Abstract
Background
Non-motor features of Parkinson's disease (PD) and dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB), such as auditory hallucinations (AH), contribute to disease burden but are not well understood.
Methods
Systematic review and random-effects meta-analyses of studies reporting AH associated with PD or DLB. Prevalence of visual hallucinations (VH) in identified studies meeting eligibility criteria were included in meta-analyses, facilitating comparison with AH. Synthesis of qualitative descriptions of AH was performed. PubMed, Web of Science and Scopus databases were searched for primary journal articles, written in English, published from 1970 to 2017. Studies reporting AH prevalence in PD or DLB were screened using PRISMA methods.
Results
Searches identified 4542 unique studies for consideration, of which, 26 met inclusion criteria. AH pooled prevalence in PD was estimated to be 8.9% [95% confidence interval (CI) 5.3–14.5], while in DLB was estimated to be 30.8% (±23.4 to 39.3). Verbal hallucinations, perceived as originating outside the head, were the most common form of AH. Non-verbal AH were also common while musical AH were rare. VH were more prevalent, with an estimated pooled prevalence in PD of 28.2% (±19.1 to 39.5), while in DLB they were estimated to be 61.8% (±49.1 to 73.0). Meta-regression determined that the use of validated methodologies to identify hallucinations produced higher prevalence estimates.
Conclusions
AH and VH present in a substantial proportion of PD and DLB cases, with VH reported more frequently in both conditions. Both AH and VH are more prevalent in DLB than PD. There is a need for standardised use of validated methods to detect and monitor hallucinations.
Key words: Auditory hallucinations, dementia with Lewy bodies, Lewy body disease, non-motor symptoms, Parkinson's disease, visual hallucinations
Introduction
Parkinson's disease (PD) and dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) are neurodegenerative diseases associated with α-synuclein dysfunction. Estimates suggest PD prevalence is 1% in people over 60 (De Lau and Breteler, 2006), while DLB has a prevalence of 0.4% in people over 65 (Vann Jones and O'Brien, 2014). Both conditions are characterised by motor dysfunction but non-motor features contribute extensively to their presentation and disease burden.
Hallucinations, spontaneous aberrant perceptions, occur in a significant proportion of cases (Diederich et al., 2009). Hallucinations can be induced by medications such as anticholinergics (Celesia and Wanamaker, 1972), dopamine agonists (Baker et al., 2009) and a range of medications modulating diverse neurochemical pathways (Porteous and Ross, 1956; Lees et al., 1977; Gondim et al., 2010; Friedman et al., 2011; Wand, 2012). This presents challenges to determining the causes and nature of AH in PD and DLB. The majority of hallucinations in PD and DLB are chronic, recurring and progressive in spite of stable medication regimens (Fénelon et al., 2000). Indeed, cognitive, sensory and circadian aspects also contribute to hallucinosis (Mosimann et al., 2006). Neuroleptics are often administered on presentation of AH, yet have moderate efficacy and potentially severe side effects, including increased mortality (McKeith et al., 1992a; Weintraub et al., 2016).
Visual hallucinations (VH) constitute a core feature of DLB diagnosis (McKeith et al., 2017) and have been described as a hallmark of PD (Onofrj et al., 2007). An associated, but distinct condition, PD dementia (PDD) (Dubois et al., 2007), also presents with motor and non-motor features, including hallucinations, but is under-reported in the literature. In PD, PDD and DLB, auditory hallucinations (AH) are generally considered of secondary concern, in spite of the progressive nature of AH, their contribution to loss of insight, decreased quality of life and consequent influence on the decision to move patients into long-term care (Goetz and Stebbins, 1993; Aarsland et al., 2000).
Previous reports vary widely in reported prevalence of AH in PD from 2% (Leu-Semenescu et al., 2011) to 45% (Amar et al., 2014; Llorca et al., 2016). Prevalence rates also range widely in DLB, from 18% (Suárez-González et al., 2014) to 43% (Piggott et al., 2007). Previous studies reporting prevalence of AH have predominantly been cross-sectional, with limited focus on the nature of AH reported. Furthermore, methods of determining the presence of hallucinations are diverse, potentially leading to differing reporting rates.
Aims of this study: This complex picture suggested a need to characterise, with increased precision, the prevalence and nature of AH in PD and DLB. Therefore, in this study, we aimed to conduct a systematic review and meta-analysis of studies reporting AH prevalence in PD and DLB. Furthermore, we aimed to assess the types of AH in both conditions and compared their prevalence with that of VH, which are more commonly investigated.
Methods
Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines were used to standardise the conduct and reporting of this study. The protocol for this study was registered in advance on PROSPERO (registration number: CRD42017067337). Ethical approval for this study was awarded by the Faculty of Science and Engineering Ethics Committee at Manchester Metropolitan University (EthOS Reference Number: 0240).
Search strategy
Literature searches for candidate studies were undertaken in the following databases: PubMed, Web of Science and Scopus. Search terms were text words: auditory, auditory hallucinations, hearing, dementia, Lewy bodies, dementia with Lewy bodies, Lewy body dementia, Parkinson's disease. The Boolean operator AND was used to maximise the number of identified papers containing combinations of search terms. A search matrix was used to ensure all paired combinations of search terms were searched for in each database.
Study selection
The search was conducted from December 2016 to November 2017. Papers published from 1 January 1970 to 13 November 2017 were considered for inclusion. Titles and abstracts were examined to remove duplications and irrelevant studies. To be included, studies needed to (i) be written in English, (ii) report measures of AH prevalence in patients with PD or DLB, and (iii) be structured as a prospective cohort, case–control or cross-sectional study. Unpublished data were not pursued or included. Both investigators examined full-text versions of studies meeting the above criteria to assess compliance with inclusion criteria and extract data. We reviewed reference lists of all included articles to identify other potentially eligible studies.
Data extraction and risk of bias assessment
Both authors extracted data from included papers, including study authors; publication year; study title; journal; volume; issue; pages; study design; number of participants; number of female participants; number of participants diagnosed with PD/DLB; mean age of disease onset; number of participants with AH and/or VH; qualitative descriptions of AH; time window for hallucination presentation; method of hallucination assessment. Both reviewers independently evaluated risk of bias for each study using criteria adapted from the NIH Quality Assessment Tool for Observational Cohort and Cross-Sectional Studies (https://www.nhlbi.nih.gov/health-topics/study-quality-assessment-tools). One point was awarded for each question we felt could be answered in the affirmative. Our implementation of this tool assessed study quality across nine domains: aims/objectives stated; experimental protocol appropriately described; selection bias (see Results for details); participant inclusion/exclusion criteria (selected from similar populations/appropriate diagnosis of PD or DLB); statistical analyses appropriate; condition assessed prior to outcomes; dropouts reported; timeframe for hallucination presentation sufficient; outcome measures clearly defined, valid, reliable and implemented consistently across all study participants; and presence of detailed qualitative description of AH. Scores were summed for each study to provide an overall score of bias and quality. No weighting was used in bias assessment. Studies were then grouped into those of highest (scores = 9/9), high (score = 8/9), moderate-to-high (score = 7/9), moderate (score = 6/9) and poor (score = 1–5/9) quality. Poor quality studies were excluded (Inzelberg et al., 1998; Katzen et al., 2010; Goetz et al., 2011; Grau-Rivera et al., 2013). A comparison of all binary decisions made found 77.4% agreement between the authors. Discrepancies were settled by discussion and consensus.
Data analysis
Our primary outcome measure was AH prevalence in patients with PD or DLB. All included studies also reported prevalence of VH, which we also extracted as a secondary outcome. Where studies reported longitudinal data, we extracted the maximum values reported. Prevalence estimates in longitudinal studies were not higher than other study designs. Indeed, one longitudinal estimate (Goetz et al., 1998) reported the lowest prevalence of both auditory and VH in PD, suggesting this method did not bias our findings.
We conducted meta-analyses of AH and VH in Lewy body disease (LBD; pooled PD and DLB), and PD and DLB. Due to the range of study designs and different patient populations we anticipated would be included in our study, and the consequent assumption that effect sizes would be sampled from a population of effect sizes that could vary due to factors other than just sampling error, we planned to carry out random-effects models a priori.
We constructed random-effects models using Comprehensive Meta-Analyses software (Borenstein et al., 2009). We calculated pooled prevalence estimates with 95% confidence intervals (CIs) and assessed heterogeneity using the I2 statistic. Possible publication bias was assessed via the fail-safe N, Begg's funnel plot and Begg and Mazumdar's rank correlation tests. If publication bias was suspected, we used Duval and Tweedie's trim and fill to adjust our prevalence estimates. Outputs from these analyses were imported to an online forest plot generator to create figures (https://www.evidencepartners.com/resources/forest-plot-generator/).
Meta-regression models were created to investigate the potential contribution of study-level covariates to the observed heterogeneity in our pooled prevalence estimates. Log pooled prevalence estimate was the dependent variable, while study quality score, mean age at disease onset and the use of validated methods to detect hallucinations were set as predictive variables. Due to the diversity of methods employed to detect hallucinations, it was not possible to compare each technique. However, a clear distinction could be drawn between those studies that employed validated methods [NeuroPsychiatric Inventory (NPI), Manchester and Oxford Universities Scale for the Psychopathological Assessment of Dementia (MOUSPAD), Columbia University Scale for Psychopathology in Alzheimer's Disease (CUSPAD), Psycho-Sensory hAllucinations Scale (PSAS), University of Miami Parkinson's Disease Hallucinations Questionnaire (UM-PDHQ), Parkinson's Psychosis Rating Scale (PPRS) or Queen Square Visual Hallucination Inventory (QSVHI)] and those that did not (Rush Hallucination Inventory, semi-structured interview, questionnaire, screening hospital records or diagnostic interview and checklist).
We undertook sensitivity analyses to assess the robustness of our pooled prevalence estimate of AH and VH. We investigated the effect of year of publication by sequentially excluding studies published before 2000, 2005 and 2010. We also examined whether study design influenced outcomes by examining cross-sectional studies only and examined the effect of different quality score cut-off values for inclusion by sequentially excluding studies with scores of less than seven or eight out of nine.
Results
Study selection
After duplicate removal, we identified 4542 unique articles through primary database searches. Screening titles and abstracts led to the elimination of 4499 irrelevant articles. Full-text versions of the remaining 43 potentially eligible articles were assessed. Of these, 13 did not meet inclusion criteria, leaving 30 articles, published between 1992 and 2016, in the qualitative synthesis. A further four articles were excluded from quantitative meta-analyses due to low-quality assessment scores (Fig. 1). This produced 26 studies eligible for inclusion in the meta-analysis of AH prevalence (online Supplementary Table S1).
Characteristics of included studies
Of the included studies, 19 were cross-sectional studies, four were longitudinal studies and three were case–control studies. These studies represent data from 10 countries, the majority of which were conducted in Europe (n = 15), while others were undertaken in North America (n = 6) and Asia (n = 5).
Five studies (Ballard et al., 1999; Aarsland et al., 2000; Ballard et al., 2001; Piggott et al., 2007; Williams et al., 2008) diagnosed DLB using McKeith et al. (1996) consensus criteria, two (Suárez-González et al., 2014; Shea et al., 2015) employed the McKeith (2006) consensus criteria, while one (Klatka et al., 1996) used post-mortem neuropathologic diagnosis.
Fifteen studies (Fernandez et al., 1992; Graham et al., 1997; Fénelon et al., 2000; Holroyd et al., 2001; Gupta et al., 2004; de Maindreville et al., 2005; Pacchetti et al., 2005; Papapetropoulos et al., 2008; Williams et al., 2008; Fénelon et al., 2010; Mack et al., 2012; Svetel et al., 2012; Amar et al., 2014; de Chazeron et al., 2015; Llorca et al., 2016) diagnosed PD according to a version of the United Kingdom Parkinson's Disease Society Brain Bank Criteria (Gibb and Lees, 1988). Five other studies used different diagnostic criteria, including definite or probable PD based on Larsen et al. (1994) criteria (Aarsland et al., 2000); Calne et al. (1992) criteria (Paleacu et al., 2005); Gelb et al. (1999) criteria (Lee and Weintraub, 2012); Core Assessment Program for Intracerebral Transplantations; Langston et al. (1992) criteria (Goetz et al., 1998); or criteria of Hughes et al. (1992) for idiopathic PD (Leu-Semenescu et al., 2011).
Our quality assessment rated four studies as highest quality (score = 9/9), 12 as high quality (8/9), seven as moderate-to-high quality (7/9) and four as moderate quality (6/9) (online Supplementary Table S2). As a population, study quality was weakest in reporting of qualitative descriptions of hallucinations, with other areas of quality assessment scoring being consistently high among included studies.
Demographics of dataset
The mean age at onset of diagnosis was 61.9 years (s.d. = 7.6). We attempted to conduct group wise comparisons between diagnoses of PD without dementia (PDWD), PDD and DLB; however, PDD and PDWD were only separately reported in one study; consequently, these data were pooled into one PD group. Data from 3774 patients were identified, of which 3420 (90.6%) had PD, with the remainder DLB, and 1178 (31.2%) were female (range = 3.3–60.4%). A higher proportion of females were found in the DLB group (mean = 48.6%; range = 20.0–56.1%), than PD (mean = 30.6%; range = 3.3–60.4%). Mean age at onset for DLB was 74.1 years (s.d. = 8.1), while mean age at onset for PD was 58.8 years (s.d. = 10.6).
Overall pooled prevalence of AH
The overall random-effects model pooled prevalence of AH in LBD (Fig. 2) was 11.9% (95% CI ±7.9 to 17.7). To compare the relative prevalence of AH in DLB and PD, two further random-effects models were constructed for each condition, independent of the other. Pooled prevalence of AH in DLB (Fig. 3a) was 30.8% (±23.4 to 39.3), while in PD (Fig. 3b), it was 8.9% (±5.3 to 14.5).
Overall pooled prevalence of VH
We also extracted information on VH from the 26 included studies. The study by Leu-Semenescu et al. (2011) reported selection bias for this analysis as all 100 PD patients had VH due to the study design, and was therefore excluded leaving 25 studies in this analysis. The overall random-effects pooled prevalence of VH in LBD (Fig. 4) was 35.9% (±26.2 to 47.0). Pooled prevalence of VH in DLB (Fig. 5a) was 61.8% (±49.1 to 73.0), while in PD (Fig. 5b), it was 28.2% (±19.1 to 39.5).
Low prevalence of single modality auditory or VH
Pure sensory hallucinations (i.e. those in only one single sensory domain) were less common. There were two (0.6%) reports of pure AH in DLB and 23 (0.7%) in PD. Pure VH were more common than auditory in both conditions, being found in 36 (10.2%) DLB cases and 122 (3.6%) PD cases. Qualitative analysis of included longitudinal studies (Graham et al., 1997; Goetz et al., 1998; Ballard et al., 2001; de Maindreville et al., 2005) revealed that pure VH tended to predate AH in both PD and DLB. As each condition progressed, AH tended to bind with recurrent complex VH to form multi-modal hallucinations that increased in prevalence from 1.5 to 10 years post baseline assessment (Goetz et al., 2011).
Types of AH
The rate of reporting qualitative descriptions of AH was low, with data only available from six studies; five of which described data from PD patients, while the other described data from DLB patients.
The most commonly reported type of AH were verbal, which were reported in all six studies. Verbal AH were reported as human voices originating from outside the patient's head, often indistinct or incomprehensible, originating outside the visual field. Verbal AH were described as ‘non-threatening’, ‘non-imperative’, ‘non-congruent’ and ‘non-paranoid’. Three studies reported the proportion of AH that were verbal: Amar et al. (2014) found 11/18 (61.1%) of patents had verbal AH in PD; Fénelon et al. (2000) reported a similar proportion in PD – 13/21 (61.9%); while Suárez-González et al. (2014) found 5/14 (35.7%) had verbal AH in DLB.
Non-verbal sounds, such as inanimate (bullet fired, doorbell ringing, tinkling of bells, walking on steps, cracking sounds or squeaking) or animate sounds (dogs barking, tigers and lions roaring) were also common. Musical hallucinations were rare, being reported in only three patients by Fénelon et al. (2000), two of whom were described as ‘deaf’.
Hallucination assessment
Across the 26 studies, there were 10 different methods employed to determine the presence of hallucinations (online Supplementary Table S1). The most common method was a semi-structured interview (n = 10), followed by NPI (n = 5), CUSPAD (n = 2), PSAS (n = 2), unnamed questionnaire (n = 2), UM-PDHQ (n = 1), MOUSPAD (n = 1), PPRS (n = 1), QSVHI (n = 1), screening of hospital records (n = 1) and not stated (=1).
Meta-regression analyses
We observed considerable heterogeneity in all meta-analyses (I2 range = 51.2–96.3), suggesting a large proportion of the observed variance may be due to real differences between studies. To investigate whether some of the observed heterogeneity could be explained by moderator variables, such as study quality score, mean age of disease onset or method of hallucination assessment, we constructed meta-regression models for those meta-analyses comprised of sufficient study numbers (LBD and PD but not DLB). The results of these four models (online Supplementary Table S3) revealed the use of validated hallucination assessment methods could explain a significant proportion of the variance for each meta-regression (R2 range = 0.09–0.38), but study quality, mean age at disease onset and disease duration could not.
The fail-safe N for our meta-analysis of AH in LBD was 5649 (Z = −28.9; p < 0.0001), while there was only minor asymmetry in Begg's funnel plot (online Supplementary Fig. S6A). Begg and Mazumdar's rank correlation test suggested that publication bias was not present (Kendall's Τ-b = −0.23; p = 0.09) and employing Duval and Tweedie's trim and fill did not modify the random-effects effect size estimate. Similar values were found for our meta-analyses of AH in PD (online Supplementary Fig. S6B) and DLB (online Supplementary Fig. S6C), though trim and fill on the latter analysis imputed three studies that increased the pooled prevalence estimate to 37.5% (±27.7 to 48.5).
The fail-safe N for our meta-analysis of VHs in LBD was 1280 (Z = −14.16; p < 0.0001), while there was only minor asymmetry in Begg's funnel plot (online Supplementary Fig. S6D). Begg and Mazumdar's rank correlation test suggested that publication bias was not present (Kendall's Τ-b = −0.11; p = 0.44) and employing Duval and Tweedie's trim and fill did not modify the random-effects estimate. Similar values were found for our meta-analyses of VH in PD (online Supplementary Fig. S6E) and DLB (online Supplementary Fig. S6F), though trim and fill on the former analysis imputed four studies and increased the pooled prevalence estimate to 33.4% (±31.4 to 35.4).
We further investigated the potential role of cognition in hallucination status. Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) scores were reported in 16 studies (10 PD cases only; four DLB only; two both conditions). Pooled MMSE scores ranged from 13.2 to 29.2 (mean = 24.1). For PD, MMSE scores were lower (mean = 25.9; s.d.± = 2.3) for cases with hallucinations than those without (27.6 ± 1.2). For DLB, MMSE scores were similar for cases with hallucinations (17.3 ± 3.0) and those without hallucinations (17.2 ± 3.1). A correlation analysis found a negative relationship between MMSE scores and AH prevalence [r(s) = −0.61; p = 0.008].
Sensitivity analyses
We assessed the impact of year of study, study design and quality score on the robustness of our pooled prevalence estimates of auditory and VH in LBD. These analyses indicated that our estimates were robust, but were a few percentage points lower than analyses only including studies published from 2010 onwards or analyses only including moderate-to-high and high-quality studies (online Supplementary Table S4).
Discussion
We report that AH and VH present in a significant proportion of PD and DLB cases, with both forms of hallucination being more prevalent in DLB. We found that VH have a higher prevalence than AH in both conditions, both occurring at rates much higher than those found in the general population (Ohayon, 2000; Waters et al., 2018). Of note were the wide variety of methods used to determine the presence of hallucinations. We found that more recently published studies, using validated methods, produced higher estimates of hallucination prevalence, suggesting a need for wider adoption of such approaches. Taken together, these data demonstrate that AH have a higher prevalence in PD and DLB than commonly assumed.
Challenges to existing models of hallucinations
Existing models of recurrent complex VH have considered VH to exist in isolation from other modalities. Some models highlight dysfunctional attentional, cognitive and perceptual networks (Collerton et al., 2005; Diederich et al., 2005; Shine et al., 2011). Our data suggest that in PD and DLB, most cases of VH progress to become multi-modal hallucinations, incorporating a bound AH to the VH (e.g. hallucinations of people progress such that they can be heard talking). Attentional-cognitive models could account for these observations; however, sensory deficits seem incongruous with bottom-up perceptual elements of existing models. The contribution of bottom-up sensory aspects to VH has been shown via studies detailing ocular (Urwyler et al., 2014) and occipital lobe dysfunction (Meppelink et al., 2009), while central, top-down contributions involving frontal (Sanchez-Castaneda et al., 2010) and temporal (Harding et al., 2002) lobes also play a role.
However, hearing loss and auditory dysfunction are common at ages associated with PD and DLB diagnosis (Lin et al., 2011). It is therefore challenging to account for VH progressing to multi-modal hallucinations, binding with AH, due to a visual perceptual deficit occurring in these cases, followed by an auditory deficit. Models of simple AH, such as tinnitus, incorporate loss of peripheral drive with adaptive changes in gain (Eggermont, 1990), reductions in inhibition throughout the auditory pathway (Wang et al., 2011) and mismatches with central predictive coding (Sedley et al., 2016), yet rarely do these changes lead to more complex AH.
Attentional networks may tend to be directed more towards the visual scene, leading to more prevalent reporting of VH when these networks dysfunction. This may be due to attentional focus being more easily directed towards visual than auditory objects (Shinn-Cunningham, 2008). As widespread degeneration progresses, attentional deficits may facilitate widespread connectivity and phantom binding of VH with AH, perhaps acting via hyperexcitable cortical and subcortical networks (Grossberg, 2000; Robson et al., 2018).
Strengths and limitations
Our findings are supported by the large proportion of moderate-to-high quality studies included in our meta-analyses (online Supplementary Table S2). The accuracy of our prevalence estimates are supported by the majority of studies being cross-sectional, the best experimental design by which to estimate prevalence (Mann, 2003). Furthermore, our sensitivity analyses showed that only including cross-sectional studies had little effect on our estimates (online Supplementary Table S4). However, the time window for detection of auditory or VH ranged up to 30 years post-diagnosis (Graham et al., 1997), limiting the temporal precision of our estimates. A selection bias may exist in our estimates due to studies selecting patients from movement disorder clinics with few community-based samples; future studies of different populations may allow insights into hallucinations in different populations.
The use of various methods to identify hallucinations was a major contributor to the high degree of heterogeneity in our meta-analyses (online Supplementary Table S3). Meta-regression models found that validated methods produced higher prevalence estimates than non-validated, suggesting that there are advantages to such approaches. However, within the range of validated measures reported in our sample (UM-PDHQ, MOUSPAD, CUSPAD, PSAS, PPRS, NPI and QSVHI) exist substantial differences in approach and outcomes. Comparisons between these approaches are beyond the scope of this study. Future work comparing these methods within the same study population may be useful. Method of hallucination assessment is an important issue, as most patients do not report AH when they first perceive them (Chou et al., 2005). This may be due to AH being less easily identified than visual, but could also be due to patient knowledge that AH are commonly associated with psychiatric conditions.
Our initial focus was to estimate AH prevalence in PD and DLB. As all studies included in this analysis also reported the number of participants who had VH, we also extracted and analysed these data. Numerous papers in the literature report VH but do not report other forms of hallucinations. Consequently, our estimates of VH do not contain all available evidence, but do provide comparison data that allow us to have confidence that VH are more common than AH.
Across the timespan of included studies, diagnostic criteria for PD have largely remained unchanged; while there have been multiple iterations of the consensus criteria for DLB diagnosis, each of which has modified the specificity and sensitivity of this diagnosis (Rizzo et al., 2017). This may be another source of between-studies heterogeneity; however, the low number of DLB studies included in our analyses did not allow meta-regression models to be constructed for these data. The lowest I2 values we observed in our meta-analyses were found for DLB studies (Figs 3a and 5a), which argues against the possible contribution of diagnostic criteria to the observed heterogeneity.
Two potentially confounding covariates were later age of diagnosis of DLB than PD and the inclusion of lower quality studies (online Supplementary Table S2). Importantly, neither of these was found to account for a significant proportion of the variance in our meta-regression models (online Supplementary Table S3). This does not exclude the possibility that age of diagnosis or age per se contributes to hallucinosis. Indeed, the presentation of VH in age-matched DLB and PDD patients shows extensive overlap, suggesting age-related changes may contribute to their generation (Mosimann et al., 2006). We were able to include data from four longitudinal cohort studies; however, two of these studies reported data for 1-year post-diagnosis, meaning that estimates of point prevalence over time were not possible in the present study. There is evidence that hallucination point prevalence in PD increases over time to affect a majority of patients (Goetz et al., 2005; Hely et al., 2008). Indeed, there is evidence that hallucinations increase in prevalence with age, post-PD diagnosis (Graham et al., 1997; Biglan et al., 2007), with VH progressing towards polysensory phenotypes (Goetz et al., 2011). Furthermore, once perceived, hallucinations generally recur and are accompanied by a lack of insight (Goetz et al., 2006), leading to increased risk of requiring placement in care facilities (Goetz and Stebbins, 1993; Aarsland et al., 2000).
Types of AH
AH in LDB are complex, typically polymodal and varied in their presentation, although there is a paucity of high-quality, qualitative descriptions. Most common were verbal AH, perceived as originating outside the head, which differentiates verbal AH in PD or DLB from those found in schizophrenia. Interestingly, of the three studies to report relative rates of different types of AHs, two found that verbal hallucinations formed the majority in PD (Fénelon et al., 2000; Amar et al., 2014) while Suárez-González et al. (2014) reported that these were a minority in DLB. This may suggest a difference in AH presentation between the two conditions.
Non-verbal AH (acouasms) were also common and complex, whether animate or inanimate. These findings suggest auditory cortex and wider temporal and frontal lobe involvement in AH in PD and DLB, a speculation that is supported by neuroimaging data (Matsui et al., 2006). Some reports of simpler acouasms were also reported. The potential overlap between acouasms and tinnitus suggests that our estimated pooled prevalence of AH may be lower than the true prevalence in the population. Indeed, a recent cross-sectional study found that in a sample of 1000 patients in a cognitive neurology clinic, verbal and musical hallucinations had a prevalence of 0.9%, while tinnitus was present in 6.9% (Bayón et al., 2017).
AH were described as providing a soundtrack to VH (Fénelon et al., 2000), such as when a patient hears conversations of visually hallucinated people talking. This presentation is of note, as numerous authors described VH as preceding AH, while the polymodal combination of VH and AH may provide diagnostic utility in differentiating cognitive and functional impairment in DLB from Alzheimer's disease (Suárez-González et al., 2014). These data agree with early operation criteria for DLB diagnosis (McKeith et al., 1992b). While the first consensus guidelines for DLB diagnosis included AH as supportive features (McKeith et al., 1996), more recent updates have removed them from consideration (McKeith, 2006; McKeith et al., 2017). A recent analysis found that the consensus criteria for DLB had become more sensitive but less specific through these iterations, with little change in diagnostic accuracy (Rizzo et al., 2017). Whether the use of validated methods to detect AH is of any diagnostic utility in DLB or PD requires further investigation.
It is interesting to note that two of the three participants who reported musical hallucinations in our sample were described as ‘deaf’ (Fénelon et al., 2000). Musical hallucinations have been associated primarily with hearing impairment (Gordon, 1997; Cope and Baguley, 2009; Perez et al., 2017), though they have been reported in PD (Ergün et al., 2009) and DLB (Golden and Josephs, 2015) without hearing impairment. The contributions and potential interactions between hearing impairment and PD or DLB require further investigation, as there are suggestions that hearing impairment may present as a non-motor feature of PD (Lai et al., 2014) and hearing impairment may be more common in PD than age-matched controls (Yýlmaz et al., 2009).
Conclusion
This study is the first, to our knowledge, to summarise, synthesise and contrast evidence for AH and VH prevalence in PD and DLB. AH and VH contribute to disease burden in a significant proportion of LDB cases. Methods of identification and assessment of AH and VH requires investigation to standardise measurements. Successful developments in this field may improve the accuracy of hallucination diagnosis and inform disease progression monitoring and interventions.
Contributors
CLE and LDO were involved in the conception, design, planning, data extraction, analysis, writing and overseeing completeness of the manuscript. Both authors gave final approval of the manuscript.
Conflict of interest
None.
Financial support
This work was supported by an Action on Hearing Loss Flexi grant to LDO (grant number F68_ORTON).
Author ORCIDs
Llwyd David Orton https://orcid.org/0000-0002-8578-2296.
Supplementary material
For supplementary material accompanying this paper visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291718003161.
References
- Aarsland D, Larsen JP, Tandberg E and Laake K (2000) Predictors of nursing home placement in Parkinson's disease: a population-based, prospective study. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 48, 938–942. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Amar B, Yadav R, Reddy YJ and Pal PK (2014) A clinical profile of patients with Parkinson's disease and psychosis. Annals of Indian Academy of Neurology 17, 187. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baker WL, Silver D, White CM, Kluger J, Aberle J, Patel AA and Coleman CI (2009) Dopamine agonists in the treatment of early Parkinson's disease: a meta-analysis. Parkinsonism & Related Disorders 15, 287–294. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ballard C, Holmes C, Mckeith I, Neill D, O'Brien J, Cairns N, Lantos P, Perry E, Ince P and Perry R (1999) Psychiatric morbidity in dementia with Lewy bodies: a prospective clinical and neuropathological comparative study with Alzheimer's disease. American Journal of Psychiatry 156, 1039–1045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ballard CG, O'Brien JT, Swann AG, Thompson P, Neill D and Mckeith IG (2001) The natural history of psychosis and depression in dementia with Lewy bodies and Alzheimer's disease: persistence and new cases over 1 year of follow-up. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 62, 46–49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bayón AR, De Sagrario MT and Sampedro FG (2017) Auditory hallucinations in cognitive neurology. Neurología (English Edition) 32, 345–354. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Biglan KM, Holloway RG, Mcdermott MP, Richard IH and Investigators PSGC-P (2007) Risk factors for somnolence, edema, and hallucinations in early Parkinson disease. Neurology 69, 187–195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Borenstein M, Hedges LV, Higgins J and Rothstein HR (2009) Introduction to Meta-Analysis. UK: Wiley Online Library. [Google Scholar]
- Calne D, Snow B and Lee C (1992) Criteria for diagnosing Parkinson's disease. Annals of Neurology 32, 125–127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Celesia GG and Wanamaker WM (1972) Psychiatric disturbances in Parkinson's disease. Diseases of the Nervous System 33, 577–583. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chou KL, Messing S, Oakes D, Feldman PD, Breier A and Friedman JH (2005) Drug-induced psychosis in Parkinson disease: phenomenology and correlations among psychosis rating instruments. Clinical Neuropharmacology 28, 215–219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Collerton D, Perry E and Mckeith I (2005) Why people see things that are not there: a novel perception and attention deficit model for recurrent complex visual hallucinations. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28, 737–757. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cope T and Baguley D (2009) Is musical hallucination an otological phenomenon? A review of the literature. Clinical Otolaryngology 34, 423–430. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Chazeron I, Pereira B, Chereau-Boudet I, Brousse G, Misdrahi D, Fénelon G, Tronche A-M, Schwan R, Lançon C and Marques A (2015) Validation of a Psycho-Sensory hAllucinations Scale (PSAS) in schizophrenia and Parkinson's disease. Schizophrenia Research 161, 269–276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Lau LM and Breteler MM (2006) Epidemiology of Parkinson's disease. The Lancet Neurology 5, 525–535. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Maindreville AD, Fénelon G and Mahieux F (2005) Hallucinations in Parkinson's disease: a follow-up study. Movement Disorders 20, 212–217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diederich NJ, Goetz CG and Stebbins GT (2005) Repeated visual hallucinations in Parkinson's disease as disturbed external/internal perceptions: focused review and a new integrative model. Movement Disorders 20, 130–140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diederich NJ, Fénelon G, Stebbins G and Goetz CG (2009) Hallucinations in Parkinson disease. Nature Reviews Neurology 5, 331. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dubois B, Burn D, Goetz C, Aarsland D, Brown RG, Broe GA, Dickson D, Duyckaerts C, Cummings J and Gauthier S (2007) Diagnostic procedures for Parkinson's disease dementia: recommendations from the movement disorder society task force. Movement Disorders 22, 2314–2324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eggermont JJ (1990) On the pathophysiology of tinnitus; a review and a peripheral model. Hearing Research 48, 111–123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ergün U, Bozbas A, Akin Ü and Inan L (2009) Musical hallucinations and Parkinson disease. The Neurologist 15, 150–152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fénelon G, Mahieux F, Huon R and Ziégler M (2000) Hallucinations in Parkinson's disease: prevalence, phenomenology and risk factors. Brain 123, 733–745. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fénelon G, Soulas T, Zenasni F and De Langavant LC (2010) The changing face of Parkinson's disease-associated psychosis: a cross-sectional study based on the new NINDS-NIMH criteria. Movement Disorders 25, 763–766. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fernandez W, Stern G and Lees A (1992) Hallucinations and parkinsonian motor fluctuations. Behavioural Neurology 5, 83–86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friedman JH, Agarwal P, Alcalay R, Black K, Chou K, Cote L, Dayalu P, Frank S, Hartlein J and Hauser R (2011) Clinical vignettes in Parkinson's disease: a collection of unusual medication-induced hallucinations, delusions, and compulsive behaviours. International Journal of Neuroscience 121, 472–476. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gelb DJ, Oliver E and Gilman S (1999) Diagnostic criteria for Parkinson disease. Archives of Neurology 56, 33–39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gibb W and Lees A (1988) The relevance of the Lewy body to the pathogenesis of idiopathic Parkinson's disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry 51, 745–752. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goetz CG and Stebbins GT (1993) Risk factors for nursing home placement in advanced Parkinson's disease. Neurology 43, 2222–2222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goetz C, Vogel C, Tanner CM and Stebbins GT (1998) Early dopaminergic drug-induced hallucinations in parkinsonian patients. Neurology 51, 811–814. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goetz CG, Wuu J, Curgian LM and Leurgans S (2005) Hallucinations and sleep disorders in PD. Six-year prospective longitudinal study. Neurology 64, 81–86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goetz CG, Fan W, Leurgans S, Bernard B and Stebbins GT (2006) The malignant course of ‘benign hallucinations’ in Parkinson disease. Archives of Neurology 63, 713–716. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goetz CG, Stebbins GT and Ouyang B (2011) Visual plus nonvisual hallucinations in Parkinson's disease: development and evolution over 10 years. Movement Disorders 26, 2196–2200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Golden EC and Josephs KA (2015) Minds on replay: musical hallucinations and their relationship to neurological disease. Brain 138, 3793–3802. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gondim FDAA, Costa HA, Taunay TC, De Oliveira GR, Ferreira JM and Rola FH (2010) Transient amantadine-induced musical hallucinations in a patient with Parkinson's disease. Movement Disorders 25, 1505–1506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gordon A (1997) Insight into auditory hallucinations and psychosis. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 12, 410–411. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Graham JM, Grünewald RA and Sagar HJ (1997) Hallucinosis in idiopathic Parkinson's disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry 63, 434–440. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grau-Rivera O, Gelpi E, Rey M, Valldeoriola F, Tolosa E, Compta Y and Martí M (2013) Prominent psychiatric symptoms in patients with Parkinson's disease and concomitant argyrophilic grain disease. Journal of Neurology 260, 3002–3009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grossberg S (2000) How hallucinations may arise from brain mechanisms of learning, attention, and volition. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 6, 583–592. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gupta M, Singh G, Khwaja GA and Mehndiratta M (2004) Hallucinations in Parkinson's disease – a study of forty three patients. JAPI 52, 703. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harding A, Broe G and Halliday G (2002) Visual hallucinations in Lewy body disease relate to Lewy bodies in the temporal lobe. Brain 125, 391–403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hely MA, Reid WG, Adena MA, Halliday GM and Morris JG (2008) The Sydney multicenter study of Parkinson's disease: the inevitability of dementia at 20 years. Movement Disorders 23, 837–844. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holroyd S, Currie L and Wooten G (2001) Prospective study of hallucinations and delusions in Parkinson's disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry 70, 734–738. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hughes AJ, Daniel SE, Kilford L and Lees AJ (1992) Accuracy of clinical diagnosis of idiopathic Parkinson's disease: a clinico-pathological study of 100 cases. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry 55, 181–184. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Inzelberg R, Kipervasser S and Korczyn AD (1998) Auditory hallucinations in Parkinson's disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry 64, 533–535. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Katzen H, Myerson C, Papapetropoulos S, Nahab F, Gallo B and Levin B (2010) Multi-modal hallucinations and cognitive function in Parkinson's disease. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders 30, 51–56. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klatka LA, Louis ED and Schiffer RB (1996) Psychiatric features in diffuse Lewy body disease. A clinicopathologic study using Alzheimer's disease and Parkinson's disease comparison groups. Neurology 47, 1148–1152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lai SW, Liao KF, Lin CL, Lin CC and Sung FC (2014) Hearing loss may be a non-motor feature of Parkinson's disease in older people in Taiwan. European Journal of Neurology 21, 752–757. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Langston JW, Koller W and Giron L (1992) Etiology of Parkinson's disease In Koller W (ed.), Handbook of Parkinson's Disease, 2nd Edn New York: Marcel Dekker, pp. 369–381. [Google Scholar]
- Larsen JP, Dupont E and Tandberg E (1994) Clinical diagnosis of Parkinson's disease. Proposal of diagnostic subgroups classified at different levels of confidence. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica 89, 242–251. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee AH and Weintraub D (2012) Psychosis in Parkinson's disease without dementia: common and comorbid with other non-motor symptoms. Movement Disorders 27, 858–863. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lees A, Clarke C and Harrison M (1977) Hallucinations after withdrawal of baclofen. The Lancet 309, 858. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leu-Semenescu S, De Cock VC, Le Masson VD, Debs R, Lavault S, Roze E, Vidailhet M and Arnulf I (2011) Hallucinations in narcolepsy with and without cataplexy: contrasts with Parkinson's disease. Sleep Medicine 12, 497–504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lin FR, Niparko JK and Ferrucci L (2011) Hearing loss prevalence in the United States. Archives of Internal Medicine 171, 1851–1853. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Llorca P, Pereira B, Jardri R, Chereau-Boudet I, Brousse G, Misdrahi D, Fénelon G, Tronche A-M, Schwan R and Lançon C (2016) Hallucinations in schizophrenia and Parkinson's disease: an analysis of sensory modalities involved and the repercussion on patients. Scientific Reports 6, 38152. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mack J, Rabins P, Anderson K, Goldstein S, Grill S, Hirsch ES, Lehmann S, Little JT, Margolis RL and Palanci J (2012) Prevalence of psychotic symptoms in a community-based Parkinson disease sample. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 20, 123–132. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mann C (2003) Observational research methods. Research design II: cohort, cross sectional, and case-control studies. Emergency Medicine Journal 20, 54–60. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matsui H, Nishinaka K, Oda M, Hara N, Komatsu K, Kubori T and Udaka F (2006) Hypoperfusion of the auditory and prefrontal cortices in Parkinsonian patients with verbal hallucinations. Movement Disorders 21, 2165–2169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mckeith IG (2006) Consensus guidelines for the clinical and pathologic diagnosis of dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB): report of the Consortium on DLB International Workshop. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease 9, 417–423. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mckeith, Fairbairn A, Perry R, Thompson P and Perry E (1992a) Neuroleptic sensitivity in patients with senile dementia of Lewy body type. British Medical Journal 305, 673–678. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mckeith, Perry R, Fairbairn A, Jabeen S and Perry E (1992b) Operational criteria for senile dementia of Lewy body type (SDLT). Psychological Medicine 22, 911–922. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mckeith IG, Galasko D, Kosaka K, Perry E, Dickson DW, Hansen LA, Salmon D, Lowe J, Mirra S and Byrne E (1996) Consensus guidelines for the clinical and pathologic diagnosis of dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB). Report of the Consortium on DLB International Workshop. Neurology 47, 1113–1124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mckeith IG, Boeve BF, Dickson DW, Halliday G, Taylor J-P, Weintraub D, Aarsland D, Galvin J, Attems J and Ballard CG (2017) Diagnosis and management of dementia with Lewy bodies Fourth consensus report of the DLB Consortium. Neurology 89, 88–100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meppelink AM, De Jong BM, Renken R, Leenders KL, Cornelissen FW and Van Laar T (2009) Impaired visual processing preceding image recognition in Parkinson's disease patients with visual hallucinations. Brain 132, 2980–2993. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mosimann UP, Rowan EN, Partington CE, Collerton D, Littlewood E, O'Brien JT, Burn DJ and Mckeith IG (2006) Characteristics of visual hallucinations in Parkinson disease dementia and dementia with Lewy bodies. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 14, 153–160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ohayon MM (2000) Prevalence of hallucinations and their pathological associations in the general population. Psychiatry Research 97, 153–164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Onofrj M, Thomas A and Bonanni L (2007) New approaches to understanding hallucinations in Parkinson's disease: phenomenology and possible origins. Expert Review of Neurotherapeutics 7, 1731–1750. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pacchetti C, Manni R, Zangaglia R, Mancini F, Marchioni E, Tassorelli C, Terzaghi M, Ossola M, Martignoni E and Moglia A (2005) Relationship between hallucinations, delusions, and rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder in Parkinson's disease. Movement Disorders 20, 1439–1448. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paleacu D, Schechtman E and Inzelberg R (2005) Association between family history of dementia and hallucinations in Parkinson disease. Neurology 64, 1712–1715. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Papapetropoulos S, Katzen H, Schrag A, Singer C, Scanlon BK, Nation D, Guevara A and Levin B (2008) A questionnaire-based (UM-PDHQ) study of hallucinations in Parkinson's disease. BMC Neurology 8, 21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perez PA, Garcia-Antelo MJ and Rubio-Nazabal E (2017) ‘Doctor, I hear music’: a brief review about musical hallucinations. The Open Neurology Journal 11, 11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Piggott MA, Ballard CG, Rowan E, Holmes C, Mckeith IG, Jaros E, Perry RH and Perry EK (2007) Selective loss of dopamine D2 receptors in temporal cortex in dementia with Lewy bodies, association with cognitive decline. Synapse 61, 903–911. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Porteous H and Ross D (1956) Mental symptoms in parkinsonism following benzhexol hydrochloride therapy. British Medical Journal 2, 138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rizzo G, Arcuti S, Copetti M, Alessandria M, Savica R, Fontana A, Liguori R and Logroscino G (2017) Accuracy of clinical diagnosis of dementia with Lewy bodies: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Neurology Neurosurgery & Psychiatry 89, 358–366. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robson E, Tweedy C, Manzanza N, Taylor J-P, Atkinson P, Randall F, Reeve A, Clowry GJ and Lebeau FE (2018) Impaired fast network oscillations and mitochondrial dysfunction in a mouse model of alpha-synucleinopathy (A30P). Neuroscience 377, 161–173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanchez-Castaneda C, Rene R, Ramirez-Ruiz B, Campdelacreu J, Gascon J, Falcon C, Calopa M, Jauma S, Juncadella M and Junque C (2010) Frontal and associative visual areas related to visual hallucinations in dementia with Lewy bodies and Parkinson's disease with dementia. Movement Disorders 25, 615–622. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sedley W, Friston KJ, Gander PE, Kumar S and Griffiths TD (2016) An integrative tinnitus model based on sensory precision. Trends in Neurosciences 39, 799–812. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shea YF, Ha J and Chu LW (2015) Comparisons of clinical symptoms in biomarker-confirmed Alzheimer's disease, dementia with Lewy bodies, and frontotemporal dementia patients in a local memory clinic. Psychogeriatrics 15, 235–241. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shine JM, Halliday GM, Naismith SL and Lewis SJ (2011) Visual misperceptions and hallucinations in Parkinson's disease: dysfunction of attentional control networks? Movement Disorders 26, 2154–2159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shinn-Cunningham BG (2008) Object-based auditory and visual attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 12, 182–186. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suárez-González A, Serrano-Pozo A, Arroyo-Anlló EM, Franco-Macías E, Polo J, García-Solís D and Gil-Néciga E (2014) Utility of neuropsychiatric tools in the differential diagnosis of dementia with Lewy bodies and Alzheimer's disease: quantitative and qualitative findings. International Psychogeriatrics 26, 453–461. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Svetel M, Smiljković T, Pekmezović T and Kostić V (2012) Hallucinations in Parkinson's disease: cross-sectional study. Acta Neurologica Belgica 112, 33–37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Urwyler P, Nef T, Killen A, Collerton D, Thomas A, Burn D, Mckeith I and Mosimann UP (2014) Visual complaints and visual hallucinations in Parkinson's disease. Parkinsonism & Related Disorders 20, 318–322. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vann Jones S and O'Brien J (2014) The prevalence and incidence of dementia with Lewy bodies: a systematic review of population and clinical studies. Psychological Medicine 44, 673–683. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wand AP (2012) Transient citalopram-induced auditory hallucinations in a patient with Parkinson's disease and depression. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 46, 178–178. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang H, Brozoski TJ and Caspary DM (2011) Inhibitory neurotransmission in animal models of tinnitus: maladaptive plasticity. Hearing Research 279, 111–117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waters F, Blom J, Jardri R, Hugdahl K and Sommer I (2018) Auditory hallucinations, not necessarily a hallmark of psychotic disorder. Psychological Medicine 48, 529–536. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weintraub D, Chiang C, Kim HM, Wilkinson J, Marras C, Stanislawski B, Mamikonyan E and Kales HC (2016) Association of antipsychotic use with mortality risk in patients with Parkinson disease. JAMA Neurology 73, 535–541. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams DR, Warren JD and Lees AJ (2008) Using the presence of visual hallucinations to differentiate Parkinson's disease from atypical parkinsonism. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry 79, 652–655. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yýlmaz S, Karalý E, Tokmak A, Güçlü E, Koçer A and Öztürk Ö (2009) Auditory evaluation in Parkinsonian patients. European Archives of Oto-Rhino-Laryngology 266, 669–671. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
For supplementary material accompanying this paper visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291718003161.