Abstract
We provide the first evidence that prolactin is a neuromodulator of behavioral and neuroendocrine stress coping in the rat. In virgin female and male rats, intracerebral infusion of ovine prolactin (oPRL) into the lateral cerebral ventricle (intracerebroventricular) exerted an anxiolytic effect on the elevated plus-maze in a dose-dependent manner (0.1 and 1.0 μg/5 μl; p< 0.01). In contrast, downregulation of the expression of the long form of brain prolactin receptors by chronic intracerebroventricular infusion of an antisense oligodeoxynucleotide (ODN) (osmotic minipump, 0.5 μg · 0.5 μl−1 · hr−1; 5 d) increased anxiety-related behavior on the plus-maze compared with mixed bases-treated and vehicle-treated rats (p < 0.01), again demonstrating an anxiolytic effect of PRL acting at brain level. Furthermore, in jugular vein-catheterized female rats, the stress-induced increase of corticotropin secretion was decreased after chronic intracerebroventricular infusion of oPRL (osmotic minipump, 1.0 μg · 0.5 μl−1 · hr−1;p < 0.05) and, in contrast, was further elevated by antisense targeting of the brain prolactin receptors (p < 0.01). This provides evidence for a receptor-mediated attenuation of the responsiveness of the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis by prolactin. The antisense ODN sequence was selected on the basis of secondary structure molecular modeling of the target mRNA to improve antisense ODN–mRNA hybridization. Receptor autoradiography confirmed the expected improvement in the efficacy of downregulation of prolactin receptor expression [empirically designed antisense, 30%; p > 0.05, not significant; adjustment of target position after mRNA modeling, 72%; p < 0.05). Taken together, prolactin acting at brain level has to be considered as a novel regulator of both emotionality and HPA axis reactivity.
Keywords: ACTH, anxiety, choroid plexus, plus-maze, HPA axis, mRNA secondary structure
Anxiety is a complex trait that has evolved to help animals evaluate and circumvent potentially dangerous situations. The neurobiological basis of anxiety-related behavior involves various neuropeptidergic systems, including corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH) (Mitchell, 1999), neuropeptide Y (Wahlestedt et al., 1993), substance P (Kramer et al., 1998), and vasopressin (Liebsch et al., 1996). Under pathological conditions of hyperanxiety (Holsboer and Barden, 1996) or in rats displaying high innate anxiety-related behavior (Landgraf et al., 1999), an aberrant hyperactivity of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis has been described, indicating a close interaction between emotionality and neuroendocrine systems.
Prolactin (PRL), in addition to its role during lactation, may influence both emotional responses and HPA axis activity. On the basis of different techniques, this neuropeptide has been shown to participate in the regulation of maternal behavior (Bridges et al., 1990; Lucas et al., 1998), grooming (Drago et al., 1983), and food intake in mammalian (Noel and Woodside, 1993) and nonmammalian species (Li et al., 1995). A possible involvement of PRL in stress response mechanisms is suggested by the findings that (1) PRL is released into the blood from pituitary lactotroph cells in response to exposure to different stressors (Neill, 1970; Seggie and Brown, 1975), (2) chronic stress induced the expression of the long form of the PRL receptors (PRL-R) in choroid plexus cells (Fujikawa et al., 1995), and (3) administration of PRL into the cerebral ventricles prevented the stress-induced formation of gastric ulcers and showed antidepressant effects during forced swimming (Drago et al., 1985, 1990). Furthermore, neuroendocrine stress responses were found to be attenuated in states of hyperprolactinemia (Schlein et al., 1974; Carter and Lightman, 1987). However, evidence for a receptor-mediated regulation of behavioral and neuroendocrine stress coping by PRL is still missing, possibly because of the lack of a specific PRL-R antagonist.
The PRL gene is expressed in several hypothalamic areas (DeVito et al., 1992; Emanuele et al., 1992), including the paraventricular nucleus (PVN) (Clapp et al., 1994). PRL-like immunoreactive fibers innervate, among others, the parvocellular portion of the PVN (Paut-Pagano et al., 1993) in which neurons synthesizing the adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) secretagogue CRH are located (Merchenthaler et al., 1982). PRL-like immunoreactive fibers have also been found in extrahypothalamic limbic brain regions, such as the bed nucleus of stria terminalis and the amygdala (Paut-Pagano et al., 1993), which are known to be critically involved in the regulation of emotionality (Charney et al., 1998). Both the long and short forms of the PRL-R have been mapped in these brain areas (Chiu and Wise, 1994;Bakowska and Morrell, 1997). However, the highest density of PRL-R, with predominance of the long form, exists in choroid plexus cells (Crumeyrolle-Arias et al., 1993; Bakowska and Morrell, 1997) in which active uptake of blood PRL into the brain takes place (Walsh et al., 1987), raising the possibility of a coordinated action in the brain of both peripheral and brain PRL.
In this study, to reveal receptor-mediated effects of PRL on anxiety-related behavior and on basal as well as stress-induced HPA axis activity in rats, we used complementary methodological approaches, including acute and chronic intracerebroventricular or intravenous infusion of ovine PRL (oPRL) into the lateral cerebral ventricle. Furthermore, because of the lack of a selective PRL-R antagonist, antisense oligodeoxynucleotides (ODNs) targeting the PRL-R mRNA (long form) were administered intracerebroventricularly to efficiently and transiently downregulate intracerebral receptor expression. Improved effectiveness of an antisense ODN sequence based on secondary structure prediction (Ho et al., 1998; Cairns et al., 1999) of the PRL-R mRNA molecule was confirmed for the first time in vivo.
Preliminary results of this study have been published previously in abstract form (Torner and Neumann, 1998; Torner et al., 1999).
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Virgin female (250–280 gm body weight) and male (280–300 gm) Wistar rats were maintained under standard laboratory conditions. Surgical procedures were performed under halothane anesthesia, after which animals were housed individually and handled daily to reduce nonspecific stress responses on the experimental day. Behavioral and neuroendocrine studies were performed between 9:00 A.M. and 12:00 P.M., at least 4 d after surgery. All experimental procedures were approved by the local government of Bavaria, Germany.
Acute intracerebroventricular oPRL administration. A guide cannula (21 ga) was stereotaxically cemented above the right lateral ventricle (0.8 mm behind bregma, 1.6 mm lateral, 1.8 mm below the skull) (Paxinos and Watson, 1986) of virgin female and male rats. Five days after surgery, rats were administered oPRL (0.1 or 1 μg/5 μl, i.c.v.; Sigma-Aldrich, Steinheim, Germany) or vehicle (5 μl of Ringer's solution, pH 7.4) under stress-free conditions via an intracerebroventricular infusion cannula (25 ga) inserted into the guide cannula 2 hr earlier and attached to the infusion microsyringe via 40-cm-long polyethylene tubings to allow remote injection into the freely moving, mostly sleeping animals over 30 sec. Ten minutes after the injection, rats were subjected to the elevated plus-maze (EPM; day 1), forced swim (day 3), and/or open-field test (day 7).
Acute intravenous oPRL administration. Virgin female rats were implanted with a chronic jugular vein catheter as described previously (Neumann et al., 1998). Five days thereafter, the catheter was attached to an infusion line, and oPRL (5 mg · kg−1 · 0.5 ml−1) or vehicle (0.5 ml/kg) was infused intravenously. After 20 min, the animals were tested on the EPM.
Acute intraperitoneal diazepam administration. To compare the anxiolytic effect of PRL with that of an established anxiolytic drug (Fernandez-Guasti and Picazo, 1997; Liebsch et al., 1998), we injected virgin female and male rats with either diazepam (1 mg/kg, i.p.) or vehicle (1 ml/kg) 30 min before exposure to the EPM and behavioral recordings.
Chronic intracerebroventricular oPRL or PRL-R antisense ODN administration. Osmotic minipumps (model 1007D; Alzet Corp., Palo Alto, CA) filled with oPRL (1 μg · 0.5 μl−1 · hr−1), PRL-R antisense ODN (0.5 μg · 0.5 μl−1 · hr−1), mixed bases ODN (0.5 μg · 0.5 μl−1 · hr−1), or vehicle (Ringer's solution; 0.5 μl/hr) were connected to an intracerebroventricular infusion cannula positioned into the right lateral ventricle of virgin female rats 5 d before anxiety-related behavior was tested on the EPM.
Jugular vein catheter. To study the effect of acute or chronic intracerebroventricular oPRL treatment or chronic intracerebroventricular PRL-R antisense treatment on HPA axis reactivity, we also implanted rats with a jugular vein catheter filled with sterile saline (0.9%) containing gentamicin (30,000 IU/ml; Centravet, Bad Bentheim, Germany). After acute intracerebroventricular administration or 5 d of intracerebroventricular treatment, blood samples were taken under basal conditions and in response to an emotional stressor (EPM). Plasma ACTH was measured by radioimmunoassay using commercially available kits (ICN Biomedicals, Costa Mesa, CA).
Elevated plus-maze. The EPM test was used to assess the emotional state and as a mild emotional stressor (novel environment) in our experiments. This test is based on the creation of a conflict between the exploratory drive of the rat and its innate fear of open and exposed areas; it has been validated for the detection of emotional responses to anxiogenic and anxiolytic substances (Pellow et al., 1985). Thus, increased open-arm exploration indicates reduced anxiety-related behavior. The EPM consists of a plus-shaped platform elevated 70 cm above the floor. Two of the opposing arms (50 × 10 cm) are closed by 40-cm-high side and end walls, whereas the other two arms have no walls (open arms). At the beginning of the test, the rats were placed on the central area (10 × 10 cm) of the maze. The following parameters of anxiety-related behavior were recorded by means of a video camera–computer setup during the 5 min testing period: (1) entries into open arms (ratio of open-arm entries to total number of entries into all arms), (2) time spent on the open arms (ratio of time spent on open arms to total time spent on all arms), (3) overall activity (total number of entries into closed arms), and (4) number of full entries into the open arms (Liebsch et al., 1998).
Forced swim test. Forced swimming represents an ethologically relevant physical and emotional stressor for rats (Abel, 1994) and was used to assess active versus passive stress-coping strategies and to reveal antidepressive drug effects (Marti and Armario, 1993). Two days after exposure on the EPM, rats were forced to swim for 10 min, and their behavior was scored by a trainer–observer pressing preset PC keys. The following behaviors were noted: (1) time spent struggling, defined as strongly moving all four limbs with the front paws breaking the water surface or scratching the tank wall; (2) time spent swimming, defined as moving all four limbs, swimming around the tank, or diving; (3) time spent floating, defined as remaining immobile with only occasional slight movements to keep the body balanced and the nose above water; and (4) latency until first floating.
Open-field test. The open-field test has been used to assess the effect of intracerebroventricular PRL on the locomotor activity of the rats. During the 15 min exposure time in the gray plastic box (60 × 60 × 40 cm), the total path traveled was recorded by a video setup.
To determine the stage of the estrous cycle, cytological examinations from vaginal smears were monitored after completion of the respective experiments. Behavioral scores of animals differing in cycle stage showed no significant differences within the same experimental group. Thus, results were compiled and analyzed regardless of estrous cycle.
Antisense and mixed bases ODN sequences. All antisense ODNs were endcapped phosphorothioated (MWG-Biotech AG, Ebersberg, Germany). A mixture of two antisense (AS) ODNs (AS1; 5′-CAC TTC CTG CAA AGG ACG-3′; 5′-CTG CCA TAC CAG AGG CTG-3′) or mixed bases ODN (5′-ACA CGT CTC GAA GAG CGA-3′) was applied initially in behavioral studies. These antisense ODNs were complementary to nucleotides 62–79 and 82–99 (Fig. 1A) of the mRNA coding for the PRL-R (Shirota et al., 1990; GenBank accession no. M57668). Antisense ODN design was then improved on the basis of two-dimensional molecular modeling results (AS2; 5′-GTA AGT CAG TGA ATA ATT GG-3′; mixed bases ODN, 5′-AACA CGT CTC GAA GAG CGAA-3′). AS2 was complementary to nucleotides 354–373 of the mRNA coding for the PRL-R (Fig. 1B) and was used for all neuroendocrine and behavioral studies presented here.
Autoradiography. The efficiency of PRL-R antisense ODNs in downregulating brain PRL-R was analyzed by receptor autoradiography. Brains were collected 1 hr after stress exposure and immediately frozen until processing. Mounted serial coronal 20 μm cryostat sections were treated as described by Crumeyrolle-Arias et al. (1993), and all sections were processed in the same assay (eight sections per rat). Briefly, mounted sections were preincubated in Tris buffer (50 mm, 2 mmCaCl2, 5 mm KCl, pH 7.4), rinsed twice (Tris 50 mm), and air dried. Then sections were incubated with 0.6 nm125I-human growth hormone (DuPont NEN, Wilmington, DE) in Tris buffer (120 mm and 0.2% bovine serum albumin, pH 7.4, at 20°C, 1 hr), rinsed twice, dipped in deionized water, and air dried. Nonspecific binding was determined by adding 1 μm oPRL. Labeled slides were exposed to Biomax MR films (Kodak, Rochester, NY) in x-ray cassettes for 18 d. Quantitative assessment of autoradiograms was performed using image analysis software (OPTIMAS, Optimas Corp.; 10× magnification; 10 mm objective). Optical density values of autoradiograms (based on 256 grayscale units), averaged over the left or right choroid plexus (1.0 mm behind bregma), were converted into relative label concentrations by logarithmic transformation (natural logarithm), because the degree of darkening of the film is known to be logarithmically related to the amount of exposing label. Nonspecific binding values were subtracted from total binding after adjusting the backgrounds to similar values.
Statistical analysis. Values are expressed as the mean ± SEM. One-way ANOVA (behavioral data, autoradiography) or two-way ANOVA (factors time × treatment; ACTH) followed by Newman–Keuls test (females, EPM and ACTH data, autoradiography) or Fisher (males, EPM data) post hoc test were used as indicated (GB-Stat 6.0; Dynamic Microsystems, Silver Spring, MD). p ≤ 0.05 was considered statistically significant.
RESULTS
Behavioral effects of oPRL
Compared with vehicle-treated controls, intracerebroventricular oPRL resulted in a significant and dose-dependent decrease in anxiety-related behavior in both female and male rats on the EPM (Fig.2A,B). PRL affected the different measures of anxiety-related behavior, including the percentage of entries into [ANOVA, females,F(2,24) = 19.6; p < 0.0001; males, F(2,17) = 3.38;p = 0.05], the percentage of time on [females,F(2,24) = 8.14; p = 0.002; males, F(2,17) = 2.53;p = 0.10], and the number of full entries into [females, F(2,24) = 7.98;p = 0.002; males,F(2,17) = 2.08; p = 0.15] the open arms of the EPM.
Systemic (intravenous) administration of oPRL to female rats at doses resembling stress-induced plasma PRL concentrations (Neill, 1970) also resulted in reduced anxiety, because the percentage of entries into the open arms of the EPM was significantly increased [F(1,16) = 16.3; p = 0.001] (Fig. 2C).
For comparison of the anxiolytic effect of oPRL, we tested the established anxiolytic drug diazepam (1 mg/kg, i.p.). Diazepam significantly reduced anxiety-related behavior on the EPM in male (percentage of time on the open arms; p < 0.05), but not female rats compared with vehicle treatment (Fig.2A,B, insets).
After intracerebroventricular (1 μg; both males and females;p < 0.05) as well as intravenous (p < 0.05) oPRL treatment, the locomotor activity was decreased, as indicated by a reduced number of entries into the closed arms (Fig. 2, right). This finding was confirmed in the open-field test because oPRL reduced the distance traveled during the 15 min exposure [F(1,11) = 4.80; p = 0.035] (Table 1). In contrast, during the 10 min forced swimming, oPRL-treated rats showed a more active coping strategy as revealed by an increased time spent struggling [F(1,19) = 4.68;p = 0.043; males] and a prolonged latency to floating [F(1,14) = 4.69; p = 0.048; females] (Table 1).
Table 1.
Vehicle | Prolactin | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Female | Male | Female | Male | |
Forced swim | n = 8 | n = 10 | n = 8 | n = 11 |
Struggling (sec) | 108 ± 33.0 | 79.3 ± 9.9 | 126 ± 61.3 | 114 ± 12.6* |
Floating (sec) | 16.0 ± 15.5 | 12.2 ± 5.0 | 5.2 ± 3.2 | 6.8 ± 2.5 |
Latency (sec) | 224 ± 124 | 167 ± 50.6 | 416 ± 238* | 293 ± 58.9 |
Open field | n = 6 | n = 7 | ||
Distance (cm) | 5634 ± 261 | 4354 ± 395* |
Rats were treated with oPRL [0.1 μg/5μl (swim) or 1.0 μg/5 μl (open field), i.c.v.] or vehicle (5 μl of sterile saline) 10 min before testing as described in Results. During forced swimming, the time spent struggling and floating and the latency until first floating were recorded. In the open field, the total distance traveled was monitored. Data are the means ± SEM.
p < 0.05 versus vehicle.
Antisense ODN targeting of brain PRL-R: effects on anxiety-related behavior
Compared with vehicle-treated or mixed bases ODN-treated controls (which did not differ from each other), downregulation of PRL-R expression by antisense targeting significantly increased the anxiety-related behavior on the EPM in female rats, because both the percentage of entries into [ANOVA;F(2,18) = 13.2; p = 0.001] and the percentage of time on [F(2,18) = 6.50; p = 0.007] the open arms of the EPM were reduced (Fig.3). Also, the number of full entries into the open arms was reduced [F(2,18) = 3.18; p = 0.041), whereas locomotor activity was increased in PRL-R antisense-treated rats as indicated by an increased number of entries into the closed arms [F(2,18) = 4.52; p = 0.02].
Inhibition of neuroendocrine responses to stressors by PRL: involvement of brain PRL-R
After 5 d of chronic intracerebroventricular infusion of vehicle or oPRL, two-way ANOVA did not reveal significant differences in basal and stress-induced ACTH between groups [factor time,F(3,42) = 36.9; p < 0.0001; factor treatment, F(1,14) = 0.62; p = 0.44]. Separate statistics performed on basal hormone concentrations [one-way ANOVA;F(1,14) = 5.05; p = 0.04] as well as the stress-induced increase in hormone secretion as a percentage of basal levels [F(1,14) = 6.03; p = 0.02] revealed increased basal ACTH plasma concentrations and an attenuated stress-induced increase in ACTH secretion in oPRL-treated rats (Fig. 4A).
Downregulation of PRL-R expression by intracerebroventricular infusion of PRL-R antisense ODN significantly affected ACTH secretion [interaction time × treatment;F(6,57) = 2.81; p = 0.018]. Although basal ACTH plasma levels did not differ between groups, antisense ODN treatment further elevated stress-induced ACTH secretion, compared with vehicle-treated and mixed bases-treated rats, 5 and 15 min after stressor exposure (Fig. 4B).
Acute intracerebroventricular oPRL (0.1 and 1.0 μg/5 μl) failed to alter basal or stress-induced ACTH plasma levels compared with vehicle-treated controls [factor time,F(3,57) = 23.1; p < 0.0001; factor treatment, F(2,19) = 0.10; p = 0.9; data not shown].
Antisense ODN design and receptor autoradiography
Three different antisense ODNs were used in these studies; they differed in the position of the target sites within the mRNA molecule coding for the long form of the PRL-R (Fig. 1).
For comparative purposes, a mixture of two antisense ODNs (see Materials and Methods) directed against two target sites upstream of the initiation codon of the PRL-R mRNA (Fig. 1B) was used initially. This resulted in only a trend toward a reduction (29% vs vehicle and 39% vs mixed bases) in PRL-R binding at the choroid plexus of the right cerebral ventricle [ANOVA;F(2,13) = 0.58; p = 0.56] (Fig. 5A).
For behavioral and neuroendocrine studies, we selected the mRNA target site on the basis of a kinetic folding simulation (Breton et al., 1997) aimed at predicting the secondary structure of the PRL-R mRNA. Following the finding that regions which yield high hybridization efficiency include part of an open loop and a complete stem (Mir and Southern, 1999), we selected a portion of the target mRNA molecule that was predicted to meet these criteria (Fig. 1A). Indeed, receptor autoradiography revealed a significant downregulation of receptor binding at the choroid plexus of the right ventricle of 72%, compared with both vehicle and mixed bases [F(6,14) = 2.93; p < 0.05] (Fig. 5B,C).
DISCUSSION
Using complementary approaches, including application of the receptor ligand and downregulation of PRL-R expression by antisense targeting, we provided the first evidence that PRL acts as an endogenous anxiolytic and reduces HPA axis responses to stress in the rat. Prediction of PRL-R mRNA secondary structure by molecular modeling succeeded in increasing downregulation efficacy of PRL-R binding when antisense targeting was performed in vivo.
The behavioral and neuroendocrine responses to intracerebral PRL and antisense targeting, respectively, suggest a critical involvement of brain PRL-R. PRL-R expression in the rat brain has been described recently (Chiu and Wise, 1994; Bakowska and Morrell, 1997). Moreover, hypothalamic expression of PRL (Emanuele et al., 1992) and PRL release from hypothalamic explants in response to depolarizing media and angiotensin II (DeVito et al., 1991; Torner et al., 1995) have been observed. This points toward the possibility of intracerebral PRL release, which may occur for example under stressful conditions, as found for various other neuropeptides such as vasopressin and oxytocin (Landgraf, 1995). In our study, to reveal receptor-mediated actions of PRL in the brain in the absence of a specific PRL-R antagonist, we used several complementary approaches. First, an acute intracerebroventricular infusion of oPRL caused a dose-dependent reduction in anxiety-related behavior on the EPM, the standard test validated for the detection of emotional responses to anxiolytic or anxiogenic substances in rodents (Pellow et al., 1985). Second, an intravenous injection of oPRL also exerted an anxiolytic effect, consistent with the hypothesis of an active and selective uptake of circulating PRL by choroid plexus cells into the cerebrospinal fluid (Walsh et al., 1987). Third, the reverse effect, i.e., an increased anxiety-related behavior on the EPM, was observed after downregulation of PRL-R expression by antisense targeting, further supporting an anxiolytic action of the brain PRL system.
It is worth mentioning that the involvement of PRL in the regulation of anxiety-related behavior does not seem to be restricted to female rats, because an anxiolytic effect of intracerebroventricular oPRL was also found in male rats, although to a lesser extent (Fig.2B). Several studies addressing behavioral effects of PRL have also demonstrated PRL actions in both genders (Li et al., 1995; Heil, 1999). PRL-R have been mapped in the brain of both female (Crumeyrolle-Arias et al., 1993; Chiu and Wise, 1994; Bakowska and Morrell, 1997; Pi and Grattan, 1998, 1999) and male rats (Muccioli et al., 1991; Roky et al., 1996; Pi and Grattan, 1998). However, evidence exists for a higher density of PRL-R in the female rat brain compared with the male (Muccioli et al., 1991; Pi and Grattan, 1998), and this could account for the difference in the anxiolytic potency of oPRL between female and male rats found in the present study. Gender-dependent effects of established anxiolytic drugs such as diazepam have also been described (Fernandez-Guasti and Picazo, 1990,1997) and could be confirmed in our study (Fig.2A,B). It is of interest to note that, in contrast to PRL, diazepam seems to be more efficient in male rats, indicating different mechanisms of action. The finding of reduced locomotor activity after intracerebral PRL during both EPM (Fig. 2) and open-field (Table 1) exposure confirms recent observations in female rats (Alvarez and Banzan, 1994). Interestingly, during 10 min forced swimming, PRL-treated rats showed a more active stress-coping strategy; for example, the latency to floating was prolonged, and the time spent struggling was increased (Table 1), which strengthens the hypothesis of an antidepressive-like action of PRL that has been suggested by Drago et al. (1990).
A possible involvement of PRL in the regulation of emotionality is supported by recent animal (Steimer et al., 1997; Landgraf et al., 1999) and human (Baumgartner et al., 1988; Reavley et al., 1997) studies demonstrating, for example, differing plasma PRL concentrations in rats bred for either high or low innate levels of anxiety-related behavior (Landgraf et al., 1999) or for high and poor acquisition of active avoidance behavior (Steimer et al., 1997). During lactation, when the brain PRL receptors are upregulated (Pi and Grattan, 1999), reduced anxiety scores were reported in various species, including humans (Asher et al., 1995).
Emotional aberrations, in particular an increased level of anxiety, are often accompanied by an enhanced reactivity of the HPA axis (Holsboer and Barden, 1996; Arborelius et al., 1999; Landgraf et al., 1999). In the present study, brain PRL was also shown to regulate ACTH secretion. Chronic intracerebroventricular oPRL infusions, mimicking a hyperprolactinemic state and presumably resulting in enhanced brain PRL-R expression measurable in the choroid plexus (Mangurian et al., 1992; Sugiyama et al., 1994), caused a shift of HPA axis activity toward elevated basal ACTH plasma concentrations and an attenuated ACTH secretory response to exposure to an emotional stressor. In contrast, downregulation of brain PRL-R expression and, consequently, PRL-R binding by antisense ODN targeting were accompanied by a higher magnitude of ACTH secretory stress responses, underlining the PRL-R-mediated “anti-stress” effect. An explanation for the elevated basal plasma ACTH secretion is provided by the finding of a PRL-induced CRH release into the hypophysial portal blood under basal conditions (Kooy et al., 1990), possibly mediated by the noradrenergic system at hypothalamic level (Calogero et al., 1996). An attenuated responsiveness of the HPA axis is seen in lactation (Walker et al., 1995; Windle et al., 1997; Neumann et al., 2000), a time when the brain PRL system is activated as shown, for example, by an increase in brain PRL-R expression (Pi and Grattan, 1999). Thus, PRL either released into blood and transported to the CSF via choroid plexus cells or released within the brain might be significantly involved in the blunted HPA response observed in lactation.
Supporting our hypothesis of PRL regulating stress responses, chronic restraint stress induces the expression of PRL-R in choroid plexus cells probably because of stress-induced PRL secretion (Fujikawa et al., 1995); administration of PRL into the cerebral ventricles prevents stress-induced hyperthermia (Drago and Amir, 1984) and formation of gastric ulcers (Drago et al., 1985). Furthermore, neuroendocrine stress responses are attenuated in hyperprolactinemia (Schlein et al., 1974;Carter and Lightman, 1987).
Because a PRL-R antagonist is lacking, we selectively inhibited the translation of its receptor mRNA using antisense targeting. The approach of intracerebroventricular infusion of an antisense ODN has been recognized as a useful tool to evaluate the function of a given neuropeptide system, because ODN diffusion to, for example, extrahypothalamic and hypothalamic regions has been described in the rat brain (Wahlestedt et al., 1993). However, a major problem of antisense targeting is the position of the target site within the mRNA molecule (Patzel and Sczakiel, 1998). In the present study this critical issue was successfully addressed by performing a computer-assisted prediction of the secondary structure exhibited by the target mRNA, hence applying recently established criteria that describe the influence of mRNA self-structure on hybridization (Mir and Southern, 1999). A modeling approach is currently the only way of accessing structural information, because x-ray crystallography or nuclear magnetic resonance techniques are still not feasible for large chain lengths. Changing the target position from empirically selected sites that are predicted to include fractions of helical stems (Fig.1A) to one that is expected to form a complete stem, an internal loop, and a hairpin loop (Fig. 1B) significantly increased the treatment efficiency. Here it should be mentioned that (1) the different length of the ODNs (18 vs 20 bases), (2) possible differences in self-complementarity of the molecules, and (3) presence of two separate molecules in the AS1 mixture could have intrinsically affected the relative efficiency of the treatments, regardless of the structural considerations. However, given the well known rarity of highly efficient antisense molecules, the differences in predicted molecular structures of the target regions should be made responsible for the largest portion of the improvement in treatment efficiency presented here. Receptor autoradiography confirmed the expected improvement in downregulation efficiency in vivoafter molecular modeling (30 vs 72% in the choroid plexus after adjustment of target positioning).
Quantification of antisense ODN-induced downregulation of PRL-R binding was performed over the choroid plexus, where labeling was strongest. Significant PRL-R binding was not detected in other relevant brain regions, possibly resulting from either low brain density of PRL-R (Muccioli et al., 1991) and lack of respective sensitivity of the receptor autoradiography used or occupancy of brain PRL-R by endogenous PRL, because decapitation of the animals was performed 1 hr after stressor exposure. However, extensive brain penetration and cellular uptake have been demonstrated using the route of continuous intracerebroventricular antisense ODN administration (Whitesell et al., 1993). Therefore, the behavioral and neuroendocrine effects seen in PRL-R antisense-treated animals are likely to be caused by downregulation of PRL-R expression in relevant brain regions, including the hypothalamic PVN, amygdala, and septal nuclei. In these regions, interactions with the CRH system have to be considered in future studies. An alternative explanation for the antisense effects that were observed, however, would be a reduced uptake of circulating PRL into CSF and, consequently, to the brain as a result of downregulation of PRL-R expression and receptor-mediated transport processes in the choroid plexus. This hypothesis includes questions regarding the origin of the endogenous ligand of brain PRL-R because PRL may reach relevant brain targets after either selective uptake by choroid plexus cells or local release from neuronal structures. The latter question is currently under investigation using intracerebral microperfusion techniques.
We propose that the complementary methodological approach used to specifically modulate brain neuropeptide–receptor interactions, both increasing the availability of the receptor ligand and reducing neuropeptide receptor expression by antisense targeting, is still a valid alternative to genetic knockout strategies for the characterization of novel physiological functions of any neuropeptidergic system. However, more work, including additional behavioral tests and characterization of detailed PRL interactions with other relevant neuropeptides such as CRH, will be required to further establish the general role of brain PRL in the regulation of both emotionality and neuroendocrine stress coping at brain level.
Footnotes
This work was supported by Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft Grant Ne 465 (I.D.N., N.T., A.P., L.T.) and CONACyT (Mexico) (L.T.). We thank P. Lörscher for technical support, Dr. C. Probst for discussing the antisense design, Dr. C. Jacob for providing the secondary structure prediction computer program (SEQFOLD), and Dr. J. A. Russell for critical comments on a previous version of this manuscript.
Correspondence should be addressed to Inga D. Neumann, Max Planck Institute of Psychiatry, Kraepelinstrasse 2, D 80804 Munich, Germany. E-mail: INEU@MPIPSYKL.MPG.DE.
REFERENCES
- 1.Abel EL. A further analysis of physiological changes in rats in the forced swim test. Physiol Behav. 1994;56:795–800. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(94)90245-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Alvarez EO, Banzan AM. Behavioural actions of prolactin locally applied into the hippocampus of adult female rats. J Neural Transm Gen Sect. 1994;95:17–28. doi: 10.1007/BF01283027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Arborelius L, Owens MJ, Plotsky PM, Nemeroff CB. The role of corticotropin-releasing factor in depression and anxiety disorders. J Endocrinol. 1999;160:1–12. doi: 10.1677/joe.0.1600001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Asher I, Kaplan B, Modai I, Neri A, Valevski A, Weizman A. Mood and hormonal changes during late pregnancy and puerperium. Clin Exp Obstet Gynecol. 1995;22:321–325. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Bakowska JC, Morrell JI. Atlas of the neurons that express mRNA for the long form of the prolactin receptor in the forebrain of the female rat. J Comp Neurol. 1997;386:161–177. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1096-9861(19970922)386:2<161::aid-cne1>3.0.co;2-#. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Baumgartner A, Graf K, Kurten I. Prolactin in patients with major depressive disorder and in healthy subjects. I. Cross-sectional study of basal and post-TRH and postdexamethasone prolactin levels. Biol Psychiatry. 1988;24:249–267. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(88)90195-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Breton N, Jacob C, Daegelen P. Prediction of sequentially optimal RNA secondary structures. J Biomol Struct Dyn. 1997;14:727–739. doi: 10.1080/07391102.1997.10508175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Bridges RS, Numan M, Ronsheim PM, Mann PE, Lupini CE. Central prolactin infusions stimulate maternal behavior in steroid-treated, nulliparous female rats. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1990;87:8003–8007. doi: 10.1073/pnas.87.20.8003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Cairns MJ, Hopkins TM, Witherington C, Wang L, Sun LQ. Target site selection for an RNA-cleaving catalytic DNA. Nat Biotechnol. 1999;17:480–486. doi: 10.1038/8658. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Calogero AE, Burrello N, Ossino AM, Weber RFA, D'Agata R. Interaction between prolactin and catecholamines on hypothalamic GnRH release in vitro. J Endocrinol. 1996;151:269–275. doi: 10.1677/joe.0.1510269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Carter DA, Lightman SL. Oxytocin responses to stress in lactating and hyperprolactinemic rats. Neuroendocrinology. 1987;46:532–537. doi: 10.1159/000124876. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Charney DS, Grillon CCG, Bremner JD. The neurobiological basis of anxiety and fear: circuits, mechanisms, and neurochemical interactions (Part II). The Neuroscientist. 1998;4:122–132. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Chiu S, Wise PM. Prolactin receptor mRNA localization in the hypothalamus by in situ hybridization. J Neuroendocrinol. 1994;6:191–199. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2826.1994.tb00572.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Clapp C, Torner L, Gutierrez-Ospina G, Alcantara E, Lopez-Gomez FJ, Nagano M, Kelly PA, Mejia S, Morales MA, Martinez de la Escalera G. The prolactin gene is expressed in the hypothalamic-neurohypophyseal system and the protein is processed into a 14-kDa fragment with activity like 16-kDa prolactin. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1994;91:10384–10388. doi: 10.1073/pnas.91.22.10384. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Crumeyrolle-Arias M, Latouche J, Jammes H, Djiane J, Kelly PA, Reymond HJ, Haour F. Prolactin receptors in the rat hypothalamus: autoradiographic localization and characterization. Neuroendocrinology. 1993;57:457–466. doi: 10.1159/000126392. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.DeVito WJ, Stone S, Avakian C. Stimulation of hypothalamic prolactin release by veratridine and angiotensin II in the female rat: effect of ovariectomy and estradiol administration. Neuroendocrinology. 1991;54:391–398. doi: 10.1159/000125919. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.DeVito WJ, Avakian C, Stone S, Ace CI. Estradiol increases prolactin synthesis and prolactin messenger ribonucleic acid in selected brain regions in the hypophysectomized female rat. Endocrinology. 1992;131:2154–2160. doi: 10.1210/endo.131.5.1425416. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Drago F, Amir S. Effects of hyperprolactinemia on core temperature of the rat. Brain Res Bull. 1984;12:355–358. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(84)90103-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Drago F, Bohus B, Gispen WH, Scapagnini U, De Wied D. Prolactin-enhanced grooming behavior: interaction with ACTH. Brain Res. 1983;263:277–282. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(83)90320-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Drago F, Continella G, Conforto G, Scapagnini U. Prolactin inhibits the development of stress-induced ulcers in the rat. Life Sci. 1985;36:191–197. doi: 10.1016/0024-3205(85)90099-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Drago F, Pulvirenti L, Spadaro F, Pennisi G. Effects of TRH and prolactin in the behavioral despair (swim) model of depression in rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1990;15:349–356. doi: 10.1016/0306-4530(90)90060-m. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Emanuele NV, Jurgens JK, Halloran MM, Tentler JJ, Lawrence AM, Kelley MR. The rat prolactin gene is expressed in brain tissue: detection of normal and alternatively spliced prolactin messenger RNA. Mol Endocrinol. 1992;6:35–42. doi: 10.1210/mend.6.1.1738369. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Fernandez-Guasti A, Picazo O. The actions of diazepam and serotonergic anxiolytics vary according to the gender and the estrous cycle phase. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1990;37:77–81. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(90)90044-i. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Fernandez-Guasti A, Picazo O. Anxiolytic actions of diazepam, but not buspirone, are influenced by gender and the endocrine stage. Behav Brain Res. 1997;88:213–218. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(97)00047-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Fujikawa T, Soya H, Yoshizato H, Sakaguchi K, Doh-Ura K, Tanaka M, Nakasima K. Restraint stress enhances the gene expression of prolactin receptor long form at the choroid plexus. Endocrinology. 1995;136:5608–5613. doi: 10.1210/endo.136.12.7588315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Heil SH. Sex-specific effects of prolactin on food intake by rats. Horm Behav. 1999;35:47–54. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.1998.1495. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Ho SP, Bao Y, Lesher T, Malhotra R, Ma LY, Fluharty SJ, Sakai RR. Mapping of RNA accessible sites for antisense experiments with oligonucleotide libraries. Nat Biotechnol. 1998;16:59–63. doi: 10.1038/nbt0198-59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Holsboer F, Barden N. Antidepressants and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical regulation. Endocr Rev. 1996;17:187–205. doi: 10.1210/edrv-17-2-187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Kooy A, deGreef WJ, Vreeburg JTM, Hackeng WHL, Ooms MP, Lamberts SWJ, Weber RFA. Evidence for the involvement of corticotropin-releasing factor in the inhibition of gonadotropin release induced by hyperprolactinemia. Neuroendocrinology. 1990;51:261–266. doi: 10.1159/000125348. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Kramer MS, Cutler N, Feghner J, Shrivastava R, Carman J, Sramek JJ, Reines SA, Liu G, Snavely D, Wyatt-Knowles E, Hale JJ, Mills SG, MacCoss M, Swain CJ, Harrison T, Hill RG, Hefti F, Scolnick EM, Chicchi GG, Sadowski S, Williams AR, Hewson L, Smith D, Carlson EJ, Hargreaves RJ, Rupniak NMJ. Distinct mechanism for antidepressant activity by blockade of central substance P receptors. Science. 1998;281:1640–1645. doi: 10.1126/science.281.5383.1640. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Landgraf R. Intracerebrally released vasopressin and oxytocin: measurement, mechanisms and behavioural consequences. J Neuroendocrinol. 1995;7:243–253. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2826.1995.tb00754.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Landgraf R, Wigger A, Holsboer F, Neumann ID. Hyper-reactive hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenocortical axis in rats bred for high anxiety-related behavior. J Neuroendocrinol. 1999;11:405–407. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2826.1999.00342.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Li C, Kelly PA, Buntin JD. Inhibitory effects of anti-prolactin receptor antibodies on prolactin binding in brain and prolactin-induced feeding behavior in ring doves. Neuroendocrinology. 1995;61:125–135. doi: 10.1159/000126833. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Liebsch G, Wotjak CT, Landgraf R, Engelmann M. Septal vasopressin modulates anxiety-related behavior in rats. Neurosci Lett. 1996;217:101–104. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Liebsch G, Linthorst ACE, Neumann ID, Reul JMHM, Holsboer F, Landgraf R. Behavioral, physiological, and neuroendocrine stress responses and differential sensitivity to diazepam in two Wistar rat lines selectively bred for high- and low-anxiety-related behavior. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1998;19:381–396. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(98)00042-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Lucas BK, Ormandy CJ, Binart N, Bridges RS, Kelly PA. Null mutation of the prolactin receptor gene produces a defect in maternal behavior. Endocrinology. 1998;139:4102–4107. doi: 10.1210/endo.139.10.6243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Mangurian LP, Walsh RJ, Posner BI. Prolactin enhancement of its uptake at the choroid plexus. Endocrinology. 1992;131:698–702. doi: 10.1210/endo.131.2.1639017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Marti J, Armario A. Effects of diazepam and desipramine in the forced swimming test: influence of previous experience with the situation. Eur J Pharmacol. 1993;236:295–299. doi: 10.1016/0014-2999(93)90601-d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Merchenthaler I, Vigh S, Petrusz P, Schally A. Immunocytochemical localization of corticotropin releasing factors (CRF) in the rat brain. Am J Anat. 1982;165:385–396. doi: 10.1002/aja.1001650404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Mir KU, Southern EM. Determining the influence of structure on hybridization using oligonucleotide arrays. Nat Biotechnol. 1999;17:788–792. doi: 10.1038/11732. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Mitchell AJ. The role of corticotropin releasing factor in depressive illness: a critical review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1999;22:635–651. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(97)00059-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Muccioli G, Ghe C, Di Carlo R. Distribution and characterization of prolactin binding sites in the male and female rat brain: effects of hypophysectomy and ovariectomy. Neuroendocrinology. 1991;53:47–53. doi: 10.1159/000125696. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Neill JD. Effects of “stress” on serum prolactin and luteinizing hormone levels during the estrous cycle of the rat. Endocrinology. 1970;87:1192–1197. doi: 10.1210/endo-87-6-1192. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Neumann ID, Johnstone HA, Hatzinger M, Liebsch G, Shipston M, Russell JA, Landgraf R, Douglas AJ. Attenuated neuroendocrine responses to emotional and physical stressors in pregnant rats involve adenohypophyseal changes. J Physiol (Lond) 1998;508:289–300. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.1998.289br.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Neumann ID, Torner L, Wigger A. Brain oxytocin: differential inhibition of neuroendocrine stress responses and anxiety-related behaviour in virgin, pregnant and lactating rats. Neuroscience. 2000;95:567–575. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(99)00433-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Noel MB, Woodside B. Effects of systemic and central prolactin injections on food intake, weight gain, and estrous cyclicity in female rats. Physiol Behav. 1993;54:151–154. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(93)90057-m. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Patzel V, Sczakiel G. Theoretical design of antisense RNA structures substantially improves annealing kinetics and efficacy in human cells. Nat Biotechnol. 1998;16:64–68. doi: 10.1038/nbt0198-64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Paut-Pagano L, Roky R, Valatx JL, Kitahama K, Jouvet M. Anatomical distribution of prolactin-like immunoreactivity in the rat brain. Neuroendocrinology. 1993;58:682–695. doi: 10.1159/000126609. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Paxinos G, Watson C. The rat brain in stereotaxic coordinates. Academic; Sydney: 1986. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Pellow S, Chopin P, File SE, Briley M. Validation of open:closed arms entries in an elevated plus-maze as a measure of anxiety in the rat. J Neurosci Methods. 1985;14:149–167. doi: 10.1016/0165-0270(85)90031-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Pi XJ, Grattan DR. Distribution of prolactin receptor immunoreactivity in the brain of estrogen-treated, ovariectomized rats. J Comp Neurol. 1998;394:462–474. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1096-9861(19980518)394:4<462::aid-cne5>3.0.co;2-#. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Pi XJ, Grattan DR. Increased prolactin receptor immunoreactivity in the hypothalamus of lactating rats. J Neuroendocrinol. 1999;11:693–705. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2826.1999.00386.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Reavley S, Fisher AD, Owen D, Creed FH, Davis JRE. Psychological distress in patients with hyperprolactinemia. Clin Endocrinol (Oxf) 1997;47:343–348. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2265.1997.2701073.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Roky R, Paut-Pagano L, Goffin V, Kitahama K, Valatx JL, Kelly PA, Jouvet M. Distribution of prolactin receptors in the rat forebrain. Immunohistochemical study. Neuroendocrinology. 1996;63:422–429. doi: 10.1159/000127067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Schlein PA, Zarrow MX, Denenberg VH. The role of prolactin in the depressed or “buffered” adrenocorticosteroid response of the rat. J Endocrinol. 1974;62:93–99. doi: 10.1677/joe.0.0620093. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Seggie JA, Brown GM. Stress response patterns of plasma corticosterone, prolactin, and growth hormone in the rat, following handling or exposure to novel environment. Can J Physiol Pharmacol. 1975;53:629–637. doi: 10.1139/y75-087. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Shirota M, Banville D, Ali S, Jolicoeur C, Boutin J-M, Edery M, Djane J, Kelly PA. Expression of two forms of prolactin receptor in rat ovary and liver. Mol Endocrinol. 1990;4:1136–1143. doi: 10.1210/mend-4-8-1136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Steimer T, la Fleur S, Schulz PR. Neuroendocrine correlates of emotional reactivity and coping in male rats from the Roman high (RHA/Verh)- and low (RLA/Verh)- avoidance lines. Behav Genet. 1997;27:503–512. doi: 10.1023/a:1021448713665. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Sugiyama T, Minoura H, Kawabe N, Tanaka M, Nakashima K. Preferential expression of long form prolactin receptor mRNA in the rat brain during the estrous cycle, pregnancy and lactation: hormones involved in its gene expression. J Endocrinol. 1994;141:325–333. doi: 10.1677/joe.0.1410325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Torner L, Neumann I. Effects of intracerebral prolactin on anxiety-related behavior and HPA axis activity in rats. Soc Neurosci Abstr. 1998;371:19. [Google Scholar]
- 61.Torner L, Mejia S, Lopez-Gomez FJ, Quintanar A, Martinez de la Escalera G, Clapp C. A 14-kilodalton prolactin-like fragment is secreted by the hypothalamo-neurohypophyseal system of the rat. Endocrinology. 1995;136:5454–5460. doi: 10.1210/endo.136.12.7588295. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Torner L, Toschi N, Pohlinger A, Probst JC, Lörscher P, Neumann I. Blockade of prolactin (PRL) receptors by antisense targeting and icv infusion of PRL affect anxiety-related behavior and neuroendocrine responses to stress. Soc Neurosci Abstr. 1999;64:8. [Google Scholar]
- 63.Wahlestedt C, Merlo-Pich E, Koob GF, Yee F, Heilig M. Modulation of anxiety and neuropeptide Y-Y1 receptors by antisense oligodeoxynucleotides. Science. 1993;259:528–531. doi: 10.1126/science.8380941. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Walker CD, Trottier G, Rochford J, Lavallée D. Dissociation between behavioral and hormonal responses to the forced swim stress in lactating rats. J Neuroendocrinol. 1995;7:615–622. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2826.1995.tb00799.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Walsh RJ, Slaby FJ, Posner BI. A receptor-mediated mechanism for the transport of prolactin from blood to cerebrospinal fluid. Endocrinology. 1987;120:1846–1850. doi: 10.1210/endo-120-5-1846. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Whitesell L, Geselowitz D, Chavany C, Fahmy B, Walbridge S, Alger JR, Neckers LM. Stability, clearance, and disposition of intraventricularly administered oligodeoxynucleotides: implications for therapeutic application within the central nervous system. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1993;90:4665–4669. doi: 10.1073/pnas.90.10.4665. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Windle RJ, Wood S, Shanks N, Perks P, Conde GL, da Costa APC, Ingram CD, Lightman SL. Endocrine and behavioural responses to noise stress: comparison of virgin and lactating female rats during non-disrupted maternal activity. J Neuroendocrinol. 1997;9:407–414. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2826.1997.00587.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]