Abstract
We combined event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) with olfactory classical conditioning to differentiate the neural responses evoked during appetitive and aversive olfactory learning. Three neutral faces [the conditioned stimuli (CS+)] were repetitively paired with pleasant, neutral, or unpleasant odors [the unconditioned stimuli (UCS)] in a partial reinforcement schedule. A fourth face was never paired to odor [the nonconditioned stimulus (CS−)]. Learning-related neural activity, comparing unpaired (face only) CS+ stimuli with CS−, showed valence-independent activations in rostral and caudal orbitofrontal cortex (OFC). Medial OFC responded to the appetitive (app) CS+, whereas lateral OFC responded to the aversive (av) CS+. Within nucleus accumbens, neural responses showed divergent activation profiles that increased with time in response to the appCS+ but decreased in response to the avCS+. In posterior amygdala, responses were elicited by the appCS+, which habituated over time. In temporal piriform cortex, neural responses were evoked by the avCS+, which progressively increased with time. These results highlight regional and temporal dissociations during olfactory learning and imply that emotionally salient odors can engender cross-modal associative learning. Moreover, the findings suggest that the role of human primary (piriform) and secondary olfactory cortices transcends their function as mere intermediaries of chemosensory information processing.
Keywords: olfaction, odor, emotion, conditioning, associative learning, neuroimaging, fMRI, orbitofrontal cortex, nucleus accumbens, amygdala, piriform cortex
Behavior is powerfully influenced by the biological salience of environmental stimuli. Odors can impact on behavioral states by virtue of their associations with threat, food, and sex. For example, bombykol, a volatile pheromone secreted by female silkworm moths, will drive potential mates miles upwind in hot pursuit (Carde and Mafra-Neto, 1997). Despite a conspicuous phylogenetic decline in olfactory prowess, humans can exhibit behavioral vulnerability to odors in certain instances (MacFarlane, 1975;Beauchamp and Maller, 1977; Baron, 1988; Stern and McClintock, 1998).
How the brain encodes and recalls behaviorally meaningful information can be investigated through associative learning paradigms. Classical (Pavlovian) conditioning is one such example in which a previously neutral item [the conditioned stimulus (CS+)] becomes predictive of an emotionally salient item [the unconditioned stimulus (UCS)] through repeated pairings. In aversive conditioning the UCS is often represented by an electric shock or loud sound, whereas foods, liquids, or drugs commonly substitute for an appetitive (reward-based) UCS. Animal studies indicate that the amygdala is critical to aversive (fear) conditioning (LeDoux, 2000), whereas dopaminergic mesolimbic circuitry has been implicated in appetitive learning (Schultz, 1998;Kalivas and Nakamura, 1999). Human neuroimaging studies of conditioning have essentially focused on aversive paradigms (Buchel et al., 1998;LaBar et al., 1998; Morris et al., 2001), with few studies addressing reward learning (O'Doherty et al., 2002).
Given the hedonic primacy of odor perception (Schiffman, 1974), odors can act as rewarding or aversive UCS stimuli in conditioning experiments. In aversion therapy for overeaters, desserts become endowed with the aversive properties of an unpleasant odor after multiple pairings (Frohwirth and Foreyt, 1978), although this effect is short-lived (Cole and Bond, 1983). When an odor UCS is paired with a visual (face) CS+, both subjective preferences of the faces and physiological indices of conditioning are significantly altered (Todrank et al., 1995; Hermann et al., 2000). In the olfactory neuroimaging literature, few studies have explored higher-order aspects of odor processing [although see Savic et al. (2000), Royet et al. (2001), and Dade et al. (2002)], and none has attempted to characterize the brain regions involved in olfactory associative learning.
In this report we describe the use of event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) techniques to delineate the neural substrates underlying human olfactory conditioning. By manipulating the affective properties of the odor UCS, we indexed alternate modes of conditioning within the same experiment: appetitive learning in the case of a pleasant odor and aversive learning in the case of an unpleasant odor. We predicted that variations in the emotional valence of learning would elicit differential responses in orbital frontal cortex (OFC), amygdala, and ventral striatum, regions strongly implicated in appetitive and aversive learning. We also hypothesized that olfactory structures involved in low-level sensory processing [as defined in a companion paper in this issue (Gottfried et al., 2002)] would show modulation of their response as a function of associative learning.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
The detailed methodology has been reported in the accompanying paper (Gottfried et al., 2002), and only distinct aspects are described here.
Subjects. Informed consent was obtained from 17 healthy right-handed subjects (10 women; mean age, 23 years), although data from 2 subjects (1 woman) was discarded because of technical problems. The study was approved by the joint National Hospital for Neurology and Neurosurgery and Institute of Neurology Ethics Committee.
Stimuli and experiment. Subjects participated in an olfactory form of classical conditioning that was similar to previous experiments in our laboratory (Buchel et al., 1998; Morris et al., 2001). Here, a series of neutral faces was paired with odors that varied in pleasantness. Four neutral faces (two male, two female) comprised the one nonconditioned (CS−) and the three conditioned (CS+) stimuli and were projected to the subjects as they lay inside the scanner. Three odors comprised the pleasant (8% vanillin), neutrally valenced (0.1% phenethyl alcohol), and unpleasant (5% 4-methyl-pentanoic acid) UCS stimuli. These were delivered using a four-channel computer-controlled olfactometer suitable for the fMRI environment (Lorig et al., 1999). Under 50% reinforcement, only one-half of all faces (CS+) were paired with their corresponding odor, resulting in seven discrete conditions: (1) “appetitive” face, paired with pleasant odor (appCS+P); (2) appetitive face, unpaired (appCS+U); (3) “neutral” face, paired with neutral odor (ntCS+P); (4) neutral face, unpaired (ntCS+U); (5) “aversive” face, paired with unpleasant odor (avCS+P); (6) aversive face, unpaired (avCS+U); and (7) face never paired with odor (CS−). This scheme allowed us to model responses to the appetitive and aversive CS+ without interference from the odor UCS.
During the task, subjects made a forced-choice push-button response regarding facial gender. Trial onset was marked by the appearance of a face. After 500 msec, the brightening of a dull-red cross-hair cued subjects to sniff, and the olfactometer was simultaneously triggered to deliver odor or control air, depending on the condition type. In this manner, subjects completed a sniff on each trial, regardless of odor presence. The stimulus duration of faces, sniff cues, and odor pulses was 750 msec in all cases. Thus, the CS+ and UCS overlapped for 250 msec. The intertrial interval was 7.5 sec, and stimulus presentation was randomized. Each event type was repeated 25 times, except for the CS− (50 times), for an experimental duration of 25 min. Subject-specific respiratory patterns (sniffs) were monitored on-line via corrugated breathing belts (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) coupled to a differential pressure sensor (Honeywell, Morristown, NJ).
Conditioning index. Subject reaction times (RTs), which have been used successfully in our laboratory as a reliable index of conditioning (Critchley et al., 2002), were measured to gauge the efficacy of learning. RTs were recorded using Cogent 2000 software (Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, London, UK). Off-line analysis was performed using Matlab 6.0 (The Mathworks Inc., Natick, MA). Condition-specific RTs for each subject were averaged over three successive temporal blocks of the experiment. Differences between unpaired CS+ and CS− conditions were calculated for each valence level (aversive, appetitive, neutral) and pooled across subjects. RT data from six subjects (appetitive, aversive events) and eight subjects (neutral events) were unavailable because of technical complications.
Image acquisition. Gradient echo T2*-weighted echoplanar images (EPI) were acquired with blood–oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) contrast on a 2-Tesla Siemens Vision MRI scanner (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany), using an optimized sequence to reduce signal dropout in orbitofrontal and ventral temporal lobes (Deichmann and Turner, 2002). There were 33 slices per volume (1.8 mm thickness, 1.2 mm gap), which covered ∼80% of the whole brain, and they were acquired continuously every 2.31 sec. Images were spatially realigned (Friston et al., 1995a), slice-time corrected, normalized, and smoothed with an 8 mm kernel. T1-weighted anatomical images were also obtained from each subject and co-registered to the mean normalized EPI.
Analysis of learning-evoked responses. Event-related fMRI data were analyzed in SPM99 (Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience) (Friston et al., 1995b) using a random-effects model as described in the companion paper (Gottfried et al., 2002), with the following exceptions. Regressors of interest included the six CS+ conditions, plus the CS− condition divided randomly into two time series of 25 events each (designated “appCS−” and “avCS−”). The estimation of two independent CS− baselines was necessary to implement conjunction analysis (see below), although it is important to note that appCS− and avCS− represented experimentally identical event types. To model learning, the design incorporated condition × time interactions, using a time constant of one-quarter session length (372 sec) similar to that used previously in neuroimaging studies of conditioning from our laboratory (Buchel et al., 1998;Morris et al., 2001). Neural responses specific to learning were isolated by contrasting the unpaired CS+ events with their designated CS− baselines.
Activations common to both appetitive and aversive learning were tested using conjunction analysis (Price and Friston, 1997). Subject-specific parameter estimates were calculated for four effects of interest: appCS+U, avCS+U, appCS−, and avCS−. (This process was repeated for the four time-modulated regressors.) These estimates from all 15 subjects were then entered into a repeated-measures ANOVA, using nonsphericity correction. By testing appropriate pairs of linear contrasts, we were able to identify learning-related responses independent of specific valence. As detailed in the accompanying paper (Gottfried et al. 2002), a descriptive model of the data was also constructed to depict time courses of activation within significant (accumbens) voxels derived from the primary model.
We report activations surviving a threshold of p < 0.001 uncorrected in regions predicted a priori, including brain areas implicated in associative learning (OFC, amygdala, nucleus accumbens, ventral tegmental area) (O'Doherty et al., 2002a). In medial temporal sites involved in olfactory sensory processing (piriform cortex) and learning (hippocampus), the threshold was set atp < 0.005 uncorrected. Correction for multiple comparisons was performed across small volumes of interest (Worsley et al., 1996) by constructing binary anatomical masks of amygdala, nucleus accumbens, OFC (including medial, lateral, anterior, and posterior subdivisions), and piriform cortex to limit the effective search space. The statistical threshold used to report these activations was set atp < 0.05 corrected for small volumes of interest or, where indicated, for whole-brain volume. All voxels of interest are reported in Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) coordinate space.
RESULTS
Behavioral
Subjects responded more slowly to the avCS+Uface compared with the CS− during early phases of aversive learning, but this differential effect habituated later in the experiment (Fig.1A). The main effect across experimental blocks approached significance (n = 9 subjects; F = 3.52; df = 2; p = 0.054; one-way repeated-measures ANOVA). Post hoc pairedt tests revealed that RTs were significantly slower in the first compared with the second block (p < 0.05) and approached significance in a comparison of first and third blocks (p = 0.068). Similar differential responses were seen during appetitive learning in response to the appCS+U face (Fig. 1B). There was a significant main effect across the three sessions (n = 9 subjects; F = 3.69; df = 2;p < 0.05; repeated-measures ANOVA), and post hoc comparisons revealed the RTs were significantly more delayed in the first than the second block (p < 0.05). By comparison, there was no significant main difference of RTs across experimental blocks for neutral learning (n = 7 subjects; F = 3.21; df = 1.17; p = 0.114; repeated-measures ANOVA, Greenhouse-Geisser adjusted).
Neuroimaging
Valence-independent olfactory learning
Sustained responses. To highlight brain regions involved in olfactory learning, regardless of specific valence, we tested for regions showing a common response during both forms of learning based on the conjunction of (appCS+U − appCS−) and (avCS+U − avCS−). This analysis revealed confluent activations in OFC with distinct peaks that encompassed medial, anterior, posterior, and lateral orbital gyri (Fig. 2A, Table1). The most significant peaks were in the right rostral OFC [medial: x = 14,y = 46, z = −18; Z = 5.85; and anterior: 28, 46, −6; Z = 4.79; bothp < 0.05 corrected for whole-brain volume), although activations were also detected on the left (medial: −18, 46, −12;Z = 3.45; p < 0.001 uncorrected; and anterior: −28, 50, −12; Z = 4.36; p< 0.05 small-volume corrected (SVC) over the region of interest]. Learning-evoked responses in bilateral centroposterior OFC (−24, 30, −12; Z = 4.41; p < 0.05 SVC; and 22, 32, −16; Z = 3.65; p = 0.052 SVC) closely overlapped caudal OFC regions defined previously in our companion study (Gottfried et al., 2002) as “secondary olfactory” cortex (at 20, 30, −20, and −24, 34, −16) (Fig.2B). Significant effects were also observed in lateral orbital gyri (right: 34, 36, −10; Z = 4.38; and 38, 34, −12; Z = 4.28; left: −30, 54, −10;Z = 4.41; and −34, 46, −12; Z = 3.87;p < 0.05 SVC). Outside of OFC, the only other neural responses elicited by the learning conjunction were in retrosplenial cortex, which closely bordered significance (0, −48, 10;Z = 4.75; p = 0.051 whole-brain corrected).
Table 1.
Brain region | MNI coordinates (mm) | Peak Z | p value | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
x | y | z | |||
Main effect (conjunction) | |||||
Right medial rostral OFC | 14 | 46 | −18 | 5.85 | <0.051-160 |
Left medial rostral OFC | −18 | 46 | −12 | 3.45 | <0.001 |
Right anterior OFC | 28 | 46 | −6 | 4.79 | <0.051-160 |
Left anterior OFC | −28 | 50 | −12 | 4.36 | <0.05* |
Right centroposterior OFC | 22 | 32 | −16 | 3.65 | 0.052* |
Left centroposterior OFC | −24 | 30 | −12 | 4.41 | <0.05* |
Right lateral OFC | 34 | 36 | −10 | 4.38 | <0.05* |
38 | 34 | −12 | 4.28 | <0.05* | |
Left lateral OFC | −30 | 54 | −10 | 4.41 | <0.05* |
−34 | 46 | −12 | 3.87 | <0.05* | |
Retrosplenial cortex | 0 | −48 | 10 | 4.75 | 0.051* |
Condition × time interaction (conjunction): time-dependent increases | |||||
Right ventromedial PFC | 4 | 34 | −14 | 3.20 | <0.001 |
Right antero-lateral OFC | 32 | 46 | −12 | 3.17 | <0.001 |
Left cerebellar hemisphere | −12 | −40 | −46 | 4.81 | <0.051-160 |
Corrected for small volume of interest;
F1-160: corrected for whole-brain volume.
Time-dependent responses. We also performed conjunctions between condition × time interactions [namely, (appCS+U × time − appCS− × time) and (avCS+U × time − avCS− × time)] to detect time-dependent changes in neural activity common to appetitive and aversive learning. Common learning-related activations that showed progressive increases over time (response potentiation) were evident in ventromedial prefrontal cortex (PFC) (4, 34, −14; Z = 3.20; p < 0.001 uncorrected), anterolateral OFC (32, 46, −12; Z = 3.17; p < 0.001 uncorrected), and cerebellum (−12, −40, −46; Z = 4.81; p < 0.05 whole-brain corrected) (Table 1). No significant time-dependent decreases were found in this analysis.
Appetitive olfactory learning
Sustained responses. We next tested for the main effect of appetitive learning in the contrast (appCS+U− appCS−), which showed several activation foci in medial OFC (18, 44, −16; Z = 4.03; p < 0.05 SVC; 16, 50, −10; Z = 3.80; p < 0.001 uncorrected; and 10, 36, −14; Z = 3.56;p < 0.001 uncorrected) (Fig.3A). These responses extended into gyrus rectus and medial PFC. An additional focus of activation was observed in anterior OFC (30, 42, −10; Z = 3.60;p < 0.001 uncorrected; p = 0.058 SVC). The direct contrast (appCS+U − avCS+U) demonstrated bilateral activity in ventral striatum (Fig. 3B). Neural activations in the right hemisphere bordered the ventral caudate head (14, 6, 2;Z = 3.12; p < 0.001 uncorrected), whereas left-sided responses were localized to nucleus accumbens (−8, 4, −4; Z = 3.07; p = 0.051 SVC). Bilateral responses were observed in dorsomedial areas of posterior amygdala, bounded superiorly and posteriorly by ventral pallidum (left: −20, −14, −12; Z = 3.37; p < 0.001 uncorrected; and right: 16, −10, −12; Z = 2.94;p < 0.005 uncorrected), and also in a more ventral region of the right medial temporal lobe (14, −12, −22; Z = 3.07; p < 0.005 uncorrected) that extended anteriorly into amygdala (Fig. 3C, Table2).
Table 2.
Brain region | MNI coordinates (mm) | Peak Z | p value | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
x | y | z | |||
(appCS+U − appCS−) | |||||
Right medial OFC | 18 | 44 | −16 | 4.03 | <0.05* |
16 | 50 | −10 | 3.80 | <0.001 | |
10 | 36 | −14 | 3.56 | <0.001 | |
Right anterior OFC | 30 | 42 | −10 | 3.60 | 0.058* |
(appCS+U − avCS+U) | |||||
Right ventral striatum | 14 | 6 | 2 | 3.12 | <0.001 |
Left nucleus accumbens | −8 | 4 | −4 | 3.07 | <0.051* |
Left pallidum/dorsomedial amygdala | −20 | −14 | −12 | 3.37 | <0.001 |
Right uncus/ventromedial amygdala | 14 | −12 | −22 | 3.07 | <0.005 |
Right dorsomedial amygdala | 16 | −10 | −12 | 2.94 | <0.005 |
(appCS+U − appCS−) × time: time-dependent increases | |||||
Right nucleus accumbens | 14 | 16 | −12 | 3.82 | <0.05* |
Right ventromedial PFC | 4 | 34 | −16 | 3.24 | <0.001 |
Right cerebellar hemisphere | 26 | −46 | −34 | 5.06 | <0.052-160 |
(appCS+U − avCS+U) × time: time-dependent decreases | |||||
Left anterior hippocampus | −32 | −14 | −16 | 3.34 | <0.001 |
Right anterior hippocampus | 36 | −14 | −20 | 3.12 | <0.001 |
Left uncus/ventromedial amygdala | −16 | −12 | −24 | 3.05 | <0.005 |
Corrected for small volume of interest;
F2-160: corrected for whole-brain volume.
Time-dependent responses. Time-dependent modulations of neural activity specific to appetitive learning were examined using condition × time interactions [that is, (appCS+U × time) relative to (appCS− × time)]. Learning-related increases (response potentiation) were evident in nucleus accumbens (14, 16, −12; Z = 3.82;p < 0.05 SVC) (Fig. 4) and ventromedial PFC (4, 34, −16; Z = 3.24;p < 0.001 uncorrected). Activations in right cerebellar hemisphere were also elicited (26, −46, −34;Z = 5.06; p < 0.05 whole-brain corrected). A direct comparison between (appCS+U× time) and (avCS+U × time) revealed significant habituating responses in bilateral anterior hippocampus (right: −36, −14, −20; Z = 3.12; p< 0.001 uncorrected; and left: −32, −14, −16; Z = 3.07; p < 0.005 uncorrected) and in a region of right medial temporal lobe (−16, −12, −24; Z = 3.05;p < 0.005 uncorrected) that was contiguous with posterior ventromedial amygdala (at y = −10) (Table2).
Aversive olfactory learning
Sustained responses. By testing the contrast (avCS+U − avCS−), we determined neural activity evoked by aversive olfactory learning. This revealed significant activations in rostral OFC with peaks in left lateral (−32, 52, −12;Z = 4.19; p < 0.05 SVC) (Fig.5A) and right medial (14, 46, −18; Z = 3.88; p < 0.001 uncorrected) orbital gyri. No significant responses were observed in the direct comparison of avCS+U and appCS+U conditions.
Time-dependent responses. Time-dependent neural responses during aversive learning were examined by contrasting condition × time interactions between avCS+U and its baseline (avCS−). Time-dependent increases in activity (response potentiation) were observed in left OFC (−18, 22, −14; Z = 3.99;p = 0.058 SVC) and in left temporopolar cortex (−26, 6, −38; Z = 3.26; p < 0.001 uncorrected) (Fig. 5B,i, Table3). This latter response extended caudally into periamygdaloid and posterior (temporal) piriform cortices (y = 2). Similar response enhancements in anterior mesiotemporal structures (peak: −30, 6, −38;Z = 4.74; p < 0.001 uncorrected) were identified in the direct contrast of time-modulated aversive (avCS+U × time) and appetitive (appCS+U × time) learning conditions (Fig.5B,ii.). Conversely, the only brain region that showed a significant decline in activity over time (response habituation) was nucleus accumbens (−10, 16, −12; Z = 3.10;p < 0.05 SVC) (Fig. 5C). This habituating profile was opposite to the temporal pattern evoked by appetitive learning in nucleus accumbens, where neural activity increased progressively over time (Fig. 4). These divergent responses are depicted in Figure 6.
Table 3.
Brain region | MNI coordinates (mm) | Peak Z | p value | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
x | y | z | |||
(avCS+U− avCS−) | |||||
Left lateral OFC | −32 | 52 | −12 | 4.19 | <0.053-150 |
Right medial OFC | 14 | 46 | −18 | 3.88 | <0.001 |
(avCS+U − avCS−) × time: time-dependent decreases | |||||
Left nucleus accumbens | −10 | 16 | −12 | 3.10 | <0.053-150 |
(avCS+U − avCS−) × time: time-dependent increases | |||||
Left medial OFC | −18 | 22 | −14 | 3.99 | 0.0583-150 |
Left temporal pole/piriform cortex | −26 | 6 | −38 | 3.26 | <0.001 |
(avCS+U − appCS−) × time: time-dependent increases | |||||
Left temporal pole/piriform cortex | −30 | 6 | −38 | 4.74 | <0.001 |
F3-150: Corrected for small volume of interest.
DISCUSSION
Our results indicate that emotionally salient odors can drive associative learning processes. We identified regional and temporal dissociations between appetitive and aversive conditioning within several key areas implicated in studies of associative learning, including OFC, nucleus accumbens, and amygdala. In addition, the activation of brain areas previously found to participate in low-level odor processing, such as piriform cortex and caudal OFC, implies that these regions play an active role in the transfer of affective value between olfactory and visual modalities.
The behavioral data (Fig. 1) show that to the extent that conditioning occurs, it is mostly completed by the second block of trials. This temporal pattern resembles those obtained in previous imaging investigations using autonomic markers, whereby learning-based differences were expressed in the early phase of conditioning (Buchel et al., 1998; LaBar et al., 1998). In addition, these behavioral profiles mirror the time course (exponential function, one-quarter time constant) used to model learning-specific responses in the fMRI data. The fact that significant differential responses to the neutral (ntCS+U) stimulus were not seen supports the idea that successful olfactory learning is contingent on odor valence rather than the presence of odor per se. We should point out that the absence of a significant behavioral effect in the third block of trials suggests that other processes (e.g., fatigue, boredom) may be interfering with olfactory conditioning. By itself this does not invalidate the responses that are seen, but raises the possibility that other learning-specific responses might have been missed.
Neural responses common to both appetitive and aversive learning were evident in widespread regions of rostral and caudal OFC (Fig. 2). The extent of these activations indicates that OFC is critical to the learning of olfactory–visual associations, regardless of specific valence. These findings concur with animal studies implicating OFC in many higher-order aspects of olfactory learning (Schoenbaum and Eichenbaum, 1995; Critchley and Rolls, 1996; Schoenbaum et al., 1998). Although studies of human patients with orbitofrontal lesions have advanced the idea that OFC is essential to odor discrimination (Potter and Butters, 1980; Zatorre and Jones-Gotman, 1991), the impact of these lesions on odor learning has not been addressed. Likewise, although OFC is consistently detected in olfactory neuroimaging studies (Zald and Pardo, 2000), its role in more complex processes has received limited attention (O'Doherty et al., 2000; Savic et al., 2000; Dade et al., 2001, 2002; Royet et al., 2001). Our study provides evidence that higher-order olfactory computations, specifically those related to learning, are engaged by human OFC.
In our companion paper (Gottfried et al., 2002) on olfactory sensory processing, we showed that odor-evoked activity in OFC was restricted specifically to caudal areas, corresponding anatomically to secondary olfactory cortex in animals (Yarita et al., 1980; Carmichael et al., 1994). This closely overlapped with posterior OFC activations evoked by olfactory learning (Fig. 2B). In contrast, significant effects within rostral OFC were associated exclusively with olfactory learning (Fig. 2A) but not with sensory odor processing. On the basis of these observations, we argue that the learning-evoked responses in caudal OFC may reflect modulation of secondary olfactory cortex by higher-order centers. Such influences might help increase the gain in structures that are primarily involved in unimodal olfactory discrimination, thereby promoting more efficient processing of behaviorally salient odors. Conversely, higher-order operations pertaining to learning would be specifically executed in rostral OFC. This idea is bolstered by animal findings suggesting an anatomical hierarchy of orbitofrontal specialization, whereby caudal regions (such as olfactory OFC) converge on medial and anterior territories to permit more complex information processing (Carmichael and Price, 1996; Ongur and Price, 2000). In this regard, neural activity in anterior OFC has been observed in recent neuroimaging studies of olfactory working memory [at 32, 44, −18 (Dade et al., 2001)] and short- and long-term odor recognition [at 27, 44, −14, and 20, 48, −18 (Dade et al., 2002)], lending further credence to the notion that this region is an important functional component of human olfactory learning.
Recent neuroimaging studies indicate that the neural substrates of reward and punishment may be functionally compartmentalized within medial and lateral OFC, respectively (O'Doherty et al., 2001, 2003;Small et al., 2001). Moreover, our companion study (Gottfried et al., 2002) showed that these dissociations were preserved in response to pleasant and unpleasant odors. In the present context, evidence for medial–lateral distinctions is less compelling but exhibits a comparable trend. Thus, in the main effect of appetitive olfactory learning, multiple activation foci were elicited in medial OFC (Fig.3A), whereas responses in lateral OFC were not observed. By comparison, the main effect of aversive learning was associated with neural responses in lateral OFC (Fig. 5A). Although rostromedial OFC was also identified in this contrast, the fact that this area emerged both in the main effect of appetitive learning and in the conjunction analysis suggests that it is probably more responsive to nonpunishing aspects of olfactory conditioning.
We found neural responses in nucleus accumbens in the direct contrast between appCS+U and avCS+U(Fig. 3B), implying that these activations were specifically related to the learning of an olfactory reward. In addition, this differential activity progressively increased with successive appCS+U presentations (Figs. 4, 6). Although human neuroimaging studies confirm that nucleus accumbens is modulated by reward-based stimuli (Delgado et al., 2000; O'Doherty et al., 2002;Pagnoni et al., 2002), we believe that our findings are the first to demonstrate that odors drive conditioned reward responses within human mesolimbic structures.
Although nucleus accumbens is a major target of midbrain dopaminergic afferents (Kalivas and Nakamura, 1999), the characterization of dopamine-related effects on evoked BOLD activity is unclear. Nevertheless, a survey of the literature suggests parallels between our data and animal studies of odor learning. For example, single-unit accumbens activity increased in male rats conditioned to associate novel smells with sexually receptive females (West et al., 1992). Similarly, rats administered intra-accumbens cocaine concurrently in the presence of neutral odors developed conditioned place preference for these stimuli (Barr and Wang, 1992).
In primate electrophysiological studies, the output signals of dopamine neurons in the ventral tegmentum are proposed to transmit reward-prediction errors to facilitate appetitive learning (Schultz, 1998). Recent human neuroimaging studies of taste-reward anticipation are broadly consistent with this proposal (O'Doherty et al., 2002;Pagnoni et al., 2002). Interestingly, the learning curves generated by temporal-difference models of the dopamine signal (Schultz, 1998) are reminiscent of the temporal profiles observed here in response to olfactory appetitive learning, although our study was not specifically devised to test such hypotheses. On the other hand, conflicting data from lesion, pharmacological, and in vivo microdialysis studies indicate that the animal literature is far from resolved, and whether our findings are ultimately compatible with reward-prediction error or other models (Berridge and Robinson, 1998; Horvitz, 2000) warrants further investigation.
Although neural activity in nucleus accumbens progressively increased in response to appCS+U, an opposite (decreasing) pattern was seen in response to avCS+U (Figs.5C, 6). Thus, it appears that the valence of olfactory learning evokes divergent temporal profiles in the same structure. That nucleus accumbens is differentially responsive to positive and negative modes of learning is supported by accumulating evidence highlighting a role for mesolimbic dopamine in aversive processing (Horvitz, 2000). Relevant to our results is the demonstration that dopamine release in ventral striatum decreases as a result of conditioned odor aversion (Abercrombie et al., 1989; Doherty and Gratton, 1992; Besson and Louilot, 1995). In addition, microdialysis studies performed in rodentsin vivo indicate that accumbens dopamine increases or decreases in response to rewarding or aversive stimuli, respectively (Di Chiara et al., 1999). However, further work is needed to establish how synaptic dopamine levels relate to fMRI signal change.
Discrete subregions of posterior amygdala were associated exclusively with appetitive learning (Fig. 3C). Responses in dorsomedial areas were sustained over the course of the experiment, whereas ventromedial regions exhibited both sustained and habituating response patterns. The demonstration of different activation loci within the amygdala may reflect functional segregation, particularly with respect to habituating and nonhabituating profiles, as shown previously (Morris et al., 2001). Although current fMRI techniques lack sufficient spatial resolution to discern individual amygdala subnuclei, the dorsomedial activation may correspond to the central nucleus of the amygdala (Mai et al., 1997), an area linked to appetitive Pavlovian conditioning in animal lesion studies (Gallagher et al., 1990;Parkinson et al., 2000). In addition, this same region closely overlaps activations evoked by anticipation of pleasant taste (O'Doherty et al., 2002). This anatomical convergence, based on different types of natural rewards, implicates a broad role for posterior dorsal amygdala in appetitive processing. More generally, our data extend recent findings from human studies demonstrating the importance of amygdala to various reward-related learning processes (Hamann et al., 1999;Johnsrude et al., 2000).
We found no significant amygdala activity during aversive olfactory learning or in anticipation of aversive tastes (O'Doherty et al., 2002). These findings are intriguing given its reliable involvement in animal (LeDoux, 2000) and human (Buchel et al., 1998; LaBar et al., 1998) studies of fear conditioning. Indeed, one might have predicted the reverse pattern of results, in which the amygdala was a selective participant in aversive, rather than appetitive, learning. As Cahill and McGaugh (1990) have elegantly shown with aversive taste reinforcers, it is possible that the use of unpleasant odors simply might not be arousing enough to engage amygdala-dependent conditioning (although one might expect this to apply to the pleasant odor as well). Alternately, qualitative differences in the aversive UCS may elicit variable activation patterns. Loud sounds or electric shocks are more likely to produce affective responses approximating fear or startle, shown in diverse species to be potent activators of amygdala (LeDoux, 2000), whereas unpleasant odors may provoke reactions closer to disgust. To this end, faces expressing disgust (as opposed to fear) seem to activate amygdala relatively poorly (Phillips et al., 1997) and may help explain why aversive olfactory learning did not involve this structure.
A remaining possibility is that functional disparities may reflect a unique organization of olfaction, whereby aversive learning would be implemented by other brain regions. Potential candidates include areas of temporal piriform and periamygdaloid cortices, which showed increasing neural activations over time in the main effect of avCS+U (relative to avCS−) and in the direct comparison with appCS+U (Fig. 5B). The idea that primary olfactory cortex helps establish salient cross-modal associations is complementary to animal studies indicating that piriform cortex supports learning-related processes (Schoenbaum and Eichenbaum, 1995; Saar et al., 1999; Datiche et al., 2001). Indeed, the detection of learning-evoked activations in both human primary (piriform) and secondary (caudal OFC) olfactory projection sites suggests that these areas should not be considered simply as unimodal sensory relays but as active participants in more complex aspects of odor information processing.
Footnotes
This research was supported by a Physician Postdoctoral Fellowship from the Howard Hughes Medical Institute (J.A.G.) and by a Wellcome Trust Programme Grant (R.J.D.). We thank P. Aston, E. Featherstone, O. Josephs, and J. S. Winston, as well as the radiology staff of the Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience.
Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Jay A. Gottfried, Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, Functional Imaging Laboratory, 12 Queen Square, London WC1N 3BG, UK. E-mail:j.gottfried@fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk.
REFERENCES
- 1.Abercrombie ED, Keefe KA, DiFrischia DS, Zigmond MJ. Differential effect of stress on in vivo dopamine release in striatum, nucleus accumbens, and medial frontal cortex. J Neurochem. 1989;52:1655–1658. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.1989.tb09224.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Baron RA. Perfume as a tactic of impression management in social and organizational settings. In: Van Toller S, Dodd GG, editors. Perfumery: the psychology and biology of fragrance. Chapman and Hall; London: 1988. pp. 91–104. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Barr GA, Wang S. Peripheral and central administration of cocaine produce conditioned odor preferences in the infant rat. Brain Res. 1992;599:181–185. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(92)90389-q. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Beauchamp GK, Maller O. The development of flavor preferences in humans: a review. In: Kare MR, Maller O, editors. The chemical senses and nutrition. Academic; New York: 1977. pp. 291–311. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Berridge KC, Robinson TE. What is the role of dopamine in reward: hedonic impact, reward learning, or incentive salience? Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 1998;28:309–369. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(98)00019-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Besson C, Louilot A. Asymmetrical involvement of mesolimbic dopaminergic neurons in affective perception. Neuroscience. 1995;68:963–968. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(95)00255-h. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Buchel C, Morris J, Dolan RJ, Friston KJ. Brain systems mediating aversive conditioning: an event-related fMRI study. Neuron. 1998;20:947–957. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80476-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Cahill L, McGaugh JL. Amygdaloid complex lesions differentially affect retention of tasks using appetitive and aversive reinforcement. Behav Neurosci. 1990;104:532–543. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.104.4.532. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Carde RT, Mafra-Neto A. Mechanisms of flight of male moths to pheromone. In: Carde RT, Minks AK, editors. Insect pheromone research: new directions. Chapman and Hall; New York: 1997. pp. 275–290. [Google Scholar]
- 10.Carmichael ST, Price JL. Connectional networks within the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex of macaque monkeys. J Comp Neurol. 1996;371:179–207. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19960722)371:2<179::AID-CNE1>3.0.CO;2-#. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Carmichael ST, Clugnet MC, Price JL. Central olfactory connections in the macaque monkey. J Comp Neurol. 1994;346:403–434. doi: 10.1002/cne.903460306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Cole AD, Bond NW. Olfactory aversion conditioning and overeating: a review and some data. Percept Mot Skills. 1983;57:667–678. doi: 10.2466/pms.1983.57.2.667. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Critchley HD, Rolls ET. Olfactory neuronal responses in the primate orbitofrontal cortex: analysis in an olfactory discrimination task. J Neurophysiol. 1996;75:1659–1672. doi: 10.1152/jn.1996.75.4.1659. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Critchley HD, Mathias CJ, Dolan RJ. Fear conditioning in humans: the influence of awareness and autonomic arousal on functional neuroanatomy. Neuron. 2002;33:653–663. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00588-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Dade LA, Zatorre RJ, Evans AC, Jones-Gotman M. Working memory in another dimension: functional imaging of human olfactory working memory. NeuroImage. 2001;14:650–660. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0868. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Dade LA, Zatorre RJ, Jones-Gotman M. Olfactory learning: convergent findings from lesion and brain imaging studies in humans. Brain. 2002;125:86–101. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Datiche F, Roullet F, Cattarelli M. Expression of Fos in the piriform cortex after acquisition of olfactory learning: an immunohistochemical study in the rat. Brain Res Bull. 2001;55:95–99. doi: 10.1016/s0361-9230(01)00499-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Deichmann R, Turner R. Improvement of local BOLD sensitivities in the presence of susceptibility gradients by using tilted slices. Proc Int Soc Magn Reson Med. 2002;10:1414. [Google Scholar]
- 19.Delgado MR, Nystrom LE, Fissell C, Noll DC, Fiez JA. Tracking the hemodynamic responses to reward and punishment in the striatum. J Neurophysiol. 2000;84:3072–3077. doi: 10.1152/jn.2000.84.6.3072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Di Chiara G, Loddo P, Tanda G. Reciprocal changes in prefrontal and limbic dopamine responsiveness to aversive and rewarding stimuli after chronic mild stress: implications for the psychobiology of depression. Biol Psychiatry. 1999;46:1624–1633. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(99)00236-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Doherty MD, Gratton A. High-speed chronoamperometric measurements of mesolimbic and nigrostriatal dopamine release associated with repeated daily stress. Brain Res. 1992;586:295–302. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(92)91639-v. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Friston KJ, Ashburner J, Frith CD, Poline J-B, Heather JD, Frackowiak RSJ. Spatial registration and normalization of images. Hum Brain Mapp. 1995a;2:165–189. [Google Scholar]
- 23.Friston KJ, Holmes AP, Worsley KJ, Poline J-P, Frith CD, Frackowiak RSJ. Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: a general linear approach. Hum Brain Mapp. 1995b;2:189–210. [Google Scholar]
- 24.Frohwirth RA, Foreyt JP. Aversive conditioning treatment of overweight. Behav Res. 1978;9:861–872. [Google Scholar]
- 25.Gallagher M, Graham PW, Holland PC. The amygdala central nucleus and appetitive Pavlovian conditioning: lesions impair one class of conditioned behavior. J Neurosci. 1990;10:1906–1911. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.10-06-01906.1990. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Gottfried JA, Deichmann R, Winston JS, Dolan RJ. Functional heterogeneity in human olfactory cortex: an event-related fMRI study. J Neurosci. 2002;22:10819–10828. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-24-10819.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Hamann SB, Ely TD, Grafton ST, Kilts CD. Amygdala activity related to enhanced memory for pleasant and aversive stimuli. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2:289–293. doi: 10.1038/6404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Hermann C, Ziegler S, Birbaumer N, Flor H. Pavlovian aversive and appetitive odor conditioning in humans: subjective, peripheral, and electrocortical changes. Exp Brain Res. 2000;132:203–215. doi: 10.1007/s002210000343. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Horvitz JC. Mesolimbocortical and nigrostriatal dopamine responses to salient non-reward events. Neuroscience. 2000;96:651–656. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(00)00019-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Johnsrude IS, Owen AM, White NM, Xhao WV, Bohbot V. Impaired preference conditioning after anterior temporal lobe resection in humans. J Neurosci. 2000;20:2649–2656. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-07-02649.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Kalivas PW, Nakamura M. Neural systems for behavioral activation and reward. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1999;9:223–227. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(99)80031-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.LaBar KS, Gatenby JC, Gore JC, LeDoux JE, Phelps EA. Human amygdala activation during conditioned fear acquisition and extinction: a mixed-trial fMRI study. Neuron. 1998;20:937–945. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80475-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.LeDoux JE. Emotion circuits in the brain. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2000;23:155–184. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Lorig TS, Elmes DG, Zald DH, Pardo JV. A computer-controlled olfactometer for fMRI and electrophysiological studies of olfaction. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput. 1999;31:370–375. doi: 10.3758/bf03207734. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.MacFarlane A. Olfaction in the development of social preferences in the human neonate. Ciba Found Symp. 1975;33:103–117. doi: 10.1002/9780470720158.ch7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Mai JK, Assheuer J, Paxinos G. Atlas of the human brain. Academic; San Diego: 1997. [Google Scholar]
- 37.Morris JS, Buchel C, Dolan RJ. Parallel neural responses in amygdala subregions and sensory cortex during implicit fear conditioning. NeuroImage. 2001;13:1044–1052. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0721. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.O'Doherty J, Rolls ET, Francis S, Bowtell R, McGlone F, Kobal G, Renner B, Ahne G. Sensory-specific satiety-related olfactory activation of the human orbitofrontal cortex. NeuroReport. 2000;11:893–897. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200003200-00046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.O'Doherty J, Kringelbach ML, Rolls ET, Hornak J, Andrews C. Abstract reward and punishment representations in the human orbitofrontal cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2001;4:95–102. doi: 10.1038/82959. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.O'Doherty J, Deichmann R, Critchley HD, Dolan RJ. Neural responses during anticipation of a primary taste reward. Neuron. 2002;33:815–826. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00603-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.O'Doherty J, Winston JS, Critchley H, Perrett D, Burt DM, Dolan RJ. Beauty in a smile: the role of medial orbitofrontal cortex in facial attractiveness. Neuropsychologia. 2003;41:147–155. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00145-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Ongur D, Price JL. The organization of networks within the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex of rats, monkeys and humans. Cereb Cortex. 2000;10:206–219. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.3.206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Pagnoni G, Zink CF, Montague PR, Berns GS. Activity in human ventral striatum locked to errors of reward prediction. Nat Neurosci. 2002;5:97–98. doi: 10.1038/nn802. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Parkinson JA, Robbins TW, Everitt BJ. Dissociable roles of the central and basolateral amygdala in appetitive emotional learning. Eur J Neurosci. 2000;12:405–413. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2000.00960.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Phillips ML, Young AW, Senior C, Brammer M, Andrew C, Calder AJ, Bullmore ET, Perrett DI, Rowland D, Williams SC, Gray JA, David AS. A specific neural substrate for perceiving facial expressions of disgust. Nature. 1997;389:495–498. doi: 10.1038/39051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Potter H, Butters N. An assessment of olfactory deficits in patients with damage to prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychologia. 1980;18:621–628. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(80)90101-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Price CJ, Friston KJ. Cognitive conjunction: a new approach to brain activation experiments. NeuroImage. 1997;5:261–270. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1997.0269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Royet JP, Hudry J, Zald DH, Godinot D, Gregoire MC, Lavenne F, Costes N, Holley A. Functional neuroanatomy of different olfactory judgments. NeuroImage. 2001;13:506–519. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0704. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Saar D, Grossman Y, Barkai E. Reduced synaptic facilitation between pyramidal neurons in the piriform cortex after odor learning. J Neurosci. 1999;19:8616–8622. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-19-08616.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Savic I, Gulyas B, Larsson M, Roland P. Olfactory functions are mediated by parallel and hierarchical processing. Neuron. 2000;26:735–745. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)81209-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Schiffman SS. Physicochemical correlates of olfactory quality. Science. 1974;185:112–117. doi: 10.1126/science.185.4146.112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Schoenbaum G, Eichenbaum H. Information coding in the rodent prefrontal cortex. I. Single-neuron activity in orbitofrontal cortex compared with that in pyriform cortex. J Neurophysiol. 1995;74:733–750. doi: 10.1152/jn.1995.74.2.733. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Schoenbaum G, Chiba AA, Gallagher M. Orbitofrontal cortex and basolateral amygdala encode expected outcomes during learning. Nat Neurosci. 1998;1:155–159. doi: 10.1038/407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Schultz W. Predictive reward signal of dopamine neurons. J Neurophysiol. 1998;80:1–27. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.80.1.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Small DM, Zatorre RJ, Dagher A, Evans AC, Jones-Gotman M. Changes in brain activity related to eating chocolate: from pleasure to aversion. Brain. 2001;124:1720–1733. doi: 10.1093/brain/124.9.1720. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Stern K, McClintock MK. Regulation of ovulation by human pheromones. Nature. 1998;392:177–179. doi: 10.1038/32408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Todrank J, Byrnes D, Wrzesniewski A, Rozin P. Odors can change preferences for people in photographs: a cross-modal evaluative conditioning study with olfactory USs and visual CSs. Learn Motiv. 1995;26:116–140. [Google Scholar]
- 58.West CH, Clancy AN, Michael RP. Enhanced responses of nucleus accumbens neurons in male rats to novel odors associated with sexually receptive females. Brain Res. 1992;585:49–55. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(92)91189-l. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Worsley KJ, Marrett S, Neelin P, Vandal AC, Friston KJ, Evans AC. A unified statistical approach for determining significant voxels in images of cerebral activation. Hum Brain Mapp. 1996;4:58–73. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1996)4:1<58::AID-HBM4>3.0.CO;2-O. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Yarita H, Iino M, Tanabe T, Kogure S, Takagi SF. A transthalamic olfactory pathway to orbitofrontal cortex in the monkey. J Neurophysiol. 1980;43:69–85. doi: 10.1152/jn.1980.43.1.69. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Zald DH, Pardo JV. Functional neuroimaging of the olfactory system in humans. Int J Psychophysiol. 2000;36:165–181. doi: 10.1016/s0167-8760(99)00110-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Zatorre RJ, Jones-Gotman M. Human olfactory discrimination after unilateral frontal or temporal lobectomy. Brain. 1991;114:71–84. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]