Skip to main content
Journal of the International Association of Providers of AIDS Care logoLink to Journal of the International Association of Providers of AIDS Care
. 2018 Feb 23;17:2325958218759208. doi: 10.1177/2325958218759208

HIV-Positive Patients’ Perceptions of Antiretroviral Therapy Adherence in Relation to Subjective Time: Imprinting, Domino Effects, and Future Shadowing

David Lessard 1,, Isabelle Toupin 1, Kim Engler 1, Andràs Lènàrt 1; I-Score Consulting Team1, Bertrand Lebouché 1
PMCID: PMC6748544  PMID: 29473484

Abstract

Antiretroviral treatment adherence barriers are major concerns in HIV care. They are multiple and change over time. Considering temporality in patients’ perceptions of adherence barriers could improve adherence management. We explored how temporality manifests itself in patients’ perceptions of adherence barriers. We conducted 2 semi-structured focus groups on adherence barriers with 12 adults with HIV which were analyzed with grounded theory. A third focus group served to validate the results obtained. Three temporal categories were manifest in HIV-positive patients’ perceptions of barriers: (1) imprinting (events with lasting impacts on patients), (2) domino effects (chain of life events), and (3) future shadowing (apprehension about long-term adherence). An overarching theme, weathering (gradual erosion of abilities to adhere), traversed these categories. These temporalities explain how similar barriers may be perceived differently by patients. They could be useful to providers for adapting their interventions and improving understanding of patients’ subjective experience of adherence.

Keywords: HIV, medication adherence, time perception, patient care management, antiretroviral therapy

Introduction

HIV is a chronic infection manageable with different regimens of antiretroviral drugs self-administered daily in pill form. Older regimens are still in use, but switching to newer and less-constraining ones (in terms of side effects and number of pills) is considered a means of improving patients’ adherence.1,2 “Adherence,” or the extent to which a person’s medication uptake corresponds with their clinician’s indications,3 is a major concern in HIV clinical practice.46 It is a condition for therapeutic success, particularly, a high CD4 count and an undetectable viral load, which reduces risks of disease progression, development of resistance, and secondary transmission.7,8 A meta-analysis suggests that almost half (45%) of HIV-positive patients in North America have suboptimal adherence (below 80%-100%, depending on the study).9

Antiretroviral treatment (ART) adherence is complex and difficult to measure. Adherence barriers vary between patients and over time.1013 Moreover, studies demonstrate that clinicians’ estimations of patients’ adherence can be erroneous14,15 and that ART adherence barriers are not always adequately discussed during clinical consultations.1618 Patients’ biomedical outcomes give only a partial assessment of adherence19 and supply no information on patients’ subjective experiences of ART-taking.

Given this complexity, a review of interpretative models of adherence in chronic conditions concluded that adherence interventions should include analyses of patients’ perceptions.10 “Patients’ perceptions” refer here to patients’ cognitive frameworks to make sense of their medical condition and treatments.20 Patients build these frameworks based on received information, individual experiences, and beliefs. They may differ in important ways from clinicians’ frameworks21 and influence how patients manage their treatment adherence.20,22 For instance, the association between HIV-positive patients’ negative perception of ART and low adherence has been demonstrated.23 In the investigation of patient’s perceptions in relation to adherence, a growing literature analyzes their subjective experiences of time.24

In HIV, few studies have examined how temporality affects patients’ perceptions of adherence barriers or have done so while focusing on specific dimensions of temporality, such as biographical disruptions25 or dailyART-taking.1 A recent narrative review highlighted the need for analyses accounting for different patients’ experiences of time.26 This analysis could shed light on the meanings, for HIV-positive patients, of past events, ART-taking routines, and their future, and help improve adherence interventions. Our objective is thus to explore how time is experienced in patients’ perceptions of their adherence barriers.

Methods

Context of the Study

Data were collected in the context of the Interference-Score (I-Score) Study, a 2-year multisited Canada/France project that aims to develop and validate a patient-reported outcome measure on ART adherence barriers for use in HIV care. A patient engagement project was initiated to integrate patients’ perspective in the I-Score Study. It consists of a Montreal-based group of 10 HIV-positive patients, the I-Score Consulting Team (henceforth, the Team),27 which meets periodically to discuss progress on the study. This article presents the results of the first research activity conducted with the Team. We obtained approval for the present study from the research ethics board of the McGill University Health Centre’s Research Institute, in Montreal, Canada.

Design and Conceptual Framework

This exploratory study draws on the concept of life course28,29 which is based on specific assumptions: one’s life events take place at specific moments, sometimes take time to fully unfold, and are influenced by those of others.28,29 It promises an understanding of complex phenomena that situates individual and dynamic experiences in relation to broader influences.30 It grounds descriptive observations and analyses in theories on people’s everyday lives1,31 and biographical disruptions.32 This framework has been employed in sociological studies of medical issues focused on chronic patients’ “real life” experiences.24,26,32,33

Sample, Data Collection, and Analysis

Participants were mainly recruited from the Team, constituted using a maximum variation sampling technique, a type of purposive sampling aimed at capturing a wide range of perspectives relating to a topic.34 We recruited one additional individual for each focus group, to compensate for any cancellations or no-shows.35 A total of 11 participants took part in 2 sex-specific focus groups in November 2015 led by I.T. and observed by D.L. and A.L. Focus groups lasted 120 and 135 minutes, respectively. They employed a semistructured format including an introductory round table discussion and open-ended questions on ART adherence barriers participants would like to report to their clinician and how they would like to report them. Participants were compensated (CAN$50).

Focus groups were audiotaped and transcribed. D.L. entered the transcriptions into the software Altas.ti (version 7.5.10, 1993-2016, by ATLAS.ti GmbH) and analyzed the data following the first 5 stages of the Grounded Theory method (coding, categorizing, association, integration, and modeling).36,37 These concurrent stages can inductively generate descriptions of “real life” dynamics and, by deconstructing and reconstructing the data, identify essential content, abstract concepts, and models.36 Analyses centered on all focus group content relevant to participants’ perceived adherence barriers. To improve the reliability of results, the coding grid employed was discussed with coauthors.

In order to validate our modeling,38 we conducted a third audio-recorded focus group to discuss the preliminary analyses with the Team, facilitated by D.L. (n = 8, 140 minutes). This focus group was held within one of the Team’s regular meetings. It thus included 1 participant (P12, a female Team member) who had been invited but could not participate in the first focus group. During this third focus group, D.L. asked participants to consider and explain in their own words the main categories developed and to confirm or qualify key statements of the model.38 This discussion was transcribed and integrated into the analysis, adjusting the coding grid as necessary. This exercise sought to validate our analysis, avoid biases, increase the soundness of our interpretations and conclusions in relation to the data,39 and refine our understanding of the categories and their interrelations.40

Results

Table 1 presents participants’ characteristics. Our analysis identified 3 temporal categories manifest in participants’ perceptions of adherence barriers: (1) imprinting, (2) domino effects, and (3) future shadowing. An essential theme, weathering, ran through these categories. We illustrate these temporal categories and the overarching theme in Table 2.

Table 1.

Characteristics of Participants in Each Focus Group, at the Exploratory and Validation Phases of Data Collection.

Participant Age Group (in Years) Group Number of Years on ART Exploratory Phase Validation Phase
Focus Group 1 Focus Group 2 Focus Group 3
P1 20-29 African woman self-identified as queer Over 10 years x
P2 50-59 African WSM Over 10 years x x
P3 50-59 White WSM 1-3 years x
P4 50-59 Latin American WSM Over 10 years x
P5 40-49 African WSM Over 10 years x x
P6 30-39 White MSM 3-6 years x x
P7 60-69 African MSW Over 10 years x x
P8 30-39 White MSM Over 10 years x x
P9 50-59 White MSW Over 10 years x
P10 30-39 European White MSM Less than 1 year x x
P11 50-59 White MSM Over 10 years x x
P12 50-59 White WSM Over 10 years x

Abbreviations: ART, antiretroviral treatment; MSM, man who has sex with men; MSW, man who has sex with women; WSM, woman who has sex with men.

Table 2.

Illustration of the Participants’ ART Adherence Barriers in Relation to the Temporal Categories.

Temporal Category 1: Imprinting
Focus Group Excerpt Imprinting Process and Impacts
Example 1
P11: “I was diagnosed in the winter of 83-84. My doctor told me my chances of survival, with the first cocktails, were almost nothing. (…) If I’ve been here this long, I must have been doing something right. Imprint Unpleasant and emotionally charged event: Survival despite the threat of death due to a lack of access to efficient ART for a long period after diagnosis
Today, it’s not enough for me just to take the drug, the right dose, at the right time. No, I’d be dead if I did just that. So many people who just took the drugs, relying on the drugs like a crutch, are dead. Impact on perception of ART Perception that only taking ART is not enough to maintain health and that doing so may be hazardous to one’s health.
I did my own research; I’m very aggressive when it comes to my health, very proactive, it’s my responsibility. The doctor’s there to help you with the diagnostic, but the first question I asked was what can I do? P1: “What interests me as a patient [who had limited access to ART for years before I learned my diagnosis] is to understand how I survived before I was officially HIV positive. What are the other drugs? Or, my aunts who raised me, what did they give me on the nutrition side? What kept me healthy while being HIV-positive? (…) I ate well. Do I need another advice like doing sports to simulate my immunity? I would like (the clinician) to inform me on natural products that we sometimes look for elsewhere.” Impact on ART adherence Proactive approach where adherence becomes one practice among others (researching, eating well, exercising, etc) to maintain health and manage HIV combined with an interest in alternative medicine
Example 2
P2: “In the past, I saw people with lipodystrophy. It was scary. When I began treatment, I thought: I will begin to be like this. (…) Imprint Unpleasant and emotionally charged event: Seeing HIV-infected persons with severe lipodystrophy
When I received my diagnosis, I thought about these people with HIV I had seen: they did not look good. I could see they were sick. Nowadays, there are no signs. People look healthy. But I put myself in this image. I thought: ‘This is what will happen to me.’ People will reject me if I start to lose weight. If I look sick, they will be afraid.(…) Impact on perception of ART While recognizing that the side effects of newer ART regimens have changed, an association between ART and lipodystrophy persists, with the fear that ART will make one visibly sick, leading to social exclusion
I would skip doses: ‘If I do not take it every day, maybe the effects will be less strong. It will not show as much’.” Impact on ART adherence Skipped doses to limit lipodystrophy
Example 3
P9: “I noticed my body odor changed with a drug. It is not really important, but when I have sexual intercourse, it is obvious for my partner. (…) Imprint Unpleasant and emotionally charged event: Side effect (odor) that impacts one’s sex life
It’s the first thing I think about when I switch drugs: ‘Ah! It will maybe change my smell.’ Impact on perception of ART View that other ART regimens may have a similar effect
We’ll take the drug anyway. At the end of the day, I’m still alive. It doesn’t matter if I stink like Pepe le Pew.” Impact on ART adherence Adherence for survival, despite possible side effects

Abbreviations: ART, antiretroviral treatment, The significance of boldface is to emphasize mechanisms of, respectively, imprinting, domino effects, future-shadowing, and weathering, in each of the examples.

Imprinting

“Imprinting” refers to the processes by which unpleasant and emotionally charged ART- or HIV-related events etch persistent memories with long-lasting and significant impacts on perceptions of ART and, consequently, adherence. This occurred in participants who lived for an extended period of time with HIV either without ART or with limited access to it (Example 1) and participants who witnessed (Example 2) or had (Example 3) unpleasant ART-related experiences (eg, side effects, interactions between ART and treatments for other health conditions, discrimination).

Domino Effects

“Domino effects” denote chain reactions in personal (eg, moving to another province or country, finding a new partner), professional (eg, academic or professional career changes), and/or medical events (eg, an ART regimen switch, the appearance of another health condition) that can impact perceptions of ART and/or adherence. Domino effects can reactivate a previously dormant barrier (Example 4), a barrier experienced in the past. They can also lead patients to consider ART a lower priority (Example 5) or as potentially detrimental to their well-being (Example 6). Finally, they can impact patients’ life choices which, in turn, can affect their behavior, contributing to adherence barriers (Example 7).

Future Shadowing

“Future shadowing” characterizes participants’ concerns raised by the long-term ART-taking, which can impact perceptions of ART and/or adherence. These concerns include the difficulty of projecting themselves into the long term (Example 8), preoccupations for ART toxicity and long-term side effects (Example 9), and the anticipation of repeatedly facing similar barriers (Example 10).

Overarching Theme: The Weathering of Adherence

Weathering refers here to the gradual erosion of patients’ well-being or ability (Example 11) or resolve (Example 12) to adhere by impacting perceptions of ART and/or adherence. It is caused by an accumulation over time of ART-related concerns or episodes of managing adherence barriers that stem from the dynamics of imprinting, domino effects, and future shadowing.

Discussion

Our analysis of focus groups conducted with HIV-positive patients revealed 3 temporalities and one theme relevant to patients’ perceptions of adherence barriers. First, imprinting refers to a linear temporality in which unpleasant events have lasting impacts on adherence. Second, domino effects form a processual temporality in which barriers emerge from life circumstances. Third, future shadowing refers to a prospective temporality underlining patients’ ambivalence toward life-long adherence. Like in other studies reporting on temporalities in chronic patients’ perceptions,26 temporal narratives “collide” in patients’ accounts. An overarching theme, “weathering,” related to participants’ reports of gradually losing motivation and capacity for adherence, runs through these categories.

Similar metaphorical usages of these terms were found in other scientific publications. “Imprint” has been used in psychology and ethology to designate phase-sensitive unconscious learning processes,41 to illustrate the impacts of experiences of care on patients’ memory and perception of treatment,42 and to refer to repercussions of sociopolitical conditions on how people define the HIV epidemic.43 The “domino effect” metaphor has been used to highlight interactions between different dimensions of a health condition and its treatment.44 The notion of “future shadowing” was inspired by that of a “dark future” which is used in a measure of pessimism.45 Weathering refers to the breaking down of minerals due to contact with elements.46 It has been used to refer to premature health deterioration due to adversity.47 We chose these terms as they illustrated well the participants’ temporal perceptions of adherence barriers.

Participants mentioned perceived barriers as diverse as side effects, meanings of life-long ART and of its costs, and discrimination. These barriers coincide with studies that show that nonadherence to ART is exacerbated by socioeconomic vulnerability,48 age,49,50 discrimination,51 and ART characteristics (eg, number of doses, side effects).52 Our results are coherent with an ART adherence model which presents adherence as a compromise between the conflicting demands of everyday life (including work demands, [disrupted] routines, and eating habits), side effects, depression, perceived ART effectiveness, and social support.53 They also provide rationales for how patients may give differential meaning to a similar barrier depending on its temporal quality and highlight how adherence barriers can gain traction in patient perceptions to ultimately impact adherence. Moreover, we show that adherence barriers may stem from concerns for one’s health or situation, for example, when one looks for alternative health practices, seeks to avoid discrimination, or faces financial difficulties.

Tentative implications for practice include the following points: (1) adherence should be recognized as a process, (2) perfect life-long adherence as a universal, “one-size-fits-all” objective is unrealistic, (3) adherence barriers must be considered in the context of each patient’s particular circumstances, and (4) interventions should include regular assessments of patients’ perceptions of adherence barriers and employ multidimensional tools that cover such elements as patient-reported barriers, values, perception of alternative health practices, significant life changes, and key moments of vulnerability, such as regimen switches. Indeed, the latter may jeopardize adherence by exposing patients to different side effects, indications, and costs.52 These could help identify potential sources of imprinting, domino effects, or future shadowing.

The 2 focus groups give this study an exploratory status, relevant for the subsequent steps of the I-Score Study. Further research and a larger sample could lead to a more complete model of adherence and temporality that is transferable to different contexts. The focus groups’ centering on adherence barriers may have emphasized negative aspects of ART at the expense of a more balanced perspective. We did not reach saturation with our analyses. However, rich data were collected from a diverse sample of people living with HIV and included a third focus group to maximize the analyses’ trustworthiness. Moreover, our results indicate that inquiring about HIV-positive patients’ temporal experiences of adherence may represent an interesting and relevant avenue for further research. The strength and originality of this study lie in its articulation of adherence barriers within a temporal framework that could prove useful for clinical evaluations.

Footnotes

Authors’ Note: D.L., I.T., and A.L. are the recipients of a Mitacs Accelerate postdoctoral internship award (#IT05857), and D.L. received an Interdisciplinary Postdoctoral Award of the Health Research Foundation for this study. The opinions expressed in this manuscript are those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of Merck Canada Inc, Gilead Sciences Canada Inc, or their affiliates or related companies.

Declaration of Conflicting Interests: The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Funding: The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: B.L. is supported by a Research Scholar award of the FRSQ (Quebec Health Research Funds). The I-Score Study is supported by the Canadian Institutes of Health Research’s HIV Clinical Trials Network (CTN 283). The study is also supported by a research grant from the Investigator Initiated Studies Program of Merck Canada (IISP-53538), and the Patient Engagement Project is supported by an unrestrictive grant from Gilead Sciences Canada Inc.

References

  • 1. McCoy L. Time, self and the medication day: a closer look at the everyday work of ‘adherence’. Soc Health Illness. 2009;31(1):128–146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Kim MJ, Kim SW, Chang HH, et al. Comparison of antiretroviral regimens: adverse effects and tolerability failure that cause regimen switching. Infect Chemother. 2015;47(4):231–238. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. World Health Organization. Defining Adherence. 2003. http://www.who.int/chp/knowledge/publications/adherence_Section1.pdf. Accessed February 12, 2018.
  • 4. Gunthard HF, Saag MS, Benson CA, et al. Antiretroviral drugs for treatment and prevention of HIV infection in adults: 2016 recommendations of the international antiviral society-USA panel. JAMA. 2016;316(2):191–210. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Cohen MS, Chen YQ, McCauley M, et al. ; HPTN 052 Study Team. Prevention of HIV-1 infection with early antiretroviral therapy. N Engl J Med. 2011;365(6):493–505. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Granich RM, Gilks CF, Dye C, De Cock KM, Williams BG. Universal voluntary HIV testing with immediate antiretroviral therapy as a strategy for elimination of HIV transmission: a mathematical model. Lancet. 2009;373(9657):48–57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Ammassari A, Trotta MP, Shalev N, Marconi P, Antinori A. Beyond virological suppression: the role of adherence in the late HAART era. Antivir Ther. 2012;17(5):785–792. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Lima VD, Harrigan R, Bangsberg DR, et al. The combined effect of modern highly active antiretroviral therapy regimens and adherence on mortality over time. J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr. 2009;50(5):529–536. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Mills EJ, Nachega JB, Buchan I, et al. Adherence to antiretroviral therapy in sub-Saharan Africa and North America: a meta-analysis. JAMA. 2006;296(6):679–690. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Tarquinio C, Tarquinio MP. L’observance thérapeutique: déterminants etmodèles théoriques. Pratiq Psycholo. 2007;13(1):1–19. [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Langebeek N, Gisolf EH, Reiss P, et al. Predictors and correlates of adherence to combination antiretroviral therapy (ART) for chronic HIV infection: a meta-analysis. BMC Med. 2014;12:142. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Atkinson MJ, Petrozzino JJ. An evidence-based review of treatment-related determinants of patients’ nonadherence to HIV medications. AIDS Patient Care STDS. 2009;23(11):903–914. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Lebouché B, Engler K, Levy JJ, et al. Minimal interference: a basis for selecting ART for prevention with positives. AIDS Care. 2013;25(10):1284–1290. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Phillips LA, Leventhal EA, Leventhal H. Factors associated with the accuracy of physicians’ predictions of patient adherence. Patient Educ Couns. 2011;85(3):461–467. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Miller LG, Liu H, Hays RD, et al. How well do clinicians estimate patients’ adherence to combination antiretroviral therapy? J Gen Intern Med. 2002;17(1):1–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Wilson IB, Laws MB, Safren SA, et al. Provider-focused intervention increases adherence-related dialogue but does not improve antiretroviral therapy adherence in persons with HIV. J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr. 2010;53(3):338–347. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Laws MB, Beach MC, Lee Y, et al. Provider-patient adherence dialogue in HIV care: results of a multisite study. AIDS Behav. 2013;17(1):148–159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Fredericksen R, Crane PK, Tufano J, et al. Integrating a web-based, patient-administered assessment into primary care for HIV-infected adults. J AIDS HIV Res (Online). 2012;4(2):47–55. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Adefolalu A, Nkosi Z. The complex nature of adherence in the management of HIV/AIDS as a chronic medical condition. Diseases. 2013;1(1):18–35. [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Keith JP, John W. Patients’ perceptions of their illness: the dynamo of volition in health care. Cur Direct Psychol Sci. 2012;21(1):60–65. [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Hagger M, Orbell S. A meta-analytic review of the common-sense model of illness representations. Psychol Health. 2003;18(2):141–184. [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Karamanidou C, Weinman J, Horne R. Improving haemodialysis patients’ understanding of phosphate-binding medication: a pilot study of a psycho-educational intervention designed to change patients’ perceptions of the problem and treatment. Br J Health Psychol. 2008;13(Pt 2):205–214. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Godin G, Cote J, Naccache H, Lambert LD, Trottier S. Prediction of adherence to antiretroviral therapy: a one-year longitudinal study. AIDS Care. 2005;17(4):493–504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Gergel TL. Illness perception, time perception and phenomenology—an extended response to Borrett. J Eval Clin Pract. 2013;19(3):501–508. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Carricaburu D, Pierret J. From biographical disruption to biographical reinforcement: the case of HIV-positive men. Soc Health Illness. 1995;17(1):65. [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Jowsey T. Time and chronic illness: a narrative review. Qual Life Res. 2016;25(5):1093–1102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Lessard D, Engler K, Toupin I, Lebouché B. Developing and evaluating a patient engagement intervention in the I-Score Study: involving HIV patients in the improvement of treatment adherence. In: Research CAFH, ed. 25th Annual Canadian Conference on HIV/AIDS Research: Realizing our potential Winnipeg, MA: Local to Global and Back; 2016. [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Bidart C. Crises, décisions et temporalités: autour des bifurcations biographiques. Cah internationaux de sociologie. 2006;120(1):29–57. [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Giele JZ, Elder GH. Life course research. Development of a field In: Giele JZ, Elder GH, eds. Methods of Life Course Research Qualitative and Quantitative Approaches. Thousand Oaks: SAGE Publications; 1998. [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Scott J, Alwin D. Retrospective versus prospective measurement of life histories in longitudinal research In: Giele JZ, Elder GH, eds. Methods of Life Course Research Qualitative and Quantitative Approaches. Thousand Oaks: SAGE Publications; 1998: 98–127. [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Conrad P. The meaning of medications: another look at compliance. Soc Sci Med. 1985;20(1):29–37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Bury M. The sociology of chronic illness: a review of research and prospects. Soc Health Illness. 1991;13(4):451–468. [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Strauss AL. Chronic Illness and the Quality of Life. St Louis, MO: Mosby; 1975. [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Marshal MN. Sampling for qualitative research. Fam Pract. 1996;13(6):522–525. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Marchand J, Giroux C. L’utilisation des groupes de discussion en marketing commercial et social. Rec Qual. 2010;29(1):99–109. [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Glaser BG, Strauss AL. The Discovery of Grounded Theory: Strategies for Qualitative Research. Chicago: Aldine Pub. Co; 1967. [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Paillé P, Lacroix JG, Descarries F, Vandelac L. L’analyse par théorisation ancrée. Cahiers de recherche sociologique. 1994(23):147–181. [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Van der Maren JM. Méthodes De Recherche Pour l’Éducation, 2e éd. Paris: De Boeck & Larcier s.a; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Noble H, Smith J. Issues of validity and reliability in qualitative research. Evid Based Nurs. 2015;18(2):34–35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Trudel L, Simard C, Vonarx N. La recherche qualitative est-elle nécessairement exploratoire? In: Guillemette F, Baribeau C, eds. Colloque pour la recherche qualitative de l’Association canadienne francophone pour l’avancement de la science. Montréal, Association pour la recherche qualitative; 2006: 38–45. [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Immelmann K. Sexual and other long-term aspects of imprinting in birds and other species. Advances in the Study of Behavior. 1972;4:147–174. [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Samuelson KA. Unpleasant and pleasant memories of intensive care in adult mechanically ventilated patients—findings from 250 interviews. Intensive Crit Care Nurs. 2011;27(2):76–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Fassin D. The embodied past. From paranoid style to politics of memory in South Africa. Soc Anthropol. 2008;16(3):312–328. [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Lidón RM. Domino effect in quality of care: improving one aspect of a disease can improve its entire spectrum. Rev Esp Cardiol. 2016;69(8):730–731. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Beck AT, Weissman A, Lester D, Trexler L. The measurement of pessimism: the hopelessness scale. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1974;42(6):861–865. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Hall K. The role of thermal stress fatigue in the breakdown of rock in cold regions. Geomorphology. 1999;31(1):47–63. [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Geronimus AT, Hicken M, Keene D, Bound J. “Weathering” and age patterns of allostatic load scores among blacks and whites in the United States. Am J Public Health. 2006;96(5):826–833. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Public Health Agency of Canada. Population-Specific HIV/AIDS Status Report: People Living with HIV/AIDS 2015. 2015; [Available from: http://www.phac-aspc.gc.ca/aids-sida/publication/ps-pd/people-personnes/chapter-chapitre-2-eng.php.
  • 49. High KP, Brennan-Ing M, Clifford DB, et al. HIV and aging: state of knowledge and areas of critical need for research. A report to the NIH Office of AIDS Research by the HIV and Aging Working Group. J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr. 2012;(60 Suppl 1):S1–S18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Ettenhofer ML, Hinkin CH, Castellon SA, et al. Aging, neurocognition, and medication adherence in HIV infection. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2009;17(4):281–290. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Katz IT, Ryu AE, Onuegbu AG, et al. Impact of HIV-related stigma on treatment adherence: systematic review and meta-synthesis. J Int AIDS Soc. 2013;16(3 Suppl 2):18640. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Nachega JB, Parienti JJ, Uthman OA, et al. Lower pill burden and once-daily antiretroviral treatment regimens for HIV infection: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Clin Infect Dis. 2014;58(9):1297–1307. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Adam BD, Maticka-Tyndale E, Cohen JJ. Adherence practices among people living with HIV. AIDS Care. 2003;15(2):263–274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of the International Association of Providers of AIDS Care are provided here courtesy of SAGE Publications

RESOURCES