Abstract
Placebo effects are well established in healthy participants experiencing experimental or acute pain. Yet, little is known about the mechanisms of placebo analgesia effects in patients with chronic pain and even less is known in patients suffering from central nervous system (CNS) diseases where pain is prevalent, difficult to manage, and often undertreated. This article briefly reviews the current knowledge of placebo analgesia effects in healthy participants with the aim of discussing how the mechanisms in placebo analgesia differ between healthy participants and patients. The focus will be on placebo analgesia effects in chronic pain conditions as well as in 2 CNS diseases: Alzheimer disease and Parkinson disease. Finally, strengths and weaknesses of the current knowledge will be discussed and it will be demonstrated how insights from the placebo literature may point to new ways of improving treatments among patients experiencing pain in relation to CNS diseases.
Keywords: Placebo analgesia effects, Chronic pain, Central nervous system diseases, Open/hidden design
1. Introduction
1.1. Definition of placebo analgesia effects
Placebo analgesia effects are related to healthy participants or patients' perception of the therapeutic intervention90 and they are likely to contribute to the magnitude of various types of analgesic treatments across different diseases. Traditionally, placebo effects have been investigated through administration of inert agents; however, recent designs use active treatments and simply manipulate participants' perception of whether they receive the treatment or not.2,14,45,58,59 The so-called open/hidden design exemplifies how participant's perception can be manipulated. In the “open” condition, the active treatment is given in full view of the patient, so the patient perceives that a treatment is given, whereas in the “hidden” condition, the same active treatment is delivered unknown to the patient.11,71 To calculate the placebo analgesia effect, it is important to control for the natural history of the pain34 so that the changes in pain between the open and hidden conditions should be compared with a no treatment control condition.70,71
1.2. Placebo analgesia effect in healthy participants and patients with chronic pain
Placebo analgesia is well studied in healthy participants exposed to experimentally induced painful stimulations.5,38,62,76 Different psychological factors trigger this phenomenon, for instance, expectations, verbal information, emotions, and learning mechanisms. The neurobiological underpinnings accompanying placebo analgesic effects in healthy participants have been extensively documented, and a complex interplay of biological events has been identified.21,39,96 However, the question is whether it is possible to transfer the knowledge obtained in healthy participants to populations for whom it is highly relevant, namely patients experiencing pain. Relatively few studies have examined placebo analgesia effects in patients with chronic pain. Comparing the placebo analgesia effect in healthy participants and patients with chronic pain, there seems to be some overlap in the psychological mechanisms, but not necessarily in the neurobiological mechanisms. Although central nervous system (CNS) diseases are frequently accompanied by pain conditions, pain is often undertreated in these patient populations66 and very few studies have examined the possible contribution from placebo analgesia effects.
The aim of this article is to illustrate that knowledge of placebo analgesia effects obtained from studies on healthy participants cannot necessarily be transferred to patients experiencing pain either as the main disease or as part of a CNS disease. This suggests that placebo analgesia should be examined more stringently across CNS diseases to specify the underlying mechanisms and to clarify whether these patient populations actually benefit from placebo analgesia effects in clinical practice. Central nervous system diseases include various specific conditions such as Alzheimer disease (AD), Parkinson disease (PD), multiple sclerosis, autism, epilepsy, and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, and several of these conditions are accompanied by frequent pain symptoms.27 Although pain treatment has been studied, eg, in multiple sclerosis18,80 and in poststroke spasticity,96 these studies only include a placebo condition and not a no-treatment control condition, which makes it impossible to separate the changes in the placebo condition from confounding factors such as spontaneous remission and regression to the mean.34 To the best of our knowledge, only studies on AD and PD have included sufficient control conditions to allow for an estimation of the actual placebo effect and therefore, only these studies are reviewed here.
2. What do we know
2.1. Placebo analgesia effects in healthy participants
Most of the knowledge of placebo analgesia effects derives from studies on healthy participants5,38,62,76 exposed to experimental pain through electrical stimulations,19,67 heat stimulations,18,20,24,95 visceral pain,33,73 or ischemic pain.1,9,15 From these studies, we know that healthy participants can experience large placebo analgesia effects38,68,83 and that these placebo effects can be mediated by different psychological mechanisms such as expectations, verbal suggestions, emotions, and learning.14,31,35,51,52,54,63,73
2.2. Psychological factors
Whereas expectations of receiving low pain are associated with a larger magnitude of the placebo analgesia effect,14,77 high levels of fear decrease the magnitude of the placebo analgesia effect.61 Furthermore, it is well established that the information given about the treatment can substantially change the effect of a treatment, producing placebo analgesia effects or nocebo hyperalgesic effects.2,8,98,99 For example, Bingel et al. have shown that different information about the same potent painkiller can induce different magnitudes of analgesia. In this study, after a no-treatment condition, participants received remifentanil and were tested under 3 different conditions: (1) hidden administration in which participants had no expectations for analgesia, (2) open administration accompanied by verbal suggestions for pain relief (positive expectation condition), and (3) hidden administration accompanied by verbal suggestions for hyperalgesia (negative expectation condition). Using this extended open/hidden design, Bingel et al. demonstrated that positive expectations significantly increased pain relief. This suggests that the overall analgesic treatment effect benefits from the contribution of a placebo effect. In addition, the study also demonstrates the importance of our perception of and expectations toward the treatment by showing that verbal suggestions contributing to negative expectations block the analgesic capacity of remifentanil.17 Thus, positive and negative expectations may greatly influence the treatment outcome.
Learning also plays a pivotal role in placebo analgesia. It has been extensively documented that after different exposures to active treatments, the administration of a placebo produces robust placebo analgesia effects.23,25,74 In particular, the longer the conditioning procedure, the higher the analgesic effect is.23 Negative learning effects have been documented as well. For example, after different ineffective treatments (negative treatment history), the analgesic responses to a new treatment are substantially reduced.49 Moreover, changing the route of administration inducing positive expectations of treatment efficacy does not counteract the negative carryover effects on treatment efficacy.98
2.3. Neurobiological underpinning
Substantiating placebo analgesia at a neurophysiological level, the placebo analgesia effect is associated with an increased activity in the prefrontal cortices during the anticipation phase when participants expect to obtain an analgesic effect of the treatment.4,93,94 This result has been confirmed by Krummenacher et al., who demonstrated that expectation-induced placebo analgesia in healthy participants can be blocked by disruption of prefrontal cortex (PFC) function using repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation.56 Furthermore, when participants experience analgesia after a placebo treatment, decreased activity can be observed in the so-called pain matrix involving areas such as the thalamus, the insula, the somatosensory cortices, and the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC)4,96 and even at the level of the spinal cord.32 These findings suggest that placebo analgesia alters the perception of pain at early levels of pain processing, although a recent meta-analysis questions this finding.99 Studies using pharmacological manipulations and brain imaging techniques have contributed with knowledge of how neurotransmitter systems modulate placebo analgesia effects in healthy participants. The involvement of the endogenous opioid system and the endocannabinoid system has been substantiated by means of opioid and cannabinoid antagonists (naloxone and rimonabant, respectively). Specifically, these antagonist drugs compete for the opioid and endocannabinoid receptors blocking these systems and, correspondingly, blocking or diminishing the placebo analgesia effect.1,2,6–8,14,26,32,40,57 More ambiguous is the involvement of the dopaminergic system. Whereas one study using a similar antagonist paradigm has shown that the placebo analgesia effect in healthy participants cannot be blocked or diminished by means of a dopamine antagonist,87 studies using brain scanning techniques or genetic analysis have supported the involvement of the dopaminergic system.42,81–83 Even so, the role of these neurotransmitter systems in placebo analgesia effects in patients with chronic pain is less understood, and the neurobiological foundation for placebo analgesia effects may constitute an important argument as to why it is necessary to distinguish between placebo analgesia effects in healthy participants exposed to pain and placebo analgesia effects in patients experiencing pain.
2.4. Placebo analgesia effects in patients with chronic pain
In pain research, it is important to differentiate chronic pain from experimental and acute pain. Specifically, chronic pain conditions, eg, neuropathic pain, are often characterized by a combination of hyperalgesia (increased pain sensitivity) and allodynia (lowered threshold for pain),77 reflecting a fundamental change in pain processing and pain perception. Furthermore, studying patients with chronic pain in an experimental setting requires a distinction between ongoing and evoked pain.78 A few studies have investigated placebo effects in both spontaneous and evoked pain and found large effects for both types of pain.20,88 A meta-analysis looking at clinical pain and evoked pain further found that patients with chronic pain are able to experience large placebo analgesic effects, just as healthy participants, and that they may even experience more clinically relevant pain reduction.38
2.4.1. Psychological factors
As shown in relation to healthy participants, expectations of low pain levels as well as high levels of positive emotions and low levels of negative emotions also contribute to the magnitude of placebo effects in patients with chronic pain.71,89 Furthermore, desire for pain relief has been suggested to play a role in placebo analgesia especially in patients with chronic pain.70,71,86,89,91
The role of learning mechanisms in patients with chronic pain has not been systematically investigated. Thus, it is not clear whether the robust placebo conditioning effect observed in healthy volunteers can be reproduced in patients with chronic pain. Only one study investigated the role of negative treatment history in patients with chronic pain, showing that patients with a more negative pain-related treatment history reported significantly larger placebo responses to their own chronic pain,67 which seems to be in contrast to the findings in healthy volunteers.98
2.4.2. Neurobiological underpinning
At a neurophysiological level, neuroimaging studies have demonstrated that placebo analgesia in patients with irritable bowel syndrome is associated with reductions in neural activity in areas of the pain matrix, such as the thalamus, the insula, the somatosensory cortices, and the ACC.27,78,79 Accordingly, the neurophysiological foundation for placebo analgesia effects in patients with chronic pain seems to be similar to the neurophysiology underlying placebo analgesia effects in healthy participants. In contrast to the findings in healthy participants, however, the few studies examining the involvement of neurotransmitters in placebo analgesia effects have not directly demonstrated the involvement of the endogenous opioid and dopaminergic systems.55,86,91 At present, no studies have examined the involvement of the endocannabinoid system in placebo analgesia effects in patients with chronic pain. The neurobiological underpinnings for placebo analgesia effects hereby accentuate the necessity to distinguish between placebo analgesia effects in healthy participants and placebo analgesia effects in patients with chronic pain.
Part of this difference in the neurobiological foundation for placebo analgesia may be associated with more fundamental differences in the processing of chronic pain as opposed to acute and experimental pain as previously described. Furthermore, psychological factors such as experience with treatment failure and negative expectations, anxiety, and depression are likely to accompany chronic pain conditions and, accordingly, are also likely to affect the prolonged pain experience44,77,89,91,92 and potentially treatment outcome, although this awaits further investigations. This accentuates the importance of considering prior treatment history—and accompanying expectancy and emotions embedded in all treatment contexts—when treating patients with chronic pain.22,36,37,70,71
2.4.3. The open-hidden design
Petersen et al. have used the open/hidden design to investigate placebo analgesia effects in patients with chronic pain suffering from neuropathic pain.70,71 Open vs hidden administration of lidocaine compared with a no treatment control condition showed significant placebo analgesia effects on spontaneous ongoing neuropathic pain71 as well as on a range of evoked pain measures including area of hyperalgesia.70,71 The pain reduction in the open administration of lidocaine was related to expectations of lower pain levels and positive emotions.71
Recently, Skyt et al84 have demonstrated that the open/hidden design can also be used to investigate the involvement of neurotransmitter systems in placebo analgesia effects in neuropathic pain patients. Using the open/hidden administration of lidocaine together with administration of a dopamine agonist and a dopamine antagonist on different test days, the study demonstrated that the placebo analgesia effect was not increased or blocked, respectively, by the agonist and the antagonist drugs. In this extended version, the open/hidden design thereby allows us to demonstrate a placebo analgesia effect in patients with chronic pain that relies on the patient's perception of the treatment but not on the underlying dopaminergic activity.
The abovementioned studies point to the open/hidden design as a promising methodological approach for examining placebo analgesia effects and the underlying mechanisms in a clinically relevant setting across patient populations experiencing pain. Similarly, it could profitably be used to explore placebo mechanisms in patients experiencing pain in relation to CNS diseases as well.
2.5. Placebo analgesia effects in central nervous system diseases
At present, relatively few studies have examined placebo analgesia effects in patients with AD and PD. The majority of these studies have, however, used the open/hidden design or related designs.
2.6. Alzheimer disease
To the best of our knowledge, only one study has investigated placebo-related mechanisms in patients with AD. Benedetti et al. studied patients with AD at the initial stage of their disease and after one year and compared them to healthy participants matched for sex and age. Both patients with AD and healthy participants were treated with open and hidden lidocaine following pain, induced via venipuncture. A significant difference in pain levels occurred between the open and hidden application of lidocaine in both patients with AD and healthy volunteers at the initial stage, indicating that both groups obtained a placebo analgesia effect.11 Yet, at the 1-year retest, when the disease has progressed, only the healthy participants obtained the placebo analgesic effect, whereas the patients with AD no longer experienced a placebo analgesia effect.
2.6.1. Psychological factors
The literature on placebo analgesia effects in healthy participants has demonstrated that factors such as verbal suggestions and expectations are involved in obtaining placebo analgesia effects. Although the study by Benedetti et al. suggests a disruption of placebo analgesic effect in patients with AD at their late stage of disease, this phenomenon still needs to be examined more systematically. For example, it will be important to directly measure AD patients' level of expectations, apply standardized pain measurement, and control for the natural history of pain to fully understand how these factors influence the placebo effect. Still, the study by Benedetti et al.11 indicates that the potential loss of expectancy-related mechanisms may make analgesic treatments less effective. Therefore, one could argue that the analgesic treatment should be increased progressively to compensate for the loss of placebo- and expectation-related mechanisms.11
2.6.2. Neurobiological underpinning
The study by Benedetti et al. also investigated the relationship between placebo mechanisms and frontal lobe connectivity by recording electrical activity of the brain with an electroencephalogram. Interestingly, patients with AD showed reduced connectivity between the prefrontal lobes and the rest of the brain at the 1-year retest, thereby indicating that the disruption of the placebo mechanisms co-occurred with reduced connectivity. It was therefore suggested that the impaired prefrontal connectivity reduces the communication between the prefrontal lobes and the rest of the brain, thereby obstructing placebo mechanisms such as expectancy to evolve.11 In light of the findings previously mentioned by Krummenacher et al. demonstrating that expectation-induced placebo analgesia in healthy participants can be blocked by disruption of PFC function, the reduced connectivity of the prefrontal lobes may at least in part explain why patients with AD do not experience placebo analgesia effects as the disease progresses.56
2.7. Parkinson disease
Studies of placebo effects in patients with PD have shown prominent effects.28 The studies have primarily been conducted in relation to motor symptoms and several have been conducted in patients implanted with electrodes in the subthalamic nuclei for deep brain stimulation (DBS).46,48,75 On this basis, placebo effects have been observed on motor symptoms when modulating expectations related to the subthalamic stimulation.46,57,64,75
2.7.1. Psychological factors
Pollo et al. have investigated how verbal suggestions for either good or bad motor performance influence the velocity of hand movement. A significant change in motor performance occurred when patients with PD expected a good motor performance, thereby suggesting that expectations induce neural changes within a very short time.75
In another study by Benedetti et al., patients with PD were tested for the velocity hand movement according to a double-blind experimental design in which neither the patient nor the experimenter knew whether the stimulator was turned on or off. On the day of the experimental session, the stimulator was kept on but the patients were told it had been turned off to induce negative expectations of motor performance. Although the stimulator was on, the patients' motor performance worsened.9
Both studies thereby demonstrate that motor performance in patients with PD, as has been seen with pain in other populations, can be modulated in 2 opposite directions by placebo and nocebo effects, and this modulation occurs on the basis of positive and negative verbal suggestions about motor performance.8,71 Recently, the role of learning mechanisms has been documented in patients with PD, showing promising results. In particular, it has for the first time been demonstrated that placebo administration induces neither clinical nor neuronal improvement in patients with PD who undergo DBS implantation without prior conditioning.16 However, substantial placebo responses occurred after repeated administrations of the anti-Parkinson agent apomorphine. As the number of apomorphine administrations increased from 1 to 4, both the clinical motor response and the neuronal activity in the ventral anterior and anterior ventrolateral thalamus increased.16 Importantly, beyond motor symptoms, DBS has shown potential for significant pain alleviation.43,44,50,60,68 These findings suggest that it is possible to investigate placebo analgesia effects in patients with PD using the open/hidden design either in relation to nonpharmacological treatments such as DBS and/or in relation to pharmacological analgesic treatment.
2.7.2. Neurobiological underpinning
As for the neurobiological mechanisms underlying placebo effects in patients with PD, neuroimaging studies have demonstrated that placebo effects in patients with PD are associated with dopamine release in the striatum.29,30,59 Specifically, de la Fuente-Fernández et al. conducted a neuroimaging study of placebo effects with a positron emission tomography. In the study, patients were aware that they would either receive an injection of an active drug or a placebo. The results suggested that the dopamine released in the ventral striatum was associated with the patient's expectation of improvement in motor symptoms. de la Fuente-Fernández et al.28 were the first to relate placebo effects to reward mechanisms and dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens, but other studies have later confirmed these findings.28,29,61,75 These reward mechanisms could also be expected to be central to analgesia effects of DBS.46,50 It would therefore be important to test whether dopamine is involved in placebo analgesia effects in patients with PD and especially investigate whether dopamine has a central role in the anticipation of pain relief and/or in the evaluation of the actual pain.
2.8. Nocebo effects
Compared to placebo effects, much less is known about nocebo effects. Nocebo effects were originally introduced to describe the negative effect of a placebo treatment,41 but today, they are primarily conceptualized as an independent phenomenon that mirror placebo effects. Generally, nocebo effects can be conceptualized as negative effects related to the patients' perception of the treatment13,71,72,83 More specifically, a nocebo effect can be seen as the effect that follows the administration of an inert treatment along with behavioral procedures and/or verbal suggestions that worsen the symptom and it should be separated from changes in natural history.16,71,72
2.8.1. Psychological factors
Like placebo effects, nocebo effects may contribute greatly to the outcome of a treatment. Specifically, suggestions and expectations of worsening can induce negative effects on disease symptoms and treatment outcome.24 Colloca et al. investigated the role of verbal suggestions and learning in nocebo hyperalgesia in healthy participants. By using a nocebo procedure, in which verbal suggestions of painful stimulation were given before the administration of either tactile or low-intensity painful electrical stimuli, their study indicated that verbal suggestions of a negative outcome can produce both hyperalgesic and allodynic effects and that conditioning did not produce an additional effect.24
In line with these findings, the open/hidden approach has been useful in the understanding of nocebo effects in patients with acute and chronic. In a study by Benedetti et al., morphine treatment in postoperative patients were interrupted either openly or hidden for the patient. Accordingly, the patients underwent one of 2 conditions; one where they were told that the morphine had been stopped (open condition) or one where the morphine was stopped without informing the patient (hidden condition). The result showed that pain was larger in the group that had undergone the open condition compared with the group in the hidden condition, suggesting that negative expectations of pain relapse may play an important role.10 The above finding is important, especially from a clinical point of view. If nocebo verbal suggestions are producing a negative response, it is important to think about how information about a treatment is delivered to the patient.24 Besides influencing expectations of improvement or worsening through information, it has been demonstrated that the interaction between the doctor and the patients does influence the treatment outcome.44
However, both open-hidden design and more traditional designs have not been able to demonstrate nocebo effects in patients with chronic pain tested in a laboratory setting.53,71,90 This is surprising and may be related to the relatively safe environment in the laboratory setting. In uncontrolled randomized controlled trial studies, however, adverse events are frequently found in the placebo arm, thereby suggesting that negative nocebo-like effects do occur in chronic populations3
2.8.2. Neurobiological underpinning
Brian imaging techniques have been fundamental in the understanding of the neurobiology of nocebo effects and negative expectations. Overall, the brain imaging studies indicate that negative expectations may result in the amplification of pain, with anticipation of pain-activating brain regions such as the ACC, the PFC, and the insula.48,54,61,74 However, besides neuroimaging, pharmacological studies provide an insight into the biochemistry of nocebo effects and negative expectations. Benedetti et al. have investigated neurotransmitters and found a release of the opioid antagonists CCK during nocebo hyperalgesia.12 In general, when investigating the placebo–nocebo phenomenon, there seems to be opposite effects and mechanisms. The opioidergic and the CCK-ergic systems seem to be activated by opposite expectations of either analgesia or hyperalgesia, meaning that verbal suggestions of a positive outcome activate endogenous µ-opioid neurotransmission, whereas suggestions of a negative outcome activates CCK-A and CCk-B receptors.12,13
2.8.3. Nocebo effects in central nervous system conditions
Nocebo-like effects have primarily been investigated in AD, PD, and across CNS diseases such as depression and scleroses through meta-analyses of adverse events in the nocebo arm of RCTs.65,69,73,85,90,97 For example, many patients with AD report adverse events, and they may be at greater risk of developing them, which could reflect nocebo responses. Yet, there is a need for further exploring nocebo effects in experimental studies. In PD, nocebo responses have been induced through negative verbal suggestions in experimental DBS trials. Studies have found some patients and certain symptoms to be susceptible to nocebo,46,64 whereas others have not found nocebo effects.47 Thus, further research is needed to identify exact nocebo effects and mechanisms in patients with AD and PD as well as across CNS diseases.
3. Future—where do we need to go?
Placebo analgesia effects are well documented in healthy participants. In this population, large placebo analgesia effects have been reported, and the knowledge of the psychological and neurobiological mechanisms is increasing substantially over time. Placebo analgesia is also reported in different pathological conditions, ranging from chronic pain to CNS diseases. Even if the study of placebo analgesia on pathological conditions is pivotal, our knowledge of the psychological and neurobiological underpinnings is still poor. For example, expectations for pain relief seem to be central for placebo effects across CNS diseases; it is, however, unclear whether a common neurobiological substrate can be delineated across diseases. Moreover, the role of learning mechanisms is still uncertain. On one hand, healthy participants experience robust placebo analgesic effects after a conditioning procedure, but, on the other hand, only few evidences of the role of learning come from patient populations. This aspect seems crucial because patients will often have negative treatment experiences during their lifetime and this may change the effectiveness of future pain treatments.
Brain imaging techniques have demonstrated neurobiological underpinning of placebo effects across all the reviewed CNS diseases. Yet, looking at the involvement of neurotransmitters, diverse findings emerge across diseases. In healthy participants, both the endogenous opioid, cannabinoid, and dopaminergic systems have been implicated in placebo analgesia effects, but none of these neurotransmitters seem to be involved in placebo effects in patients with chronic pain. Interestingly, the dopaminergic system seems to be central for placebo effects in patients with PD but the direct involvement of the dopaminergic system in placebo analgesia effects of patients with PD still needs to be investigated. Thus, this review indicates that the mechanisms of placebo analgesia effects are likely to differ across CNS diseases. Therefore, to understand the mechanisms and the contribution of placebo analgesia effects to pain treatments across CNS diseases, it is pivotal to investigate the placebo analgesia effect specifically within each disease, preferably using comparable designs.
In this article, it has been shown that the open/hidden design can be used to investigate placebo analgesia effects across CNS diseases, involving both pharmacological and nonpharmacological treatments, and specifying both psychological and neurobiological mechanisms. Importantly, this can be done within a clinically relevant context where active pain medication is given. We therefore suggest that this can be used as a comparable design across diseases to estimate the magnitude of placebo analgesia effects across CNS diseases. In this manner, we can begin to understand similarities and differences of placebo analgesia effects across CNS diseases. Currently, the primary knowledge of placebo effects is in pain, AD, and PD, and some in depression. Yet, it will be important to expand this knowledge to other CNS diseases including multiple sclerosis, autism, and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Both placebo and nocebo effects are important to consider in clinical practice because they may both have a great influence on treatment outcome through various factors and mechanisms.
The more we learn about placebo effects in specific CNS diseases, the better we can use this knowledge to optimize pain treatments. Specifically, it will be important to know which CNS diseases may benefit from optimized perception of the treatment and hence a placebo effect to adjust pain treatments accordingly. In this manner, the investigation of placebo analgesia effects across CNS diseases holds the potential to improve our understanding of placebo analgesia mechanisms and to offer better treatments for large groups of patients who currently suffer from insufficient pain treatment.
Disclosures
The authors have no conflict of interest to declare.
Footnotes
Sponsorships or competing interests that may be relevant to content are disclosed at the end of this article.
References
- [1].Amanzio M, Benedetti F. Neuropharmacological dissection of placebo analgesia: expectation-activated opioid systems versus conditioning-activated specific subsystems. JNeurosci 1999;19:484–94. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [2].Amanzio M, Pollo A, Maggi G, Benedetti F. Response variability to analgesics: a role for non-specific activation of endogenous opioids. PAIN 2001;90:205–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [3].Amanzio M, Benedetti F, Vase L. A systemactic review of adverse events in the placebo arm of donepezil trials: the role of cognitive impairment. Int Psychogeriatr 2012;24:698–707. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [4].Amanzio M, Benedetti F, Porro CA, Palermo S, Cauda F. Activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of brain correlates of placebo analgesia in human experimental pain. Hum Brain Mapp 2013;34:738–52. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [5].Aslaksen PM, Flaten MA. The roles of physiological and subjective stress in the effectiveness of a placebo on experimentally induced pain. Psychosom Med 2008;70:811–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [6].Benedetti F. The opposite effects of the opiate antagonist naloxone and the cholecystokinin antagonist proglumide on placebo analgesia. PAIN 1996;64:535–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [7].Benedetti F. Cholecystokinin type –A and type- B receptors and their modulation of opioid analgesia. News Physiol Sci 1997;12:263–8. [Google Scholar]
- [8].Benedetti F, Vighetti S, Ricco C, Lagna E, Bergamasco B, Pinessi L, Rainero I. Pain threshold and tolerance in Alzheimer's disease. PAIN 1999;80:377–82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [9].Benedetti F, Pollo A, Lopiano L, Lanotte M, Vighetti S, Rainero I. Conscious expectation and unconscious conditioning in analgesic, motor, and hormonal placebo/nocebo responses. J Neurosci 2003;23:4315–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [10].Benedetti F, Maggi G, Lopiano L. Open versus hidden medical treatments: the patient's knowledge about a therapy affects the therapy outcome. Prev Treat 2003:61.61a. [Google Scholar]
- [11].Benedetti F, Arduino C, Costa S, Vighetti S, Tarenzi L, Rainero I, Asteggiano G. Loss of expectation-related mechanisms in Alzheimer's disease makes analgesic therapies less effective. PAIN 2006;121:133–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [12].Benedetti F, Amanzio M, Vighetti S, Asteggiano G. The biochemical and neuroendocrine bases of the hyperalgesic nocebo effect. J Neurosci 2006;26:12014–22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [13].Benedetti F, Lanotte M, Lopiano L, Colloca L. When words are painful: unraveling the mechanisms of the nocebo effect. Neuroscience 2007;147:260–71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [14].Benedetti F, Amanzio M. The response: how words and rituals change the patient's brain. Patient Educ Couns 2011;84:413–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [15].Benedetti F, Thoen W, Blanchard C, Vighetti S, Arduino C. Pain as a reward: changing the meaning of pain from negative to positive co-activates opioid and cannabinoid systems. PAIN 2013;154:361–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [16].Benedetti F, Frisaldi E, Carlino E, Giudetti L, Pampallona A, Zibetti M, Lanotte M, Lopiano L. Teaching neurons to respond to placebo. J Physiol 2016;594:5647–60. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [17].Bingel U, Wanigasekera V, Wiech K, Ni Mhuircheartaigh R, Lee MC, Ploner M, Tracey I. The effect of treatment expectation on drug efficacy: imaging the analgesic benefit of the opioid remifentanil. Sci Transl Med 2011;3:70ra14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [18].Brown TR, Slee A. A randomized placebo-controlled trial of duloxetine for central pain in multiple sclerosis. Int J MS Care 2015;17:83–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [19].Carlino E, Torta DM, Piedimonte A, Frisaldi E, Vighetti S, Benedetti F. Role of explicit verbal information in conditioned analgesia. Eur J Pain 2015;19:546–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [20].Charron J, Rainville P, Marchand S,. Direct comparison of placebo effects on clinical and experimental pain. Clin J Pain 2006;22:204–11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [21].Colagiuri B, Schenk LA, Kessler MD, Dorsey SG, Colloca L. The placebo effect: from concepts to genes. Neuroscience 2015;307:171–90. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [22].Colloca L, Benedetti F. How prior experience shape placebo analgesia. PAIN 2006;124:126–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [23].Colloca L, Tinazzi M, Recchia S, Le Pera D, Fiaschi A, Benedetti F, Valeriani M. Learning potentiates neurophysiological and behavioral placebo analgesic responses. PAIN 2008;139:306–14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [24].Colloca L, Sigaudo M, Benedetti F. The role of learning in nocebo and placebo effects. PAIN 2008;136:211–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [25].Colloca L, Benedetti F. Placebo analgesia induced by social observational learning. PAIN 2009;144:28–34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [26].Craggs JG, Price DD, Robinson ME. Enhancing the placebo response: functional resonance imaging evidence of memory and semantic processing in placebo analgesia. J Pain 2014;15:435–46. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [27].de Tommaso M, Kunz M, Valeriani M,. Therapeutic approach to pain in neurodegenerative diseases: current evidence and perspectives. Expert Rev Neurother 2017;17:143–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [28].de la Fuente-Fernández R, Ruth TJ, Sossi V, Schulzer M, Calne DB, Stoessl AJ. Expectation and dopamine release: mechanism of the placebo effect in Parkinson's disease. Science 2001;293:1164–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [29].de la Fuente-Fernández R, Philips AG, Zamburlini M, Sossi V, Calne DB, Ruth TJ, Stoessl AJ. Dopamine release in human ventral striatum and expectation of reward. Behav Brain Res 2002;136:359–63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [30].de la Fuente-Fernández R. The placebo-reward hypothesis: dopamine and the placebo effect. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 2009;15:72–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [31].De Pascalis V, Chiaradia C, Carotenuto E. The contribution of suggestibility and expectation to placebo analgesia phenomenon in an experimental setting. PAIN 2002;96:393–402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [32].Eippert F, Bingel U, Schoell ED, Yacubian J, Klinger R, Lorenz J, Büchel C. Activation of the opioidergic descending pain control system underlies placebo analgesia. Neuron 2009;63:533–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [33].Elsenbruch S. How positive and negative expectations shape the experience of visceral pain. Handb Exp Pharmacol 2014;225:97–119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [34].Fields HL, Levine JD. Placebo analgesia- a role for endorphins? Viewpoint. Treds Neurosci 1984;7:271–3. [Google Scholar]
- [35].Finniss DG, Kaptchuk TJ, Miller F, Bennedetti F. Placebo effects: biological, clinical and ethical advances. Lanced 2010;375:686–95. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [36].Flaten MA, Aslaksen PM, Finset A, Simonsen T, Johansen O. Cognitive and emotional factors in placebo analgesia. J Psychosom Res 2006;61:81–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [37].Flatten MA, Aslaksen PM, Lyby P, Bjørkedal E. The relation of emotions of placebo responses. Phil Trans R Soc 2011;366:1818–27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [38].Forsberg JT, Martinussen M, Flaten MA. The placebo analgesic effect in healthy individuals and patients: a meta-analysis. Psychosom Med 2017;79:388–94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [39].Geuter S, Koban L, Wager TD. The cognitive neuroscience of placebo effects: concepts, predictions, and physiology. Annu Rev Neurosci 2017;40:167–88. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [40].Grevert P, Albert LH, Goldstein A. Partial antagonism of placebo analgesia by naloxone. PAIN 1983;16:129–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [41].Hahn RA. The nocebo phenomenon, concept, evidence, and implications for public health. Prev Med 1997;26:207–611. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [42].Hall KT, Lembo AJ, Kirsch I, Ziogas DC, Douaiher J, Jensen KB, Conbiy LA, Kelley JM, Kokkotou E, Kaptchuk TJ. Catechol-O-methyltransferase val158met polymorphism predicts placebo effect in irritable bowel syndrome. PLoS One 2012;7:e48135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [43].Jung YJ, Kim HJ, Jeon BS, Park H, Lee WW, Paek SH. An 8-year follow-up on the effect of subthalamic nucleus deep brain stimulation on pain in Parkinson disease. JAMA Neurol 2015;72:504–10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [44].Kaptchuk TJ, Kelley JM, Conboy LA, Davis RB, Kerr CE, Jacobson EE, Kirsch I, Schyner RN, Nam BH, Nguyen LT, Park M, Rivers AL, McManus C, Kokkotou E, Drossman DA, Goldman P, Lembo AJ,. Components of placebo effect: randomised controlled trial in patients with irritable bowel syndrome. BMJ 2008;336:999–1003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [45].Kam-Hansen S, Jakubowski M, Kelley JM, Kirsch I, Hoaglin DC, Kaptchuk TJ, Burstein R. Altered placebo and drug labeling changes the outcome of episodic migraine attacks. Sci Transl Med 2014;8:6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [46].Keitel A, Ferrea S, Südmeyer M, Schnitzler A, Wojtecki L. Expectation modulates the effect of deep brain stimulation on motor and cognitive function in tremor-dominant Parkinson's disease. PLoS One 2013;8:e81878. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [47].Keitel A, Wojtecki L, Hirschmann J, Hartmann CJ, Ferrea S, Südmeyer M, Schnitzler A. Motor and cognitive placebo-/nocebo-responses in Parkinson's disease patients with deep brain stimulation. Behav Brain Res 2013;250:199–205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [48].Keltner JR, Furst A, Fan C, Redfern R, Inglis B, Fields HL,. Isolating the modulatory effect of expectation on pain transmission: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Neurosci 2006;26:4437–43. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [49].Kessner S, Forkmann K, Ritter C, Wiech K, Ploner M, Bingel U. The effect of treatment history on therapeutic potcome: psychological and neurobiological underpinnings. PLoS One 2014;9:e109014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [50].Kim HJ, Jeon BS, Lee JY, Paek SH, Kim DG. The benefit of subthalamic deep brain stimulation for pain in Parkinson disease: a 2-year follow-up study. Neurosurgery 2012;70:18–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [51].Kirsch I. The placebo effect revisited: lessons learned to date. Complement Ther Med 2013;21:102–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [52].Kirsch I, Kong J, Sadler P, Spaeth R, Cook A, Kaptchuk T, Gollub R. Expectancy and conditioning in placebo analgesia: separate or connected processes? Psychol Conscious (Wash D C) 2014;1:51–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [53].Klinger R, Schmitz J, Benson S, Elsenbruch S, Kamping S, Flor H. Schmerzreduction durch variation von kontextreizen: Möglichketen zur selbstwirksamkeit in der schmerztherapie? Der Schmerz 2013:47–9. [Google Scholar]
- [54].Koyama T, McHaffie JG, Laurienti PJ, Coghill RC. The subjective experience of pain: where expectations become reality. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2005;102:12950–5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [55].Kupers R, Maeyaert J, Boly M, Faymonville ME, Laureys S. Naloxone-insensitive epidural placebo analgesia in a chronic pain patient. Anesthesiology 2007;106:1239–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [56].Krummenacher P, Candia V, Folkers G, Schedlowski M, Schönbächler G. Prefrontal cortex modulates placebo analgesia. PAIN 2010;148:368–74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [57].Lanotte M, Lopiano L, Torre E, Bergamasco B, Colloca L, Benedetti F. Expectation enhances autonomic responses to stimulation of the human subthalamic limbic region. Brain Behav Immun 2005;19:500–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [58].Levine JD, Gordon NC, Fields HL. The mechanism of placebo analgesia. Lancet 1978;2:654–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [59].Levine JD, Gordon NC. Influence of the method of drug administration on analgesic response. Nature 1984;312:755–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [60].Lidstone SC, Schulzer M, Dinelle K, Mak E, Sossi V, Ruth TJ, de la Fuente-Fernández R, Phillips AG, Stoessl AJ. Effects of expectation on placebo-induced dopamine release in Parkinson disease. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2010;67:857–65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [61].Lorenz J, Hauck M, Paur RC, Nakamura Y, Zimmermann R, Bromm B, Engel AK. Cortical correlates of false expectations during pain intensity judgments—a possible manifestation of placebo/nocebo cognitions. Brain Behav Immun 2005;19:283–95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [62].Lyby PS, Aslaksen PM, FlatenMA Is fear of pain related to placebo analgesia? J Psychosom Res 2010;68:369–77. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [63].Lyby PS, Forsberg JT, Asli O, Flaten MA. Induced fear reduces the effectiveness od a placebo intervention on pain. PAIN 2012;153:1114–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [64].Mercado R, Constantoyannis C, Mandat T, Kumar A, Schulzer M, Stoessl AJ, Honey CR. Expectation and the placebo effect in Parkinson's disease patients with subthalamic nucleus deep brain stimulation. Mov Disord 2006;21:1457–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [65].Mitsikostas DD, Mantonakis L, Chalarakis N. Nocebo in clinical trials for depression: a meta-analysis. Psychiatry Res 2014;215:82–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [66].Morrison RS, Siu AL. A comparison of pain and its treatment in advanced dementia and cognitively intact patients with hip fracture. J Pain 2000;19:240–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [67].Müller M, Kamping S, Benrath J, Skowronek H, Schmitz J, Klinger R, Flor H. Treatment history and placebo responses to experimental and clinical pain in chronic pain patients. Eur J Pain 2016;20:1530–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [68].Oshima H, Katayama Y, Morishita T, Sumi K, Otaka T, Kobayashi K, Suzuki Y, Fukaya C, Yamamoto T. Subthalamic nucleus stimulation for attenuation of pain related to Parkinson disease. J Neurosurg 2012;116:99–106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [69].Papadopoulos D, Mitsikostas DD. Nocebo effects in multiple sclerosis trials: a meta-analysis. Mult Scler 2010;16:816–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [70].Petersen GL, Finnerup NB, Nørskov KN, Grosen K, Pilegaard HK, Benedetti F, Price DD, Jensen TS, Vase L. Placebo manipulations reduce hyperalgesia in neuropathic pain. PAIN 2012;153:1292–300. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [71].Petersen GL, Finnerup NB, Grosen K, Pilegaard HK, Tracy I, Benedetti F, Price DD, Jensen TS, Vase L. Expectations and positive emotional feelings accompany reductions in ongoing and evoked neuropathic pain following placebo interventions. PAIN 2014;155:2687–98. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [72].Petersen GL, Finnerup NB, Colloca L, Amanzio M, Price DD, Jensen TS, Vase L. The magnitude of nocebo effects in pain: a meta-analysis. PAIN 2014;155:1426–34. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [73].Piedimonte A, Guerra G, Vighetti S, Carlino E. Measurin expectation of pain: contingent negative variation in placebo and nocebo effects. Eur J Pain 2017;21:874–85. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [74].Ploghaus A, Tracey I, Gati JS, Clare S, Menon RS, Matthews PM, Rawlins JN. Dissociating pain from its anticipation in the human brain. Science 1999;284:1979–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [75].Pollo A, Torre E, Lopiano L, Rizzone M, Lanotte M, Cavanna A, Bergamasco B, Benedetti F. Expectation modulates the response to subthalamic nucleus stimulation in Parkinsonian patients. Neuroreport 2002;13:1383–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [76].Posner J, Burke CA. The effects of naloxone on opiate and placebo analgesia in healthy volunteers. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1985;87:468–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [77].Price DD, Milling LS, Kirsch I, Duff A, Montgomery GH, Nicholls SS. An analysis of factors that contribute to the magnitude of placebo analgesia in an experimental paradigm. PAIN 1999;83:147–56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [78].Price DD, Craggs J, Verne GN, Perlstein WM, Robinson ME. Placebo analgesia is accompanied by large reductions in pain-related brain activity in irritable bowel syndrome patients. PAIN 2007;127:63–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [79].Price DD, Craggs JG, Zhou Q, Verne GN, Peristein WM, Robinson ME. Widespread hyperalgesia in irritable bowel syndrome is dynamically maintained by tonic visceral impulse input and placebo/nocebo factors: evidence from human psychophysics, animal models, and neuroimaging. Neuroimage 2009;47:995–1001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [80].Schimrigk S, Marziniak M, Neubauer C, Kugler EM, Werner G, Abramov-Sommariva D,. Dronabinol is a safe long-term treatment option for neuropathic pain patients. Eur Neurol 2017;78:320–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [81].Schmid J, Theysohn N, Gaß F, Benson S, Gramsch C, Forsting M, Gizewski ER, Elsenbruch S. Neural mechanisms mediating positive and negative treatment expectations in visceral pain: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study on placebo and nocebo effects in healthy volunteers. PAIN 2013;154:2372–80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [82].Scott DJ, Stohler CS, Egnatuk CM, Wang H, Koeppe RA, Zubieta JK. Individual differences in reward responding explain placebo-induced expectations and effects. Neuron 2007;55:325–36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [83].Scott DJ, Stohler CS, Egnatuk CM, Wang H, Koeppe RA, Zubieta JK. Placebo and nocebo effects are defined by opposite opioid and dopaminergiv responses. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2008;65:220–31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [84].Skyt I, Moslemi K, Baastrup C, Grosen K, Benedetti F, Petersen GL. Dopaminergic tone does not influence pain levels during placebo interventions in patients with chronic neuropathic pain. PAIN 2018;0:1–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [85].Stathis P, Smpiliris M, Konitsiotis S, Mitsikostas DD. Nocebo as a potential confounding factor in clinical trials for Parkinson's disease treatment: a meta-analysis. Eur J Neurol 2013;20:527–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [86].Strafella AP, Ko JH, Monchi O. Therapeutic application of transcranial magnetic stimulation in Parkinson's disease: the contribution of expectation. Neuroimage 2006;31:1666–72. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [87].Vase L, Riley JL, III, Price DD. A comparison of placebo effects in clinical analgesic trials versus studies of placebo analgesia. PAIN 2002;99:443–52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [88].Vase L, Robinson ME, Verne GN, Price DD. The contributions of suggestion, desire, and expectation to placebo effects in irritable bowel syndrome patients: an empirical investigation. PAIN 2003;105:17–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [89].Vase L, Price DD, Verne GN, Robinson ME. The contribution of changes in expected pain levels and desire for pain relief to placebo in analgesia. In: Price DD, Bushnell MC, editors. Psychological methods of pain control: basic science and clinical perspectives. Seattle: IASP Press, 2004, pp. 207–34. [Google Scholar]
- [90].Vase L, Robinson ME, Verne GN, Price DD. Increased placebo analgesia over time in irritable bowel syndrome (IBS) patients is associated with desire and expectation but not endogenous opioid mechanisms. PAIN 2005;115:338–47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [91].Vase L, Skyt I, Petersen GL, Price DD. Placebo and nocebo effects in chronic pain patients: how expectations and emotional feelings contribute to the experience of pain. Z für Psychol 2014;222:135–9. [Google Scholar]
- [92].Voudouris NJ, Peck CL, Coleman G. The role of conditioning and verbal expectancy in the placebo response. PAIN 1990;43:121–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [93].Wager TD, Rilling JK, Smith EE, Sokolik A, Casey KL, Davidson RJ, Kosslyn SM, Rose RM, Cohen JD. Placebo-induced changes in FMRI in the anticipation and experience of pain. Science 2004;303:1162–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [94].Wager TD, Matre D, Casey KL. Placebo effects in laser-evoked pain potentials. Brain Behav Immun 2006;20:219–30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [95].Wager TD, Atlas LY. The neuroscience of placebo effects: connecting context, learning and health. Nat Rev Neurosci 2015;16:403–18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [96].Wrobel N, Wiech K, Forkmann K, Ritter C, Bingel U. Haloperidol blocks dorsal striatum activity but not analgesia in a placebo paradigm. Cortex 2014;57:60–73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [97].Zis P, Mitsikostas DD. Nocebo in Alzheimer's disease; meta-analysis of placebo-controlled clinical trials. J Neurol Sci 2015;355:94–100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [98].Zunhammer M, Ploner M, Engelbrecht C, Bock J, Kessner SS, Bingel U. The effects of treatment failure generalize across different routes of drug administration. Sci Transl Med 2017;7:9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [99].Zunhammer M, Bingel U, Wager TD. Placebo imaging consortium. Placebo effects on the neurologic pain signature: a meta-analysis of individual participant functional magnetic resonance imaging data. JAMA Neurol 2018;75:1321. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]