Abstract
The way in which the brain deals with sensory information relies not only on feedforward processing of signals from the periphery but also on feedback inputs. This is the case of the massive projection back from layer 6 in the visual cortex to the thalamus, for which, despite being the greatest single source of synaptic contacts, the functional role still remains unclear. In the cat lateral geniculate nucleus, part of this cortical feedback is mediated by type 1 metabotropic glutamate receptors (mGluR1s), which are exclusively located on distal segments of the relay-cell dendrites. Here we show that in adult cats the cortex uses a synaptic drive mediated by these receptors (mGluR1) specifically to enhance the excitatory center of the thalamic receptive field. Moreover the effect is maximum in response to those stimuli that effectively drive cortical cells, and importantly, it does not affect the spatiotemporal structure of the thalamic receptive field. Therefore, cortex, by closing this corticofugal “loop,” is able to increase the gain of its thalamic input within a focal spatial window, selecting key features of the incoming signal.
Keywords: LGN, visual cortex, corticothalamic, metabotropic receptors, feedback modulation, layer 6
In the early visual pathway, information travels from the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) to the primary visual cortex. At this level, the relay of visual information is regulated by a network of synaptic connections, including modulatory projections from brainstem nuclei (Ahlsen and Lo, 1982; De Lima and Singer, 1987a,b; Steriade et al., 1988; Bickford et al., 1993; Uhlrich et al., 1995; Cudeiro and Rivadulla, 1999), GABAergic inputs from local interneurons and perigeniculate neurons (Montero, 1986, 1987), and a feedback projection from the primary visual cortex using both ionotropic and metabotropic [metabotropic glutamate receptor (mGluR)] excitatory amino acid receptors (Montero and Wenthold, 1989; Scharfman et al., 1990; McCormick and von Krosigk, 1992). Two characteristics differentiate the cortical feedback from the other modulatory inputs: first, the number of fibers largely outnumbers any other projection (Van Horn et al., 2000), and second, the retinotopy of the pathway is preserved precisely (Updyke, 1975, 1977), making the visual cortex an ideal candidate to exert a point-to-point control onto the flow of information traveling through the LGN (Murphy et al., 1999). Previous investigations over several decades (Singer, 1977) have shown that corticofugal projections modify the excitability of thalamic neurons. Growing evidence favors an active role of feedback connections in visual processing, influencing properties of visual stimulus such as, for example, orientation and direction of movement (Sillito et al., 1993; Murphy et al., 1999) and contributing to the generation of the sensitivity of a cell to the length of a moving contour, the so-called length tuning (LT) (Murphy and Sillito, 1987), which was first observed in the cortex (Hubel and Wiesel, 1965).
In the cat, the LGN represents an excellent model for the study of the role exerted by corticofugal axons, because retinal and cortical inputs are segregated on relay cells, with retinal afferents contacting proximal dendrites and cortical terminals contacting distal dendrites (Wilson et al., 1984; Erisir et al., 1997). In addition, there is also segregation of the distribution of subtypes of the metabotropic glutamate receptors that matches this synaptic input distribution, with type 1 mGluRs (mGluR1s) found in the distal dendrites of relay cells (associated with cortical terminals) and mGluR5s being in the synaptic terminal of interneurons, coupled to retinal inputs (Godwin et al., 1996) (see Fig. 1).
To study the role of corticogeniculate inputs in visual processing, we extracellularly recorded the visual responses of LGN cells before, during, and after the iontophoretic ejection of (+)2-methyl-4-carboxyphenylglycine (LY367385), a highly specific mGluR1 antagonist (Clark et al., 1997; Salt et al., 1999; Marino et al., 2001). We used two sets of visual stimuli: static stimuli (flashing spots), considered to drive LGN neurons strongly but not to activate corticothalamic cells, and moving patterns (bars or gratings), considered to better activate corticothalamic cells. To dissect out specific effects from global changes in firing rate caused by changes in inhibitory balance, we have compared responses obtained during mGluR1 blockade with those recorded during GABA ejection. Our results indicate that cortical feedback mediated by mGluR1 receptors is likely to enhance thalamic center-surround antagonism, with major consequences for properties like LT, but it does not affect the spatiotemporal structure of the thalamic receptive field measured with static stimuli.
Parts of these results have been published previously in abstract form (Varela et al., 2000).
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Adult cats, 2.5–3.5 kg, were anesthetized with halothane (0.1–1% in 70% N2O/30% O2) and paralyzed with gallamine triethiodide (10 mg · kg−1 · hr−1). Full details are given in Cudeiro et al. (2000). To further reduce possible eye movement artifacts, posts were fixed to the sclera. The procedures conformed to the Spanish Physiology Society and the International Council for Laboratory Animal Science and the European Union (statute 86/809).
Extracellular recording and iontophoresis. All of our observations were made in the A laminas of the LGN. Multibarreled pipettes (three to five barrels) were used for extracellular recording and iontophoretic ejection of drugs. Waveforms and time stamps were stored (Plexon Inc.), and we carefully performed off-line spike sorting to assess the isolation of the selected spike and recording stability. The barrels were filled with NaCl (3m) for recording, and an aqueous solution of 50 mm LY367385, pH 8. Ach (1m, pH 5) was included in the pipette for identifying relay cells on the basis of its excitatory effect (McCormick and Prince, 1987). Ejection currents were in the range of 50–80 nA; this range was selected on the basis of initial qualitative observations. When not in use, each drug barrel was subject to a constant retention current of 5–25 nA of appropriate polarity. When a cell was isolated, we measured its response to the visual stimuli and repeated the same protocol during ejection of LY367385 and once more after a period of recovery. Only cells that showed recovery of responses >80% of control values were included in this study.
Visual stimulation. Computer-controlled visual stimuli (Lohmann Research Equipment) were presented monocularly on a monitor with a mean luminance of 14 cd/m2 at a contrast of 0.7, refresh rate 128 Hz. Visual stimuli consisted of sinusoidal drifting wave gratings, spots of different size flashing on the receptive field (RF), and moving bars of different length. The spatiotemporal properties of the stimuli were optimally set for each cell. Stimuli were presented in pseudorandom order and included two blank presentations to obtain spontaneous activity. Responses were collected for the entire duration of the stimulus and were averaged from 8 to 15 presentations. For drifting gratings, each presentation included 10 cycles of the stimulus. Receptive fields were mapped with moving bars and in 10 cells by reverse correlation with sparse noise (Jones and Palmer, 1987; Hirsch et al., 1998). The sparse noise protocol consisted of light or dark squares flashed singly for 40 msec in pseudorandom order, 30 times each, on a 20 × 20 grid (square size, 0.4–1°; contrast ranged from 80 to 100%). RFs are plotted as pixel maps where the magnitude of the response is coded in a grayscale. All maps are constructed for just one polarity of the stimulus. LT index was calculated as (spikes evoked by optimal stimulus − spikes evoked by non-optimal)/spikes evoked by optimal. Number of spikes was calculated from the accumulated count in all the bins constituting the response area in the peristimulus time histograms (PSTHs), after subtraction of the background discharge level. We defined the response area from PSTHs showing the optimal response including those bins underlying the center response but not the secondary peaks (Jones and Sillito, 1994). Thus the LT index could vary from 0 (not length tuned) to 1 (no response to non-optimal, i.e., 100% length tuned); bar length = 0 represents spontaneous activity.
RESULTS
Visual information from the retina reaches layer 4 of primary visual cortex indirectly via a relay in the LGN of the thalamus. After extensive laminar processing in primary visual cortex, the loop is closed by means of a dense feedback connection arising from pyramidal cells in layer 6 (Fig. 1). Although feedback mechanisms of processing are common to all neuronal systems, their precise role remains undetermined.
The results presented in this paper were obtained from 55 cells (28 X, 24 Y, and 3 unclassified) recorded in the A laminas of the LGN of adult cats. We found no difference regarding LGN cell types.
Effect of blocking cortical inputs mediated by metabotropic receptors on thalamic response properties
Effect of LY367385 on moving stimuli
Thalamic and cortical cells differ not only in their receptive field structure but also in other response properties such as their sensitivity to moving versus static stimuli. To maximize the effect of blocking mGluR1 receptors, we used those stimuli that more strongly activate cortical cells. Figure 2 shows the response of an X ON LGN cell to a drifting grating of optimal spatial and temporal frequency before, during, and after the ejection of LY367385. Blocking of mGluR1s significantly decreased the neuron response (by 64%), which recovered to control levels after ∼15 min. This result was found consistently in all cells examined (average diminution ± SD; 42 ± 4%; n = 21). Occasionally we tested the action of LY367385 on the responses evoked by gratings with orthogonal orientations. The observed effect showed no difference (n = 4; data not shown) regarding the orientation of the stimulus.
Other stimuli that effectively drive cortical and thalamic cells are moving bars. In addition, LGN cells (like some cortical cells) are selective for the bar length and are known as length-selective, end-stopped, or length tuned (LT). In the LGN, this property is highly dependent on the integrity of the visual cortex, and it is considered to be an emergent property of the geniculocortical loop (Murphy and Sillito, 1987). Figure 3shows an example of a representative cell. In the center of the figure, the tuning curve illustrates the responses of the neuron during control conditions (black) and during ejection of LY367385 (gray). The histograms show a detailed response for each of the three selected lengths of the bar. During the control period, the cell shows the classical LT curve, the response reaching a maximum when the length of the bar fits the dimensions of the RF center (3° in this example), declining in response to longer bars. After 6 min of continuous ejection of LY367385, the responses of the cell are reduced. However, it is important to note that the reduction affects most the responses to the optimal length. Recovery was obtained after 10 min (data not shown). The same result was obtained in 56% of the cells studied (n = 25). On average, the LT index (see Materials and Methods) decreased by 55%. The scatter plot in Figure4A shows the LT index in control conditions against LT index during LY367385 ejection for all cells. In 14 cases the LT index clearly decreased during the mGluR1 blockade (Wilcoxon test; p < 0.01). In the remaining 11 cells there was a similar change to all stimuli, and therefore the LT index remained unaffected. The bar histogram in Figure4B represents the average decrease in LT index for the entire population (n = 25).
Effect of LY367385 on static stimuli
We have shown that metabotropic glutamate receptors are functionally active in the LGN in response to stimuli that optimally activate cortical cells. What happens with static stimuli? We examined the effect of LY367385 on the responses evoked by flashed spots covering the RF center. During the blockade of mGluR1 receptors, the response to the stimulus remains unaffected (Fig.5A). In these cases, the response to flashing spots was not reduced, but when the presentation of the stimulus was long enough, we observed that the sustained phase of the response clearly decreased during LY367385 application (Fig.5B) (notice that the transient phase of the response is not affected). This effect, and the concomitant reduction in spontaneous activity, suggests that the cortical feedback, mediated by mGluR1, plays an important role in regulating LGN excitability. In some cases, we did observe a slight diminution in the magnitude of the response, but this reduction was always much smaller than that obtained when moving stimuli were used. The average decrease was 9% (n = 25), clearly below the one obtained when moving bars of similar length were used. This small diminution in response could be a consequence of a reduction in excitability, because spontaneous activity was also reduced when mGluR1s were blocked (56%). Interestingly, spontaneous activity did not decrease in 30% of the cells studied (n = 37).
LY367385 does not produce an unspecific decrease in excitability
The results presented above suggest that blocking mGluR1-mediated cortical input reduces LGN excitability. This action is reflected in a decrease in the spontaneous activity and the sustained component of the response to long-lasting flashing spots. However, the effect of LY367385 on responses to moving stimuli suggests that the role of the cortical feedback is more complex than a global, unspecific modulation in firing rate as a result of, for instance, increased hyperpolarization. To further exclude this as a possibility, we directly compared the effects of LY367385 and GABA (the neurotransmitter responsible for inhibition in the LGN) on visual responses of six LGN cells. Figure6A shows results obtained from a X ON cell. In control conditions (Fig.6A, left histogram, black line), the cell had an LT index of 0.47. During LY367385 ejection (dotted line), selectivity to bar length was mostly abolished because the response to the optimal stimulus was strongly affected. During GABA application (Fig. 6A,center histogram, black line), visual responses markedly decreased to all stimuli (note the different y-axis scale). However, the cell clearly maintained selectivity for stimulus length. Moreover, the response of this cell to flashing spots of increasing diameters was not affected by LY367385 (Fig.6B).
Effect of LY367385 on the organization of LGN receptive fields
A fundamental property of LGN receptive fields is the center-surround antagonism. This attribute is retinal in origin, but it has been shown to be influenced by visual cortex (Murphy and Sillito, 1987; Cudeiro and Sillito, 1996). To study the effect of blocking mGluR1 receptors on the basic organization of the thalamic receptive field, we followed two strategies. First, we studied the effect of LY367385 application on the degree of center-surround antagonism measured by comparing responses to flashed spots of two different sizes: one just covering the receptive-field excitatory center and the other covering both center and surround. Figure7 illustrates the results obtained from a Y ON cell. Before the application of LY367385 (black), the cell showed rather strong center-surround antagonism, i.e., the optimal response was obtained for a stimulus size (3°) that matched the size of the receptive-field center (top histogram). When the size of the spot was increased to 8°, covering simultaneously center and surround, the response dropped markedly (bottom histogram). This effect was detected in both the transient and sustained phases of the response. When mGluR1s were blocked (Fig. 7, gray), the transient response to the small and large spots was conserved, but the sustained component was clearly affected in both cases.
The second approach consisted of using a sparse noise protocol (see Materials and Methods) to obtain a precise representation of the spatiotemporal structure of the RF center. Figure8A shows the RF of an X ON cell, obtained at 20–60 msec, before and during LY367385 application. The sparse noise consisted of white spots flashed on a dark background (contrast, 100%). The bar histograms next to the pixel maps represent the response of the cell to stimuli that fell on pixels located along two imaginary lines crossing the RF center in thex- and y-axis. The only difference between the two maps is a decrease in the background activity during the blockade of mGluR1, visible as a darker background in the map and a lower level in the baseline of the histograms. Figure 8B shows that neither the time course of the response at the peak of the receptive field nor its spatial structure were affected during LY367385 application (gray line). Although the stimulus was updated every 40 msec, spike counts were taken in 2 msec bins to precisely check potential changes in latency, duration, or magnitude of the response.
DISCUSSION
LY367385 is a highly specific mGluR1 antagonist (Clark et al., 1997; Marino et al. 2001), and it has previously been shown in vivo that this compound does not affect responses mediated by either other metabotropic or ionotropic glutamate receptors (Salt et al., 1999). Furthermore, there is clear evidence that the corticofugal axons are the only source of synaptic input to the LGN, which uses mGluR1 (Godwin et al., 1996). Given these data, we believe that the results reported in this paper are therefore the direct consequence of modifying the corticothalamic input mediated by metabotropic receptors.
Here we have demonstrated that ∼60% of LGN neurons, in the presence of the specific mGluR1 antagonist LY367385, decrease their selectivity for the length of effective moving bar stimuli. Specifically, this property is lost because the response to the optimal stimulus is strongly reduced, rather than there being an increase in response magnitude to longer stimuli. Second, this effect does not correlate with changes in the spatiotemporal structure of the RF as measured with a sparse noise protocol. Third, spontaneous firing is reduced. On the other hand, responses to simple, static stimuli, like flashed spots, remain largely unaffected when the mGluR1 antagonist LY367385 is applied. However, when the presentation of the stimulus is long enough to evoke a sustained response, this phase of the response is notably reduced.
The most parsimonious interpretation of our results is that mGluR1-mediated cortical input modulates LGN responses via two different mechanisms acting simultaneously. First, it would modulate excitability, as indicated by the decrease in spontaneous activity when ejecting LY367385. Probably this is the result of withdrawing some tonic component of the excitatory input from cortex to LGN that is mediated by mGluR1s (Turner and Salt, 2000).
A second mechanism would specifically enhance thalamic responses to those stimuli that strongly activate cortical neurons. We have demonstrated that a simple hyperpolarization, such as that induced by local application of GABA, affected the responses to all the stimuli by a similar amount, without affecting the LT of the cells. Hence, the effect of LY367385 on thalamic responses cannot be explained solely on the basis of a nonspecific hyperpolarization, but rather requires that the normal excitatory drive is “permissive” specifically to the geniculate expression of its excitatory retinal input, revealed when effective cortical stimuli are used.
The role of the corticothalamic input on thalamic response properties has been a matter of intense work, both in vivo and in vitro, during the past several years (Murphy and Sillito, 1987;McCormick and von Krosigk, 1992; Sillito et al., 1994; Marrocco et al., 1996; Murphy et al., 1999; Bal et al., 2000). Previous experimental approaches included suppressing cortical input by surgically removing or cooling the cortex, pharmacological modulation of cortical activity, etc. Our experimental design, which was based on two recent findings—the discovery that at the level of the LGN, mGluR1s are restricted to corticothalamic synapses onto relay cells, and the development of an antagonist specific for mGluR1—has two major advantages: it restricts the extent of the blockade, and it recovers in a reasonable time, which allowed us to test the response of the same cell in several conditions.
Effects on moving stimuli
The main effect described in this paper is a decrease in thalamic LT when cortical input is blocked. This decrease in bar-length selectivity is not produced by an increase in the response to the non-optimal stimuli, as demonstrated by Sillito and colleagues (Murphy and Sillito, 1987), but rather by a specific reduction of the response to the optimal stimulus. Murphy and Sillito (1987) reported a loss of LT in LGN cells when visual cortex was surgically removed, eliminating all cortical input to the thalamus. This loss of LT was the result of increased responses to longer stimuli, indicating that length tuning was the result of cortically driven geniculate inhibition. In the data above we reveal a second component, by locally suppressing part of the excitatory cortical input (mediated by mGluR1 receptors) without directly affecting cortically driven inhibition [cortical terminals activate only ionotropic receptors of LGN interneurons (Pape and McCormick, 1995)]. To account for the changes in LT that we show here, we suggest that this mGluR1-mediated corticofugal excitation should be insufficient on its own to provide an effective drive for the LGN center. Rather, the more weakly length-tuned retinal input must summate, probably nonlinearly, with the cortical drive to create the center response to moving stimuli. As a result, if we remove the cortical drive, the response at the center collapses, producing a distortion in length summation (flattening the length-tuning curve).
Our findings fit nicely with recent data obtained in the somatosensory system by Canedo and Aguilar (2000), who showed that cortical input enhances thalamic and cuneate responses produced by stimuli located at the center of the RF. Our data seem to indicate that visual cortex modulates thalamic activity to promote information transfer through specific locations in sensory space. Because we are locally blocking mGluR1, we cannot rule out the possibility that removing cortical inputs at increasing distances in the LGN would further modify thalamic response properties, particularly to those stimuli that specifically engage the surround of the receptive field. Importantly, a significant percentage of cells in our sample (11 of 25) did not show a reduction in LT index in the presence of LY367385; rather, responses to stimuli of all sizes were reduced by a similar amount. To a first approximation, our sample then seems to comprise cells that belong to two separate classes of LGN neurons distinctly affected by the cortical input. However, none of the tests performed systematically to characterize all recorded cells allowed us to detect further consistent differences between the two cell types. Thus both groups contained a similar proportion of X and Y cells. Moreover, cells belonging to the two groups were found during the same penetration, with no trace of clustering according to laminar location or distance from the area centralis. An attractive possibility is that the two groups represent relay cells and interneurons. On the basis of their responses to iontophoretical ejection of Ach (McCormick and Prince, 1987), we conclude that most of our recordings are from relay cells. One could argue that the two groups represent two novel cell classes in the LGN. However, because we are locally blocking mGluR1 receptors using iontophoretic application of the drug, we cannot discard the possibility that more effective removal of cortical inputs at different distances in the LGN (by increased ejection currents, longer durations, etc.), or even a differential distribution of such inputs on a given dendritic tree, would further modify thalamic response properties.
Effects on static stimuli
Cortical feedback has been related to changes in the structure, and even the position, of LGN receptive fields (Wang et al., 2000). Our results using sparse noise, however, show that blocking mGluR1-mediated cortical input has no effect on the spatiotemporal structure of thalamic receptive fields. This apparent disagreement could arise from two important methodological differences. First, Wang et al. (2000)topically applied bicuculline to the cortex, which increases cortical input over a large area of LGN. Second, our experimental approach does not directly affect cortically driven inhibitory events in the LGN, which may “shape” the RF dynamically. Whether or not the spatial structure of each component of the field changes in the presence of LY367385, stronger effects would be expected on responses to stimuli that do induce complex center-surround interactions, like concentric stimuli with different orientations (Sillito et al., 1993).
Blocking mGluR1 reduced spontaneous activity in 70% of the cells recorded. This is in good agreement with previous reports by Tsumoto et al. (1978) showing that cortical input to LGN is excitatory when it comes from cortical regions in good retinotopic alignment. Furthermore, mGluR1-mediated cortical input has been shown to evoke a long-lasting excitatory postsynaptic potential from the reduction of a potassium conductance (McCormick and von Krosigk, 1992) that is associated with an increased neuronal excitability (Turner and Salt, 2000). This excitatory effect could explain the observed reduction in the sustained responses when mGluR1s are blocked.
Our results suggest that cortical feedback, mediated by mGluR1, modulates center-surround interactions by specifically enhancing the responses at the center of the field. In addition, results by others have demonstrated cortically mediated lateral inhibition in the LGN (Tsumoto et al., 1978; Murphy and Sillito, 1987). Thus the corticothalamic projection, using a push–pull mechanism that selectively enhances center-excitation or surround-inhibition, or both, may be capable of modulating thalamic activity to promote transfer through specific locations in sensory space. Furthermore, the time scale of the metabotropic corticothalamic EPSP is on the order of seconds (McCormick and von Krosigk, 1992) and might be implicated in different plastic processes, mediating experience-dependent changes in thalamic responses (Cudeiro et al., 2000). In the early visual system, feedforward connections shape the classical receptive field (e.g., from the retina to LGN to V1) (Alonso et al., 1996; Tavazoie and Reid, 2000), and by means of feedback pathways, neurons receive information form the surround. Here we have shown that by acting on metabotropic receptors, feedback connections reinforce the center of the RF and hence the salience of the message passed to the visual cortex.
Footnotes
This work was supported by Comision Interministerial de Ciencia y Tecnologia (PB1998-0179), Spain. We thank K. L. Grieve for helpful comments and corrections on this manuscript.
Correspondence should be addressed to Javier Cudeiro, Departamento de Medicina, Campus de Oza, 15006 A Coruña, Spain. E-mail: jcud@udc.es.
REFERENCES
- 1.Ahlsen G, Lo FS. Projection of brain stem neurons to the perigeniculate nucleus and the lateral geniculate nucleus in the cat. Brain Res. 1982;238:433–438. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(82)90119-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Alonso JM, Usrey WM, Reid RC. Precisely correlated firing in cells of the lateral geniculate nucleus. Nature. 1996;383:815–819. doi: 10.1038/383815a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Bal T, Debay D, Destexhe A. Cortical feedback controls the frequency and synchrony of oscillations in the visual thalamus. J Neurosci. 2000;20:7478–7488. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-19-07478.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Bickford ME, Gunluk AE, Guido W, Sherman SM. Evidence that cholinergic axons from the parabrachial region of the brainstem are the exclusive source of nitric oxide in the lateral geniculate nucleus of the cat. J Comp Neurol. 1993;334:410–430. doi: 10.1002/cne.903340307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Canedo A, Aguilar J. Spatial and cortical influences exerted on cuneothalamic and thalamocortical neurons of the cat. Eur J Neurosci. 2000;12:2515–2533. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2000.00107.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Clark BP, Baker SR, Goldsworthy J, Harris JR, Kingston AE. 2-Methyl-4-carboxyphenylglycine (LY367385) selectively antagonizes metabotropic glutamate mGluR1 receptors. Bioorg Med Chem Lett. 1997;7:2777–2870. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Cudeiro J, Rivadulla C. Sight and insight: on the physiological role of nitric oxide in the visual system. Trends Neurosci. 1999;22:109–116. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(98)01299-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Cudeiro J, Sillito AM. Spatial frequency tuning of orientation-discontinuity-sensitive corticofugal feedback to the cat lateral geniculate nucleus. J Physiol (Lond) 1996;490:481–492. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1996.sp021159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Cudeiro J, Rivadulla C, Grieve KL. Visual response augmentation in cat (and macaque) LGN: potentiation by corticofugally mediated gain control in the temporal domain. Eur J Neurosci. 2000;12:1135–1144. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2000.00000.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.De Lima AD, Singer W. The serotoninergic fibers in the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus of the cat: distribution and synaptic connections demonstrated with immunocytochemistry. J Comp Neurol. 1987a;258:339–351. doi: 10.1002/cne.902580303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.De Lima AD, Singer W. The brainstem projection to the lateral geniculate nucleus in the cat: identification of cholinergic and monoaminergic elements. J Comp Neurol. 1987b;259:92–121. doi: 10.1002/cne.902590107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Erisir A, Van Horn SC, Bickford ME, Sherman SM. Immunocytochemistry and distribution of parabrachial terminals in the lateral geniculate nucleus of the cat: a comparison with corticogeniculate terminals. J Comp Neurol. 1997;377:535–549. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Godwin DW, Van Horn SC, Eriir A, Sesma M, Romano C, Sherman SM. Ultrastructural localization suggests that retinal and cortical inputs access different metabotropic glutamate receptors in the lateral geniculate nucleus. J Neurosci. 1996;16:8181–8192. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-24-08181.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Hirsch JA, Alonso JM, Reid RC, Martinez LM. Synaptic integration in striate cortical simple cells. J Neurosci. 1998;18:9517–9528. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-22-09517.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Hubel DH, Wiesel TN. Receptive fields and functional architecture in two non-striate visual areas (18 and 19) of the cat. J Neurophysiol. 1965;28:229–287. doi: 10.1152/jn.1965.28.2.229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Jones HE, Sillito AM. The length-response properties of cells in the feline perigeniculate nucleus. Eur J Neurosci. 1994;6:1199–1204. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1994.tb00618.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Jones JP, Palmer LA. The two-dimensional spatial structure of simple receptive fields in cat striate cortex. J Neurophysiol. 1987;58:1187–1211. doi: 10.1152/jn.1987.58.6.1187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Marino MJ, Wittmann M, Bradley SR, Hubert GW, Smith Y, Conn PJ. Activation of group I metabotropic glutamate receptors produces a direct excitation and disinhibition of GABAergic projection neurons in the substantia nigra pars reticulata. J Neurosci. 2001;21:7001–7012. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-18-07001.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Marrocco RT, McClurkin JW, Alkire MT. The influence of the visual cortex on the spatiotemporal response properties of lateral geniculate nucleus cells. Brain Res. 1996;737:110–118. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(96)00660-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.McCormick DA, Prince DA. Actions of acetylcholine in the guinea-pig and cat medial and lateral geniculate nuclei, in vitro. J Physiol (Lond) 1987;392:147–165. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1987.sp016774. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.McCormick DA, von Krosigk M. Corticothalamic activation modulates thalamic firing through glutamate “metabotropic” receptors. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1992;89:2774–2778. doi: 10.1073/pnas.89.7.2774. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Montero VM. Localization of gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) in type 3 cells and demonstration of their source to F2 terminals in the cat lateral geniculate nucleus: a Golgi-electron-microscopic GABA-immunocytochemical study. J Comp Neurol. 1986;254:228–245. doi: 10.1002/cne.902540207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Montero VM. Ultrastructural identification of synaptic terminals from the axon of type 3 interneurons in the cat lateral geniculate nucleus. J Comp Neurol. 1987;264:268–283. doi: 10.1002/cne.902640210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Montero VM, Wenthold RJ. Quantitative immunogold analysis reveals high glutamate levels in retinal and cortical synaptic terminals in the lateral geniculate nucleus of the macaque. Neuroscience. 1989;31:639–647. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(89)90429-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Murphy PC, Sillito AM. Corticofugal feedback influences the generation of length tuning in the visual pathway. Nature. 1987;329:727–729. doi: 10.1038/329727a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Murphy PC, Duckett SG, Sillito AM. Feedback connections to the lateral geniculate nucleus and cortical response properties. Science. 1999;286:1552–1554. doi: 10.1126/science.286.5444.1552. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Pape H-C, McCormick DA. Electrophysiological and pharmacological properties of interneurons in the cat dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus. Neuroscience. 1995;68:1105–1125. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(95)00205-w. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Salt TE, Turner JP, Kingston AE. Evaluation of agonists and antagonists acting at group I metabotropic glutamate receptors in the thalamus in vivo. Neuropharmacology. 1999;38:1505–1510. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3908(99)00081-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Scharfman HE, Lu SM, Guido W, Adams PR, Sherman SM. N-methyl-d-aspartate receptors contribute to excitatory postsynaptic potentials of cat lateral geniculate neurons recorded in thalamic slices. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1990;87:4548–4552. doi: 10.1073/pnas.87.12.4548. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Sillito AM, Cudeiro J, Murphy PC. Orientation sensitive elements in the corticofugal influence on center-surround interactions in the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus. Exp Brain Res. 1993;93:6–16. doi: 10.1007/BF00227775. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Sillito AM, Jones HE, Gerstein GL, West DC. Feature-linked synchronization of thalamic relay cell firing induced by feedback from the visual cortex. Nature. 1994;369:479–482. doi: 10.1038/369479a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Singer W. Control of thalamic transmission by corticofugal and ascending reticular pathways in the visual system. Physiol Rev. 1977;57:386–420. doi: 10.1152/physrev.1977.57.3.386. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Steriade M, Pare D, Parent A, Smith Y. Projections of cholinergic and non-cholinergic neurons of the brainstem core to relay and associational thalamic nuclei in the cat and macaque monkey. Neuroscience. 1988;25:47–67. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(88)90006-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Tavazoie SF, Reid RC. Diverse receptive fields in the lateral geniculate nucleus during thalamocortical development. Nat Neurosci. 2000;3:608–616. doi: 10.1038/75786. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Tsumoto T, Creutzfeldt OD, Legendy CR. Functional organization of the corticofugal system from visual cortex to lateral geniculate nucleus in the cat (with an appendix on geniculo-cortical mono-synaptic connections). Exp Brain Res. 1978;32:345–364. doi: 10.1007/BF00238707. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Turner JP, Salt TE. Synaptic activation of the group I metabotropic glutamate receptor mGlu1 on the thalamocortical neurons of the rat dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus in vitro. Neuroscience. 2000;100:493–505. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(00)00280-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Uhlrich DJ, Tamamaki N, Murphy PC, Sherman SM. Effects of brain stem parabrachial activation on receptive field properties of cells in the cat's lateral geniculate nucleus. J Neurophysiol. 1995;73:2428–2447. doi: 10.1152/jn.1995.73.6.2428. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Updyke BV. The patterns of projection of cortical areas 17, 18, and 19 onto the laminae of the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus in the cat. J Comp Neurol. 1975;163:377–396. doi: 10.1002/cne.901630402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Updyke BV. Topographic organization of the projection from cortical areas 17, 18, and 19 onto the thalamus, pretectum and superior colliculus in the cat. J Comp Neurol. 1977;173:81–122. doi: 10.1002/cne.901730106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Van Horn SC, Erisir A, Sherman SM. Relative distribution of synapses in the A-laminae of the lateral geniculate nucleus of the cat. J Comp Neurol. 2000;416:509–520. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Varela C, Rivadulla C, Martinez L, Cudeiro J. Stimulus-dependent modulation of LGN activity by cortically activated metabotropic glutamate receptors. Soc Neurosci Abstr. 2000;26:1197. [Google Scholar]
- 42.Wang W, Andolina IM, Jones HE, Salt TE, Sillito AM. Influence of enhanced feedback from V1 on visual responses in the LGN. Soc Neurosci Abstr. 2000;26:447. [Google Scholar]
- 43.Wilson JR, Friedlander MJ, Sherman SM. Fine structural morphology of identified X- and Y-cells in the cat's lateral geniculate nucleus. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1984;221:411–436. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1984.0042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]