Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2020 Jun 18.
Published in final edited form as: Cogn Neuropsychol. 2019 Jun 18;36(3-4):178–192. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2019.1619544

Direct electrical stimulation of the left Frontal Aslant Tract disrupts sentence planning without affecting articulation

Benjamin L Chernoff 1, Max H Sims 2, Susan O Smith 3, Webster H Pilcher 3, Bradford Z Mahon 1,2,3
PMCID: PMC6744286  NIHMSID: NIHMS1041090  PMID: 31210568

Abstract

Sentence production involves mapping from deep structures that specify meaning and thematic roles to surface structures that specify the order and sequencing of production ready elements. We propose that the frontal aslant tract is a key pathway for sequencing complex actions with deep hierarchical structure. In the domain of language, and primarily with respect to the left FAT, we refer to this as the ‘Syntagmatic Constraints On Positional Elements’ (SCOPE) hypothesis. One prediction made by the SCOPE hypothesis is that disruption of the frontal aslant tract should disrupt sentence production at grammatical phrase boundaries, with no disruption of articulatory processes. We test this prediction in a patient undergoing direct electrical stimulation mapping of the frontal aslant tract during an awake craniotomy to remove a left frontal brain tumor. We found that stimulation of the left FAT prolonged inter-word durations at the start of grammatical phrases, while inter-word durations internal to noun phrases were unaffected, and there was no effect on intra-word articulatory duration. These results provide initial support for the SCOPE hypothesis, and motivate novel directions for future research to explore the functions of this recently discovered component of the language system.

Keywords: Frontal Aslant Tract, Sentence Production, Diffusion MRI, Direct Electrical Stimulation, Neurosurgery, Aphasia

Introduction

Sentence production requires planning at multiple levels of processing. Planning is needed to grammatically structure phrases, position lexical elements and grammatical morphemes, and retrieve and ultimately produce phonological and articulatory information. Understanding the neural circuitry that supports sentence production is critical for understanding how the brain processes language. Strong constraints on cognitive models are provided by careful studies of how the system can fail, either in the healthy system in the form of slips of the tongue or under conditions of neurological injury. The Frontal Aslant Tract is a recently discovered white matter pathway that connects, in one branch, the inferior frontal gyrus to the pre-supplementary motor area, and in another branch, the inferior frontal gyrus with the anterior cingulate cortex. Within the inferior frontal gyrus it is believed to project principally to pars opercularis, but there is also evidence that it may project to pars orbitalis (Szmuda et al., 2017). The anatomy of the Frontal Aslant Tract was formalized in human Diffusion Tensor Imaging (DTI) tractography studies by Catani et al, (2012; 2013) and previously described by others (Aron et al., 2007; Lawes et al., 2008; Ford et al., 2010). Post-mortem dissection of human brains (Bozkurt et al., 2016; Koutsarnakis et al., 2017; Goryainov et al., 2017) and tractography studies in nonhuman primates (Thiebaut de Schotten et al., 2012) have also provided important information about the anatomy of the frontal aslant tract.

Lesions to the left frontal aslant tract can result in halting and dysfluent speech that is otherwise semantically, morpho-syntactically, and phonologically correct; furthermore, the dysfluency in spontaneous sentence production does not manifest during sentence repetition (Chernoff et al., 2018). A broader literature has implicated the FAT in verbal fluency, in primary progressive aphasia (Catani et al, 2013; Mandelli et al., 2014), autism (Chenausky et al, 2017), and post-stroke language difficulties (Li et al., 2017). There is also evidence for a role of the FAT in speech initiation. Direct electrical stimulation intra-operatively of left frontal aslant tract has been shown to be able to induce speech arrest (Vassal et al., 2014). Further, when examining post-operative outcome after damage to the frontal aslant tract as compared to the fronto-striatal tract (which is located more medially), damage to the former was more likely to cause speech initiation impairments, while damage to the fronto-striatal tract caused non-speech motor initiation impairments (Kinoshita et al., 2015; Fujii et al., 2015). Damage to the frontal aslant tract has also been related to a reduction in functional connectivity between its cortical endpoints (Chernoff et al., 2018), suggesting that functional connectivity of those structures critically depends on the FAT and not other pathways.

The frontal aslant tract, in addition to the corticospinal and corticobulbar (Cai et al., 2014; Chang et al., 2008; Dick et al., 2014) has been associated with stuttering, both developmentally (Kronfeld-Duenias, 2016a; 2016b) and intra-operatively (Kemerdere et al., 2016). While the right frontal aslant tract has not been as systematically studied, Dick and colleagues (2018) have suggested that the right FAT may play a critical role in executive function, especially inhibitory control. That proposal resonates with the putative role of the right inferior frontal gyrus in inhibition (Aron et al., 2003; Chikazoe et al., 2007; Verbruggen et al., 2008; Hampshire et al., 2010), and the pre-supplementary motor area in selection of movements (Amador & Fried, 2004) and resolving conflicting motor plans (Nachev et al., 2007).

Here we propose that the left frontal aslant tract serves as an interface between distinct levels of sentence planning—specifically, between planning syntagmatic relations and positional planning of morphophonological elements—we refer to this as the ‘Syntagmatic Constraints On Positional Elements’ (SCOPE) hypothesis. The key aspect of this proposal is that the frontal aslant tract interfaces grammatical specifications of sentence structure with (already accessed) lexical representations, by hypothesis, in anticipation of planning articulatory phrases. This generates the prediction that disruption of the frontal aslant tract should specifically disrupt sentence production at phrasal boundaries, with no impairment for articulatory duration. The purpose of the current investigation was to test that hypothesis by measuring sentence fluency, in a patient who was anticipated to have direct electrical stimulation of the left frontal aslant tract during surgery for resection of a tumor.

Prior documented impairments for speech fluency in patients with damage to the FAT have been based on neuropsychological tests of speech production - such as the BDAE “Cookie Theft” (Chernoff et al., 2018) test, the “Cinderella test” (Catani et al., 2013), and indices of stuttering severity, such as the Stuttering Severity Instrument (SSI-III; Riley 1994). While powerful, those tests capture gross fluency measures (such as mean length of utterance), and are less constrained than may be preferable for evaluating our proposal about the functions supported by the frontal aslant tract. We thus designed a task that could be used intra-operatively while the frontal aslant tract was stimulated, and which would allow us to examine different components of sentence production. The patient was presented with a 2x2 arrangement of colored geometric shapes on each trial, with one of the four shapes cued via a thick black outline. The patient’s task was to generate a sentence describing the spatial relation of the cued shape in relation to the shape that was either immediately above or below it—for instance, ‘The red square is above the yellow circle.’ If stimulation of the frontal aslant tract disrupts sentence production by disrupting lexical retrieval, then we predict that inter-word durations (i.e., pauses between words) will be longer prior to all content words in the sentence. By contrast, if the frontal aslant tract supports integration of grammatical information with positionally specified elements, then stimulation of that tract should lead to prolonged inter-word durations at the boundaries of grammatical phrases, but not within grammatical phrases. Finally, if the frontal aslant tract supports articulatory processes, then stimulation of that tract should prolong or disrupt articulation of all words in the sentence.

Methods

Participants.

Patient AI was, at the time of testing, a 46 year-old man with a left frontal Oligodendroglioma (Figure 1A) who was recruited as part of an ongoing prospective longitudinal study evaluating preoperative fMRI and DTI for delineating the maximal safe surgical resection. Eleven age matched neurosurgical controls completed the same neuropsychological tests (see Garcea et al., 2017 for precedent with those controls). All patients gave informed consent in compliance with procedures set by the institutional review board of the University of Rochester.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

A. Coronal series showing the frontal aslant tract (blue-light blue gradient), the u-shaped fibers connecting the middle and superior frontal gyrus (red-yellow gradient), and the lesion visible in the T1 image, with track counts in each voxel B. Three dimensional reconstruction of the tumor (red) and the left frontal aslant tract (blue) in the patient’s pre-operative T1-weighted MRI.

MRI Data Acquisition Parameters.

A series of BOLD fMRI, T1 and DTI studies were conducted with AI to localize language, sensorimotor, and praxis networks as part of pre-surgical planning. Those scans constitute a standard regimen of scans conducted on all neurosurgery patients studied within the Program for Translational Brain Mapping at the University of Rochester (www.tbm.urmc.edu).

MRI data were acquired on a 3T Siemens PRISMA scanner with a 32-channel head coil located at the Rochester Center for Brain Imaging. High-resolution structural T1 contrast images were acquired using a magnetization prepared rapid gradient echo (MPRAGE) pulse sequence at the start of each session (TR=2530, TE=3.44ms, flip angle=71, FOV=256x256 mm2, matrix=256x256, voxel size 1x1x1 mm3, 192 sagittal slices). Functional images were acquired using a BOLD echo-planar imaging pulse sequence (TR=2200ms, TE=30ms, flip angle=70, FOV=256x256mm2, matrix=128x128, voxel size=2x2x2 mm3, 90 axial slices). DTI acquisition used a single shot echo-planar sequence (60 diffusion directions with b = 1000 s/mm2, 10 images with b=0 s/mm2, TR=6500ms, TE=56ms, FOV=256x256mm2, matrix=128x128, voxel size=2x2x2 mm3, 70 axial slices, anterior to posterior phase encoding). One additional b=0 volume was collected with posterior to anterior phase encoding, to allow for susceptibility induced artifact correction.

fMRI Preprocessing.

fMRI data were analyzed with the BrainVoyager software package (Version 2.8) and in-house scripts drawing on the BVQX toolbox written in MATLAB. The first six volumes of each run were discarded to allow for signal equilibration (four at image acquisition and two at preprocessing). Preprocessing of the functional data consisted of (in order) slice scan time correction (sinc interpolation), motion correction with respect to the first volume of the first functional run, and linear trend removal in the temporal domain (cutoff: two cycles within the run). Functional data were registered (after contrast inversion of the first volume) to high-resolution de-skulled anatomy of each subject in native space. Functional data were smoothed at 6 mm FWHM (1.5 mm voxels), and interpolated to 2mm3 voxels. These analysis pipelines are described in prior work from our group (e.g., Chernoff et al., 2018; Chen et al., 2016; 2017; Fintzi & Mahon, 2013; Garcea & Mahon, 2014; Garcea et al., 2016; 2017; Kristensen et al., 2016; Mahon et al., 2013).

Diffusion MRI Preprocessing.

DTI preprocessing was performed with the FMRIB Software Library (FSL; http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/). Susceptibility induced artifacts were corrected using FSL’s TOPUP tool (Andersson et al, 2003; Smith et al., 2004). Next, the coefficients from TOPUP were fed to FSL’s Eddy tool (Andersson & Sotiropoulos, 2016), which corrects eddy currents and motion. Lastly, reconstruction of the whole brain tensor was performed using StarTrack (https://www.mr-startrack.com/) with a step size of 1mm, angle threshold of 45 degrees, and FA threshold of 0.2.

Definition of functional ROIs for Tractography.

One of the language scans—a category fluency experiment—was used to functionally localize regions of interest (ROI) to perform tractography. In the category fluency experiment, the patient viewed a cue that could be a letter (e.g. words that start with the letter “a”), a noun (e.g. fruit), or an action category (e.g. actions performed in the kitchen), and had 30 seconds to overtly generate as many items from that category as possible. Stimulus blocks alternated with 20-second fixation periods. Regions of interest (ROI) for tractography were functionally defined using the peak BOLD-contrast from the category verbal fluency tasks, in the pre-supplementary motor area and the posterior inferior frontal gyrus. Ten-millimeter radius spheres were drawn around the peak voxel and used as seeds for fiber tracking. An exclusion mask was drawn around consecutive sagittal slices in the right hemisphere, four to eight millimeters from the midline, in order to exclude crossing callosal fibers. Deterministic tractography was performed using TrackVis (Wang & Weeden, http://www.TrackVis.org).

Neuropsychological tests.

Patient AI was evaluated pre-operatively to broadly assess language, praxis, visual processing, memory, and attention. All testing was video and/or audio recorded for offline analysis (for a detailed descriptions of the testing, see Garcea et al., 2013; Stasenko et al., 2014). We conduct a large array of tests as part of a standard battery for a broader longitudinal study in the Program for Translational Brain Mapping, but in this case we were particularly interested in language function. The purpose of testing patients more broadly is to understand the limits of each patient’s impairments, which is critical for deriving inferences about cognitive organization based on the underling cognitive processes that are disrupted in any given patient (i.e., the ‘sufficiency’ condition, as in Caramazza, 1984).

AI completed a neuropsychological screener prior to surgery and again one month after surgery. Language assessment included picture naming (Snodgrass and Vanderwart, 1980), word reading (Psycholinguistic Assessment of Language Processing in Aphasia, PALPA, subtest 33, Kay, Lesser & Colthart, 1992), pseudoword reading (PALPA subtest 36), number reading (one, two, and three digits), category fluency (1 minute to generate items to letter, category or action-context cues), sentence repetition (PALPA, subtest 12, subset of n = 18), picture-word matching (Stasenko et al., 2014), and object decision (Barbarotto et al., 2002). AI was also evaluated for praxis knowledge using in-house materials and procedures described previously (Garcea et al., 2012; 2013; for precedent, see Buxbaum and Saffran, 2002). AI was unimpaired for all neuropsychological tests preoperatively (see Table 1).

Table 1.

Neuropsychological performance of patient AI and 11 age matched neurosurgical controls. Values in the table represent percent correct, with the exception of category fluency (mean number of items)

Patient AI Neurosurgical Controls
Category Test Accuracy Mean Accuracy SD
Naming Word Reading 100% 96% 5%
Pseudoword Reading 96% 90% 16%
Number Naming 100% 77% 17%
Picture Naming 95% 94% 5%
Semantic Processing Object Decision 96% 89% 10%
Picture-Word Match 100% 97% 3%
Other Sentence Repetition 100% 93% 14%
Function Knowledge 100% 96% 7%
Manipulation Knowledge 100% 83% 25%
Cambridge Face Test 80% 71% 16%
Verbal Fluency Average Number of Items
Letter Fluency 18.33 19 2
Noun Fluency 18.50 14.1 4.7
Action Fluency 17.67 11.5 3.62

Task for Intra-operative Direct Electrical Stimulation Mapping.

Intra-operative stimulation of the frontal aslant tract has been performed almost exclusively using picture naming and limb movements, with the exception of Sierpowska (2015), who also used a verb-from-noun generation task. In order to assess sentence planning within the constraints of the operating room, we designed a task with simple geometric shapes, and which involved an overt response to facilitate real-time scoring of accuracy in the service of proximate clinical decisions about how to procede with the tumor resection. We also designed the task to be amenable to detailed response time analysis in order to evaluate the core prediction made by the SCOPE hypothesis (see Introduction). To elicit sentences that were standardized and predictable, geometric shapes (square, circle, triangle, and diamond) that were each one of four colors (blue, yellow, red, or green) were arranged in a 2 by 2 array on each trial. On each trial, one of the four shapes had a thick black outline, which indicated it was the target shape, and hence the grammatical subject of the sentence (Figure 3). The patient’s task was to describe the spatial relation between the target shape and the shape that was either above or below it, using as much information as is required. For example, when presented with two triangles above two circles, the patient could say “The blue triangle is above the red circle.” The task allows for pragmatic constraints to influence syntactic structure (e.g., it would be as informative in that example to say ‘The triangle is above the red circle’ because there is only one red circle). In contrast, for other trials, in which all four items were different shapes, the task could be accurately completed without using color terms (e.g., “The triangle is above the square.”). This manipulation, in principle, introduces variation in the frame of the sentence. Similar approaches using displays of shapes to elicit sentences have been used to test utterance planning and message formulation (e.g. Brown-Schmidt & Tanenhaus, 2006). We note, that in the end, the patient always produced sentences of the form ‘The [color] [shape] is [above/below] the [color][shape]’. Because the patient was otherwise fully on task during the awake portion of the surgery, he was not corrected or encouraged to use the alternate (i.e., more minimal) sentence frames.

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

A. Inter- vs. Intra-word durations. Mean inter-word durations for each correct word and mean articulation time for each correct word. Averages are compared for stimulation trials (grey bars) and non-stimulation trials (white bars). Error bars show the standard error of the mean, over words. B. Phrase head vs. content word inter-word duration; Mean inter-word duration is compared across all phrase head words and across all content words, for each correct word. Averages are compared for stimulation trials (grey bars) and non-stimulation trials (white bars). Error bars show the standard error of the mean, over words. C. Mean inter-word duration for each ordinal position of the sentence. Averages are compared for stimulation trials (grey bars) and non-stimulation trials (white bars). Error bars show the standard error of the mean, over trials.

There are many permutations of shapes*color*position*target so we randomly sampled 100 combinations for materials to be used, with the constraint that there were 25 trials of each of the four possible minimalistically correct sentence frames (i.e., no color terms, two color terms, or color term for first or second shape only). AI practiced the task in the lab prior to surgery, laying down on his right side, to simulate the ergonomics of the operating room. During those practice sessions, he understood the task and performed all trials with no errors, hesitations, or paraphasias. During the surgery, an auditory cue (a click) initiated each trial; this auditory cue signaled to the surgeon the start of a new trial. At the discretion of the attending surgeon (WHP) direct current stimulation was then applied coincident with trial onset, with the duration of stimulation lasting ~4 seconds.

Intra-operative testing.

Intra-operative experiments were performed using StrongView, which has been developed within the Program for Translational Brain Mapping at the University of Rochester. StrongView includes a PC with a keyboard for experiment control, an independent backup battery power source, and speakers (Bose Companion 2 Series III). An articulating arm (Tether Tools Rock Solid Master Articulating Arm) that can rotate along multiple joints with several degrees of freedom is attached via a rail clamp to the surgical bed. Attached to the arm is a small touch screen (Elo 1002 L 10” Touchscreen Monitor), a microphone (Sennheiser Professional Shotgun Microphone), and a webcam (Logitech HD Pro Webcam C920), all of which are trained on the patient’s mouth. The microphone feeds through a splitter (M-Audio M-Track) that goes to i) an amplifier (Behringer Tube Ultragain Mic100) and directly to a speaker, so that both the surgeons and the researchers can hear the patient with no delay/echo, and ii) to the PC where it is time-stamped and recorded for offline analysis. In-house software (StrongView) controls presentation and randomization of stimuli, and inter-trial and inter-stimulus intervals. StrongView records the timestamp at which each stimulus is presented, and a photodiode on the patient screen is fed to the electrocorticography amplifier to mark trial onsets (data not analyzed herein). A camcorder (Sony HDR-CX900 HD Handicam) positioned on an 11-foot tripod was used to record the surgery in high definition. In addition, an overhead camera built into the operating lights recorded the entire surgery. Cranial navigation was accomplished with BrainLab, which is an optical camera system that aligns the patient’s brain to the preoperative MRI using facial physiognomy. Prior to mapping, a BrainLab registration star was attached to the bipolar direct current electrical stimulator and registered using the fixed registration star on the field. In this way, we acquired the exact location of each point of direct electrical stimulation with respect to the preoperative MRI. Those data points were exported after the case for offline analysis, and are shown as red spheres in Supplemental Video 1.

Response time analysis of the experimental task.

Inter-word (i.e., pauses between words) and intra-word (i.e., articulatory) durations were calculated by manually transcribing the audio using Audacity by author BC. At the time of analysis, BC was blinded to which trials were with stimulation and which were without stimulation (he did not attend the mapping session and reviewed video records of the surgical field only after performing the latency analyses. We coded each word with its ordinal position within the sentence (i.e. “is” is position number four; <above/below> is position number five, etc.). In addition, a binary variable was coded for each word that represents whether or not the word was part of a trial where subcortical direct electrical stimulation was applied. The inter-word duration for a given word was defined as the difference between the offset of the last phoneme of the previous word, and the onset of the first phoneme of the current word. The intra-word duration (articulatory duration) of a given word was defined as the difference between the onset of the first phoneme of a word and the offset of the last phoneme of that word. A schematic of the sentence parcellation scheme adopted herein is depicted in Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Schematic of the sentence latency measures used in the present investigation- The black square represents the onset of each trial. The words are grouped into noun phrases (orange and red) and the verb phrase (magenta), where the first word of each phrase represents the phrasal boundary. The gaps highlighted in green represent articulatory time (i.e., intra-word durations), while the gaps highlighted in blue represent inter-word durations. The purple gap represents the entire sentence duration.

Determining Proximity of Stimulation points to the frontal aslant tract.

In order to calculate Euclidean distance from each stimulation point to the frontal aslant tract, we used FSL to extract the entire set of coordinates (n=2,580) that correspond to the FAT, based on pre-operative DTI. FSL’s linear registration tool (FLIRT) (Jenkinson & Smith, 2001) was used to register the pre-operative frontal aslant tract to the T1 anatomy used for intra-operative navigation. Separately, and as described above, we used BrainLab for cranial navigation in the operating room to record the coordinate of each location of intraoperative stimulation. For each stimulation coordinate, we calculated the Euclidean distance to each of the 2,580 voxels of the frontal aslant tract. The minimum value from the resulting vector was taken as the estimate of the Euclidean distance between the stimulation point and the closest point along the frontal aslant tract.

Results

Intra-operative mapping to identify eloquent cortical sites initiated with motor mapping and picture naming. Direct electrical stimulation was delivered with a bipolar Ojemann stimulator (Nicolet). There were no motor- or language-positive sites in the area of planned corticectomy. On that basis, the surgeon began the tumor resection. From preoperative MRI (fMRI + DTI), we knew that the tumor was located anterior to motor cortex, with the anterior margin of the tumor abutting the posterior aspect of the frontal aslant tract, at the tract’s superior/medial end.

After the incision was made and the tumor resection had initiated, AI performed 19 trials of the sentence production experiment without stimulation. This provided a ‘warm-up’ for the patient on the critical task. During this ‘practice’ session, only one error was produced, which was self-corrected (diamond to triangle). He also performed sentence repetition using sentences from subtest 12 of the PALPA (Kay et al., 1992). No repetition errors, paraphasias, or stuttering were observed. Those observations are important because they indicate that the corticectomy itself had no effect on AI’s language abilities.

Toward the end of the tumor resection, when the frontal aslant tract was exposed after resection of the anterior margin of tumor, the experimental task was initiated. Thirty-eight trials of the experiment were completed by AI, with 18 of those 38 trials accompanied by subcortical stimulation along the deep anterior margin of the tumor, and 20 without stimulation. For all of those trials, AI produced sentence frames that included color terms for both shapes (e.g., The COLOR SHAPE is ABOVE/BELOW the COLOR SHAPE). Of the 304 words he produced across the 38 sentences (38 trials * 8 words), 251 were included in the latency analyses. The words excluded from latency analysis included instances of speech arrest or hesitation (n=20), coordinate substitution (n=9), and word productions preceded by non-verbal sounds such as coughing (n=9). After the first pass latency analysis, statistical outliers (n=15) were excluded, defined as words with a preceding inter-word pause that was 2 standard deviations or greater than the mean inter-word pause for all words at that sentence position. There were no phonemic substitutions or articulatory errors. The mean distance of all stimulation points to the FAT was 10.2 millimeters (SD= 5.4; range 1.14 to 17.69).

Accuracy.

We considered all correct words in the inter-word pause analysis, but we also looked at accuracy at the trial level. While words were excluded if they were preceded by a cough or were outliers, those sentences were considered accurate for sentence-level analyses, in that he eventually produced the correct description of the shapes with no paraphasias. Of the 18 stimulation trials, AI made errors on 9 trials, including coordinate substitutions (orientation, shape, or color words), and speech arrest. The other 50 of the stimulation trials were correct. Conversely, of the 20 non-stimulation trials, AI made two errors. On one trial he said “below” instead of “above,” and on another trial he failed to complete the full sentence prior to the onset of the next trial. The difference in accuracy between stimulation and non-stimulation trials was significant (X238= 7.37, p=0.007).

Latency Analysis.

Total sentence duration was not different for correct stimulation trials compared to non-stimulation trials(t25 = 0.41, p = 0.68). Sentence duration was calculated from the onset of the stimulus to the offset of the last word.

We examined articulation time of stimulation and non-stimulation trials, in order to test whether stimulation affected the time required to produce the speech motor movements of each word. The mean articulation time for words with stimulation was 413 ms (SD=236 ms; range= 127 to 885 ms) and for words without stimulation was 449 ms (SD=257 ms; range= 129 to 1162 ms); the difference was not significant (t249= 1.15, p= 0.25, d= 0.15) (Figure 3A).

We then examined the effect of stimulation on inter-word durations. We conducted a two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) to test the influence of stimulation (2 levels: stimulation, no stimulation) and sentence position (8 levels: first | second | third | fourth | fifth | sixth | seventh | and eighth word in the sentence) on inter-word durations. There were main effects of stimulation (F(1,235) = 11.15; p= 0.001, ηp2 = 0.05) and position (F(7,235) = 191.52, p < 0.00001; p < 0.02, ηp2 =0.85), and an interaction between stimulation and position (F(7,235)= 5.92, p < 0.00001, ηp2 = 0.15). Hypothesis driven tests evaluated the key question of whether inter-word durations were differentially prolonged at phrasal heads, including for the verb (“is”) and noun phrases (“the”). For this analysis, we binned across sentence positions according to grammatical structure. Specifically, inter-word durations prior to the noun phrases (prior to ‘the’) and prior to the verb (‘is’) were combined, while inter-word durations prior to all other words were combined (Figure 3B). There was a two-way interaction between position and stimulation (F(1,247) = 13.386, p= 0.0003, ηp2 = 0.05). This interaction is reflected in the higher mean for “the” and “is” after stimulation (mean = 1908 ms, SD = 1076 ms) compared to without stimulation (mean= 1407 ms, SD= 699 ms), but a lower mean for all other words when produced in the context of stimulation (mean = 221 ms, SD = 91 ms) compared to without stimulation (mean = 261 ms, SD = 175 ms).

Finally, we conducted t-tests for stimulation vs. non-stimulation for inter-word durations at each position in the sentence (Figure 3C). There were significant differences for words in the first ordinal position (“The”) (t25 = 2.23, p = 0.04, d = 0.86); the third ordinal position (shape) (t31 = −2.64, p = 0.01, d = 0.91); the fourth ordinal position (“is”) (t34 = 2.14, p = 0.04, d = 0.71); and the sixth ordinal position (“the”) (t29 = 3.08, p = 0.004, d = 1.1). There were no significant differences in inter-word durations between stimulation and non-stimulation words for the other ordinal positions.

Discussion

The frontal aslant tract is a recently described white matter pathway that connects the inferior frontal gyrus with supplementary motor cortex, and has been implicated in verbal fluency in a series of reports. We propose a new hypothesis for understanding the contribution of the frontal aslant tract to language production, emphasizing the role of this pathway in mapping grammatical specified planning of sentence structure and positional level planning—referred to as the Syntagmatic Constraints On Positional Elements (SCOPE) hypothesis. One prediction made by this proposal is that disruption of the FAT will disrupt sentence production at the level at which syntagmatic relations interface with positional specifications of words, which by hypothesis, should correspond to the boundaries of grammatical phrases. To test this prediction, we designed a cued sentence production task for use during intraoperative awake language mapping, and analyzed both accuracy and the distribution of intra- and inter-word durations as a function of stimulation. We found that neither the overall duration of sentences nor the articulation time (intra-word durations) of words was affected by stimulation. However, inter-word durations were prolonged for words preceded by stimulation of the frontal aslant tract, but only if those words were at the boundaries of grammatical phrases. Other words were either not significantly different, or facilitated by stimulation (shorter inter-word durations for stimulation than non-stimulation). One possibility is that the facilitation observed for inter-word durations within noun phrases is a byproduct of the prolonged durations at the boundary of the noun phrase; i.e., there was more time devoted to planning the entire phrase, and some of that time allowed articulatory planning to proceed as well. To evaluate this possibility, we correlated the inter-word durations for the boundary of the second noun phrase in each sentence with the within-phrase inter-word durations (also for the second noun phrase in each sentence). This was done separately for stimulation and non-stimulation trials. If facilitation effects are a byproduct of more time devoted to planning the phrase at its initiation, then there should be a negative correlation. In contrast, we observed a positive correlation (r=0.44) between inter-word durations for the head of the second noun phrase and inter-word durations within that phrase. Thus, at present, it is not obvious why stimulation would lead to shorter inter-word durations within noun phrases, and this issue merits additional empirical scrutiny with future research.

All models of speech production agree that sentence production involves the i) formulation of a message, ii) construction of a syntactic frame together with access to words’ grammatical properties, and iii) phonological encoding and articulation (e.g., Dell et al., 1997; Caramazza 1997; Garrett 1980a; 1980b; Rapp & Goldrick, 2000). While models disagree on issues such as the dynamics of information flow in the system, whether there are 1 or 2 lexical levels, and whether access to syntactic and phonological properties occurs in that order or in parallel, there is broad agreement that sentence production involves the translation of a hierarchical representation specifying sentence-level grammatical dependencies into a specific surface form. Furthermore, there is general agreement that grammatical processing is a separable process in language production, and there is substantial neural evidence as well (Shapiro et al., 2000; 2003; 2006; Indefrey & Levelt, 2004; Friederici et al., 2006; Hickok & Poeppel, 2007). Thus, all models must posit a process whereby grammatical specifications are ‘realized’ as surface forms. Our proposal is that the frontal aslant tract is a key component of that interface.

Our findings are relevant as well to previous research on the functional consequences of damage to the cortical regions connected by the Frontal Aslant Tract—the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) and pre-supplementary motor area (pre-SMA). The IFG is associated with agrammatism in primary progressive aphasia, both in production (Grossman et al., 2012) and comprehension (Charles et al., 2014). Lesion studies have also implicated the IFG in syntactic processing (Kaan & Swaab, 2002; Grodzinsky, 2006). Importantly, the IFG has been implicated in a number of other aspects of language processing, including phonological encoding, selection among alternatives and general control processes (e.g., Thompson-Schill et al., 1997; 2015; Kan et al., 2006; Nozari & Hepner, 2018; Anders et al., 2017), and it has been argued that this brain region is composed of functionally distinct subregions (Amunts et al., 1999; Anwander et al., 2006; Helm et al., 2008; Sahin et al., 2009). If the IFG is composed of functionally distinct sub-regions, then it is important to consider if the FAT consists of functionally dissociable tracts with different endpoints in different subregions of the of IFG. Pars opercularis and pars triangularis are the regions of the IFG most commonly described as the endpoints of the FAT (Dick et al., 2018), and an important open question is whether there may be different subcomponents of the FAT, projecting to different regions of the IFG, that support different functions.

At the other end of the FAT, the pre-SMA has been shown to support the organization of action sequences (Kennerley, Sakai, & Rushworth, 2004), especially when it comes to initiation (Eccles, 1982; Nachev et al., 2008 for review). There is some evidence that the right hemisphere FAT may support the initiation and control of motor movements generally, as damage to the right FAT can result in Foix-Chavany-Marie syndrome, an impairment of voluntary control of certain facial and pharyngeal movements (e.g., laughing, coughing) with intact reflexes in the same muscles (Martino et al., 2012; Brandao et al., 2013). These impairments have not been observed after damage to the left FAT, which may support a dissociation between the left and right hemisphere FAT (Dick et al., 2018). It is important to emphasize that while we have framed the SCOPE hypothesis as applying to sentence production, there is nothing in our data that precludes a role for the FAT in relating other hierarchically organized action sequences to planning processes necessary for their production. Whether or not the left FAT supports similar functions outside of language as it does, by hypothesis, in language is an important and open empirical question.1

There are several limitations of the current investigation that should be considered in interpreting our findings and in thinking forward to future empirical investigations of the FAT. First, other white matter pathways share a common endpoint with the frontal aslant tract in the inferior frontal gyrus, such as the Arcuate Fasciculus (AF). However, it is unlikely those other pathways were directly affected by stimulation because all subcortical stimulations were at the level of the superior frontal gyrus and not the inferior frontal gyrus. It is possible, however, that the effect on behavior of frontal aslant tract stimulation was a consequence of stimulation of U-shaped fibers that connect the superior and middle frontal gyri (Catani et al., 2012). The functional role of these fibers, both as considered on their own, and in support of the FAT, is an open question (Chernoff et al., 2018). To illustrate the proximity of these U-Fibers to the FAT in our data, Figure 1 shows tractography of the FAT as well as the U-Fibers connecting the superior and middle, and middle and inferior frontal gyri. In addition, the constraints of our task, which were motivated by the desire to elicit stereotyped responses that still required planning, adds some ambiguity that invites further consideration in future research. In particular, future research should involve the production of sentences that can differentiate between coordinate errors and errors that are specifically semantic in nature. In the context of our task, it is not clear if when the patient slipped for instance, between ‘triangle’ and ‘square,’ whether that error constitutes a ‘semantic error’ or rather a coordinate substitution that respects task constraints. We are developing a version of this task in which pictures of common objects and animals are used in place of geometric shapes, specifically to be able to distinguish whether lexical errors are coordinate substitutions or (proper) semantic substitutions.

It should also be emphasized that care is required when interpreting brain-behavior relation in patients with tumors, as there may have been reorganization over a period of time as the tumor grew. This concern is assuaged by our ability to use functional MRI to localize eloquent regions in the left frontal lobe in AI that are similar in location to healthy participants. In addition, it is worth emphasizing that while AI experienced some weakness in his right foot after the surgery, his language was intact both before and after the surgery. The potential limitation associated with studying patients with brain tumors, which must be considered and managed, should not outweigh the pragmatic reality that neurosurgery patients represent the only opportunity to test the effects of direct electrical stimulation on language processing in the human brain.

Prior studies using direct electrical stimulation mapping have emphasized accuracy and not latency analyses (e.g., Sanai et al., 2008; Herbet et al., 2018; Leonard et al., 2018; Orena et al., 2018; Rofes et al., 2018; Ojemann et al., 1989), with a few notable exceptions (e.g. Hirshorn et al., 2016). There are several reasons why caution must be exercised when using response time in the context of direct electrical stimulation, including that patients are under anesthetic agents, that the ergonomics of testing in the operating room are highly constrained, and that typically it is not possible to obtain a large number of trials during an awake craniotomy. Nonetheless, if tasks are designed with those constraints in mind, and patients sufficiently practiced so as to be able to execute the task fluently, then there is no reason why response time cannot be used to infer subtle effects of direct electrical stimulation on cognitive function. For instance, in the current investigation, the key comparison is between stimulation and non-stimulation trials, and those trials were intermixed; thus any general effects of anesthesia would affect both conditions equally. The current investigation constitutes a demonstration of the potential power of using response times to test hypotheses about underlying cognitive function using direct electrical stimulation mapping during awake craniotomies. Nevertheless, reaction time analyses must be carefully measured and due diligence is required to ensure that response times are measuring the intended aspects of patient performance (e.g., through redundancy of patient recordings in the operating rooms, careful post-processing; for further relevant discussion, van der Linden and colleagues, 2014; Riès and colleagues, 2012). For instance, it should be emphasized that a key component of our approach has been to optimize the quality of the audio recordings (using directional microphones) so as to be able to filter out the many extraneous background noises in the operating room.

Conclusion

Sentence production requires the integration of syntactic and phonological planning with lexical retrieval. Patients with damage to the left frontal aslant tract demonstrate an impairment in sentence production, but lexical retrieval is intact. The findings reported in this case study, together with previous related work from our group (Chernoff et al., 2018), suggest a hypothesis about why that may be the case. Future work is needed in order to integrate the SCOPE hypothesis with recent neurocognitive models of lexical access (Anders et al., 2017; Schnur, 2017; Belke, 2017; Nozari & Hepner, 2018), existing neurobiological models of speech production such as GODIVA (Bohland et al., 2010), and network level hodotopic models of the language system (Duffau et al., 2014; 2015) . Finally, to properly evaluate the SCOPE hypothesis it will be necessary to systematically test speech fluency across a wider array of grammatical structures in the context of direct stimulation or frank injury to the frontal aslant tract.

Supplementary Material

video

Supplemental Video 1. Intra-operative video showing subcortical stimulation of the frontal aslant tract. For each stimulation event, a coronal and sagittal slice is shown. The frontal aslant tract is shown in blue in the MRI images. The red dot depicts the location in the brain of the current stimulation event. Prior stimulation coordinates are shown as white spheres. This video shows representative stimulation and non-stimulation trials. Video from: www.tbm.urmc.org

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to Dr. Michael Tanenhaus for his discussion and contributions to the development of the experimental paradigm used during intraoperative mapping and for his insights throughout the planning phase of this study. The authors thank Keith Parkins for software development (StrongView), Sarah Gannon, Steve Erickson, Kelly Wright, and Michael Schmidt for assistance in the operating room, and Raouf Belkhir and for assistance with the development of response time measures. This research was supported by NSF (BCS1349042) and NIH (R01NS089069, R01EY028535) grants to BZM, a core grant to the Center for Visual Science (P30 EY001319), and by support to the Department of Neurosurgery by Norman and Arlene Leenhouts. BLC was supported by an NSF training grant (NSF DGE-1449828).

Footnotes

1

We are grateful to Dr. Anthony Dick for raising this possibility.

References

  1. Amador N, & Fried I (2004). Single-neuron activity in the human supplementary motor area underlying preparation for action. Journal of Neurosurgery, 100(2), 250–259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Amunts K, Schleicher A, Bürgel U, Mohlberg H, Uylings HB, & Zilles K (1999). Broca’s region revisited: cytoarchitecture and intersubject variability. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 412(2), 319–341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Anders R, Riès S, Van Maanen L, & Alario FX (2017). Lesions to the left lateral prefrontal cortex impair decision threshold adjustment for lexical selection. Cognitive neuropsychology, 34(1-2), 1–20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Andersson JL, Skare S, & Ashburner J (2003). How to correct susceptibility distortions in spin-echo echo-planar images: application to diffusion tensor imaging. NeuroImage, 20(2), 870–888. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Andersson JL, & Sotiropoulos SN (2016). An integrated approach to correction for off-resonance effects and subject movement in diffusion MR imaging. NeuroImage, 125, 1063–1078. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Anwander A, Tittgemeyer M, von Cramon DY, Friederici AD, & Knösche TR (2006). Connectivity-based parcellation of Broca’s area. Cerebral Cortex, 17(4), 816–825. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Aron AR, Fletcher PC, Bullmore ET, Sahakian BJ, & Robbins TW (2003). Stop-signal inhibition disrupted by damage to right inferior frontal gyrus in humans. Nature Neuroscience, 6(2), 115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Aron AR, Behrens TE, Smith S, Frank MJ, & Poldrack RA (2007). Triangulating a cognitive control network using diffusion-weighted magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and functional MRI. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(14), 3743–3752. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Barbarotto R, Laiacona M, Macchi V, & Capitani E (2002). Picture reality decision, semantic categories and gender: A new set of pictures, with norms and an experimental study. Neuropsychologia, 40(10), 1637–1653. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Belke E (2017). Effects of lesions to the left lateral prefrontal cortex on task-specific top-down biases and response strategies in blocked-cyclic naming. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 34(1-2), 26–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bock K, & Levelt W (1994). Language production: Grammatical encoding In Gernsbacher MA (Ed.), Handbook of Psycholinguistics (pp. 945–984). San Diego: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
  12. Bohland JW, Bullock D, & Guenther FH (2010). Neural representations and mechanisms for the performance of simple speech sequences. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 1504–1529. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bozkurt B, Yagmurlu K, Middlebrooks EH, Karadag A, Ovalioglu TC, Jagadeesan B, … Grande AW (2016). Microsurgical and Tractographic Anatomy of the Supplementary Motor Area Complex in Humans. World Neurosurgery, 95, 99–107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Brandao E, Ferreria A, & Leal Loureiro J (2013). Anterior biopercular syndrome caused by unilateral infarction. Acta Med Port, 26(2), 177–179. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Brown-Schmidt S & Tanenhaus M (2006). Watching the eyes when talking about size: An investigation of message formulation and utterance planning. Journal of Memory and Language, 54, 592–609. [Google Scholar]
  16. Buxbaum LJ, & Saffran EM (2002). Knowledge of object manipulation and object function: dissociations in apraxic and nonapraxic subjects. Brain and language, 82(2), 179–199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Cai S, Tourville JA, Beal DS, Perkell JS, Guenther FH, & Ghosh SS (2014). Diffusion imaging of cerebral white matter in persons who stutter: evidence for network-level anomalies. Frontiers In Human Neuroscience, 8, 54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Caramazza A (1997). How many levels of processing are there in lexical access? Cognitive Neuropsychology, 14, 177–208. [Google Scholar]
  19. Caramazza A (1984). The logic of neuropsychological research and the problem of patient classification in aphasia. Brain and Language, 21(1), 9–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Catani M, Dell’Acqua F, Vergani F, Malik F, Hodge H, Roy P, … Thiebaut de Schotten M (2012). Short frontal lobe connections of the human brain. Cortex, 48(2), 273–291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Catani M, Mesulam MM, Jakobsen E, Malik F, Martersteck A, Wieneke C, … Rogalski E (2013). A novel frontal pathway underlies verbal fluency in primary progressive aphasia. Brain, 136(8), 2619–2628. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Chang SE, Erickson KI, Ambrose NG, Hasegawa-Johnson MA, & Ludlow CL (2008). Brain anatomy differences in childhood stuttering. Neuroimage, 39(3), 1333–1344. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Charles D, Olm C, Powers J, Ash S, Irwin DJ, McMillan CT, … & Grossman M (2014). Grammatical comprehension deficits in non-fluent/agrammatic primary progressive aphasia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry, 85(3), 249–256. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Chen Q, Garcea FE, & Mahon BZ (2016). The representation of object-directed action and function knowledge in the human brain. Cerebral Cortex, 26, 1609–1618. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Chen Q, Garcea FE, Jacobs RA, & Mahon BZ (2017). Abstract Representations of Object-Directed Action in the Left Inferior Parietal Lobule. Cerebral Cortex, 1–13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Chenausky K, Kernbach J, Norton A, & Schlaug G (2017). White Matter Integrity and Treatment-Based Change in Speech Performance in Minimally Verbal Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 11, 175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Chernoff BL, Teghipco A, Garcea FE, Sims M, Paul DA, Tivarus M, Smith SO, Pilcher WH, & Mahon BZ (2018). A role for the Frontal Aslant Tract in speech planning: A neurosurgical case study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 30(5), 752–769. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Chikazoe J, Konishi S, Asari T, Jimura K, & Miyashita Y (2007). Activation of right inferior frontal gyrus during response inhibition across response modalities. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(1), 69–80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Dick AS, Bernal B, & Tremblay P (2014). The language connectome new pathways, new concepts. The Neuroscientist, 20(5), 453–467. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Dick AS, Garic D, Graziano P, & Tremblay P (2018). The frontal aslant tract (FAT) and its role in speech, language and executive function. bioRxiv, 249912. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Duffau H, Moritz-Gasser S, & Mandonnet E (2014). A re-examination of neural basis of language processing: Proposal of a dynamic hodotopical model from data provided by brain stimulation mapping during picture naming. Brain and Language, 131, 1–10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Duffau H (2015). Stimulation mapping of white matter tracts to study brain functional connectivity. Nature Reviews Neurology, 11(5), 255265. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Eccles JC (1982). The initiation of voluntary movements by the supplementary motor area. Archiv für Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten, 231(5), 423–441. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Fintzi AR, & Mahon BZ (2013). A bimodal tuning curve for spatial frequency across left and right human orbital frontal cortex during object recognition. Cerebral Cortex, 24(5), 1311–1318. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Ford A, McGregor KM, Case K, Crosson B, & White KD (2010). Structural connectivity of Broca’s area and medial frontal cortex. NeuroImage, 52(4), 1230–1237. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Friederici AD, Bahlmann J, Heim S, Schubotz RI, & Anwander A (2006). The brain differentiates human and non-human grammars: functional localization and structural connectivity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103(7), 2458–2463. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Fujii M, Maesawa S, Motomura K, Futamura M, Hayashi Y, Koba I, & Wakabayashi T (2015). Intraoperative subcortical mapping of a language-associated deep frontal tract connecting the superior frontal gyrus to Broca’s area in the dominant hemisphere of patients with glioma. Journal of Neurosurgery, 122(6), 1390–1396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Garcea FE, Dombovy M, and Mahon BZ (2013). Preserved tool knowledge in the context of impaired action knowledge: Implications for models of semantic memory. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 1–18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Garcea FE, & Mahon BZ (2014). Parcellation of left parietal tool representations by functional connectivity. Neuropsychologia, 60, 131–143. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Garcea FE, Kristensen S, Almeida J, & Mahon BZ (2016). Resilience to the contralateral visual field bias as a window into object representations. Cortex, 81, 14–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Garcea FE, Chernoff BL, Diamond B, Lewis W, Sims MH, Tomlinson SB, Teghipco A, Belkhir R, Gannon SB, Erickson S, Smith SO, Stone J, Liu L, Tollefson T, Langfitt J, Marvin E, Pilcher WH, & Mahon BZ (2017). Direct electrical stimulation of the human brain disrupts music processing. Current Biology. 27(17), 2684–2691. e7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Garrett M (1980a). Levels of processing in sentence production In Butterworth B (Ed.), Language production (Vol. 1, pp. 177–220). London: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
  43. Garrett M (1980b). The limits of accommodation: Arguments for independent processing levels in sentence production In Fromkin VA (Ed.), Errors in linguistic performance: Slips of the tongue, ear, pen, and hand (pp. 263–271). New York: Academic Press [Google Scholar]
  44. Goryainov SA, Kondrashov AV, Gol’dberg MF, Batalov AI, Sufianov RA, Zakharova NE, … Potapov AA (2017). Long association tracts of the human white matter: an analysis of 18 hemisphere dissections and in vivo HARDI-CSD tractography. Problems of Neurosurgery Named After N.N Burdenko, 81(1), 13–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Grodzinsky Y (2006). A blueprint for a brain map of syntax In Grodzinsky Y & Amunts K (Eds.), Broca’s region. New York: Oxford University Press [Google Scholar]
  46. Grossman M (2012). The non-fluent/agrammatic variant of primary progressive aphasia. The Lancet Neurology, 11(6), 545–555. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Hampshire A, Chamberlain SR, Monti MM, Duncan J, & Owen AM (2010). The role of the right inferior frontal gyrus: inhibition and attentional control. Neuroimage, 50(3), 1313–1319. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Heim S, Eickhoff SB, & Amunts K (2008). Specialisation in Broca’s region for semantic, phonological, and syntactic fluency?. Neuroimage, 40(3), 1362–1368. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Herbet G, Moritz-Gasser S, Lemaitre AL, Almairac F, & Duffau H (2018). Functional compensation of the left inferior longitudinal fasciculus for picture naming. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 1–18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Hickok G, & Poeppel D (2007). The cortical organization of speech processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8(5), 393. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Hirshorn EA, Li Y, Ward MJ, Richardson RM, Fiez JA, & Ghuman AS (2016). Decoding and disrupting left midfusiform gyrus activity during word reading. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(29), 8162–8167. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Indefrey P, & Levelt WJ (2004). The spatial and temporal signatures of word production components. Cognition, 92(1-2), 101–144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Jenkinson M, & Smith S (2001). A global optimisation method for robust affine registration of brain images. Medical Image Analysis, 5(2), 143–156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Kaan E, & Swaab TY (2002). The brain circuitry of syntactic comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(8), 350–356. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Kan IP, Kable JW, Van Scoyoc A, Chatterjee A, & Thompson-Schill SL (2006). Fractionating the left frontal response to tools: Dissociable effects of motor experience and lexical competition. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 18(2), 267–277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Kay J, Coltheart M, Lesser R. 1992. Palpa: Psycholinguistic Asessments of Language Processing in Aphasia: Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]
  57. Kemerdere R, de Champfleur NM, Deverdun J, Cochereau J, Moritz-Gasser S, Herbet G, & Duffau H (2016). Role of the left Frontal Aslant Tract in stuttering: a brain stimulation and tractographic study. Journal of Neurology, 263(1), 157–167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Kennerley SW, Sakai K, & Rushworth MF (2004). Organization of action sequences and the role of the pre-SMA. Journal of neurophysiology, 91(2), 978–993. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Kinoshita M, de Champfleur NM, Deverdun J, Moritz-Gasser S, Herbet G, & Duffau H (2014). Role of fronto-striatal tract and Frontal Aslant Tract in movement and speech: an axonal mapping study. Brain Structure and Function, 220(6), 3399–3412. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Koutsarnakis C, Liakos F, Kalyvas A, Skandalakis G, Komaitis S, Christidi F,… Stranjalis G (2017). The Superior Frontal Transsulcal Approach to the Anterior Ventricular System: Exploring the Sulcal and Subcortical Anatomy Using Anatomic Dissections and Diffusion Tensor Imaging Tractography. World Neurosurgery, 106, 339–354. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Kristensen S, Garcea FE, Mahon BZ, & Almeida J (2016). Temporal frequency tuning reveals interactions between the dorsal and ventral visual streams. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 28(9), 1295–1302. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Kronfeld-Duenias V, Amir O, Ezrati-Vinacour R, Civier O, & Ben-Shachar M (2016a). Dorsal and ventral language pathways in persistent developmental stuttering. Cortex, 81, 79–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Kronfeld-Duenias V, Amir O, Ezrati-Vinacour R, Civier O, & Ben-Shachar M (2016b). The frontal aslant tract underlies speech fluency in persistent developmental stuttering. Brain Structure and Function, 221(1), 365–381. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Lawes INC, Barrick TR, Murugam V, Spierings N, Evans DR, Song M, & Clark CA (2008). Atlas-based segmentation of white matter tracts of the human brain using diffusion tensor tractography and comparison with classical dissection. NeuroImage, 39(1), 62–79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Leonard MK, Desai M, Hungate D, Cai R, Singhal NS, Knowlton RC, & Chang EF (2018). Direct cortical stimulation of inferior frontal cortex disrupts both speech and music production in highly trained musicians. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 1–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Levelt WJ (1993). Speaking: From intention to articulation (Vol. 1). MIT press. [Google Scholar]
  67. Li M, Zhang Y, Song L, Huang R, Ding J, Fang Y, … & Han Z (2017). Structural connectivity subserving verbal fluency revealed by lesion-behavior mapping in stroke patients. Neuropsychologia, 101, 85–96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Mahon BZ, Kumar N, & Almeida J (2013). Spatial frequency tuning reveals interactions between the dorsal and ventral visual systems. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25, 862–871. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Mandelli ML, Caverzasi E, Binney RJ, Henry ML, Lobach I, Block N, … Gorno-Tempini ML (2014). Frontal white matter tracts sustaining speech production in primary progressive aphasia. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(29), 9754–9767. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Martino J, de Lucas EM, Ibanez-Plagaro FJ, Valle-Folgueral JM, & Vazquez-Barquero A (2012). Foix-Chavany-Marie syndrome caused by a disconnection between the right pars opercularis of the inferior frontal gyrus and the supplementary motor area. Journal of Neurosurgery, 117(5), 844–850. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Nachev P, Wydell H, O’Neill K, Husain M, & Kennard C (2008). The role of the pre-supplementary motor area in the control of action. Neuroimage, 36, T155–T163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Nozari N, & Hepner CR (2018). To select or to wait? The importance of criterion setting in debates of competitive lexical selection. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 1–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Ojemann G, Ojemann J, Lettich EREEGT, & Berger M (1989). Cortical language localization in left, dominant hemisphere: an electrical stimulation mapping investigation in 117 patients. Journal of Neurosurgery, 71(3), 316–326. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Orena EF, Caldiroli D, Acerbi F, Barazzetta I, & Papagno C (2018). Investigating the functional neuroanatomy of concrete and abstract word processing through direct electric stimulation (DES) during awake surgery. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 1–11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Rapp B, & Goldrick M (2000). Discreteness and interactivity in spoken word production. Psychological Review, 107(3), 460. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Riès S, Legou T, Burle B, Alario FX, & Malfait N (2012). Why does picture naming take longer than word reading? The contribution of articulatory processes. Psychonomic bulletin & review, 19(5), 955–961. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Riley G (1994) Stuttering severity instrument for children and adults, 3rd Edition Austin: Pro-Ed. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Rofes A, Mandonnet E, de Aguiar V, Rapp B, Tsapkini K, & Miceli G (2018). Language processing from the perspective of electrical stimulation mapping. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 1–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Sahin NT, Pinker S, Cash SS, Schomer D, & Halgren E (2009). Sequential processing of lexical, grammatical, and phonological information within Broca’s area. Science, 326(5951), 445–449. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Sanai N, Mirzadeh Z, & Berger MS (2008). Functional outcome after language mapping for glioma resection. New England Journal of Medicine, 358(1), 18–27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Schnur TT (2017). Word selection deficits and multiword speech. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 34(1-2), 21–25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Shapiro K, Shelton J, & Caramazza A (2000). Grammatical class in lexical production and morhpological processing: Evidence from a case of fluent aphasia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 17(8), 665–682. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Shapiro K, & Caramazza A (2003). Grammatical processing of nouns and verbs in left frontal cortex?. Neuropsychologia, 41(9), 1189–1198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Shapiro KA, Moo LR, & Caramazza A (2006). Cortical signatures of noun and verb production. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103(5), 1644–1649. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Sierpowska J, Gabarrós A, Fernandez-Coello A, Camins A, Castañer S, Juncadella M, … Rodríguez-Fornells A (2015). Morphological derivation overflow as a result of disruption of the left Frontal Aslant white matter tract. Brain and Language, 142, 54–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Woolrich MW, Beckmann CF, Behrens TE, Johansen-Berg H, … & Niazy RK (2004). Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. NeuroImage, 23, S208–S219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Snodgrass J, Vanderwart M. 1980. A standardized set of 260 pictures: Norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Stasenko A, Garcea F, Dombovy M, & Mahon B (2014). When concepts lose their color: A case of object-color knowledge impairment. Cortex, 58, 217–238. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Szmuda T, Rogowska M, Sloniewski P, Abuhaimed A, Szmuda M, Springer J, … Skorek A (2017). Frontal aslant tract projections to the inferior frontal gyrus. Folia Morpholologica, 76(4), 574–581. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Thiebaut de Schotten M, Dell’Acqua F, Valabregue R, & Catani M (2012). Monkey to human comparative anatomy of the frontal lobe association tracts. Cortex, 48(1), 82–96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Thompson-Schill SL, D’Esposito M, Aguirre GK, & Farah MJ (1997). Role of left inferior prefrontal cortex in retrieval of semantic knowledge: a reevaluation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 94(26), 14792–14797. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Thompson-Schill SL, Bedny M, & Goldberg RF (2005). The frontal lobes and the regulation of mental activity. Current opinion in neurobiology, 15(2), 219–224. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Van der Linden L, Riès S, Legou T, Burle B, Malfait N and Alario A (2014). A comparison of two procedures for verbal response time fractionation. Frontiers in Psychology, section Language Sciences, 5: 1213. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Riès S, Legou T, Burle B, Alario A and Malfait N (2012). Why does picture naming take longer than word reading? The contribution of articulatory processes. Psychonomics Bulletin and Review. See also corrigendum here: doi: 10.3758/s13423-014-0668-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Vassal F, Boutet C, Lemaire J-J, & Nuti C (2014). New insights into the functional significance of the frontal aslant tract-An anatomo-functional study using intraoperative electrical stimulations combined with diffusion tensor imaging-based fiber tracking. British Journal of Neurosurgery, 28(October), 685–687. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Verbruggen F, & Logan GD (2008). Response inhibition in the stop-signal paradigm. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(11), 418–424. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Wang R & Wedeen VJ http://www.TrackVis.org Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital.

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

video

Supplemental Video 1. Intra-operative video showing subcortical stimulation of the frontal aslant tract. For each stimulation event, a coronal and sagittal slice is shown. The frontal aslant tract is shown in blue in the MRI images. The red dot depicts the location in the brain of the current stimulation event. Prior stimulation coordinates are shown as white spheres. This video shows representative stimulation and non-stimulation trials. Video from: www.tbm.urmc.org

RESOURCES