 2, 71–94. [Google Scholar]
- Crombag HS, Badiani A, Maren S, & Robinson TE (2000) The role of contextual versus discrete drug-associated cues in promoting the induction of psychomotor sensitization to intravenous amphetamine. Behav. Brain Res, 116, 1–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dackis CA & Gold MS (1985) New concepts in cocaine addiction: the dopamine depletion hypothesis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev, 9, 469–477. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Vry J, Donselaar I, & Van Ree JM (1989) Food deprivation and acquisition of intravenous cocaine self-administration in rats: effect of naltrexone and haloperidol. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther, 251, 735–740. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deroche-Gamonet V, Belin D, & Piazza PV (2004) Evidence for addiction-like behavior in the rat. Science, 305, 1014–1017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Edwards S & Koob GF (2013) Escalation of drug self-administration as a hallmark of persistent addiction liability. Behav. Pharmacol, 24, 356–362. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferrario CR, Gorny G, Crombag HS, Li Y, Kolb B, & Robinson TE (2005) Neural and behavioral plasticity associated with the transition from controlled to escalated cocaine use. Biol. Psychiatry, 58, 751–759. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferris MJ, Mateo Y, Roberts DCS, & Jones SR (2011) Cocaine-insensitive dopamine transporters with intact substrate transport produced by self-administration. Biol. Psychiatry, 69, 201–207. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gerber GJ & Wise RA (1989) Pharmacological regulation of intravenous cocaine and heroin self-administration in rats: a variable dose paradigm. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav, 32, 527–531. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gueye AB, Allain F, & Samaha A-N (2018) Intermittent intake of rapid cocaine injections promotes the risk of relapse and increases mesocorticolimbic BDNF levels during abstinence. Neuropsychopharmacology, in press DOI: 10.1038/s41386-018-0249-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guillem K, Ahmed SH, & Peoples LL (2014) Escalation of cocaine intake and incubation of cocaine seeking are correlated with dissociable neuronal processes in different accumbens subregions. Biol. Psychiatry, 76, 31–39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hershey ND & Kennedy RT (2013) In vivo calibration of microdialysis using infusion of stable-isotope labeled neurotransmitters. ACS Chem. Neurosci, 4, 729–736. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hurd YL, Weiss F, Koob GF, And NE, & Ungerstedt U (1989) Cocaine reinforcement and extracellular dopamine overflow in rat nucleus accumbens: an in vivo microdialysis study. Brain Res, 498, 199–203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hursh SR (1991) Behavioral economics of drug self-administration and drug abuse policy. J. Exp. Anal. Behav, 56, 377–393. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hursh SR & Silberberg A (2008) Economic demand and essential value. Psychol. Rev, 115, 186–198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- James MH, Stopper CM, Zimmer BA, Koll NE, Bowrey HE, & Aston-Jones G (2018) Increased Number and Activity of a Lateral Subpopulation of Hypothalamic Orexin/Hypocretin Neurons Underlies the Expression of an Addicted State in Rats. Biol. Psychiatry, in press DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.07.022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kawa AB, Allain F, Robinson TE, & Samaha A-N (2019) The transition to cocaine addiction: the importance of pharmacokinetics for preclinical models. Psychopharmacology (Berl), in press DOI: 10.1007/s00213-019-5164-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kawa AB, Bentzley BS, & Robinson TE (2016) Less is more: prolonged intermittent access cocaine self-administration produces incentive-sensitization and addiction-like behavior. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 233, 3587–3602. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kawa AB & Robinson TE (2019) Sex differences in incentive-sensitization produced by intermittent access cocaine self-administration. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 236, 625–639. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kippin TE, Fuchs R. a, & See RE (2006) Contributions of prolonged contingent and noncontingent cocaine exposure to enhanced reinstatement of cocaine seeking in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 187, 60–67. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koob GF & Le Moal M (1997) Drug abuse: hedonic homeostatic dysregulation. Science, 278, 52–58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koob GF & Le Moal M (2001) Drug addiction, dysregulation of reward, and allostasis. Neuropsychopharmacology, 24, 97–129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koob GF & Volkow ND (2016) Neurobiology of addiction: a neurocircuitry analysis. The lancet. Psychiatry, 3, 760–773. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kruzich PJ, Congelton KM, & See RE (2001) Conditioned reinstatement of drug-seeking behavior with a discrete compound stimulus classically conditioned with intravenous cocaine. Behav. Neurosci, 115, 1086–1092. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu Y, Roberts DCS, & Morgan D (2005) Effects of extended-access self-administration and deprivation on breakpoints maintained by cocaine in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 179, 644–651. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lu L, Shepard JD, Scott Hall F, & Shaham Y (2003) Effect of environmental stressors on opiate and psychostimulant reinforcement, reinstatement and discrimination in rats: a review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev, 27, 457–491. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lynch WJ & Carroll ME (2001) Regulation of drug intake. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol, 9, 131–143. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mantsch JR, Baker DA, Francis DM, Katz ES, Hoks MA, & Serge JP (2008) Stressor- and corticotropin releasing factor-induced reinstatement and active stress-related behavioral responses are augmented following long-access cocaine self-administration by rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 195, 591–603. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mantsch JR, Yuferov V, Mathieu-Kia AM, Ho A, & Kreek MJ (2004) Effects of extended access to high versus low cocaine doses on self-administration, cocaine-induced reinstatement and brain mRNA levels in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 175, 26–36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marsden CA, Broch OJ, & Guldberg HC (1972) Effect of nigral and raphé lesions on the catechol-O-methyl transferase and monoamine oxidase activities in the rat striatum. Eur. J. Pharmacol, 19, 35–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mateo Y, Lack CM, Morgan D, Roberts DCS, & Jones SR (2005) Reduced dopamine terminal function and insensitivity to cocaine following cocaine binge self-administration and deprivation. Neuropsychopharmacology, 30, 1455–1463. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nicola SM & Deadwyler SA (2000) Firing rate of nucleus accumbens neurons is dopamine-dependent and reflects the timing of cocaine-seeking behavior in rats on a progressive ratio schedule of reinforcement. J. Neurosci, 20, 5526–5537. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oleson EB, Richardson JM, & Roberts DCS (2011) A novel IV cocaine self-administration procedure in rats: Differential effects of dopamine, serotonin, and GABA drug pre-treatments on cocaine consumption and maximal price paid. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 214, 567–577. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oleson EB & Roberts DCS (2009) Behavioral economic assessment of price and cocaine consumption following self-administration histories that produce escalation of either final ratios or intake. Neuropsychopharmacology, 34, 796–804. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paterson NE & Markou A (2003) Increased motivation for self-administered cocaine after escalated cocaine intake. Neuroreport, 14, 2229–2232. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paulson PE & Robinson TE (1994) Relationship between circadian changes in spontaneous motor activity and dorsal versus ventral striatal dopamine neurotransmission assessed with on-line microdialysis. Behav. Neurosci, 108, 624–635. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paxinos G & Watson C (2007) The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates, 6th edn. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Phillips GD, Howes SR, Whitelaw RB, Wilkinson LS, Robbins TW, & Everitt BJ (1994) Isolation rearing enhances the locomotor response to cocaine and a novel environment, but impairs the intravenous self-administration of cocaine. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 115, 407–418. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pierce R, Bell K, Duffy P, & Kalivas P (1996) Repeated cocaine augments excitatory amino acid transmission in the nucleus accumbens only in rats having developed behavioral sensitization. J. Neurosci, 16, 1550–1560. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pitchers KK, Kane LF, Kim Y, Robinson TE, & Sarter M (2017) ‘Hot’ vs. ‘cold’ behavioural-cognitive styles: motivational-dopaminergic vs. cognitive-cholinergic processing of a Pavlovian cocaine cue in sign- and goal-tracking rats. Eur. J. Neurosci, 46, 2768–2781. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quadros IMH & Miczek KA (2009) Two modes of intense cocaine bingeing: increased persistence after social defeat stress and increased rate of intake due to extended access conditions in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 206, 109–120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reid MS, Hsu K, & Berger SP (1997) Cocaine and amphetamine preferentially stimulate glutamate release in the limbic system: Studies on the involvement of dopamine. Synapse, 27, 95–105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson TE & Berridge KC (1993) The neural basis of drug craving: an incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Res. Rev, 18, 247–291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rowland NE (2007) Food or fluid restriction in common laboratory animals: balancing welfare considerations with scientific inquiry. Comp. Med, 57, 149–160. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saunders BT & Robinson TE (2010) A Cocaine Cue Acts as an Incentive Stimulus in Some but not Others: Implications for Addiction. Biol. Psychiatry, 67, 730–736. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schenk S, Lacelle G, Gorman K, & Amit Z (1987) Cocaine self-administration in rats influenced by environmental conditions: implications for the etiology of drug abuse. Neurosci. Lett, 81, 227–231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sharpe AL & Samson HH (2001) Effect of naloxone on appetitive and consummatory phases of ethanol self-administration. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res, 25, 1006–1011. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siciliano CA, Fordahl SC, & Jones SR (2016) Cocaine Self-Administration Produces Long-Lasting Alterations in Dopamine Transporter Responses to Cocaine. J. Neurosci, 36, 7807–7816. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simon SL, Richardson K, Dacey J, Glynn S, Domier CP, Rawson RA, & Ling W (2002) A comparison of patterns of methamphetamine and cocaine use. J. Addict. Dis, 21, 35–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Singer BF, Fadanelli M, Kawa AB, & Robinson TE (2018) Are Cocaine-Seeking “Habits” Necessary for the Development of Addiction-Like Behavior in Rats? J. Neurosci, 38, 60–73. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith JA, Qiu Mo, Guo H, Kunko PM, & Robinson SE (1995) Cocaine increases extraneuronal levels of aspartate and glutamate in the nucleus accumbens. Brain Res, 683, 264–269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Song P, Mabrouk OS, Hershey ND, & Kennedy RT (2012) In Vivo Neurochemical Monitoring Using Benzoyl Chloride Derivatization and Liquid Chromatography–Mass Spectrometry. Anal. Chem, 84, 412–419. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stuber GD, Roitman MF, Phillips PEM, Carelli RM, & Wightman RM (2005) Rapid Dopamine Signaling in the Nucleus Accumbens during Contingent and Noncontingent Cocaine Administration. Neuropsychopharmacology, 30, 853–863. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (2016) Facing Addiction in America: The Surgeon General’s Report on Alcohol, Drugs, and Health, Washington DC: HHS. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vanderschuren LJ & Everitt BJ (2004) Drug seeking becomes compulsive after prolonged cocaine self-administration. Science, 305, 1017–1019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Volkow ND, Koob GF, & McLellan AT (2016) Neurobiologic Advances from the Brain Disease Model of Addiction. N. Engl. J. Med, 374, 363–371. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wee S, Mandyam CD, Lekic DM, & Koob GF (2008) Alpha 1-noradrenergic system role in increased motivation for cocaine intake in rats with prolonged access. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol, 18, 303–311. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weeks JR (1962) Experimental morphine addiction: Method for automatic intravenous injections in unrestrained rats. Science, 138, 143–144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- West BT, Welch KB, Ga AT, & Crc H (2007) LINEAR MIXED MODELS A Practical Guide Using Statistical Software, Statistics in Medicine
- Willuhn I, Burgeno LM, Groblewski PA, & Phillips PEM (2014) Excessive cocaine use results from decreased phasic dopamine signaling in the striatum. Nat. Neurosci, 17, 704–709. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wood PL, Kim HS, & Marien MR (1987) Intracerebral dialysis: Direct evidence for the utility of 3-MT measurements as an index of dopamine release. Life Sci, 41, 1–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xue Y, Steketee JD, & Sun W (2012) Inactivation of the central nucleus of the amygdala reduces the effect of punishment on cocaine self-administration in rats. Eur. J. Neurosci, 35, 775–783. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zimmer BA, Dobrin CV, & Roberts DCS (2011) Brain-Cocaine Concentrations Determine the Dose Self-Administered by Rats on a Novel Behaviorally Dependent Dosing Schedule. Neuropsychopharmacology, 36, 2741–2749. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zimmer BA, Oleson EB, & Roberts DC (2012) The motivation to self-administer is increased after a history of spiking brain levels of cocaine. Neuropsychopharmacology, 37, 1901–1910. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]