Skip to main content
Brain and Behavior logoLink to Brain and Behavior
. 2019 Aug 25;9(9):e01376. doi: 10.1002/brb3.1376

Emotional stability is associated with the MAOA promoter uVNTR polymorphism in women

Ángel Rodríguez‐Ramos 1,2,3,4,, Juan Antonio Moriana 2,3,4, Francisco García‐Torres 2,3,4, Manuel Ruiz‐Rubio 1,3,4
PMCID: PMC6749489  PMID: 31448578

Abstract

Background

Neuroticism is associated with low emotional stability, and it is characterized by a tendency to perceive ordinary situations as threatening and difficult to manage. This personality trait has been associated with psychological distress and predicts some mental disorders. Previous studies have shown that women tend to be more neurotic than men and, in general, females have also a higher incidence of anxious and depressive disorders.

Methods

We analyzed in a sample of 99 female university students (from 18 to 26 years old) if emotional stability, measured using the Big Five Questionnaire, was linked to polymorphic variants in candidate genes related to dopaminergic and serotonergic systems, and other personality variables.

Results

We found that emotional stability and its subdimensions are genetically associated with MAOA‐uVNTR polymorphism. Thus, women carriers of the 3‐repeat allele (lower MAO‐A expression) showed higher levels of emotional stability. No associations were found with other polymorphisms analyzed, including COMT Val158Met, 5‐HTTLPR, and DAT 3′UTR VNTR. Furthermore, our results showed a negative correlation between emotional stability and depression, state anxiety, and trait anxiety. In fact, MAOA‐uVNTR and trait anxiety also explained emotional stability and its subdimensions. We also found that other genetic characteristic, phenylthiocarbamide tasting, explained impulsivity, specifically tasters controlled impulses better than nontasters.

Conclusion

Our results indicate that neuroticism might be regulated by MAOA and could be a common factor between different phenotypes, such as aggressive behaviors or personality disorders, observed in women with higher activity genotype who had been exposed to negative environments during childhood. This study could lead to a better understanding of the basis of emotional stability and could lead to future projects for this purpose.

Keywords: anxiety, emotional stability, MAOA, neuroticism, phenylthiocarbamide

1. INTRODUCTION

Neuroticism is a personality trait characterized by a tendency to perceive ordinary situations as threatening and difficult to manage. It correlates with psychological distress and predicts some mental and physical disorders (Slavish et al., 2018). Neuroticism is included in almost all major models of personality traits (Lahey, 2009) and has been associated with emotional instability (even both concepts are used as synonyms). Neurotic individuals tend to worry, experience more negative emotions, and have a poor control of their impulses and desires (Ormel et al., 2013; Servaas et al., 2013). Moreover, persons who score high in neuroticism often feel personally inadequate, and are self‐critical and sensitive to the criticism of others (Lahey, 2009).

Previous studies have shown that women tend to be more neurotic than men (Lynn & Martin, 1997; Weisberg, Deyoung, & Hirsh, 2011). The magnitude of these differences become fewer with age, although a small difference is present even in old age (Chapman, Duberstein, Sörensen, & Lyness, 2007; Soto, John, Gosling, & Potter, 2011). Higher neuroticism scores have been related to some of the most common mental disorders, including mood, anxiety, and substance use disorders (Ormel et al., 2013), and with an unfavorable course of them (Jeuring et al., 2018; Struijs, Lamers, Spinhoven, van der Does, & Penninx, 2018). The higher scores in neuroticism of women respect to men have been related to the fact that women have higher incidence of anxious and depressive disorders than men (Alonso et al., 2004; Riecher‐Rössler, 2017).

Heritability studies using twins have proposed that neuroticism is moderately heritable (~47%) (Boomsma et al., 2018; Docherty et al., 2016; Kim et al., 2017; Power & Pluess, 2015). A recent study has found an association between the dopaminergic system and neuroticism, but only in contexts where there is high climate stress (Fischer, Lee, & Verzijden, 2018). Two genes of this system have been widely associated with certain behaviors and psychiatric conditions: COMT and DAT (Gatt, Burton, Williams, & Schofield, 2015). COMT is an enzyme involved in degradation of catecholamines such as dopamine, epinephrine, and norepinephrine. The most common genetic variant is a change of valine to methionine (Val158Met) in the protein amino acid sequence. Met reduces enzyme activity at only 25% of the Val isoform (Chen et al., 2004; Lotta et al., 1995). On the other hand, DAT is involved in regulating the dopaminergic transmission, reuptaking dopamine from the synaptic cleft to the presynaptic neuron. In the 3′UTR of its coding gene exists a 40 bp variable number of tandem repeats (VNTR) that can present between 3 and 11 copies, being the most common alleles the 9‐ and 10‐repeats forms (Costa, Riedel, Müller, Möller, & Ettinger, 2011). This VNTR affects gene expression, where 9 repeats seem to be associated with higher dopamine reuptaking efficiency than the 10‐repeat allele, although these results are inconsistent with other studies (Del Hoyo et al., 2016; Guo, Roettger, & Shih, 2007).

Nevertheless, actually the most investigated pathway has been the serotonergic system. Current literature suggests that lower synaptic serotonin levels are physiologically responsible of higher neuroticism (Tuominen et al., 2017). 5‐HTT transports the serotonin from the synaptic cleft into the presynaptic neurons. A 44 bp insertion/deletion polymorphism in the promoter region of its coding gene (5‐HTTLPR) is associated with changes in the transcriptional activity of the gene, presenting the long allele (L) an increased expression and serotonin reuptake relative to the short variant (S). This polymorphism has been widely studied and has been suggested that has a pleiotropic effect in different mental disorders (Gatt et al., 2015). Human studies linking 5‐HTT and neuroticism have been inconsistent. However, a recent study has shown that neuroticism and 5‐HTT expression are linked in a sex‐dependent manner. In women, higher neuroticism scores seem to be associated with lower 5‐HTT binding in thalamus, whereas in males, higher scores were associated with higher binding (Tuominen et al., 2017).

Enzymes that regulate serotonin levels, such as monoamine oxidases (MAOs), could be also important. MAOs are a family of enzymes that catalyze the oxidative deamination of some neurotransmitters and dietary amines. In human, there are two types of MAOs: MAO‐A and MAO‐B. These protein‐coding genes, MAOA and MAOB, are located in the X‐chromosome (Grimsby, Chen, Wang, Lan, & Shih, 1991). They have differences in its substrates, tissue‐cell distribution, and other properties. Thus, dopamine, tyramine, and tryptamine are substrates for both MAO‐A and MAO‐B. Nonetheless, MAO‐A preferentially oxidizes the biogenic amines serotonin and norepinephrine (Kalgutkar, Dalvie, Castagnoli, & Taylor, 2001; Shih & Chen, 2004). Brunner syndrome is caused by a MAOA mutation that leads to a MAO‐A deficiency and therefore an excess of monoamine neurotransmitters. This rare genetic disorder is characterized by borderline mental retardation and tendency to violence (Brunner, Nelen, Breakefield, Ropers, & Oost, 1993; Hunter, 2010). MAOA has a polymorphism 1.2 kb upstream of its coding sequence. This 30 bp VNTR can present 2 (Guo, Ou, Roettger, & Shih, 2008), 3, 3.5, 4 or 5 copies (Sabol, Hu, & Hamer, 1998), affecting the transcriptional expression of the enzyme. Alleles containing 3.5 or 4 repeats are expressed more efficiently than alleles containing either 2, 3 or 5 repeats (Guo et al., 2008; Sabol et al., 1998). Alleles 3 and 4 are the most common forms (Pavlov, Chistiakov, & Chekhonin, 2012). Since MAOA is involved in controlling the levels of monoamine neurotransmitters, many studies have focused in genetic associations of MAOA‐uVNTR with psychological traits and disorders (Bortolato, Floris, & Shih, 2018; Liu, Huang, Luo, Wu, & Li, 2016). Caspi et al. (2002) provided the first evidence of a gene–environment interaction of the MAOA polymorphism that later have been confirmed by several studies (Kim‐Cohen et al., 2006). In this manner, males with low MAO‐A activity genotype who have been exposed to maltreatment tend to show more antisocial and aggressive behaviors. Simultaneously, recent evidence has suggested that this tendency would happen also in women but in those with high MAO‐A activity genotype (Aslund et al., 2011; Byrd & Manuck, 2014; McGrath et al., 2012; Prom‐Wormley et al., 2009; Sjöberg et al., 2007; Verhoeven et al., 2012; Wakschlag et al., 2010). MAOA has been related to other mental health problems in addition to antisocial behaviors. Recent studies have suggested that could exist a common factor for all these phenotypes, and that could be emotion regulation (Nilsson, Åslund, Comasco, & Oreland, 2018). Then, women with high MAO‐A activity genotype who had experienced childhood maltreatment would present higher levels of emotional reactivity that could predict some pathologies, with the reverse pattern in males. Thereby, MAOA x maltreatment would regulate indirectly through emotional reactivity a higher risk of some pathologies rather than through a direct way (Byrd et al., 2019).

Although aggressiveness and impulsivity are two separated constructs, they are closely related (García‐Forero, Gallardo‐Pujol, Maydeu‐Olivares, & Andrés‐Pueyo, 2009). In the Big Five Questionnaire (Caprara, Barbaranelli, Borgogni, & Perugini, 1993), the dimension Emotional Stability‐Neuroticism is subdivided into emotion control and impulse control. So, this construct is highly related to impulsivity, which is a multidimensional neuropsychological construct that can be defined as “a predisposition toward rapid, unplanned reactions to internal or external stimuli without regard to the negative consequences of these reactions to the impulsive individual or to others” (Moeller, Barratt, Dougherty, Schmitz, & Swann, 2001). Despite any person can present impulsivity, it is more probable to be present in people with certain psychiatric disorders, such as ADHD, mania, substance abuse/dependence, and some personality disorders (Kulacaoglu & Kose, 2017; Moeller et al., 2001).

Some psychiatric conditions as anhedonia (Thomas, Al‐Mesaabi, Bahusain, & Mutawa, 2014) and different personality traits (Kimmel & Lester, 1987; Mascie‐Taylor, McManus, MacLarnon, & Lanigan, 1983; Very & Iacono, 1968) have been related to phenylthiocarbamide (PTC) tasting. PTC is an organic compound that either tastes bitter or is tasteless. This capacity to taste the PTC appears to depend on a simple dominant Mendelian gene (tasters > nontasters), although some reports suggest a more complex inheritance and expression of this trait (Guo & Reed, 2001; Keller & Adise, 2016). This capacity has been also related to numerous conditions such as food selection and obesity (Veluswami et al., 2015), smoking (Risso et al., 2016), or disorders as schizophrenia (Moberg et al., 2007).

Since MAOA‐uVNTR x maltreatment have been associated with emotional reactivity, we proposed that MAOA could be related in some way with emotional stability–neuroticism personality trait. Likewise, various studies have investigated whether there exists a genetic association between PTC tasting, COMT, 5‐HTT, and DAT polymorphisms with this trait, and several discrepancies have been detected. In the present study, we attempted to extend in an integrative way the knowledge of how neuroticism, emotion control, and impulse control are affected by these variables in women.

2. MATERIALS AND METHODS

2.1. Participants

The initial sample was N = 132, students of the University of Córdoba (Spain). However, some subjects of the study were dismissed due to missing data, unwillingness to participate, or age higher than the established for the study. Finally, ninety‐nine healthy women between 18 and 26 years old (mean = 20.89 years; SD = 1.846 years) took part in the current research. The participants were randomly selected among the students of two university centers. All procedures were carried out with the understanding and written consent of each subject. Once informed consent was provided, participants were assigned with an individual code and completed an anonymous self‐report survey. They also permitted to collect buccal swabs for genomic DNA. The study was approved by “Comité de Ética de la Investigación de Córdoba, Hospital Universitario Reina Sofía, Córdoba (Spain)”.

2.2. Measures

2.2.1. Big Five Questionnaire (BFQ)

The Spanish version of the BFQ (Caprara, Barbaranelli, & Borgogni, 1995) consists of 132 multiple‐choice items of a 5‐points Likert scale. This questionnaire is a personality test based on the Five‐Factor Model that identifies five fundamental dimensions in the human personality (Energy, Friendliness, Conscientiousness, Emotional Stability, and Openness). In the BFQ, each dimension is subdivided into two subdimensions. These dimensions and its subdimensions are measured in a continuum where an individual might be anywhere between the two extremes of each one. The BFQ also includes a Distortion scale with the purpose of identifying altered profiles. In the present study, we will only mention the dimension emotional stability–neuroticism that is subdivided into impulse control and emotion control.

2.2.2. State‐Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI)

The STAI is a psychological questionnaire commonly used to estimate state and trait anxiety (Spielberger, Gorsuch, & Lushene, 1970). The Spanish version of this inventory consists of 20 items for assessing state anxiety and 20 for trait. In each item, subjects indicate in which extend they agree or disagree with the statements with a 4‐point Likert scale as “very much”, “likely”, “not so much,” or “not at all”, where the response scale ranges from 0 to 3 points (Buela‐Casal, Guillén‐Riquelme, & Seisdedos Cubero, 2011). Higher scores indicate greater anxiety. State anxiety reflects a transitory emotional state or “right now” condition. However, trait anxiety represents stable individual differences in the tendency to anxiety and refers to a prone to respond with “general” anxiety.

2.2.3. Beck's Depression Inventory (BDI‐II)

The Spanish version of BDI‐II is a 21‐question multiple‐choice self‐report inventory to measure symptoms of depression, in which each item was rated on a scale from 0 to 3 (Beck, Steer, & Brown, 2011). The total score was calculated by summing the score of the 21 items and oscillates between 0 and 63 points, where higher scores indicate greater symptom severity. Different cutoff ranges have been established: 0–13 minimal depression, 14–19 mild depression, 20–28 moderate depression, and 29–63 severe depression (Beck, Steer, & Brown, 1996).

2.3. Genotyping

Genomic DNA was extracted and purified from buccal swabs through the HigherPurity™ Buccal Swab Genomic DNA Extraction Kit (Canvax Biotech S.L.). Different polymorphisms were genotyped through polymerase chain reaction (PCR) amplification (Canvax Biotech S.L.) in a final volume of 10 μl, containing: 1 μl of about 100 ng DNA, 1 μl of 10x PCR Buffer, 1 μl of 25 mM MgCl2, 1 μl of 8 mM dNTP, 3 μl of 10x GC‐enhancer, 2 μl of 10x Y‐enhancer, 0.25 μl of forward primer (15 pmol/μl), 0.25 μl of forward reverse (15 pmol/μl), 0.25 μl of Mili‐Q water, and 0.25 μl Taq DNA polymerase (5 U/μl).

2.3.1. Monoamine oxidase A (MAOA)

For genotyping MAOA‐uVNTR polymorphism, we used the following primers based on Sabol et al. (1998): forward 5′‐ACA GCC TGA CCG TGG AGA AG‐3′ and reverse 5′‐GAA CGG ACG CTC CAT TCG GA‐3′. PCR conditions were an initial denaturalization of 8 min at 95°C, 40 cycles of 30 s at 94°C, 30 s at 58.2°C, and 1 min at 72°C; followed by a final elongation of 8 min at 72°C. PCR products were separated and visualized using 2.5% agarose gels with ethidium bromide. This protocol is a modification of McGrath et al. (2012).

Alleles with 3 repeats were categorized as “low” activity, while those with 4 repeats were categorized as “high” activity. Genotype frequencies of the genetic polymorphisms in the sample are collected in Table 1. Genotype frequencies of our sample are similar to previously described (Table 1), so we assumed that it was representative of the population. The population was in Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium, χ 2(1, N = 99)=1.79, < .05. Since MAOA gene is located in the X‐chromosome, it is easy to determine whether they present high or low MAOA activity in men. However, it is not clear if in women the MAOA gene of the second chromosome is silenced or not. Some studies have shown that MAOA expression is silenced on the inactive X‐chromosome in women (Nordquist & Oreland, 2006; Stabellini, Vasques, de Mello, Hernandes, & Pereira, 2009). Others, even though do not find a complete X‐inactivation of the MAOA, took to indicate the existence of an alternative process of dosage compensation in females highly variable among individuals (Pinsonneault, Papp, & Sadée, 2006). Taking this into account, if X‐inactivation/regulation is sufficiently random across cells, in heterozygous this would predict an intermediate response generally lower than the homozygous for 4‐repeat allele as other have previously observed (Meyer‐Lindenberg et al., 2006). Since it is not completely understood, we preferred to take both options into consideration. Likewise, since the 4‐repeat allele is the most common allele, we had a little percentage of women homozygous for 3 repeats. For these reasons, we considered to create only two groups, one with the highest expression (homozygous for 4‐repeat allele) and other with lower expression (heterozygous and homozygous for 3‐repeat allele).

Table 1.

Distribution of the genetic polymorphisms studied in the sample

Variable n % %a
5‐HTT     Arias et al. (2012)
L/L 24 24.2 27.4
L/S 47 47.5 48
S/S 28 28.3 24.6
DAT     Sáiz et al. (2010)
9R/9R 8 8.1 10.89
9R/10R 47 47.5 42.9
10R/10R 41 41.4 42.25
10R/11R 3 3.0 Ns
MAOA     Rivera et al. (2009)
3R/3R 12 12.1 17
3R/4R 37 37.4 39
4R/4R 50 50.5 42
COMT     Sánchez‐Morán et al. (2018)
Val/Val 27 27.3 25.0
Val/Met 49 49.5 50.0
Met/Met 23 23.2 25.0
a

Proportions previously described in Spanish populations.

2.3.2. Catechol‐O‐methyltransferase (COMT)

We genotyped the COMT Val158Met polymorphism using the following primers: forward 5′‐ACT GTG GCT ACT CAG CTG TG‐3′ and reverse 5′‐CCT TTT TCC AGG TCT GAC AA‐3′. As described by Bishop, Fossella, Croucher, and Duncan (2008), PCR conditions were an initial denaturalization of 5 min at 94°C, 12 cycles of 30 s at 94°C, 45 s at 58°C, and 35 s at 72°C; followed by 28 cycles of 30 s at 94°C, 45 s at 50°C, and 30 s at 72°C. The final point was an elongation of 5 min at 72°C. PCR products were digested using the restriction enzyme NlaIII, also known as Hin1II (Thermo Scientific). After digested, samples were separated in a 3% agarose gel with ethidium bromide. The population was in Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium, χ 2(1, N = 99) = 0, < .05.

2.3.3. Dopamine transporter (DAT)

We genotyped DAT 3′UTR VNTR using the following primers based on Vandenbergh et al. (1992): forward 5′‐ TGC GGT GTA GGG AAC GGC CTGA GA‐3′ and reverse 5′‐GTG TGG TCT GCA GGC TGC CTG CAT‐3′. PCR conditions were an initial denaturalization of 5 min at 95°C, 40 cycles of 30 s at 95°C, 30 s at 68°C, and 90 s at 72°C and a final elongation of 5 min at 72°C. PCR products were separated in a 2% agarose gel with ethidium bromide. The population was in Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium, χ 2(1, N = 99) = 2.70, < .05.

2.3.4. Serotonin transporter (5‐HTT)

We genotyped 5‐HTTLPR polymorphism using the following primers based on Lesch et al. (1996): forward 5′‐GGC GTT GCC GCT CTG AAT GC‐3′ and reverse 5′‐ GAG GGA CTG AGC TGG ACA ACC AC‐3′. PCR conditions were an initial denaturalization of 5 min at 95°C, 40 cycles of 30 s at 95°C, 30 s at 68°C, and 90 s at 72°C and a final elongation of 5 min at 72°C. PCR products were separated in a 2% agarose gel with ethidium bromide. The population was in Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium, χ 2(1, N = 99) = 0.161, < .05.

2.4. Phenylthiocarbamide (PTC) taste test strips

The PTC test strips were pre‐prepared in the laboratory using rectangular fragments of filter paper impregnate with a suprathreshold PTC solution. Stock solutions were freshly prepared containing 2.60 g of the recrystallized material per liter (Lugg & Whyte, 1955). The subjects were asked to hold a filter paper without the solution (control), and later with the solution (test), in her mouth for 5–10 s and tell if they taste a bitter flavor or not in the second one. Depending on it, they were grouped in “tasters” and “nontasters,” respectively.

2.5. Statistical analysis

Statistical analysis was carried out using IBM SPSS Statistics 20. Groups were compared using independent ANOVA test on genetic polymorphisms, PTC tasting, and sociodemographic variables. Pearson correlation coefficient was used to calculate bivariate correlations among the different quantitative variables analyzed. Finally, three multiple linear regressions were carried out using the stepwise method in order to stablish the explanatory power of all the independent variables measured herein (state and trait anxiety, depression, MAOA, COMT, 5‐HTT and DAT polymorphisms, and PTC tasting) over the selected dependent variables (emotional stability and its subdimensions). Results were accepted as significant with a confidence level of 95%.

3. RESULTS

3.1. Emotional Stability negative correlates with Anxiety and Depression levels

Mean scores of the self‐report questionnaires have been brought together in Table 2; ANOVA and correlation analysis results have been collected in Tables 3 and 4, respectively. As could be expected, we found a negative correlation between emotional stability and state and trait anxiety. There was also a negative correlation between emotional stability and depression. These negative correlations existed even with the two subdimensions of the emotional stability.

Table 2.

Mean score and standard deviation of self‐report variables

Variable n % Mean SD
Anxiety (STAI)        
State 99   14.89 9.335
Trait 99   21.48 9.693
Depression (BDI) 99   6.27 5.776
BFQ        
Emotional stability 99   51.47 10.205
Emotion control 99   51.17 9.316
Impulse control 99   51.48 10.948
PTC        
Taster 74 74.7    
Nontaster 25 25.3    

Abbreviations: BDI, Beck's Depression Inventory; BFQ, Big Five Questionnaire; PTC, phenylthiocarbamide; STAI, State‐Trait Anxiety Inventory.

Table 3.

Statistical results of ANOVA analysis

Variable 1 Variable 2 F (1,99) p value
Emotional stability MAOA 6.925 .010
COMT 1.218 .299
5‐HTT 0.248 .781
DAT 0.621 .603
Phenylthiocarbamide 1.499 .224
Impulse control MAOA 10.056 .002
COMT 1.377 .256
5‐HTT 0.118 .888
DAT 0.671 .572
Phenylthiocarbamide 6.118 .015
Emotion control MAOA 2.780 .099
COMT 0.880 .417
5‐HTT 0.564 .571
DAT 0.621 .603
Phenylthiocarbamide 0.065 .800

Table 4.

Statistical results of correlation analysis

Variable 1 Variable 2 r (97) p value
Emotional stability State anxiety −.469 .001
Trait anxiety −.674 .001
Depression −.523 .001
Impulse control State anxiety −.343 .001
Trait anxiety −.527 .001
Depression −.387 .001
Emotion control State anxiety −.498 .001
Trait anxiety −.699 .001
Depression −.566 .001

3.2. Association of low‐activity MAO‐A allelic variant with emotional stability

We also found a genetic association between MAOA‐uVNTR and neuroticism in females. Women carriers of the 3‐repeat allele showed higher levels of emotional stability, and then lower neuroticism, than those noncarriers of this allele. Referring to the subdimensions of emotional stability, carriers of the 3‐repeat allele also presented higher impulse control. However, there was no significant difference in emotion control, but it showed a tendency toward significance.

3.3. No association between COMT, 5‐HTT, and DAT polymorphisms and emotional stability

We checked out also if there was a genetic association between COMT, 5‐HTT, and DAT polymorphisms and neuroticism or its subdimensions, however, we did not find any significant result.

3.4. Phenylthiocarbamide tasting capacity is linked to a better impulse control

The frequency of phenylthiocarbamide tasters in the sample (Table 2) was similar to previous studies carried out in Spanish population (Guo & Reed, 2001; Pons, 1955). We found that phenylthiocarbamide tasters controlled better their impulses than nontasters. However, no significant differences were found in emotion control or emotional stability.

3.5. Regression models of emotional stability, impulse control, and emotion control

Furthermore, we evaluated the explanatory power of the variables measured herein over emotional stability, impulse control, and emotion control. For this purpose, we first included all the variables in the regression analysis, and later, we were discarding those that were not significant until only significant variables were included in the model.

The results showed that variables included in the different models were significant (Table 5). In our model for emotional stability, trait anxiety and MAOA polymorphism explained the 51% of the variance observed in our sample (R 2 = .512, F(2,99) = 52.453, < .001). In the case of impulse control, the 38.6% of the variance was explained by trait anxiety, MAOA polymorphism, and PTC tasting (R 2 = .386, F(3,99) = 21.503, < .001). Finally, the 50.7% of the variance observed in emotion control was explained by trait anxiety and MAOA polymorphism (R 2 = .507, F(2,99) = 51.414, < .001). Residual plots of the different models have been included in Figure S1.

Table 5.

Regression models of emotional stability, impulse control, and emotion control

Predictor variable B SE B β t p
Model for emotional stability          
Trait anxiety −0.711 0.074 −.675 −9.566 <.001
MAOA (high MAO‐A activity genotype) −5.307 1.433 −.261 −3.704 <.001
Model for impulse control          
Trait anxiety −0.594 0.089 −.526 −6.638 <.001
MAOA (high MAO‐A activity genotype) −5.936 1.762 −.272 −3.369 <.001
PTC (tasters) 4.499 2.028 .179 2.219 .029
Model for emotion control          
Trait anxiety −0.672 0.068 −.700 −9.863 <.001
MAOA (high MAO‐A activity genotype) −3.156 1.315 −.170 −2.400 .018

4. DISCUSSION

The present study was designed to examine the role that several genetic polymorphisms related to the dopaminergic and serotonergic pathways play in neuroticism. We have focused in women since they have higher incidence of mental disorders than men, especially anxious and depressive disorders (Alonso et al., 2004; Riecher‐Rössler, 2017). Several studies have reported that women often show higher scores in neuroticism than men (Lynn & Martin, 1997; Weisberg et al., 2011). At the same time, higher neuroticism scores have been related to some affective disorders (Ormel et al., 2013) and an unfavorable course of them (Jeuring et al., 2018; Struijs et al., 2018). These data agree with the results present herein that show a negative correlation between emotional stability (and its subdimensions) and anxiety and depression levels. Thereby, maybe sex differences in neuroticism might explain the fact that women suffer more from this type of mental disorders (Kendler & Gardner, 2014).

4.1. MAO‐A and the serotonergic system: Implications in emotional stability

Twin studies have suggested that neuroticism has a moderate genetic heritability (Boomsma et al., 2018; Power & Pluess, 2015). In this context, genes related to dopaminergic and serotonergic systems are good candidates to explain part of the genetic basis of this personality trait. In the present study, we have found a significant genetic association between neuroticism and MAOA 30 bp VNTR polymorphism, which protein is involved in degradation of serotonin and dopamine. Specifically, we have found that healthy women with higher MAO‐A activity genotype show higher levels of neuroticism than those with lower activity.

Referring to the serotonergic system, the current literature shows an increase in frontolimbic binding of the 5‐HT2A (Frokjaer et al., 2008) and a reduction of 5‐HT1A binding in cortical and subcortical regions in people with high neuroticism scores (Hirvonen, Tuominen, Någren, & Hietala, 2015). A recent study has shown that neuroticism and 5‐HTT expression are linked in a sex‐dependent manner, where higher neuroticism scores seem to be associated with lower 5‐HTT binding in thalamus in women (Tuominen et al., 2017). Cell surface expression of 5‐HTT quickly responds to dynamic serotonin concentrations, generally being the expression of 5‐HTT parallel to serotonin levels (Ramamoorthy, Giovanetti, Qian, & Blakely, 1998), that could be interpreted as women have lower levels of serotonin in the thalamus. This might explain why did not find a genetic association between 5‐HTT and neuroticism in our study since we only carried out it in women. So, in summary, the current literature suggests that lower synaptic serotonin levels are the physiological basis of higher neuroticism in women. The fact that our results show higher levels of neuroticism in women with higher MAO‐A activity genotype agrees with this hypothesis, considering that it is the main enzyme involved in degrading serotonin. Thus, women with higher MAO‐A activity will degrade more serotonin and then probably have lower serotonin levels.

Results shown herein are comparable to Yu et al. (2005), who reported higher harm avoidance in females with higher MAO‐A activity, which is related to depression and anxiety disorders. Eley et al. (2003) find a genetic association between high MAO‐A activity and neuroticism in men, but not in women. Otherwise, our results contrast with several studies that have found no association between MAOA and neuroticism (Garpenstrand et al., 2002; Pełka‐Wysiecka et al., 2012; Tochigi et al., 2006). These discrepancies might be due to the use of different test to measure neuroticism. In fact, our study is the first report of a genetic association between the BFQ and MAOA‐uVNTR. Another reasonable explanation could be the different range of age of the sample, in other investigations the mean age of the sample was higher than in our study (a mean of around 40 years old), and some of them present a wide range of age between participants or do not include it. As reported by Soto et al. (2011), neuroticism in women shows positive trends into adolescence, flat trends through emerging adulthood, and then a negative trend across early adulthood and middle age. So, it is likely that age might influence on finding a relationship between the variables here studied and should be considered.

4.2. MAO‐A, aggression, and impulsivity

We also found a genetic association between MAO‐A and impulse control. While males with low MAO‐A activity who have been exposed to maltreatment tend to show more antisocial and aggressive behaviors (Caspi et al., 2002), in women this tendency would happen but in those with high MAO‐A activity (Byrd & Manuck, 2014). Likewise, males with low MAO‐A activity present an increasing activity of the amygdala and the hippocampus with the level of stress during childhood, while males with high MAO‐A activity exhibit a decreasing response. The opposite occurs in women (Holz et al., 2016). Consequently, MAO‐A gene–environment interaction seems to be sex‐dependent, at least in the neural circuitry of aggression.

It has been described that excessive reactivity in the amygdala coupled with inadequate prefrontal regulation contribute to increase the likelihood of aggressive behavior (Pavlov et al., 2012; Siever, 2008). Particularly, an increased amygdala activity has been linked to impulsive aggression and not to callous‐unemotional aggression. This kind of aggression has previously been associated with MAOA (Meyer‐Lindenberg et al., 2006). Although impulsivity and aggressiveness are two separated constructs, they are closely related (García‐Forero et al., 2009). In this way, our results show that women with high MAO‐A activity would control less their impulses and hence would be more impulsive than those with low MAO‐A activity. This higher impulsivity might predict, even more, the fact that they also present more aggressive behaviors (impulsive aggression) and other risk behaviors related to mental disorders when they are exposure to childhood life stress. Then, women with high MAO‐A activity genotype would present higher neuroticism and impulsivity and, in a negative environment, are more likely to present aggressive behaviors, personality disorders, and other phenotypes. Neurotic people tend to present more emotional reactivity (Lahey, 2009). Therefore, these results would agree with Byrd et al. (2019) who found that high MAO‐A activity genotype would be related to aggressive behaviors and other disorders through an indirect pathway with emotional reactivity as an intermediary instead of through a direct pathway. Thus, we can speculate that neuroticism regulated by MAOA might be a common factor between the different phenotypes observed in women who had the high MAO‐A activity genotype as proposed by Nilsson et al. (2018).

4.3. Dopaminergic system and neuroticism

In this study, we also try to find a genetic association with other polymorphisms in genes related to the dopaminergic system. Particularly, COMT and DAT are involved in degrading dopamine and reuptaking dopamine from the synaptic cleft to the presynaptic neurons, respectively.

In accordance with our results, several studies did not find an association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and neuroticism in the revised version of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire in large samples (Henderson et al., 2000; Olsson et al., 2005; Pap et al., 2012; Urata et al., 2007; Wray et al., 2008). Other studies only found a tendency toward significance (Hatzimanolis et al., 2013; Hoth et al., 2006; Pełka‐Wysiecka et al., 2012). Otherwise, other studies have found a significant association between the COMT polymorphism and differences in neuroticism in a sex‐dependent manner on the NEO‐FFI in healthy people (Eley et al., 2003; Hettema, Chen, Sun, & Brown, 2015; Stein, Fallin, Schork, & Gelernter, 2005). Taking all together, this information seems to indicate that COMT Val158Met polymorphism is not directly associated with neuroticism or if it is, it would explain just a little percentage of neuroticism. One supposed explanation could be that the main mechanism involved in neuroticism is the serotonergic system, and serotonin is not degraded by COMT.

Some studies have been carried out associating DAT 3′UTR VNTR polymorphism and neuroticism, and most of them did not find significant results (Chmielowiec et al., 2018; Hünnerkopf, Strobel, Gutknecht, Brocke, & Lesch, 2007; Kazantseva, Gaĭsina, Malykh, & Khusnutdinova, 2009; Kazantseva, Gaysina, Malykh, & Khusnutdinova, 2011). However, a recent meta‐analysis has suggested that the dopaminergic system is related to personality trait differences in neuroticism only in contexts where there is a high climatic stress (Fischer et al., 2018). Although they consider Spain as a place with medium climate demands, we did not find a genetic association.

In summary, we found no genetic association between neuroticism and these two genes of the dopaminergic system. It is likely that since personality is a complex construct and it is influenced by both genes and environment, many different genes with small effect would contribute to its expression, and our sample is not big enough to detect its effect.

4.4. PTC might be associated with impulse control

Phenylthiocarbamide is an organic compound that either tastes bitter or is tasteless. Previous studies have tried to find an association between PTC tasting and mental conditions, but just a few of them are related to personality. Very and Iacono (1968) found that nontasters scored significantly higher on six MMPI clinical scales (D, Hy, Pd, Pa, Pt, and Sc scales), while tasters only scored higher on the Ma scale. They interpreted these results as a general maladjustment of the nontasters and, hence, a higher susceptibility to organic problems. In the present study, we have found that nontasters control worse their impulses. So, our results seem to agree with this previous study. On the other hand, Mascie‐Taylor et al. (1983) found that nontasters tend to be more placid, relaxed, and practical in comparison with the tasters using Cattell's Sixteen Personality Factor Questionnaire. Likewise, Kimmel and Lester (1987) found no difference between tasters and nontasters in the psychological traits of the Eysenck Personality Inventory. In these mentioned reports, different questionnaires were used. This could explain the discrepancies between the results found in them, suggesting that further investigation is required.

4.5. Explanatory power of the regression models

Finally, we carried out a linear regression to check out if the variables studied herein would explain emotional stability or any of it subdimensions. We found that trait anxiety highly explains emotional stability, while state anxiety and depression does not. This is not surprising since Ormel, Rosmalen, and Farmer (2004) found a strong correlation between the stable component of anxiety and neuroticism. The same was observed with its subdimensions, impulse control, and emotion control. The MAOA‐uVNTR polymorphism also explains these three variables, while PTC tasting only explains impulse control.

Our models would explain 51% of the variance in emotional stability, 38.6% in impulse control, and 50.7% in emotion control. Although all the models did not explain all the variance in these variables, it is not unexpected since these variables are complex constructs that depend on genetics and also the environment. We could expect that including other variables as exposition or not to a stressful environment would change the model, probably explaining a higher percentage of the variance observed.

4.6. Limitations and future directions

In the present research, we have studied how emotional stability is affected by personality, genetics, and other phenotypic variables such as being taster of PTC in women. It is the first time that these variables have been checked out altogether trying to explain emotional stability, impulse control, and emotion control evaluated through the BFQ. These are complex constructs that depend on multiple factors, and it would be interesting to consider other variables in family members and environmental characteristics during childhood.

As main limitation of our study, we have to mention the sample size that is relatively low for this kind of study. However, some authors mentioned that largest studies are less thoroughly performed than small studies, and therefore, it is also important to taking small studies into account for meta‐analysis (Moffitt & Caspi, 2014). Duncan and Keller (2011) argued that there is a strong publication bias toward positive findings in novel GxE reports and that probably many more tests had been carried out than reported in the literature because of that. They also found that normally replication studies are more realistic of the true rate of positive GxE findings than do novel studies. In this sense, although our study is a novel research, it is based on previous reports but with a new integrative perspective and we also showed negative results obtained for the dopaminergic system genes. Likewise, in our study we carried out a large number of analysis, so it is likely that appears a multiple testing problem. This problem is frequent in genomic studies and other fields related to biology, circumstance that must be taken into account when interpreting the results obtained. Thus, although our results are in line with previous reports, we cannot discard the possibility of type I error. In any case, this study could give rise to future projects that help to elucidate how emotional stability is influenced by both genes and environment and which mechanisms are involved in it, starting with the serotonergic system.

5. CONCLUSIONS

In the present study, we have found that women with higher MAO‐A activity genotype (homozygous for 4‐repeat allele) showed lower emotional stability, and then higher neuroticism, and impulse control than those with lower MAO‐A activity genotype (homozygous for 3‐repeat allele and heterozygous). Previous studies have shown that women with higher MAO‐A activity genotype who had been exposed to negative environments during childhood presented higher risk of presenting aggressive behavior and some mental disorders as personality disorders. We propose that in women, neuroticism might be regulated by MAOA and could be a common factor between all these phenotypes observed in women with higher MAO‐A activity genotype. We also found that depression, state anxiety, and trait anxiety were highly associated with emotional stability and its subdimensions. On the other hand, PTC tasters controlled better their impulses than nontasters and no associations were found with other polymorphisms analyzed, including COMT Val158Met, 5‐HTTLPR, and DAT 3′UTR VNTR. Finally, from all these variables, only MAOA‐uVNTR and trait anxiety explained some of the variance observed in emotional stability, impulse control, and emotion control, while PTC tasting also explained a percentage of the variance observed in impulse control.

CONFLICT OF INTEREST

None declared.

Supporting information

 

Rodríguez‐Ramos Á, Moriana JA, García‐Torres F, Ruiz‐Rubio M. Emotional stability is associated with the MAOA promoter uVNTR polymorphism in women. Brain Behav. 2019;9:e01376 10.1002/brb3.1376

Funding information

Universidad de Córdoba.

DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT

Data available on request due to privacy/ethical restrictions.

REFERENCES

  1. Alonso, J. , Angermeyer, M. C. , Bernert, S. , Bruffaerts, R. , Brugha, T. S. , Bryson, H. , … Vollebergh, W. A. M. (2004). Prevalence of mental disorders in Europe: Results from the European Study of the Epidemiology of Mental Disorders (ESEMeD) project. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 109(s420), 21–27. 10.1111/j.1600-0047.2004.00327.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Arias, B. , Aguilera, M. , Moya, J. , Sáiz, P. A. , Villa, H. , Ibáñez, M. I. , … Fañanás, L. (2012). The role of genetic variability in the SLC6A4, BDNF and GABRA6 genes in anxiety‐related traits. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 125(3), 194–202. 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2011.01764.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Aslund, C. , Nordquist, N. , Comasco, E. , Leppert, J. , Oreland, L. , & Nilsson, K. W. (2011). Maltreatment, MAOA, and delinquency: Sex differences in gene‐environment interaction in a large population‐based cohort of adolescents. Behavior Genetics, 41(2), 262–272. 10.1007/s10519-010-9356-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Beck, A. T. , Steer, R. A. , & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the beck depression inventory–II. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
  5. Beck, A. T. , Steer, R. A. , & Brown, G. K. (2011). BDI‐II. inventario de depresión de beck‐II. Madrid, Spain: Pearson. [Google Scholar]
  6. Bishop, S. J. , Fossella, J. , Croucher, C. J. , & Duncan, J. (2008). COMT val158met genotype affects recruitment of neural mechanisms supporting fluid intelligence. Cerebral Cortex, 18(9), 2132–2140. 10.1093/cercor/bhm240 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Boomsma, D. I. , Helmer, Q. , Nieuwboer, H. A. , Hottenga, J. J. , de Moor, M. H. , van den Berg, S. M. , … de Geus, E. J. (2018). An extended twin‐pedigree study of neuroticism in the Netherlands twin register. Behavior Genetics, 48(1), 1–11. 10.1007/s10519-017-9872-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bortolato, M. , Floris, G. , & Shih, J. C. (2018). From aggression to autism: new perspectives on the behavioral sequelae of monoamine oxidase deficiency. Journal of Neural Transmission, 125(11), 1589–1599. 10.1007/s00702-018-1888-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Brunner, H. G. , Nelen, M. , Breakefield, X. O. , Ropers, H. H. , & van Oost, B. A. . (1993). Abnormal behavior associated with a point mutation in the structural gene for monoamine oxidase A. Science, 262(5133), 578–580. 10.1126/science.8211186 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Buela‐Casal, G. , Guillén‐Riquelme, A. , & Seisdedos Cubero, N. (2011). Cuestionario de Ansiedad Estado‐Rasgo: Adaptación española. Madrid, Spain: TEA Ediciones. [Google Scholar]
  11. Byrd, A. L. , & Manuck, S. B. (2014). MAOA, childhood maltreatment, and antisocial behavior: Meta‐analysis of a gene‐environment interaction. Biological Psychiatry, 75(1), 9–17. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.05.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Byrd, A. L. , Manuck, S. B. , Hawes, S. W. , Vebares, T. J. , Nimgaonkar, V. , Chowdari, K. V. , … Stepp, S. D. (2019). The interaction between monoamine oxidase A (MAOA) and childhood maltreatment as a predictor of personality pathology in females: Emotional reactivity as a potential mediating mechanism. Development and Psychopathology, 31(1), 361–377. 10.1017/S0954579417001900 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Caprara, G. V. , Barbaranelli, C. , & Borgogni, L. (1995). BFQ. Cuestionario big five. Madrid, Spain: TEA. [Google Scholar]
  14. Caprara, G. V. , Barbaranelli, C. , Borgogni, L. , & Perugini, M. (1993). The “big five questionnaire”: A new questionnaire to assess the five factor model. Personality and Individual Differences, 15(3), 281–288. 10.1016/0191-8869(93)90218-R [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  15. Caspi, A. , McClay, J. , Moffitt, T. E. , Mill, J. , Martin, J. , Craig, I. W. , … Poulton, R. (2002). Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated children. Science, 297(5582), 851–854. 10.1126/science.1072290 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Chapman, B. P. , Duberstein, P. R. , Sörensen, S. , & Lyness, J. M. (2007). Gender differences in five factor model personality traits in an elderly cohort: extension of robust and surprising findings to an older generation. Personality and Individual Differences, 43(6), 1594–1603. 10.1016/j.paid.2007.04.028 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Chen, J. , Lipska, B. K. , Halim, N. , Ma, Q. D. , Matsumoto, M. , Melhem, S. , … Weinberger, D. R. (2004). Functional analysis of genetic variation in catechol‐O‐methyltransferase (COMT): Effects on mRNA, protein, and enzyme activity in postmortem human brain. American Journal of Human Genetics, 75(5), 807–821. 10.1086/425589 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Chmielowiec, J. , Chmielowiec, K. , Suchanecka, A. , Trybek, G. , Mroczek, B. , Małecka, I. , & Grzywacz, A. (2018). Associations between the dopamine D4 receptor and DAT1 dopamine transporter genes polymorphisms and personality traits in addicted patients. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 15(10), 2076 10.3390/ijerph15102076 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Costa, A. , Riedel, M. , Müller, U. , Möller, H.‐J. , & Ettinger, U. (2011). Relationship between SLC6A3 genotype and striatal dopamine transporter availability: a meta‐analysis of human single photon emission computed tomography studies. Synapse (New York, N. Y.), 65(10), 998–1005. 10.1002/syn.20927 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Del Hoyo, L. , Xicota, L. , Langohr, K. , Sánchez‐Benavides, G. , de Sola, S. , Cuenca‐Royo, A. , … TESDAD Study Group (2016). VNTR‐DAT1 and COMTVal158Met genotypes modulate mental flexibility and adaptive behavior skills in down syndrome. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 10, 193 10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00193 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Docherty, A. R. , Moscati, A. , Peterson, R. , Edwards, A. C. , Adkins, D. E. , Bacanu, S. A. , … Kendler, K. S. (2016). SNP‐based heritability estimates of the personality dimensions and polygenic prediction of both neuroticism and major depression: Findings from CONVERGE. Translational Psychiatry, 6(10), e926 10.1038/tp.2016.177 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Duncan, L. E. , & Keller, M. C. (2011). A critical review of the first 10 years of candidate gene‐by‐environment interaction research in psychiatry. American Journal of Psychiatry, 168(10), 1041–1049. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11020191 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Eley, T. C. , Tahir, E. , Angleitner, A. , Harriss, K. , McClay, J. , Plomin, R. , … Craig, I. (2003). Association analysis of MAOA and COMT with neuroticism assessed by peers. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part B, Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 120B(1), 90–96. 10.1002/ajmg.b.20046 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Fischer, R. , Lee, A. , & Verzijden, M. N. (2018). Dopamine genes are linked to Extraversion and Neuroticism personality traits, but only in demanding climates. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 1733 10.1038/s41598-017-18784-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Frokjaer, V. G. , Mortensen, E. L. , Nielsen, F. Å. , Haugbol, S. , Pinborg, L. H. , Adams, K. H. , … Knudsen, G. M. (2008). Frontolimbic serotonin 2A receptor binding in healthy subjects is associated with personality risk factors for affective disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 63(6), 569–576. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.07.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. García‐Forero, C. , Gallardo‐Pujol, D. , Maydeu‐Olivares, A. , & Andrés‐Pueyo, A. (2009). Disentangling impulsiveness, aggressiveness and impulsive aggression: An empirical approach using self‐report measures. Psychiatry Research, 168(1), 40–49. 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.04.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Garpenstrand, H. , Norton, N. , Damberg, M. , Rylander, G. , Forslund, K. , Mattila‐Evenden, M. , … Jönsson, E. G. (2002). A regulatory monoamine oxidase a promoter polymorphism and personality traits. Neuropsychobiology, 46(4), 190–193. 10.1159/000067804 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Gatt, J. M. , Burton, K. L. O. , Williams, L. M. , & Schofield, P. R. (2015). Specific and common genes implicated across major mental disorders: A review of meta‐analysis studies. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 60, 1–13. 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.09.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Grimsby, J. , Chen, K. , Wang, L. J. , Lan, N. C. , & Shih, J. C. (1991). Human monoamine oxidase A and B genes exhibit identical exon‐intron organization. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 88(9), 3637–3641. 10.1073/pnas.88.9.3637 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Guo, G. , Ou, X.‐M. , Roettger, M. , & Shih, J. C. (2008). The VNTR 2 repeat in MAOA and delinquent behavior in adolescence and young adulthood: Associations and MAOA promoter activity. European Journal of Human Genetics, 16(5), 626–634. 10.1038/sj.ejhg.5201999 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Guo, G. , Roettger, M. E. , & Shih, J. C. (2007). Contributions of the DAT1 and DRD2 genes to serious and violent delinquency among adolescents and young adults. Human Genetics, 121(1), 125–136. 10.1007/s00439-006-0244-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Guo, S. W. , & Reed, D. R. (2001). The genetics of phenylthiocarbamide perception. Annals of Human Biology, 28(2), 111–142. 10.1080/03014460151056310 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Hatzimanolis, A. , Vitoratou, S. , Mandelli, L. , Vaiopoulos, C. , Nearchou, F. A. , Stefanis, C. N. , … Stefanis, N. C. (2013). Potential role of membrane‐bound COMT gene polymorphisms in female depression vulnerability. Journal of Affective Disorders, 148(2–3), 316–322. 10.1016/j.jad.2012.12.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Henderson, A. S. , Korten, A. E. , Jorm, A. F. , Jacomb, P. A. , Christensen, H. , Rodgers, B. , … Easteal, S. (2000). COMT and DRD3 polymorphisms, environmental exposures, and personality traits related to common mental disorders. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 96(1), 102–107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Hettema, J. M. , Chen, X. , Sun, C. , & Brown, T. A. (2015). Direct, indirect and pleiotropic effects of candidate genes on internalizing disorder psychopathology. Psychological Medicine, 45(10), 2227–2236. 10.1017/S0033291715000021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Hirvonen, J. , Tuominen, L. , Någren, K. , & Hietala, J. (2015). Neuroticism and serotonin 5‐HT1A receptors in healthy subjects. Psychiatry Research, 234(1), 1–6. 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2015.04.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Holz, N. , Boecker, R. , Buchmann, A. F. , Blomeyer, D. , Baumeister, S. , Hohmann, S. , … Laucht, M. (2016). Evidence for a sex‐dependent MAOA× childhood stress interaction in the neural circuitry of aggression. Cerebral Cortex, 26(3), 904–914. 10.1093/cercor/bhu249 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Hoth, K. F. , Paul, R. H. , Williams, L. M. , Dobson‐Stone, C. , Todd, E. , Schofield, P. R. , … Gordon, E. (2006). Associations between the COMT Val/Met polymorphism, early life stress, and personality among healthy adults. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 2(2), 219–225. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Hünnerkopf, R. , Strobel, A. , Gutknecht, L. , Brocke, B. , & Lesch, K. P. (2007). Interaction between BDNF Val66Met and dopamine transporter gene variation influences anxiety‐related traits. Neuropsychopharmacology: Official Publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology, 32(12), 2552–2560. 10.1038/sj.npp.1301383 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Hunter, P. (2010). The psycho gene. EMBO Reports, 11(9), 667–669. 10.1038/embor.2010.122 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Jeuring, H. W. , Stek, M. L. , Huisman, M. , Oude Voshaar, R. C. , Naarding, P. , Collard, R. M. , … Comijs, H. C. (2018). A six‐year prospective study of the prognosis and predictors in patients with late‐life depression. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry: Official Journal of the American Association for Geriatric Psychiatry, 26(9), 985–997. 10.1016/j.jagp.2018.05.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Kalgutkar, A. S. , Dalvie, D. K. , Castagnoli, N. , & Taylor, T. J. (2001). Interactions of nitrogen‐containing xenobiotics with monoamine oxidase (MAO) isozymes A and B: SAR studies on MAO substrates and inhibitors. Chemical Research in Toxicology, 14(9), 1139–1162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Kazantseva, A. V. , Gaĭsina, D. A. , Malykh, S. B. , & Khusnutdinova, E. K. (2009). Role of dopamine transporter gene (DAT1) polymorphisms in personality traits variation. Genetika, 45(8), 1110–1117. 10.1134/S1022795409080122 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Kazantseva, A. , Gaysina, D. , Malykh, S. , & Khusnutdinova, E. (2011). The role of dopamine transporter (SLC6A3) and dopamine D2 receptor/ankyrin repeat and kinase domain containing 1 (DRD2/ANKK1) gene polymorphisms in personality traits. Progress in Neuro‐Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry, 35(4), 1033–1040. 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2011.02.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Keller, K. L. , & Adise, S. (2016). Variation in the ability to taste bitter thiourea compounds: implications for food acceptance, dietary intake, and obesity risk in children. Annual Review of Nutrition, 36(1), 157–182. 10.1146/annurev-nutr-071715-050916 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Kendler, K. S. , & Gardner, C. O. (2014). Sex differences in the pathways to major depression: A study of opposite‐sex twin pairs. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 171(4), 426–435. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.13101375 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Kim, S. E. , Kim, H.‐N. , Yun, Y.‐J. , Heo, S. G. , Cho, J. , Kwon, M.‐J. , … Kim, H.‐L. (2017). Meta‐analysis of genome‐wide SNP‐ and pathway‐based associations for facets of neuroticism. Journal of Human Genetics, 62(10), 903–909. 10.1038/jhg.2017.61 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Kim‐Cohen, J. , Caspi, A. , Taylor, A. , Williams, B. , Newcombe, R. , Craig, I. W. , & Moffitt, T. E. (2006). MAOA, maltreatment, and gene‐environment interaction predicting children's mental health: New evidence and a meta‐analysis. Molecular Psychiatry, 11(10), 903–913. 10.1038/sj.mp.4001851 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Kimmel, H. L. , & Lester, D. (1987). Personalities of those who can taste phenylthiocarbamide. Psychological Reports, 61(2), 586 10.2466/pr0.1987.61.2.586 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Kulacaoglu, F. , & Kose, S. (2017). Singing under the impulsiveness: Impulsivity in psychiatric disorders. Psychiatry and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 28(2), 205–210. 10.1080/24750573.2017.1410329 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  51. Lahey, B. B. (2009). Public health significance of neuroticism. American Psychologist, 64(4), 241–256. 10.1037/a0015309 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Lesch, K.‐P. , Bengel, D. , Heils, A. , Sabol, S. Z. , Greenberg, B. D. , Petri, S. , … Murphy, D. L. (1996). Association of anxiety‐related traits with a polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region. Science, 274(5292), 1527–1531. 10.1126/science.274.5292.1527 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Liu, Z. , Huang, L. , Luo, X. , Wu, L. , & Li, M. (2016). MAOA variants and genetic susceptibility to major psychiatric disorders. Molecular Neurobiology, 53(7), 4319–4327. 10.1007/s12035-015-9374-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Lotta, T. , Vidgren, J. , Tilgmann, C. , Ulmanen, I. , Melén, K. , Julkunen, I. , & Taskinen, J. (1995). Kinetics of human soluble and membrane‐bound catechol O‐methyltransferase: A revised mechanism and description of the thermolabile variant of the enzyme. Biochemistry, 34(13), 4202–4210. 10.1021/bi00013a008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Lugg, J. W. , & Whyte, J. M. (1955). Taste thresholds for phenylthiocarbamide of some population groups. I The thresholds of some civilized ethnic groups living in Malaya. Annals of Human Genetics, 19(4), 290–311. 10.1111/j.1469-1809.1955.tb01355.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Lynn, R. , & Martin, T. (1997). Gender differences in extraversion, neuroticism, and psychoticism in 37 nations. The Journal of Social Psychology, 137(3), 369–373. 10.1080/00224549709595447 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Mascie‐Taylor, C. G. , McManus, I. C. , MacLarnon, A. M. , & Lanigan, P. M. (1983). The association between phenylthiocarbamide (PTC) tasting ability and psychometric variables. Behavior Genetics, 13(2), 191–196. 10.1007/BF01065667 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. McGrath, L. M. , Mustanski, B. , Metzger, A. , Pine, D. S. , Kistner‐Griffin, E. , Cook, E. , & Wakschlag, L. S. (2012). A latent modeling approach to genotype‐phenotype relationships: Maternal problem behavior clusters, prenatal smoking, and MAOA genotype. Archives of Women's Mental Health, 15(4), 269–282. 10.1007/s00737-012-0286-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Meyer‐Lindenberg, A. , Buckholtz, J. W. , Kolachana, B. , R. Hariri, A. , Pezawas, L. , Blasi, G. , … Weinberger, D. R. (2006). Neural mechanisms of genetic risk for impulsivity and violence in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103(16), 6269–6274. 10.1073/pnas.0511311103 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Moberg, P. , Mcgue, C. , Kanes, S. , Roalf, D. , Balderston, C. , Gur, R. , … Turetsky, B. (2007). Phenylthiocarbamide (PTC) perception in patients with schizophrenia and first‐degree family members: Relationship to clinical symptomatology and psychophysical olfactory performance. Schizophrenia Research, 90(1–3), 221–228. 10.1016/j.schres.2006.11.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Moeller, F. G. , Barratt, E. S. , Dougherty, D. M. , Schmitz, J. M. , & Swann, A. C. (2001). Psychiatric aspects of impulsivity. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158(11), 1783–1793. 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.11.1783 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Moffitt, T. E. , & Caspi, A. (2014). Bias in a protocol for a meta‐analysis of 5‐HTTLPR, stress, and depression. BMC Psychiatry, 14(1), 179 10.1186/1471-244X-14-179 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Nilsson, K. W. , Åslund, C. , Comasco, E. , & Oreland, L. (2018). Gene–environment interaction of monoamine oxidase A in relation to antisocial behaviour: Current and future directions. Journal of Neural Transmission, 125(11), 1601–1626. 10.1007/s00702-018-1892-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Nordquist, N. , & Oreland, L. (2006). Monoallelic expression of MAOA in skin fibroblasts. Biochemical and Biophysical Research Communications, 348(2), 763–767. 10.1016/j.bbrc.2006.07.131 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Olsson, C. A. , Anney, R. J. L. , Lotfi‐Miri, M. , Byrnes, G. B. , Williamson, R. , & Patton, G. C. (2005). Association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and propensity to anxiety in an Australian population‐based longitudinal study of adolescent health. Psychiatric Genetics, 15(2), 109–115. 10.1097/00041444-200506000-00007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Ormel, J. , Jeronimus, B. F. , Kotov, R. , Riese, H. , Bos, E. H. , Hankin, B. , … Oldehinkel, A. J. (2013). Neuroticism and common mental disorders: Meaning and utility of a complex relationship. Clinical Psychology Review, 33(5), 686–697. 10.1016/j.cpr.2013.04.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Ormel, J. , Rosmalen, J. , & Farmer, A. (2004). Neuroticism: A non‐informative marker of vulnerability to psychopathology. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 39(11), 906–912. 10.1007/s00127-004-0873-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Pap, D. , Gonda, X. , Molnar, E. , Lazary, J. , Benko, A. , Downey, D. , … Juhasz, G. (2012). Genetic variants in the catechol‐o‐methyltransferase gene are associated with impulsivity and executive function: Relevance for major depression. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part B, Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 159B(8), 928–940. 10.1002/ajmg.b.32098 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Pavlov, K. A. , Chistiakov, D. A. , & Chekhonin, V. P. (2012). Genetic determinants of aggression and impulsivity in humans. Journal of Applied Genetics, 53(1), 61–82. 10.1007/s13353-011-0069-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Pełka‐Wysiecka, J. , Ziętek, J. , Grzywacz, A. , Kucharska‐Mazur, J. , Bienkowski, P. , & Samochowiec, J. (2012). Association of genetic polymorphisms with personality profile in individuals without psychiatric disorders. Progress in Neuro‐Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 39(1), 40–46. 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2012.04.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Pinsonneault, J. K. , Papp, A. C. , & Sadée, W. (2006). Allelic mRNA expression of X‐linked monoamine oxidase a (MAOA) in human brain: Dissection of epigenetic and genetic factors. Human Molecular Genetics, 15(17), 2636–2649. 10.1093/hmg/ddl192 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Pons, J. (1955). Taste sensitivity to phenylthiourea in Spaniards. Human Biology, 27(3), 153–160. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Power, R. A. , & Pluess, M. (2015). Heritability estimates of the Big Five personality traits based on common genetic variants. Translational Psychiatry, 5(7), e604 10.1038/tp.2015.96 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Prom‐Wormley, E. C. , Eaves, L. J. , Foley, D. L. , Gardner, C. O. , Archer, K. J. , Wormley, B. K. , … Silberg, J. L. (2009). Monoamine oxidase A and childhood adversity as risk factors for conduct disorder in females. Psychological Medicine, 39(04), 579 10.1017/S0033291708004170 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Ramamoorthy, S. , Giovanetti, E. , Qian, Y. , & Blakely, R. D. (1998). Phosphorylation and regulation of antidepressant‐sensitive serotonin transporters. The Journal of Biological Chemistry, 273(4), 2458–2466. 10.1074/jbc.273.4.2458 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Riecher‐Rössler, A. (2017). Sex and gender differences in mental disorders. The Lancet Psychiatry, 4(1), 8–9. 10.1016/S2215-0366(16)30348-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Risso, D. S. , Kozlitina, J. , Sainz, E. , Gutierrez, J. , Wooding, S. , Getachew, B. , … Drayna, D. (2016). Genetic variation in the TAS2R38 bitter taste receptor and smoking behaviors. PLoS ONE, 11(10), e0164157 10.1371/journal.pone.0164157 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Rivera, M. , Gutiérrez, B. , Molina, E. , Torres‐González, F. , Bellón, J. A. , Moreno‐Küstner, B. , … Cervilla, J. A. (2009). High‐activity variants of the uMAOA polymorphism increase the risk for depression in a large primary care sample. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 150B(3), 395–402. 10.1002/ajmg.b.30829 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Sabol, S. Z. , Hu, S. , & Hamer, D. (1998). A functional polymorphism in the monoamine oxidase A gene promoter. Human Genetics, 103(3), 273–279. 10.1007/s004390050816 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Sáiz, P. A. , García‐Portilla, M. P. , Arango, C. , Morales, B. , Arias, B. , Corcoran, P. , … Bobes, J. (2010). Genetic polymorphisms in the dopamine‐2 receptor (DRD2), dopamine‐3 receptor (DRD3), and dopamine transporter (SLC6A3) genes in schizophrenia: Data from an association study. Progress in Neuro‐Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 34(1), 26–31. 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2009.09.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Sánchez‐Morán, M. , Hernández, J. A. , Duñabeitia, J. A. , Estévez, A. , Bárcena, L. , González‐Lahera, A. , … Carreiras, M. (2018). Genetic association study of dyslexia and ADHD candidate genes in a Spanish cohort: Implications of comorbid samples. PLoS ONE, 13(10), e0206431 10.1371/journal.pone.0206431 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Servaas, M. N. , van der Velde, J. , Costafreda, S. G. , Horton, P. , Ormel, J. , Riese, H. , & Aleman, A. (2013). Neuroticism and the brain: A quantitative meta‐analysis of neuroimaging studies investigating emotion processing. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(8), 1518–1529. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.05.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Shih, J. C. , & Chen, K. (2004). Regulation of MAO‐A and MAO‐B gene expression. Current Medicinal Chemistry, 11(15), 1995–2005. 10.2174/0929867043364757 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Siever, L. J. (2008). Neurobiology of aggression and violence. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165(4), 429–442. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07111774 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Sjöberg, R. L. , Nilsson, K. W. , Wargelius, H.‐L. , Leppert, J. , Lindström, L. , & Oreland, L. (2007). Adolescent girls and criminal activity: Role of MAOA‐LPR genotype and psychosocial factors. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part B, Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 144B(2), 159–164. 10.1002/ajmg.b.30360 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Slavish, D. C. , Sliwinski, M. J. , Smyth, J. M. , Almeida, D. M. , Lipton, R. B. , Katz, M. J. , & Graham‐Engeland, J. E. (2018). Neuroticism, rumination, negative affect, and sleep: Examining between‐ and within‐person associations. Personality and Individual Differences, 123, 217–222. 10.1016/j.paid.2017.11.023 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Soto, C. J. , John, O. P. , Gosling, S. D. , & Potter, J. (2011). Age differences in personality traits from 10 to 65: Big Five domains and facets in a large cross‐sectional sample. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(2), 330–348. 10.1037/a0021717 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Spielberger, C. D. , Gorsuch, R. L. , & Lushene, R. E. (1970). Manual for the state‐trait anxiety inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. [Google Scholar]
  89. Stabellini, R. , Vasques, L. R. , de Mello, J. C. M. , Hernandes, L. M. , & Pereira, L. V. (2009). MAOA and GYG2 are submitted to X chromosome inactivation in human fibroblasts. Epigenetics, 4(6), 388–393. 10.4161/epi.4.6.9492 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Stein, M. B. , Fallin, M. D. , Schork, N. J. , & Gelernter, J. (2005). COMT polymorphisms and anxiety‐related personality traits. Neuropsychopharmacology, 30(11), 2092–2102. 10.1038/sj.npp.1300787 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Struijs, S. Y. , Lamers, F. , Spinhoven, P. , van der Does, W. , & Penninx, B. W. J. H. (2018). The predictive specificity of psychological vulnerability markers for the course of affective disorders. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 103, 10–17. 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2018.04.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Thomas, J. , Al‐Mesaabi, W. , Bahusain, E. , & Mutawa, M. (2014). The relationship between taste sensitivity to phenylthiocarbamide and anhedonia. Psychiatry Research, 215(2), 444–447. 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.11.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Tochigi, M. , Otowa, T. , Hibino, H. , Kato, C. , Otani, T. , Umekage, T. , … Sasaki, T. (2006). Combined analysis of association between personality traits and three functional polymorphisms in the tyrosine hydroxylase, monoamine oxidase A, and catechol‐O‐methyltransferase genes. Neuroscience Research, 54(3), 180–185. 10.1016/j.neures.2005.11.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Tuominen, L. , Miettunen, J. , Cannon, D. M. , Drevets, W. C. , Frokjaer, V. G. , Hirvonen, J. , … Hietala, J. (2017). Neuroticism associates with cerebral in vivo serotonin transporter binding differently in males and females. The International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 20(12), 963–970. 10.1093/ijnp/pyx071 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Urata, T. , Takahashi, N. , Hakamata, Y. , Iijima, Y. , Kuwahara, N. , Ozaki, N. , … Inada, T. (2007). Gene‐gene interaction analysis of personality traits in a Japanese population using an electrochemical DNA array chip analysis. Neuroscience Letters, 414(3), 209–212. 10.1016/j.neulet.2006.12.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Vandenbergh, D. J. , Persico, A. M. , Hawkins, A. L. , Griffin, C. A. , Li, X. , Jabs, E. W. , & Uhl, G. R. (1992). Human dopamine transporter gene (DAT1) maps to chromosome 5p15.3 and displays a VNTR. Genomics, 14(4), 1104–1106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Veluswami, D. , Meena, B. A. , Latha, S. , Fathima, I. G. , Soundariya, K. , & Selvi, K. S. (2015). A study on prevalence of phenyl thiocarbamide (PTC) taste blindness among obese individuals. Journal of Clinical and Diagnostic Research, 9(5), CC04‐6 10.7860/JCDR/2015/11821.5896 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Verhoeven, F. E. A. , Booij, L. , Kruijt, A.‐W. , Cerit, H. , Antypa, N. , & Does, W. (2012). The effects of MAOA genotype, childhood trauma, and sex on trait and state‐dependent aggression. Brain and Behavior, 2(6), 806–813. 10.1002/brb3.96 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Very, P. S. , & Iacono, C. (1968). Phenylthiourea taste blindness and MMPI personality patterns in adult Caucasian females. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 24(2), 187–188. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Wakschlag, L. S. , Kistner, E. O. , Pine, D. S. , Biesecker, G. , Pickett, K. E. , Skol, A. D. , … Cook, E. H. (2010). Interaction of prenatal exposure to cigarettes and MAOA genotype in pathways to youth antisocial behavior. Molecular Psychiatry, 15(9), 928–937. 10.1038/mp.2009.22 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Weisberg, Y. J. , Deyoung, C. G. , & Hirsh, J. B. (2011). Gender differences in personality across the ten aspects of the big five. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 178 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00178 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Wray, N. R. , James, M. R. , Dumenil, T. , Handoko, H. Y. , Lind, P. A. , Montgomery, G. W. , & Martin, N. G. (2008). Association study of candidate variants of COMT with neuroticism, anxiety and depression. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part B, Neuropsychiatric Genetics: the Official Publication of the International Society of Psychiatric Genetics, 147B(7), 1314–1318. 10.1002/ajmg.b.30744 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Yu, Y.‐W.‐Y. , Yang, C.‐W. , Wu, H.‐C. , Tsai, S.‐J. , Hong, C.‐J. , Chen, M.‐C. , & Chen, T.‐J. (2005). Association study of a functional MAOA‐uVNTR gene polymorphism and personality traits in Chinese young females. Neuropsychobiology, 52(3), 118–121. 10.1159/000087556 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

 

Data Availability Statement

Data available on request due to privacy/ethical restrictions.


Articles from Brain and Behavior are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES