Abstract
In visual search, the presence of a salient, yet task-irrelevant, distractor in the stimulus array interferes with target selection and slows down performance. Neuroimaging data point to a key role of the frontoparietal dorsal attention network in dealing with visual distractors; however, the respective roles of different nodes within the network and their hemispheric specialization are still unresolved. Here, we used transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) to evaluate the causal role of two key regions of the dorsal attention network in resisting attentional capture by a salient singleton distractor: the frontal eye field (FEF) and the cortex within the intraparietal sulcus (IPS). The task of the participants (male/female human volunteers) was to discriminate the pointing direction of a target arrow while ignoring a task-irrelevant salient distractor. Immediately after stimulus onset, triple-pulse 10 Hz TMS was delivered either to IPS or FEF on either side of the brain. Results indicated that TMS over the right FEF significantly reduced the behavioral cost engendered by the salient distractor relative to left FEF stimulation. No such effect was obtained with stimulation of IPS on either side of brain. Interestingly, this FEF-dependent reduction in distractor interference interacted with the contingent trial history, being maximal when no distractor was present on the previous trial relative to when there was one. Our results provide direct causal evidence that the right FEF houses key mechanisms for distractor filtering, pointing to a pivotal role of the frontal cortex of the right hemisphere in limiting interference from an irrelevant but attention-grabbing stimulus.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Visually conspicuous stimuli attract our attention automatically and interfere with performance by diverting resources away from the main task. Here, we applied transcranial magnetic stimulation over four frontoparietal cortex locations (frontal eye field and intraparietal sulcus in each hemisphere) to identify regions of the dorsal attention network that help limit interference from task-irrelevant, salient distractors. Results indicate that the right FEF participates in distractor-filtering mechanisms that are recruited when a distracting stimulus is encountered. Moreover, right FEF implements adjustments in distraction-filtering mechanisms following recent encounters with distractors. Together, these findings indicate a different hemispheric contribution of the left versus right dorsal frontal cortex to distraction filtering. This study expands our understanding of how our brains select relevant targets in the face of task-irrelevant, salient distractors.
Keywords: distractor filtering, dorsal attention network, frontal eye field, intraparietal sulcus, transcranial magnetic stimulation
Introduction
Visual selective attention supports an individual's ability to select relevant information while disregarding irrelevant but attention-grabbing stimuli (Jonides and Yantis, 1988; Yantis and Jonides, 1990; Forster and Lavie, 2008; Marini et al., 2013). Evidence that attention is involuntarily captured by salient stimuli comes, among others, from studies using visual search arrays with salient singleton distractors, such as a uniquely colored stimulus in an array of differently colored items (Theeuwes, 1992; Theeuwes and Burger, 1998; Theeuwes and Godljn, 2002). The presence of a singleton distractor in a search array results in an unwanted shift of attention to the salient stimulus (attentional capture effect), as indexed by a measurable performance cost relative to trials without the singleton distractor. Stimulus-driven mechanisms responsible for the attentional capture effects are antagonized by goal-driven mechanisms that guide attention toward the relevant target stimulus and suppress distractors (Reynolds and Chelazzi, 2004; Müller and Ebeling, 2008; Chelazzi et al., 2011; Marini et al., 2016), although the cognitive and neural mechanisms that support distractor filtering are still under debate.
Distractor filtering, which consists of the elimination, or at least the attenuation, of the behavioral costs engendered by irrelevant yet salient stimuli, has received mounting interest in recent years (Chelazzi et al., 2019). Different putative mechanisms for distractor filtering have been proposed depending on the adopted experimental paradigm and behavioral context (Chelazzi et al., 2019). These mechanisms include proactive control (Marini et al., 2013, 2016; Geng, 2014; Cosman et al., 2018), habituation of capture (Neo and Chua, 2006; Pascucci and Turatto, 2015; Turatto et al., 2018a,b), intertrial priming (Geyer et al., 2008; Müller et al., 2010), and implicit distractor probability learning (Goschy et al., 2014; Ferrante et al., 2018; Wang and Theeuwes, 2018a,b; Sauter et al., 2018, 2019; Di Caro et al., 2019). Despite the abundance of behavioral evidence about specific task conditions and behavioral contexts wherein distractor suppression occurs, much less is known about the underlying neural mechanisms (Egeth et al., 2010; Folk and Remington, 2010; Seidl et al., 2012; Gaspar and McDonald, 2014; Geng, 2014; Liesefeld et al., 2014, 2017; Marini et al., 2016; Donohue et al., 2018). Therefore, the present study sought to characterize the neural machinery for distractor filtering by investigating the causal role of different subregions within the dorsal attention network.
Numerous functional imaging studies demonstrated that attentional control in the presence of potential distraction is supported by the dorsal (mostly bilateral) frontoparietal attention network, whose core regions include the frontal eye field (FEF) and the posterior parietal cortex (PPC), notably tissue within the intraparietal sulcus (IPS) (Corbetta and Shulman, 2002; de Fockert et al., 2004; Serences et al., 2004, 2005; Leber, 2010; Talsma et al., 2010; de Fockert and Theeuwes, 2012; DiQuattro et al., 2014; Marini et al., 2016; Lee and Geng, 2017). These anatomical locations align well with electrophysiological evidence in nonhuman primates, showing that both target selection and distractor suppression mechanisms involve parietal cortex (Ipata et al., 2006) and PFC (for a specific contribution of FEF in distractor suppression, see Cosman et al., 2018), although neural responses to salient distractors are more greatly suppressed and more closely correlated with performance in prefrontal, rather than parietal cortex (Suzuki and Gottlieb, 2013).
Brain imaging studies in humans also reported a correlation between frontal (but not parietal) neural activity and the magnitude of distractor interference, strongly supporting a role for the frontal cortex in actively preventing the interference from irrelevant distractors (de Fockert et al., 2004; de Fockert and Theeuwes, 2012; Marini et al., 2016). Consistently, event-related potential (ERP) studies also converge in indicating a prominent role of frontal areas in attentional control (Grent-'t-Jong and Woldorff, 2007; Leblanc et al., 2008; Brignani et al., 2009; Ptak et al., 2011; Shomstein et al., 2012; Liesefeld et al., 2014). These findings also revealed an orderly temporal structure of neural signatures during the control of spatial attentional allocation, with attentional control signals first elicited at the level of the frontal lobe, followed by activity in the parietal lobe. This temporal advantage for the frontal over the parietal signals likely reflects processes involved in attentional deployment, but also in target selection and conflict control. Together, these results seem to indicate a leading contribution of the frontal areas in monitoring (potential) conflict and proactively preventing or reactively abating distraction. However, an inherent limitation of these studies is the inability to reveal any causal organization in the described relationship between brain activity and behavioral performance. To probe causal relationships between brain and behavior, noninvasive brain stimulation techniques are the most suitable choice (Pascual-Leone et al., 2000). Cosman et al. (2015) demonstrated that anodal transcranial direct-current stimulation (tDCS) over PFC enhanced the ability of participants to overcome distraction by decreasing the reaction time (RT) cost associated with a salient but task-irrelevant item. Along the same lines, by using repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) over PPC, Hodsoll et al. (2009) found that TMS over the right PPC, but not the left PPC, significantly reduced the RT cost of distraction. Moreover, the frontoparietal network is causally involved in filtering out not only perceptually salient, but also conflicting distractors, as demonstrated by modulations in the interference elicited by task-irrelevant flanker stimuli after tDCS of either PFC (Zmigrod et al., 2016) or parietal cortex (Weiss and Lavidor, 2012; Kajimura and Nomura, 2015).
Brain stimulation studies have revealed that the parietal and prefrontal cortices are involved in distractor filtering. However, due to the poor spatial resolution of tDCS and the lack of TMS or tDCS studies that directly compared the two crucial nodes of the dorsal frontoparietal network, namely, the FEF and IPS, it remains to be established whether distractor filtering is more distinctively supported by one or the other node of this network, or equally by both. Likewise, the available evidence is highly inconclusive as to whether the right and left dorsal attention networks are equally involved in distractor filtering. In the present study, we used TMS to comparatively investigate the causal role in distraction filtering of FEF and IPS on either side of the brain. Our general hypothesis was that the dorsal frontoparietal network is causally involved in distraction filtering, consistent with evidence in the literature (Chelazzi et al., 2019), but that this function might be more strongly supported by either node in the network. In particular, in light of previous neuroimaging observations (de Fockert et al., 2004; Melloni et al., 2012; de Fockert and Theeuwes, 2012; Marini et al., 2016) and the established temporal dynamics within the frontoparietal network, demonstrated by ERP studies (Grent-'t-Jong and Woldorff, 2007; Leblanc et al., 2008; Brignani et al., 2009; Ptak et al., 2011; Shomstein et al., 2012; Liesefeld et al., 2014), we might expect a more distinctive involvement of frontal regions in distractor suppression mechanisms, as indexed by a stronger modulation in the cost of distraction following FEF stimulation.
Cross-trial contingencies, such as the presence or absence of distractors in consecutive trials, are well known to modulate distraction filtering: for example, the RT cost engendered by a salient distractor is larger when a distractor was absent (vs present) in the immediately preceding trial (Geyer et al., 2008; Marini et al., 2013). A neuro-functional architecture for this effect may consist of an increase in top-down control following distractor encounters (Botvinick et al., 2001; Kerns et al., 2004), mediated by increased activity in the frontoparietal circuit (FEF and IPS, see Walsh et al., 2011). This view finds support in neuro-stimulation studies (Hodsoll et al., 2009; Soutschek et al., 2013) showing that the modulation of activity in the right PPC by means of TMS affected performance during conflict tasks, with the effect being strongly modulated as a function of what happened in the previous trial. Hodsoll et al. (2009) used an additional singleton paradigm to demonstrate that inhibitory 1 Hz TMS over the right PPC, but not left PPC, reduced the behavioral cost of a color-singleton distractor. Crucially, this lessening of distractor interference was mostly due to the elimination of priming effects between target and distractor singletons on consecutive trials. In this context, we therefore hypothesized an effect of TMS that varies depending on recent trial history, namely, depending on the state of the brain in the instant when it encounters a distractor. This idea also fits well with the notion of state dependency, whereby the effects of TMS on behavioral performance depend not only on the stimulation parameters themselves, but also on the brain's “state” when stimulation is applied (Silvanto and Pascual-Leone, 2008; Silvanto and Cattaneo, 2017). Based on this evidence, here we formulated the additional hypothesis that modulations of distraction filtering by cross-trial contingencies may likewise be mediated by the dorsal frontoparietal network, and therefore that FEF and/or IPS stimulation might alter such history-dependent modulations of distractor filtering. Therefore, if FEF and/or IPS are involved also in the regulation of cross-trial dynamics of distractor-filtering, we expected to identify an interaction between TMS stimulation on a given trial and the presence versus absence of a distractor in the preceding trial, a possibility that we addressed with the present work.
Materials and Methods
Participants.
Thirty-two participants took part in the experiment (22 female; mean age = 23.56 years, SD = 3.44 years). Two of them had to be excluded: 1 participant did not complete the task because FEF stimulation induced slight movements of the contralateral hand, which interfered with the task; another participant was excluded because of near-chance responses (mean accuracy: 58%). Therefore, data from 30 participants (20 females, mean age = 23.4 years, SD = 3.24 years) were used for the analyses reported below. We would like to state candidly that, following the initial reviewing of our study, in compliance with the reviewers' recommendation, we increased the original sample size, from N = 20 to the final N = 30 subjects. To the benefit of complete transparency, the key statistical results on the original sample are available online (https://osf.io/4ke56/; stored on the Open Science Framework data sharing platform). All participants were right-handed and with normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity and normal color vision. Before the TMS experiment, each subject filled out a questionnaire to evaluate eligibility for TMS. None of the participants reported any contraindications for TMS use (Rossi et al., 2009). Written informed consent was obtained from all participants before the beginning of the experiment. The study protocol was approved by the local ethical committee, and the experiment was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.
Materials and stimuli.
The protocol was adapted from the additional singleton paradigm (Theeuwes, 1992). A version of the paradigm similar to the one used in the current experiment has been used previously in our laboratory (Ferrante et al., 2018). The visual search display consisted of four stimuli (one per visual quadrant) presented equidistantly from one another and centered on a central fixation point (eccentricity: 4°). All stimuli were composed of two green or red triangles (1° × 1° each) presented on a light gray background. In 50% of trials, all display items were of the same color (e.g., red; distractor-absent condition); whereas in the remaining 50% of trials, three items were of the same color (e.g., red) and the remaining item (additional singleton) was of the alternative color (e.g., green; distractor-present condition). The target was defined as the only item in the display with both triangles pointing in the same direction (both up or both down, i.e., a double arrowhead), whereas the singleton distractor, when present, was a color-singleton stimulus with both triangles pointing outwardly. The remaining stimuli (nontargets or fillers) were always of the same color as the target and with both triangles pointing inwardly (Fig. 1A).
Procedure.
Participants were seated in front of a 17 inch CRT monitor (spatial resolution of 1280 × 1024 pixels and a refresh rate of 75 Hz) at a distance of 57 cm, in a dimly illuminated, silent room. A chin rest was used to keep the viewing distance constant during the whole session. Each trial began with a fixation point displayed in isolation for 1000 ms, and this was then accompanied for 700 ms by an array of four placeholders, which were identical to nontargets. At the end of the 700 ms period, one of the placeholders was replaced by the target; and on 50% of the trials, a different placeholder was replaced by the singleton distractor (Tommasi et al., 2015; Ferrante et al., 2018). This array remained visible for 50 ms and was followed by a blank screen until the participant responded or 2000 ms had elapsed, whichever came first. The subsequent trial started after a 4000 ms intertrial interval (Fig. 1B). The participants' task was to indicate as quickly and as accurately as possible whether the target element was pointing up or down by pressing “1” for “up” or “2” for “down” on a numeric keypad (or vice versa; counterbalanced across participants). The experiment included two sessions conducted on 2 different days. In each session, participants completed a practice block of 24 trials to familiarize with the task, followed by 6 experimental blocks (one per TMS condition).
Each block consisted of 72 trials, 36 of which were distractor-present and 36 distractor-absent. Within each block, the target and the singleton distractor were presented equally often at any given spatial location. The order of blocks was pseudo-randomized in such a way that in the first session the six different TMS conditions were equally distributed across participants (to spread out any potential learning effect equally across TMS conditions). The second session was identical to the first one, except that the order of blocks was reversed relative to the first session (to minimize any carryover effects related to stimulation site). Each experimental session lasted ∼2 h.
TMS.
Online neuro-navigated TMS was delivered using a Rapid2 stimulator (Magstim) connected to a 70 mm butterfly coil. Targeted sites in different blocks were over the left and right IPS and over the left and right FEF. To control for any possible nonspecific effects due to lateralized TMS, we identified two suitable sham conditions in a region on the scalp located between IPS and FEF of the left and right hemisphere, respectively. This yielded six different TMS conditions corresponding to the six scalp sites of stimulation (four with active stimulation and two with sham stimulation). The four active TMS sites were localized by means of stereotaxic navigation on individual estimated MRIs obtained through a 3D warping procedure fitting a high-resolution MRI template with the participant's scalp model and craniometric points (Softaxic, EMS). Neuro-navigation used anatomical Talairach coordinates (Talairach and Tournoux, 1988) obtained by converting the MNI coordinates of the sites of interest from a recent meta-analysis of fMRI studies of eye movements (Zhou and Shu, 2017). Coordinates were x = −36, y = −1, z = 48 and x = 36 y = −1, z = 48 for left and right FEF, respectively, and x = −30, y = −53, z = 49 and x = 30, y = −53, z = 49 for left and right IPS, respectively (Fig. 1C).
Since the existing literature shows that FEF can be localized based on distance from primary motor cortex (M1) (Ro et al., 1999), as an additional step, we measured the anatomical distance on the scalp between the targeted, putative FEF site and the corresponding M1 within each hemisphere. After localizing the area of primary motor cortex that produced the most robust contraction of the contralateral hand, a scalp marking was made on each subject over this location (both over the right and left M1). Then, after localizing the putative FEF hotspots by means of the neuro-navigation approach, as previously described, we calculated the distance between putative FEF and M1 in each hemisphere, separately for each participant. Across subjects, the mean distances between FEF and M1 were 2.76 cm (right) and 2.78 cm (left). More importantly, once we obtained our behavioral indexes of TMS-induced modulation of performance (see below), we asked whether such modulation was affected by the distance across individual participants between the two critical sites (M1 and putative FEF). To anticipate, by applying this method and a correlational approach, we obtained evidence to indicate that the effects of TMS across participants tended to be greatest when applied at a certain distance anteriorly from the functionally identified M1 site (see below).
The resting motor threshold (rMT) was determined using a software-based “adaptive method” developed by Awiszus (2003) (Motor Threshold Assessment Tool, version 2.0: http://www.clinicalresearcher.org/software.htm). Any visible muscle twitch was entered in the software as a “valid response.” During the experiment, TMS was delivered at 100% of the individual rMT (mean intensity = 51% of the maximum stimulator output). For the left FEF/IPS and right FEF/IPS, the coil was initially oriented with an angle of ∼45° from the nasion-inion line and the handle pointing outwards, and hence adjusted for each participant to minimize discomfort. For the two sham conditions, the coil was held perpendicular to the scalp to ensure that the magnetic field did not stimulate the underlying cortex. Three TMS pulses were delivered at 10 Hz (pulse gap of 100 ms) starting 100 ms after search array onset (Fig. 1B). We used triple-pulse 10 Hz TMS following previous studies showing that these stimulation parameters were effective in modulating the underlying cortical activity (Cohen Kadosh et al., 2010; Saad and Silvanto, 2013). Triple-pulse 10 Hz TMS starting at 100 ms covered a time window (100–300 ms) that is crucial for attentional capture effects and distractor suppression mechanisms, as shown by human scalp electrophysiology (Jannati et al., 2013; McDonald et al., 2013; Liesefeld et al., 2017). The software Open-Sesame (Mathôt et al., 2012) was used for stimulus presentation, data collection, and TMS triggering.
Statistical analysis.
RTs were log-transformed before the analysis to assuage deviations from normality (before transformation: skewness = 2.23, kurtosis = 5.52; after transformation: skewness = −0.05, kurtosis = 0.44). Only correct-response trials were included in the RT analysis (a total of 5.8% of trials were excluded). Linear mixed-effect models were used as the main statistical procedure (Baayen et al., 2008). We set up each model following the Barr et al. (2013) recommendation to model the maximal random-effects structure justified by the experimental design. Statistical significance was tested with the F test with Satterthwaite approximation of degrees of freedom. All the models were estimated using R (R Development Core Team, 2016) and the lme4 package (version 1.1–12) (Bates et al., 2014). When appropriate, post hoc tests were conducted using the R-package phia (version 0.2–0) (De Rosario-Martinez, 2015) and applying the Bonferroni–Holm correction for multiple comparisons.
TMS effects are best understood when comparing each active TMS condition with the corresponding (right or left) sham stimulation. An advantage of having two sham conditions, as opposed to only one, is to subtract out any potential nonspecific TMS effect, such as the lateralized click of the coil associated with the TMS stimulation. Indeed, previous studies demonstrated that the clicking sound of the TMS pulse induces a shift of covert spatial attention to the corresponding side of space, thus facilitating target detection ipsilateral to the stimulation (Duecker and Sack, 2013). In addition to the RT analysis, we conducted an accuracy analysis on all conditions (Table 1). However, since no significant effect of TMS emerged from the accuracy analysis, we will not report those results, except for the sham conditions (see below). Finally, for significant effects, effect size was computed as Cohen's f2 (Selya et al., 2012), which uses residual variance from the model to estimate effect size. Since mixed-effect models have two different types of R2 (variance explained), the marginal R2, which represents the variance explained by the fixed effects, and the conditional R2, which represents the variance explained by the entire model, whenever both fixed and random effects were included in the model, two different f2 values were calculated for each effect, namely, the f2m (marginal) and the f2c (conditional). Nonetheless, for multilevel models, effect sizes calculated using residual variance and proportion of explained variance should be interpreted with caution because the addition of variables to the model may increase residual variance, resulting in negative estimates of explained variance and even of effect size (Snijders and Bosker, 1994).
Table 1.
Left hemisphere |
Right hemisphere |
|||
---|---|---|---|---|
Distractor present | Distractor absent | Distractor present | Distractor absent | |
Sham | 91.8% (6.8) | 96.4% (3.8) | 91.5% (8.0) | 96.1% (5.1) |
FEF | 91.6% (6.6) | 96.6% (4.3) | 92.3% (7.5) | 97.5% (2.8) |
IPS | 91.2% (7.5) | 96.8% (4.3) | 92.4% (6.4) | 97.2% (2.9) |
aData are mean (SD).
Results
Behavioral effects
To ensure that the adopted paradigm was suitable for the given purposes, we first tested the interfering effect of distracting visual stimuli on task performance in the absence of active TMS stimulation (i.e., restricting the analysis to the sham condition). The brain hemisphere (left vs right), distractor presence (present vs absent), and their interaction were entered as fixed-effect factors in a linear mixed model that predicted log-transformed RTs on correct-response trials. Intercepts and slope for the interaction between brain hemisphere and distractor presence were also included in the model as random-effect factors across participants. This analysis revealed a significant main effect of distractor presence (F(1,28.85) = 161.32, p < 0.001; f2m = 0.071, f2c = 0.14), reflecting faster responses in the distractor-absent condition (mean 361 ms) compared with the distractor-present condition (mean 456 ms). The main effect of brain hemisphere was not significant (F(1,28.51) < 1, p = 0.88). Importantly, the interaction between distractor presence and brain hemisphere was also nonsignificant (F(1,85.98) < 1, p = 0.40), indicating that the effect of distractor did not differ significantly between the two control sham conditions (Fig. 2A). Similarly, a mixed logistic model was estimated using accuracy as the dependent variable. This analysis (Fig. 2B) revealed a significant main effect of distractor presence (χ2(1) = 34.37, p < 0.001) and no other significant effects (main effect of brain hemisphere: χ2(1) < 1, p = 0.54; interaction distractor presence by brain hemisphere: χ2(1) < 1, p = 0.94). Contrast analysis showed that, when a distractor was present, the participants' accuracy rates were lower (94%) compared with the no-distractor condition (98%, z = −4.75, p < 0.001) (Fig. 2B).
As a second step, we evaluated whether, and to what degree, experiencing a distractor in the previous trial modulated distractor interference on the current trial. Indeed, behavioral research has demonstrated larger distractor costs on a given trial N when no distractor was presented (vs when it was presented) on the previous trial N-1 (Geyer et al., 2008; Müller et al., 2010). For this analysis, trials following incorrect-response trials were omitted, as well as incorrect-response trials (8.15% of total trials). Log-transformed RTs were analyzed with a linear mixed model that included brain hemisphere (left vs right), distractor presence (present vs absent), and type of previous trial (distractor-present vs distractor-absent) as fixed-effect factors, and random intercepts and slope for factors brain hemisphere, distractor presence, and type of previous trial within participants. The analysis revealed a significant main effect of distractor presence (F(1,28.7) = 152.07, p < 0.001; f2m = 0.073, f2c = 0.14) and a quasi-significant effect of type of previous trial (F(1,73.8) = 3.81, p = 0.055). Importantly, and as predicted, the analysis also showed a significant interaction between distractor presence and type of previous trial (F(1,7390.2) = 9.37, p = 0.002; f2m = 0.0005, f2c = 0.001). This interaction emerged because the distractor cost was larger when a distractor was absent in the previous trial (mean 103 ms) compared with when it was present (mean 87 ms) (Fig. 2C). Importantly, this effect was not significantly modulated by brain hemisphere, as indicated by a nonsignificant three-way interaction between distractor presence, type of previous trial, and brain hemisphere (F(1,7384.9) < 1, p = 0.50). All of the other effects or interactions were not significant: the main effect of brain hemisphere (F(1,28) < 1, p = 0.82); the interaction brain hemisphere by distractor presence (F(1,7377.3) < 1, p = 0.66); and the interaction brain hemisphere by type of previous trial (F(1,7383.4) < 1, p = 0.69). Finally, a mixed-effect logistic model was estimated using accuracy as the dependent variable. This analysis revealed a significant main effect of distractor (χ2(1) = 35.29, p < 0.001) as before. The interaction between distractor presence and type of previous trial was nonsignificant (χ2(1) = 1.52, p = 0.21). Contrast analysis showed that the cost of distractor did not differ reliably depending on the absence versus presence of a distractor in the previous trial (cost of distraction = 4.9% and 2.8%, respectively; z = 1.62, p = 0.10; Fig. 2D). No other main effects or interactions were significant: the main effect of brain hemisphere (χ2(1) < 1, p = 0.70); the interaction distractor presence by brain hemisphere (χ2(1) < 1, p = 0.78); the interaction brain hemisphere by type of previous trial (χ2(1) < 1, p = 0.64); and the three-way interaction distractor presence by type of previous trial by brain hemisphere (χ2(1) = 1.12, p = 0.29).
Effect of TMS on visual search
We tested the effect of TMS on distractor filtering mechanisms using a linear mixed model that predicted log-transformed RTs on correct-response trials. The experimental factors TMS (sham vs FEF vs IPS), distractor presence (present vs absent), brain hemisphere (left vs right), and all their interactions were included as fixed effects. Random coefficients across participants were estimated for intercept and for factors TMS, distractor presence, and brain hemisphere. The omnibus analysis revealed a significant main effect of distractor presence (F(1,28.9) = 188.65, p < 0.001; f2m = 0.065, f2c = 0.13), indicating that participants were overall faster in the distractor-absent condition (mean 361 ms) compared with the distractor-present condition (mean 451 ms), with an average distractor cost of 90 ms. Crucially, the three-way interaction TMS by brain hemisphere by distractor presence was significant (F(2,24116.7) = 4.20, p = 0.01; f2m = 0.0002, f2c = 0.0004) (Table 2). Contrast analysis showed that the effect of the right FEF stimulation (i.e., difference in distractor cost between right sham and right FEF TMS conditions) was significantly different from the effect of the left FEF stimulation (i.e., difference in distractor cost between left sham vs left FEF TMS conditions; t = 2.883, p = 0.003). This reflected reduced distractor cost following right, but not left, FEF stimulation. The distractor cost associated with IPS stimulation did not significantly differ between brain hemispheres (t = 1.168, p = 0.24) as well as the difference between IPS and FEF TMS stimulation between the left and the right hemisphere (t = 1.718, p = 0.09) (Fig. 3A).
Table 2.
Numerator DF | Denominator DF | F | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Distractor presence | 1 | 28.9 | 188.6512 | <0.001*** |
TMS | 2 | 29.1 | <1 | 0.62 |
Brain hemisphere | 1 | 28.9 | 1.5934 | 0.22 |
Distractor presence × TMS | 2 | 24122.5 | 2.8440 | 0.06 |
Distractor presence × brain hemisphere | 1 | 24123.0 | 2.8074 | 0.09 |
TMS × brain hemisphere | 2 | 24117.3 | 8.0674 | <0.001*** |
Distractor presence × TMS × brain hemisphere | 2 | 24116.7 | 4.2069 | 0.01* |
*p < 0.05;
***p < 0.001.
To further explore the significant three-way interaction, we performed follow-up analyses separately for each hemisphere. The left hemisphere analysis revealed a nonsignificant interaction TMS by distractor presence (F(2,53.75) < 1, p = 0.85). On the contrary, the right hemisphere analysis indicated that the interaction TMS by distractor presence was significant (F(2,34.89) = 4.58, p = 0.017; f2m = 0.0005, f2c = 0.0009) (Table 3). Contrast analysis confirmed that the interaction effect reflected a significant decrease in the distractor cost with FEF versus Sham stimulation (the average distractor cost decreased by 23 ms; t = 3.023, p = 0.004). The distractor cost did not significantly differ either for the sham versus IPS contrast (t = 1.369, p = 0.18) or for the IPS versus FEF contrast (t = 1.491, p = 0.14) (Fig. 3A).
Table 3.
Numerator DF | Denominator DF | F | p | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Right hemisphere | Distractor presence | 1 | 28.803 | 218.0195 | <0.001*** |
TMS | 2 | 28.901 | 2.4004 | 0.10 | |
Distractor presence × TMS | 2 | 34.893 | 4.5807 | 0.017* | |
Left hemisphere | Distractor presence | 1 | 28.924 | 150.3842 | <0.001*** |
TMS | 2 | 29.058 | <1 | 0.84 | |
Distractor presence × TMS | 2 | 53.750 | <1 | 0.85 |
*p < 0.05;
***p < 0.001.
To further investigate whether the reduction in the distractor cost following right FEF stimulation was due to a relative RT increase in the distractor-absent condition, to a relative RT decrease in the distractor-present condition, or to a combination of both, we conducted post hoc comparisons. These revealed that FEF stimulation reliably reduced RTs compared with sham selectively in the distractor-present condition (χ2(1) = 8.01, p = 0.027). The contrast between FEF and IPS stimulation in the distractor-present condition revealed a slight trend toward significance (χ2(1) = 5.79, p = 0.080). All other comparisons were far from significant (all p values >0.56; Fig. 3B,C).
An important question to ask is whether any effects of TMS on the distractor cost depend critically on the spatial position of the distractor and/or of the target in the given search display. Therefore, we analyzed RTs in the distractor-present condition as a function of whether the target and distractor appeared in the right or left visual field, that is, ipsilateral or contralateral to the stimulation (only right TMS conditions were considered in this analysis). We implemented a linear mixed model with the experimental factors TMS (sham vs FEF vs IPS), target side (right vs left), and distractor side (right vs left) as fixed effects. The random effect structure included the random intercept for subject, as well as the by-subject random slope for TMS, target side, and distractor side. The analysis indicated a significant interaction between target side and distractor side (F(1,5821) = 32.39, p < 0.001), reflecting longer RTs when target and distractor were on the same side (mean 462 ms) compared with opposite sides (mean 440 ms), presumably due to greater competition in the former condition relative to the latter. Crucially, TMS did not interact with either target side (F(2,5819.9) < 1, p = 0.99) or distractor side (F(2,5823.1) < 1, p = 0.48), or their interaction (F(2,5821.5) < 1, p = 0.39), indicating that the reduction in the distractor cost associated with right FEF stimulation occurred regardless of the visual field wherein target and distractor stimuli were respectively presented.
An additional analysis was performed to assess whether the chosen coordinates for the FEF stimulation site were optimal with respect to the putative cortical location of FEF at the level of individual participants. Here, coordinates for the FEF site were derived from the existing literature (Zhou and Shu, 2017). However, it is also known that the distance between FEF and M1 is ∼2 cm in humans (Müri et al., 1991; Ro et al., 1999). Because the behavioral effects of TMS over right (and left) FEF varied considerably across participants, and so did the scalp distance between M1 and the stimulated FEF site, we hypothesized that the strongest behavioral effects of FEF stimulation might have occurred in those participants for whom the M1-FEF distance was around the expected, anatomically plausible value of 2 cm (due to more precise FEF targeting in those participants). To explore this possibility, we fitted a second-order polynomial function predicting the magnitude of the effect reported in the previous analysis (Δ; i.e., the RT difference between FEF and sham stimulation in the distractor-present conditions) from the distance between FEF and M1 (d) and on its square (i.e., Δ = β0 + β1d + β2d2 + ε). The squared term was included because we expected the distribution of the actual individual FEF-M1 distances to include both smaller and larger values with respect to the putative optimal distance (Müri et al., 1991; Ro et al., 1999), such that the expected relationship between those values and the strength of the measured behavioral effects might not be accommodated for by a linear trend only. Interestingly, not only the analysis revealed a significant linear relationship (β1 = −0.42; p = 0.01), indicating that the effect linearly decreased with the distance, but also a significant quadratic effect (β2 = 0.74; p = 0.006). These results imply that the effect of TMS was the strongest when the distance between the putative right FEF stimulation site and the right M1 site was ∼2.77 cm and decreased for both shorter and longer distances. This observation indicates that, when the putative FEF site, localized by means of neuro-navigation, was located anteriorly within a certain anatomical distance from M1 (i.e., ∼2–3 cm), the behavioral effects of right FEF stimulation on distractor suppression were strongest, possibly indicating a more precise targeting of the actual FEF location. This finding suggests that the localization of FEF relative to its distance from M1 (2–3 cm anterior) might provide a better strategy to target this brain area in future studies. As a control, we performed the same analysis on the left hemisphere. Therefore, we fitted an analogous second-order polynomial function predicting the magnitude of the effect (i.e., the difference in RT between FEF and sham in the distractor-present conditions) from the distance between left FEF and M1. This analysis did not reveal any significant effect (β1 = −0.13; p = 0.42; β2 = 0.26; p = 0.34) (Fig. 3D).
Finally, given the significant impact of right FEF stimulation on distractor suppression, we were also interested in testing whether these effects interacted with the recent trial history. To this aim, log-transformed RTs were analyzed with a mixed linear model that included TMS (sham vs FEF), distractor presence (present vs absent), and type of previous trial (distractor-present vs distractor-absent) as fixed-effect factors and intercept, TMS, distractor presence, and type of previous trial as random-effect coefficients across participants. As noted above, for this specific analysis, trials following incorrect-response trials were removed in addition to incorrect-response trials. This analysis revealed a significant main effect of distractor presence (F(1,28.5) = 189.92, p < 0.001; f2m = 0.060, f2c = 0.12). As expected, the analysis confirmed that TMS modulated distractor filtering by unveiling a significant interaction between TMS and distractor presence (F(1,7394.0) = 13.07, p < 0.001; f2m = 0.0008, f2c = 0.001). The interaction between distractor presence by type of previous trial revealed a trend toward significance (F(1,7398.9) = 3.04, p = 0.081). Interestingly, the three-way interaction TMS by distractor presence by type of previous trial was nearly significant (F(1,7403.6) = 3.55, p = 0.059; f2m = 0.001, f2c = 0.001). To further explore this quasi-significant interaction, we conducted separate analyses based on the type of previous trial (distractor-absent or distractor-present). When a distractor was present in the previous trial, no significant effect of FEF TMS on the distractor cost was found, TMS by distractor presence (F(1,3602.3) = 1.71, p = 0.19). Conversely, when a distractor was absent in the previous trial, TMS significantly modulated the cost of distraction (F(1,3743.2) = 15.05, p < 0.001; f2m = 0.001, f2c = 0.003) (Fig. 4). All of the other effects or interactions did not reach significance (all p values > 0.081). As a control, a similar analysis was also performed comparing sham versus right IPS stimulation. This analysis indicated a main effect of distractor presence (F(1,28.6) = 196.58, p < 0.001; f2m = 0.069, f2c = 0.13) and a significant interaction distractor presence by type of previous trial (F(1,7433.2) = 7.19, p = 0.007; f2m = 0.0004, f2c = 0.0008).
For the right IPS stimulation condition, a nonsignificant effect was found for the interaction TMS by type of previous trial (F(1,7432.4) < 1, p = 0.39), albeit a marginally significant effect was obtained for the interaction TMS by distractor presence (F(1,7424.1) = 3.10, p = 0.07). The three-way interaction TMS by distractor presence by type of previous trial was nonsignificant (F(1,7435.9) < 1, p = 0.32). All of the other effects or interactions did not reach the significance level (all p values >0.08).
Discussion
This study sought to ascertain the causal role of two key regions of the dorsal attention network, FEF and IPS, in modulating attentional capture elicited by salient distractor stimuli. Results show that TMS stimulation of the right FEF significantly reduced the behavioral interference caused by a salient singleton distractor during a visual search task. Crucially, this was reliably different from what observed following left FEF stimulation, which had no measurable effect on attentional capture compared with a suitable sham condition. The magnitude of the reduction in distractor interference associated with right FEF stimulation correlated with the anatomical distance between the putative FEF stimulation site and M1 at the single-subject level and peaked in participants for whom such distance was between 2 and 3 cm, compatible with functional localization evidence (Ro et al., 1999). Interestingly, an additional analysis attributed reduction in distractor costs following right FEF stimulation to performance improvements occurring in the distractor-present condition, which attests to the specificity of our results. Perhaps even more interestingly, further analyses indicated that this lessening of distractor costs interacted with modulations of distractor interference due to intertrial contingencies. In particular, the reduction of distractor interference observed during right FEF stimulation was maximized when the previous trial was a distractor-absent versus distractor-present trial. Together, these results attest to the pivotal role of the right FEF in both limiting attentional capture by salient distractors and modulating history-contingent distractor interference.
Our main result reveals a causal role of right FEF in online distractor filtering. This result aligns nicely with the general idea that the human PFC is responsible for controlling and filtering task-irrelevant information (Shimamura, 2000; Kane and Engle, 2002; de Fockert et al., 2004; de Fockert and Theeuwes, 2012; Geng, 2014; Marini et al., 2016). A recent brain stimulation study showed that tDCS stimulation over bilateral PFC led to a decrease in attentional capture in the additional singleton task (Cosman et al., 2015). Additional evidence that prefrontal regions directly drive the efficient filtering of irrelevant information comes from ERP studies, which identified a frontal ERP component presumably related to distractor suppression (Vogel et al., 2005; Liesefeld et al., 2014; Vissers et al., 2016) (the frontal bias signal). Several fMRI studies have also provided converging evidence on the role of PFC in filtering out irrelevant stimuli. For example, brain activity levels in the PFC correlated with the magnitude of the interfering effects engendered by salient distractors as measured behaviorally (de Fockert et al., 2004; Leber, 2010; de Fockert and Theeuwes, 2012). More relevant for the interpretation of the current results, a brain-behavior relationship has been established between activity levels in the right PFC (right inferior frontal gyrus [rIFG] and right middle frontal gyrus [rMFG]) and behavioral indexes of distractor suppression (Weissman et al., 2006; Demeter et al., 2011; Marini et al., 2016). The distinctive role of a right-lateralized network in attentional control has also been confirmed by neuropsychological evidence, pointing to the rMFG as a crucial node for regulating both top-down and bottom-up attention (see Japee et al., 2015). In the present study, the delivery of repetitive TMS immediately after target (and distractor) presentation may have modulated ongoing activity by increasing neuronal excitability within a network of right-lateralized prefrontal attentional control regions (including rIFG, rMFG, and right FEF). This may have rendered the network prompter to process upcoming visual stimuli, in turn resulting in an optimal instantiation of distraction-filtering mechanisms in the presence of a salient distractor. The existence of strong functional interactions between the FEF, which is part of the dorsal attention network, and the rMFG and rIFG, which are part of the ventral attention network, is supported by several resting-state connectivity studies (Fox et al., 2006; He et al., 2007; Shulman et al., 2009; Asplund et al., 2010). Crucially, DiQuattro and colleagues (DiQuattro and Geng, 2011; DiQuattro et al., 2014) reported that an excitatory pathway from the temporoparietal junction to IFG to FEF contributed to the optimal suppression of irrelevant yet salient distractors. The interpretation of the current results in terms of a facilitating role of TMS in the instantiation of distractor filtering mechanisms fits perfectly with very recent findings in the macaque, demonstrating a shared neural substrate for target selection and distractor suppression in FEF neuronal populations (Cosman et al., 2018). More specifically, this study demonstrated that signatures of (target selection and) salient-distractor filtering in FEF neurons preceded by ∼50 ms posterior ERP signatures of the same process, likely reflecting proactive suppression of the salient distracting stimulus before it can affect neural selection and eventually capture attention (Sawaki and Luck, 2010; see also Gaspelin et al., 2015; Gaspelin and Luck, 2018). In this framework, it is possible that ERP markers of attentional suppression reported in human participants (Liesefeld et al., 2014, 2017) reflect the successful control against distraction, which is implemented by PFC circuitry (and, in particular, right FEF). This would support the general notion that prefrontal-extrastriate projections are responsible for both enhancing task-relevant information and suppressing irrelevant, and especially distracting, information (Serences et al., 2004; Ruff and Driver, 2006; Gazzaley et al., 2007; Cosman et al., 2018).
Our results are compatible with the idea that TMS facilitated the instantiation of distractor suppression. An alternative interpretation is that the right FEF stimulation may have disrupted the attentional bias toward the salient visual feature (the odd color) because this might also lead to reduced capture by the salient distractor. Indeed, it is well known that both FEF and IPS are involved in attentional capture and salience-dependent computations (Corbetta and Shulman, 2002; Serences et al., 2005). Electrophysiological studies with nonhuman primates have consistently demonstrated that visual saliency maps are represented in multiple frontoparietal regions, including LIP (Gottlieb et al., 1998) and FEF (Schall and Hanes, 1993), as well as in subcortical structures, notably the superior colliculus (see White et al., 2017). In the present experiment, TMS may have interfered with attentional mechanisms that are responsible for biasing attention toward salient items. This, in turn, may have weakened attentional capture and reduced the RT cost engendered by salient distractors. In principle, a similar argument could be made for IPS stimulation, which, however, did not significantly modulate the behavioral cost produced by salient distractors. Possibly, the contribution of FEF to attentional biasing toward salient stimuli may be either stronger or more susceptible to TMS-induced modulations than that of IPS. Incidentally, the early involvement of prefrontal regions in bottom-up attention, as shown by research on nonhuman primates, suggests that the identification of salient visual stimuli in parietal and frontal regions may proceed in parallel rather than serially (Thompson et al., 1996; Thomas and Paré, 2007; Katsuki and Constantinidis, 2012). Although the occurrence of a TMS-induced deficit in saliency computation may seem compatible with the available evidence, for at least two reasons, we consider this possibility less likely than our previous interpretation in terms of distraction-filtering mechanisms. First, if saliency computation were compromised by the right FEF TMS stimulation, then we might expect a performance cost in the distractor-absent condition, too, because saliency computation should have also supported target selection, in addition to attentional capture by the singleton distractor. However, our data indicated no TMS-induced modulation of performance in the distractor-absent condition. Second, an interpretation in terms of saliency computation does not seem to provide an obvious account for the effects of TMS on modulations of performance related to cross-trial contingencies (see below). For these reasons, we favor an interpretation whereby right FEF TMS modulated mechanisms responsible for the effective filtering of salient distractors.
The negative results we found for the IPS stimulation may appear at odds with those reported by Hodsoll et al. (2009), as they described a significant reduction in the distractor cost following right PPC TMS. However, crucial differences in the TMS protocol may account for the discrepant results. Hodsoll et al. (2009) used an offline, 1 Hz TMS approach, known to give rise to prolonged cortical inhibition at the site of stimulation (Chen et al., 1997; Oliveri et al., 2005). Instead, our time-locked, 10 Hz stimulation protocol may have been suboptimal for inducing clear-cut behavioral changes following IPS stimulation. In line with this interpretation, previous ERP studies demonstrated a sequential involvement of FEF and IPS during distractor-filtering tasks, with frontal signals preceding those detected over parietal areas (Grent-'t-Jong and Woldorff, 2007; Leblanc et al., 2008; Brignani et al., 2009; Ptak et al., 2011; Shomstein et al., 2012; Liesefeld et al., 2014). Therefore, we cannot exclude the possibility that the early stimulation (between 100 and 300 ms after display onset) applied in the current study was adequate for affecting FEF but not IPS activity, and that a later or more prolonged stimulation may reveal the involvement of IPS in the current behavioral context. These remain open possibilities for future studies to explore.
The present results are also compatible with the proposed general role of the frontoparietal attention network in modulating the responsiveness of visual cortical regions (Grosbras and Paus, 2003; Moore and Armstrong, 2003; Silvanto et al., 2006, 2009; Bisley and Goldberg, 2010; Noudoost et al., 2010; Baluch and Itti, 2011; Buschman and Kastner, 2015; Scolari et al., 2015; Marini et al., 2016). The available evidence suggests that suppression mechanisms may be implemented through prefrontal-driven modulations of sensory processing (Serences et al., 2004; Gazzaley et al., 2007; Seidl et al., 2012), similarly to sensory enhancements of target features (Reynolds and Chelazzi, 2004; Chelazzi et al., 2011). Moreover, recent findings both in humans (Michalareas et al., 2016; Wang et al., 2016; Popov et al., 2017) and nonhuman primates (van Kerkoerle et al., 2014; Bastos et al., 2015) suggest that FEF may exert top-down control by modulating visual oscillatory alpha-band activity in sensory areas. Recently, Popov et al. (2017) used MEG to show that right FEF, but not left FEF, exerts top-down control on stimulus processing in visual cortex. This FEF hemispheric asymmetry fits well with our current results and is complemented by the typical finding that TMS modulates visual task performance in both (left and right) hemifields when applied over the right FEF, and in the right hemifield only when applied over the left FEF (Grosbras and Paus, 2003; Smith et al., 2005; Silvanto et al., 2006). In general, the present results concur with the large body of evidence suggesting a right hemispheric dominance for the control of visuo-spatial attention in the FEF (Grosbras and Paus, 2003; Silvanto et al., 2006; Capotosto et al., 2009; Duecker et al., 2013; Marshall et al., 2015; Wang et al., 2016).
Interestingly, the beneficial effects of right FEF stimulation on distractor suppression seem to interact with modulations of distractor costs that are related to the presence versus absence of a distractor in the preceding trial, suggesting a role of the right frontal attention network in the intertrial, history-dependent regulation of distraction-filtering mechanisms. More specifically, in the sham condition, the interfering effect of the distractor was greater on trials following a distractor-absent trial compared with trials following a distractor-present trial, an observation that has been reported previously (Geyer et al., 2008). The TMS stimulation of right FEF eliminated the relative disadvantage of having experienced a distractor-absent condition in the previous trial, which could be alternatively interpreted as if TMS mimicked the advantage of having encountered a distractor-present condition. We have already suggested that the effect of TMS on neuronal excitability might have consisted of an increase of activity in neuronal populations implementing distractor-filtering mechanisms in the current trial. In addition, TMS may have also facilitated the sustained maintenance of distraction-filtering mechanisms through the subsequent trial, hence resulting in a reduction of the distractor cost. This idea can be accommodated in the framework of the interactions between the conflict monitoring system (Botvinick et al., 2001) and the frontoparietal attention network. According to this framework, when response conflict occurs, the ACC signals an increased demand for cognitive control, which leads to an enhancement of top-down control mediated by the dorsal frontoparietal network (FEF and IPS) (see Walsh et al., 2011) on the subsequent trial (Carter et al., 2000; Casey et al., 2000; Botvinick et al., 2001). From an anatomical and physiological perspective, this is supported by the existence of direct axonal projections between ACC and both FEF (Huerta et al., 1987; Stanton et al., 1993) and PPC (Pandya et al., 1981; Hampson et al., 2006). Perhaps similarly to response conflict, also the presence of a salient distractor may trigger the ACC reactively (see Seeley et al., 2007) and then engage greater attentional control proactively in preparation for the subsequent trial, thus leading to reduced distractor costs. Here, magnetic stimulation may have primed the attention control network in a way that mimicked the proactive adjustments occurring spontaneously after distractor-present trials. This, in turn, would have resulted in a reduced distractor cost in the subsequent trial. The idea that right FEF TMS may have promoted proactive trial-to-trial control finds support in a study by Leber (2010), showing that stronger pretrial activity in prefrontal areas was associated with reduced distractor interference by a salient irrelevant distractor.
As a cautionary note, the design of our experiment is not optimally suited to draw firm conclusions concerning the effect of TMS on intertrial modulations. Indeed, by applying TMS pulses on every trial, we cannot cleanly disentangle whether the reported effects were due to an influence of TMS on the current trial (N) or instead on the previous trial (N-1). Regardless of this limitation, our results demonstrated that following right FEF stimulation the relative disadvantage of having experienced a distractor-absent condition in the previous trial was completely eliminated. This finding can be interpreted in two alternative ways: On the one hand, TMS on the current trial N may have strengthened reactive (online) mechanisms to deal with distraction, those very mechanisms that are especially important when the system has not been alerted by a distractor on the preceding trial. Alternatively, TMS on the previous (distractor-absent) trial may have proactively primed the attention control network, mimicking the modulation occurring spontaneously on distractor-present trials. Future and more sensitive TMS experimental designs will help clarify this distinction.
In conclusion, with a systematic approach, the present findings indicate a causal role of the right FEF in adjusting attentional filtering mechanisms. We propose that the TMS intervention engaged the cortical network that controls and regulates mechanisms for limiting interference from irrelevant, attention-capturing distractor stimuli. More specifically, TMS may have shielded the network from the impact of potential distractor interference, both by facilitating online distractor filtering mechanisms and by modulating intertrial adjustments. Moreover, our results suggest that not only right FEF is directly involved in the reactive control mechanisms that deal with distracting stimuli after their appearance, but it also mediates attentional control mechanisms that are modulated on a trial-by-trial basis. Future research will have to elucidate the contribution to distraction filtering of other cortical regions, notably ventral attention network regions, such as IFG/MFG in the frontal lobe and temporoparietal junction in the parietal lobe, and will clarify whether the role of ventral regions is similar or distinguishable from the role of the dorsal attention network characterized here.
Footnotes
This work was supported by University of Verona Ricerca di Base 2015 (Grant B32F15000700001) to L. Chelazzi.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
References
- Asplund CL, Todd JJ, Snyder AP, Marois R (2010) A central role for the lateral prefrontal cortex in goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention. Nat Neurosci 13:507–512. 10.1038/nn.2509 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Awiszus F. (2003) TMS and threshold hunting. Suppl Clin Neurophysiol 56:13–23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baayen RH, Davidson DJ, Bates DM (2008) Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. J Mem Lang 59:390–412. 10.1016/j.jml.2007.12.005 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Baluch F, Itti L (2011) Mechanisms of top-down attention. Trends Neurosci 34:210–224. 10.1016/j.tins.2011.02.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barr DJ, Levy R, Scheepers C, Tily HJ (2013) Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: keep it maximal. J Mem Lang 68:255–278. 10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bastos AM, Vezoli J, Bosman CA, Schoffelen JM, Oostenveld R, Dowdall JR, De Weerd P, Kennedy H, Fries P (2015) Visual areas exert feedforward and feedback influences through distinct frequency channels. Neuron 85:390–401. 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.12.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B, Walker S (2014) lme4: linear mixed-effect models using Eigen and S4 (R package version 1.1–7). Available at http://cran.r-project.org/package=lme4.
- Bisley JW, Goldberg ME (2010) Attention, intention, and priority in the parietal lobe. Annu Rev Neurosci 33:1–21. 10.1146/annurev-neuro-060909-152823 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Botvinick MM, Braver TS, Barch DM, Carter CS, Cohen JD (2001) Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychol Rev 108:624–652. 10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.624 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brignani D, Guzzon D, Marzi CA, Miniussi C (2009) Attentional orienting induced by arrows and eye-gaze compared with an endogenous cue. Neuropsychologia 47:370–381. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.09.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buschman TJ, Kastner S (2015) From behavior to neural dynamics: an integrated theory of attention. Neuron 88:127–144. 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Capotosto P, Babiloni C, Romani GL, Corbetta M (2009) Frontoparietal cortex controls spatial attention through modulation of anticipatory alpha rhythms. J Neurosci 29:5863–5872. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0539-09.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carter CS, Macdonald AM, Botvinick M, Ross LL, Stenger VA, Noll D, Cohen JD (2000) Parsing executive processes: strategic vs. evaluative functions of the anterior cingulate cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 97:1944–1948. 10.1073/pnas.97.4.1944 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Casey BJ, Thomas KM, Welsh TF, Badgaiyan RD, Eccard CH, Jennings JR, Crone EA (2000) Dissociation of response conflict, attentional selection, and expectancy with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 97:8728–8733. 10.1073/pnas.97.15.8728 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chelazzi L, Della Libera C, Sani I, Santandrea E (2011) Neural basis of visual selective attention. Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci 2:392–407. 10.1002/wcs.117 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chelazzi L, Marini F, Pascucci D, Turatto M (2019) Getting rid of visual distractors: the why, when, how, and where. Curr Opin Psychol 29:135–147. 10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.02.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen R, Classen J, Gerloff C, Celnik P, Wassermann EM, Hallett M, Cohen LG (1997) Depression of motor cortex excitability by low-frequency transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neurology 48:1398–1403. 10.1212/WNL.48.5.1398 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen Kadosh RC, Muggleton N, Silvanto J, Walsh V (2010) Double dissociation of format-dependent and number-specific neurons in human parietal cortex. Cereb Cortex 20:2166–2171. 10.1093/cercor/bhp273 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corbetta M, Shulman GL (2002) Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 3:201–215. 10.1038/nrn755 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cosman JD, Atreya PV, Woodman GF (2015) Transient reduction of visual distraction following electrical stimulation of the prefrontal cortex. Cognition 145:73–76. 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.08.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cosman JD, Lowe KA, Zinke W, Woodman GF, Schall JD (2018) Prefrontal control of visual distraction. Curr Biol 28:414–420.e3. 10.1016/j.cub.2017.12.023 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Rosario-Martinez (2015) phia: post-hoc interaction analysis. https://cran.r-project.org/package=phia.
- de Fockert JW, Theeuwes J (2012) Role of frontal cortex in attentional capture by singleton distractors. Brain Cogn 80:367–373. 10.1016/j.bandc.2012.07.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Fockert J, Rees G, Frith C, Lavie N (2004) Neural correlates of attentional capture in visual search. J Cogn Neurosci 16:751–759. 10.1162/089892904970762 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Demeter E, Hernandez-Garcia L, Sarter M, Lustig C (2011) Challenges to attention: a continuous arterial spin labeling (ASL) study of the effects of distraction on sustained attention. Neuroimage 54:1518–1529. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.026 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Di Caro V, Theeuwes J, Della Libera C (2019) Suppression history of spatial locations biases attentional and oculomotor control. J Vis 18:477. [Google Scholar]
- DiQuattro NE, Geng JJ (2011) Contextual knowledge configures attentional control networks. J Neurosci 31:18026–18035. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4040-11.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DiQuattro NE, Sawaki R, Geng JJ (2014) Effective connectivity during feature-based attentional capture: evidence against the attentional reorienting hypothesis of TPJ. Cereb Cortex 24:3131–3141. 10.1093/cercor/bht172 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Donohue SE, Bartsch MV, Heinze HJ, Schoenfeld MA, Hopf JM (2018) Cortical mechanisms of prioritizing selection for rejection in visual search. J Neurosci 38:4738–4748. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2407-17.2018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duecker F, Sack AT (2013) Pre-stimulus sham TMS facilitates target detection. PLoS One 8:e57765. 10.1371/journal.pone.0057765 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duecker F, Formisano E, Sack AT (2013) Hemispheric differences in the voluntary control of spatial attention: direct evidence for a right-hemispheric dominance within frontal cortex. J Cogn Neurosci 25:1332–1342. 10.1162/jocn_a_00402 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Egeth HE, Leonard CJ, Leber AB (2010) Why salience is not enough: reflections on top-down selection in vision. Acta Psychol (Amst) 135:130–132. 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.05.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferrante O, Patacca A, Di Caro V, Della Libera C, Santandrea E, Chelazzi L (2018) Altering spatial priority maps via statistical learning of target selection and distractor filtering. Cortex 102:67–95. 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.09.027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Folk CL, Remington R (2010) A critical evaluation of the disengagement hypothesis. Acta Psychol 135:103–105. 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.04.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Forster S, Lavie N (2008) Failures to ignore entirely irrelevant distractors: the role of load. J Exp Psychol Appl 14:73–83. 10.1037/1076-898X.14.1.73 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox MD, Corbetta M, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Raichle ME (2006) Spontaneous neuronal activity distinguishes human dorsal and ventral attention systems. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 103:10046–10051. 10.1073/pnas.0604187103 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gaspar JM, McDonald JJ (2014) Suppression of salient objects prevents distraction in visual search. J Neurosci 34:5658–5666. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4161-13.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gaspelin N, Luck SJ (2018) The role of inhibition in avoiding distraction by salient stimuli. Trends Cogn Sci 22:79–92. 10.1016/j.tics.2017.11.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gaspelin N, Leonard CJ, Luck SJ (2015) Direct evidence for active suppression of salient-but-irrelevant sensory inputs. Psychol Sci 26:1740–1750. 10.1177/0956797615597913 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gazzaley A, Rissman J, Cooney J, Rutman A, Seibert T, Clapp W, D'Esposito M (2007) Functional interactions between prefrontal and visual association cortex contribute to top-down modulation of visual processing. Cereb Cortex 17 [Suppl 1]:i125–i135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geng JJ. (2014) Attentional mechanisms of distractor suppression. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 23:147–153. 10.1177/0963721414525780 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Geyer T, Müller HJ, Krummenacher J (2008) Expectancies modulate attentional capture by salient color singletons. Vision Res 48:1315–1326. 10.1016/j.visres.2008.02.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goschy H, Bakos S, Müller HJ, Zehetleitner M (2014) Probability cueing of distractor locations: both intertrial facilitation and statistical learning mediate interference reduction. Front Psychol 5:1195. 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01195 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gottlieb JP, Kusunoki M, Goldberg ME (1998) The representation of visual salience in monkey parietal cortex. Nature 391:481–484. 10.1038/35135 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grent-'t-Jong T, Woldorff MG (2007) Timing and sequence of brain activity in top-down control of visual-spatial attention. PLoS Biol 5:e12. 10.1371/journal.pbio.0050012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grosbras MH, Paus T (2003) Transcranial magnetic stimulation of the human frontal eye field facilitates visual awareness. Eur J Neurosci 18:3121–3126. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2003.03055.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hampson M, Driesen NR, Skudlarski P, Gore JC, Constable RT (2006) Brain connectivity related to working memory performance. J Neurosci 26:13338–13343. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3408-06.2006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- He BJ, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Epstein A, Shulman GL, Corbetta M (2007) Breakdown of functional connectivity in frontoparietal networks underlies behavioral deficits in spatial neglect. Neuron 53:905–918. 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.02.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hodsoll J, Mevorach C, Humphreys GW (2009) Driven to less distraction: rTMS of the right parietal cortex reduces attentional capture in visual search. Cereb Cortex 19:106–114. 10.1093/cercor/bhn070 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huerta MF, Krubitzer LA, Kaas JH (1987) Frontal eye field as defined by intracortical microstimulation in squirrel monkeys, owl monkeys, and macaque monkeys: II. Cortical connections. J Comp Neurol 265:332–361. 10.1002/cne.902650304 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ipata AE, Gee AL, Gottlieb J, Bisley JW, Goldberg ME (2006) LIP responses to a popout stimulus are reduced if it is overtly ignored. Nat Neurosci 9:1071–1076. 10.1038/nn1734 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jannati A, Gaspar JM, McDonald JJ (2013) Tracking target and distractor processing in fixed-feature visual search: evidence from human electrophysiology. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 39:1713–1730. 10.1037/a0032251 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Japee S, Holiday K, Satyshur MD, Mukai I, Ungerleider LG (2015) A role of right middle frontal gyrus in reorienting of attention: a case study. Front Syst Neurosci 9:1–16. 10.3389/fnsys.2015.00023 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jonides J, Yantis S (1988) Uniqueness of abrupt visual onset in capturing attention. Percept Psychophys 43:346–354. 10.3758/BF03208805 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kajimura S, Nomura M (2015) Decreasing propensity to mind-wander with transcranial direct current stimulation. Neuropsychologia 75:533–537. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.07.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kane MJ, Engle RW (2002) The role of prefrontal cortex in working-memory capacity, executive attention, and general fluid intelligence: an individual differences perspective. Psychon Bull Rev 9:637–671. 10.3758/BF03196323 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Katsuki F, Constantinidis C (2012) Early involvement of prefrontal cortex in visual bottom-up attention. Nat Neurosci 15:1160–1166. 10.1038/nn.3164 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kerns JG, Cohen JD, MacDonald AW 3rd, Cho RY, Stenger VA, Carter CS (2004) Anterior cingulate conflict monitoring and adjustments in control. Science 303:1023–1026. 10.1126/science.1089910 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leber AB. (2010) Neural predictors of within-subject fluctuations in attentional control. J Neurosci 30:11458–11465. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0809-10.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leblanc E, Prime DJ, Jolicoeur P (2008) Tracking the location of visuospatial attention in a contingent capture paradigm. J Cogn Neurosci 20:657–671. 10.1162/jocn.2008.20051 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee J, Geng JJ (2017) Idiosyncratic patterns of representational similarity in prefrontal cortex predict attentional performance. J Neurosci 37:1257–1268. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1407-16.2016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liesefeld AM, Liesefeld HR, Zimmer HD (2014) Intercommunication between prefrontal and posterior brain regions for protecting visual working memory from distractor interference. Psychol Sci 25:325–333. 10.1177/0956797613501170 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liesefeld HR, Liesefeld AM, Töllner T, Müller HJ (2017) Attentional capture in visual search: capture and post-capture dynamics revealed by EEG. Neuroimage 156:166–173. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.05.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marini F, Chelazzi L, Maravita A (2013) The costly filtering of potential distraction: evidence for a supramodal mechanism. J Exp Psychol Gen 142:906–922. 10.1037/a0029905 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marini F, Demeter E, Roberts KC, Chelazzi L, Woldorff MG (2016) Orchestrating proactive and reactive mechanisms for filtering distracting information: brain-behavior relationships revealed by a mixed-design fMRI study. J Neurosci 36:988–1000. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2966-15.2016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marshall TR, O'Shea J, Jensen O, Bergmann TO (2015) Frontal eye fields control attentional modulation of alpha and gamma oscillations in contralateral occipitoparietal cortex. J Neurosci 35:1638–1647. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3116-14.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mathôt S, Schreij D, Theeuwes J (2012) OpenSesame: an open-source, graphical experiment builder for the social sciences. Behav Res Methods 44:314–324. 10.3758/s13428-011-0168-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McDonald JJ, Green JJ, Jannati A, Di Lollo V (2013) On the electrophysiological evidence for the capture of visual attention. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 39:849–860. 10.1037/a0030510 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Melloni L, van Leeuwen S, Alink A, Müller NG (2012) Interaction between bottom-up saliency and top-down control: how saliency maps are created in the human brain. Cereb Cortex 22:2943–2952. 10.1093/cercor/bhr384 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Michalareas G, Vezoli J, van Pelt S, Schoffelen JM, Kennedy H, Fries P (2016) Alpha-beta and gamma rhythms subserve feedback and feedforward influences among human visual cortical areas. Neuron 89:384–397. 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.12.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moore T, Armstrong KM (2003) Selective gating of visual signals by microstimulation of frontal cortex. Nature 421:370–373. 10.1038/nature01341 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Müller HJ, Töllner T, Zehetleitner M, Geyer T, Rangelov D, Krummenacher J (2010) Dimension-based attention modulates feed-forward visual processing. Acta Psychol (Amst) 135:117–122. 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.05.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Müller NG, Ebeling D (2008) Attention-modulated activity in visual cortex: more than a simple 'spotlight.' Neuroimage 40:818–827. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.11.060 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Müri RM, Hess CW, Meienberg O (1991) Transcranial stimulation of the human frontal eye field by magnetic pulses. Exp Brain Res 86:219–223. 10.1007/bf00231057 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neo G, Chua FK (2006) Capturing focused attention. Percept Psychophys 68:1286–1296. 10.3758/BF03193728 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noudoost B, Chang MH, Steinmetz NA, Moore T (2010) Top-down control of visual attention. Curr Opin Neurobiol 20:183–190. 10.1016/j.conb.2010.02.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oliveri M, Koch G, Torriero S, Caltagirone C (2005) Increased facilitation of the primary motor cortex following 1 Hz repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of the contralateral cerebellum in normal humans. Neurosci Lett 376:188–193. 10.1016/j.neulet.2004.11.053 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pandya DN, Van Hoesen GW, Mesulam MM (1981) Efferent connections of the cingulate gyrus in the rhesus monkey. Exp Brain Res 42:319–330. 10.1007/bf00237497 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pascual-Leone A, Walsh V, Rothwell J (2000) Transcranial magnetic stimulation in cognitive neuroscience-virtual lesion, chronometry, and functional connectivity. Curr Opin Neurobiol 10:232–237. 10.1016/S0959-4388(00)00081-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pascucci D, Turatto M (2015) The distracting impact of repeated visible and invisible onsets on focused attention. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 41:879–892. 10.1037/xhp0000025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Popov T, Kastner S, Jensen O (2017) FEF-controlled alpha delay activity precedes stimulus-induced gamma-band activity in visual cortex. J Neurosci 37:4117–4127. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3015-16.2017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ptak R, Camen C, Morand S, Schnider A (2011) Early event-related cortical activity originating in the frontal eye fields and inferior parietal lobe predicts the occurrence of correct and error saccades. Hum Brain Mapp 32:358–369. 10.1002/hbm.21025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- R Development Core Team (2016) R: a language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna. [Google Scholar]
- Reynolds JH, Chelazzi L (2004) Attentional modulation of visual processing. Annu Rev Neurosci 27:611–647. 10.1146/annurev.neuro.26.041002.131039 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rossi S, Hallett M, Rossini PM, Pascual-Leone A (2009) Safety, ethical considerations, and application guidelines for the use of transcranial magnetic stimulation in clinical practice and research. Clin Neurophysiol 120:2008–2039. 10.1016/j.clinph.2009.08.016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ro T, Cheifet S, Ingle H, Shoup R, Rafal R (1999) Localization of the human frontal eye fields and motor hand area with transcranial magnetic stimulation and magnetic resonance imaging. Neuropsychologia 37:225–231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruff CC, Driver J (2006) Attentional preparation for a lateralized visual distractor: behavioral and fMRI evidence. J Cogn Neurosci 18:522–538. 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.4.522 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saad E, Silvanto J (2013) How visual short-term memory maintenance modulates the encoding of external input: evidence from concurrent visual adaptation and TMS. Neuroimage 72:243–251. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.01.053 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sauter M, Liesefeld HR, Zehetleitner M, Müller HJ (2018) Region-based shielding of visual search from salient distractors: target detection is impaired with same, but not different-dimension distractors. Atten Percept Psychophys 80:622–642. 10.3758/s13414-017-1477-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sauter M, Liesefeld HR, Müller HJ (2019) Learning to suppress salient distractors in the target dimension: region-based inhibition is persistent and transfers to distractors in a nontarget dimension. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. Advance online publication. Retrieved January 28, 2019. doi: 10.1037/xlm0000691. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sawaki R, Luck SJ (2010) Capture versus suppression of attention by salient singletons: electrophysiological evidence for an automatic attend-to-me signal. Atten Percept Psychophys 72:1455–1470. 10.3758/APP.72.6.1455 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schall JD, Hanes DP (1993) Neural basis of saccade target selection in frontal eye field during visual search. Nature 366:467–469. 10.1038/366467a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scolari M, Seidl-Rathkopf KN, Kastner S (2015) Functions of the human frontoparietal attention network: evidence from neuroimaging. Curr Opin Behav Sci 1:32–39. 10.1016/j.cobeha.2014.08.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seeley WW, Menon V, Schatzberg AF, Keller J, Glover GH, Kenna H, Reiss AL, Greicius MD (2007) Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J Neurosci 27:2349–2356. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-06.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seidl KN, Peelen MV, Kastner S (2012) Neural evidence for distracter suppression during visual search in real-world scenes. J Neurosci 32:11812–11819. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1693-12.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Selya AS, Rose JS, Dierker LC, Hedeker D, Mermelstein RJ (2012) A practical guide to calculating Cohen's f2, a measure of local effect size, from PROC MIXED. Front Psychol 3:1–6. 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00111 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Serences JT, Yantis S, Culberson A, Awh E (2004) Preparatory activity in visual cortex indexes distractor suppression during covert spatial orienting. J Neurophysiol 92:3538–3545. 10.1152/jn.00435.2004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Serences JT, Shomstein S, Leber AB, Golay X, Egeth HE, Yantis S (2005) Coordination of voluntary and stimulus-driven attentional control in human cortex. Psychol Sci 16:114–122. 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00791.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shimamura AP. (2000) The role of the prefrontal cortex in dynamic filtering. Psychobiology 28:207–218. [Google Scholar]
- Shomstein S, Kravitz DJ, Behrmann M (2012) Attentional control: temporal relationships within the fronto-parietal network. Neuropsychologia 50:1202–1210. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.02.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shulman GL, Astafiev SV, Franke D, Pope DL, Snyder AZ, McAvoy MP, Corbetta M (2009) Interaction of stimulus-driven reorienting and expectation in ventral and dorsal frontoparietal and basal ganglia-cortical networks. J Neurosci 29:4392–4407. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5609-08.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silvanto J, Cattaneo Z (2017) Common framework for 'virtual lesion' and state-dependent TMS: the facilitatory/suppressive range model of online TMS effects on behavior. Brain Cogn 119:32–38. 10.1016/j.bandc.2017.09.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silvanto J, Pascual-Leone A (2008) State-dependency of transcranial magnetic stimulation. Brain Topogr 21:1–10. 10.1007/s10548-008-0067-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silvanto J, Lavie N, Walsh V (2006) Stimulation of the human frontal eye fields modulates sensitivity of extrastriate visual cortex. J Neurophysiol 96:941–945. 10.1152/jn.00015.2006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silvanto J, Muggleton N, Lavie N, Walsh V (2009) The perceptual and functional consequences of parietal top-down modulation on the visual cortex. Cereb Cortex 19:327–330. 10.1093/cercor/bhn091 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith DT, Jackson SR, Rorden C (2005) Transcranial magnetic stimulation of the left human frontal eye fields eliminates the cost of invalid endogenous cues. Neuropsychologia 43:1288–1296. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.12.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snijders TA, Bosker RJ (1994) Modeled variance in two-level models. Sociol Methods Res 22:342–363. 10.1177/0049124194022003004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Soutschek A, Taylor PC, Müller HJ, Schubert T (2013) Dissociable networks control conflict during perception and response selection: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. J Neurosci 33:5647–5654. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4768-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stanton GB, Bruce CJ, Goldberg ME (1993) Topography of projections to the frontal lobe from the macaque frontal eye fields. J Comp Neurol 330:286–301. 10.1002/cne.903300209 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suzuki M, Gottlieb J (2013) Distinct neural mechanisms of distractor suppression in the frontal and parietal lobe. Nat Neurosci 16:98–104. 10.1038/nn.3282 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Talairach J, Tournoux P (1988) Co-planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain: 3-dimensional proportional system: an Approach to cerebral imaging. Stuttgart, Germany: Theime. [Google Scholar]
- Talsma D, Coe B, Munoz DP, Theeuwes J (2010) Brain structures involved in visual search in the presence and absence of color singletons. J Cogn Neurosci 22:761–774. 10.1162/jocn.2009.21223 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Theeuwes J. (1992) Perceptual selectivity for color and form. Percept Psychophys 51:599–606. 10.3758/BF03211656 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Theeuwes J, Burger R (1998) Attentional control during visual search: the effect of irrelevant singletons. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 24:1342–1353. 10.1037/0096-1523.24.5.1342 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Theeuwes J, Godljn R (2002) Irrelevant singletons capture attention: evidence from inhibition of return. Percept Psychophys 64:764–770. 10.3758/BF03194743 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thomas NW, Paré M (2007) Temporal processing of saccade targets in parietal cortex area LIP during visual search. J Neurophysiol 97:942–947. 10.1152/jn.00413.2006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson KG, Hanes DP, Bichot NP, Schall JD (1996) Perceptual and motor processing stages identified in the activity of macaque frontal eye field neurons during visual search. J Neurophysiol 76:4040–4055. 10.1152/jn.1996.76.6.4040 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tommasi G, Fiorio M, Yelnik J, Krack P, Sala F, Schmitt E, Fraix V, Bertolasi L, Le Bas JF, Ricciardi GK, Fiaschi A, Theeuwes J, Pollak P, Chelazzi L (2015) Disentangling the role of cortico-basal ganglia loops in top-down and bottom-up visual attention: an investigation of attention deficits in Parkinson disease. J Cogn Neurosci 27:1215–1237. 10.1162/jocn_a_00770 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turatto M, Bonetti F, Pascucci D, Chelazzi L (2018a) Desensitizing the attention system to distraction while idling: a new latent learning phenomenon in the visual attention domain. J Exp Psychol Gen 147:1827–1850. 10.1037/xge0000503 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turatto M, Bonetti F, Pascucci D (2018b) Filtering visual onsets via habituation: a context-specific long-term memory of irrelevant stimuli. Psychon Bull Rev 25:1028–1034. 10.3758/s13423-017-1320-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Kerkoerle T, Self MW, Dagnino B, Gariel-Mathis MA, Poort J, van der Togt C, Roelfsema PR (2014) Alpha and gamma oscillations characterize feedback and feedforward processing in monkey visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 111:14332–14341. 10.1073/pnas.1402773111 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vissers ME, van Driel J, Slagter HA (2016) Proactive, but not reactive, distractor filtering relies on local modulation of alpha oscillatory activity. J Cogn Neurosci 28:1964–1979. 10.1162/jocn_a_01017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vogel EK, McCollough AW, Machizawa MG (2005) Neural measures reveal individual differences in controlling access to working memory. Nature 438:500–503. 10.1038/nature04171 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walsh BJ, Buonocore MH, Carter CS, Mangun GR (2011) Integrating conflict detection and attentional control mechanisms. J Cogn Neurosci 23:2211–2221. 10.1162/jocn.2010.21595 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang B, Theeuwes J (2018a) How to inhibit a distractor location? statistical learning versus active, top-down suppression. Atten Percept Psychophys 80:860–870. 10.3758/s13414-018-1493-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang B, Theeuwes J (2018b) Statistical regularities modulate attentional capture. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 44:13–17. 10.1037/xhp0000472 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang C, Rajagovindan R, Han SM, Ding M (2016) Top-down control of visual alpha oscillations: sources of control signals and their mechanisms of action. Front Hum Neurosci 10:15. 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weiss M, Lavidor M (2012) When less is more: evidence for a facilitative cathodal tDCS effect in attentional abilities. J Cogn Neurosci 24:1826–1833. 10.1162/jocn_a_00248 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weissman DH, Roberts KC, Visscher KM, Woldorff MG (2006) The neural bases of momentary lapses in attention. Nat Neurosci 9:971–978. 10.1038/nn1727 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White BJ, Kan JY, Levy R, Itti L, Munoz DP (2017) Superior colliculus encodes visual saliency before the primary visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 114:9451–9456. 10.1073/pnas.1701003114 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yantis S, Jonides J (1990) Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: voluntary versus automatic allocation. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 16:121–134. 10.1037/0096-1523.16.1.121 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhou W, Shu H (2017) A meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies of eye movements and visual word reading. Brain Behav 7:e00683. 10.1002/brb3.683 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zmigrod S, Zmigrod L, Hommel B (2016) Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) over the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex affects stimulus conflict but not response conflict. Neuroscience 322:320–325. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.02.046 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]