Skip to main content
The Journal of Nutrition logoLink to The Journal of Nutrition
. 2019 Jun 26;149(10):1812–1817. doi: 10.1093/jn/nxz120

Food Insecurity Is Associated with Lower Cognitive Functioning in a National Sample of Older Adults

Eduardo T Portela-Parra 1,, Cindy W Leung 1,
PMCID: PMC6768814  PMID: 31240308

ABSTRACT

Background

Food insecurity, a social and economic condition of inadequate food resources, is known to affect cognitive development in children. However, research is sparse among adult populations, particularly older adults who may be more susceptible to accelerated cognitive decline.

Objective

The aim of this study was to examine the associations between food insecurity and cognitive functioning among older adults.

Methods

Data came from 1823 older adults (≥60 y) with incomes ≤300% of the federal poverty level (FPL) from the 2011–2014 NHANES. Food security was measured using the 10-item Adult Food Security Survey Module. Cognitive function was measured using the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease (CERAD) word learning subtest and delayed word recall, the Animal Fluency Test (AFT), and the Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST). The cognitive assessments were then standardized and an overall cognitive function z score was created by averaging across all cognitive assessments. Associations with food insecurity were examined using multivariate linear regression models, adjusting for sociodemographic and health characteristics.

Results

In the analytic population, the prevalence of food insecurity was 23.7%. Across all cognitive assessments, the mean scores among food-insecure adults was significantly lower than the mean scores among food-secure adults. After adjusting for sociodemographic and health characteristics, food insecurity was associated with lower scores on the CERAD word learning subtest (β = −0.14, 95% CI: −0.26, −0.01), the AFT (β = −0.13, 95% CI: −0.25, −0.002), and the DSST (β = −0.24, 95% CI: −0.33, −0.15). Food insecurity was also associated with a lower score on the overall cognitive function z score (β = −0.15, 95% CI: −0.26, −0.05).

Conclusions

In this national sample of 1823 adults aged ≥60 y, food insecurity was inversely associated with cognitive function, which may translate into higher risk of cognitive impairment over time.

Keywords: food insecurity, cognitive function, cognitive impairment, NHANES, elderly

Introduction

Food insecurity, defined as the lack of consistent access to food, has been a persistent concern for older adults in the US. In 2016 ∼4.9 million older adults were food insecure and another 3.7 million older adults experienced marginal food security (1). Food insecurity is more prevalent among older adults living below the poverty line, of African American or Hispanic race/ethnicity, and living with grandchildren in the home (1).

Food insecurity is an important social determinant of health, independent of other socioeconomic factors (2). Extensive literature has shown that food insecurity is associated with several health conditions in adulthood, including hypertension (3–5), diabetes (3–6), and cardiovascular disease (4, 7). There have been fewer studies on the health consequences and underlying mechanisms of food insecurity among older adults, despite the higher burden of diseases related to aging and subsequent healthcare expenditures (8). To date, studies have demonstrated that older adults with multiple chronic diseases are at higher risk of food insecurity (9), and that food insecurity subsequently predicts poorer disease management through cost-related medication nonadherence (10, 11), leading to greater healthcare costs over time (12).

In addition to physical health, food insecurity has also been known to impact cognitive function and mental health (13–18). Much of the current research on food insecurity and cognitive function has been conducted in children (19, 20). Aging is also a risk factor for cognitive decline and cognitive impairment, which may be exacerbated by the experience of food insecurity and deficiencies in dietary intake (21) or increases in psychological stress (22). Only a few studies have explored the relation between food insecurity and cognitive function among middle-aged and older adults, 2 of which are from the Boston Puerto Rican Health Study cohort, a longitudinal cohort study of health disparities among 1500 Puerto Rican adults. In 2009, Gao et al. found that compared to food-secure adults, adults with very low food security had lower scores on a global assessment of a cognitive performance test (the Mini-Mental State Examination), and several tasks measuring executive function (23). In 2016, Wong et al. repeated this analysis using longitudinal measures (24). Their findings showed that food insecurity was associated with greater cognitive decline over a 2-y period using a composite score of cognitive assessments. The results of these studies mirror those from studies of children's cognitive development and academic achievement (2, 19, 20). However, more research is needed to determine whether the results observed from Puerto Rican adults are generalizable to the larger population of older adults at risk of food insecurity. Thus, the objective of the present study was to examine the associations between food insecurity and cognitive function in a national sample of older adults.

Methods

Study population

Data for this study was derived from the 2011–2014 NHANES (25), which is an ongoing, multistage cross-sectional survey administered by the National Center for Health Statistics. The 2011–2012 and 2013–2014 surveys represent the most recent years in which cognitive assessments were available at the time of the present study. The study population was comprised of 2127 adults aged ≥60 y with household incomes ≤300% federal poverty level (FPL). Individuals with missing data on food security (n = 1), cognitive assessments (n = 299), and other covariates, such as educational attainment (n = 2) and smoking status (n = 2), were further excluded leaving an analytic sample size of 1823 adults. Similar to prior studies, a threshold on household income was applied because food insecurity is relatively uncommon among higher-income households and restricting the analytic population to lower-income households may reduce confounding of the primary association by household income (6, 26–28).

Food insecurity

Food insecurity was measured with the 10-item US Adult Food Security Survey Module. This module includes the same questions used in the US Household Food Security Survey Module but omits questions pertaining to children in the household. The questions assess experiences or behaviors related to insufficient resources to purchase food over the past 12 mo. A score ranging from 0–10 was created by summing the affirmative responses, with a higher score denoting greater food insecurity. The score was categorized into full food security (0), marginal food security (1, 2), low food security (3–5), and very low food security (6–10). Similar to prior studies, marginal food-secure adults (n = 219) were combined with low (n = 219) and very low food-secure adults (n = 130) in the present study to create a combined “food-insecure” category (29, 30).

Cognitive function assessments

Starting in 2011, multiple assessments of cognitive functioning were administered to study participants aged ≥60 y in the mobile examination center (MEC) by trained examiners (31). These tasks were conducted in the study participant's preferred language (English, Spanish, Korean, Vietnamese, or Chinese) and were audio recorded. The cognitive assessments included: The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease (CERAD) word learning subtest and delayed word recall, the Animal Fluency Test (AFT), and the Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST).

CERAD word learning subtest assesses the ability to immediately learn new verbal information (32). In a series of 3 trials, NHANES participants first read aloud 10 unrelated words, and were then asked to recall as many of the 10 words as possible. The order of the 10 words was changed in the subsequent trials. Each trial was scored from 0 (no words recalled) to 10 (all words recalled). For the present analysis, the average of the 3 trials was computed. The CERAD delayed word recall was conducted after the AFT and DSST (∼8–10 min after the CERAD word learning subtest), assessing the ability for delayed learning of new information. The CERAD delayed word recall score is the number of words the participant was able to recall from the earlier CERAD word learning subtest, and is scored from 0–10. The AFT, which assesses verbal fluency, asks participants to name as many animals as they can in 1 min (33). The AFT score is the total number of animals named. The DSST assesses processing speed, sustained attention, and working memory, and is a component of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS III) (34). The psychometric properties of these tasks have been established in prior studies (35–38). In this paper-based test, participants are provided a key of 9 numbers matched to symbols. They are then instructed to copy the corresponding symbols into 133 boxes next to the adjacent numbers. The DSST score is the total number of correct matches.

All cognitive assessment scores were further converted to z scores to facilitate interpretation and to accommodate the different maximum scores across the various cognitive assessments. An overall cognitive function score was also created by averaging the 4 z scores, with a higher score denoting higher cognitive functioning. Creation of the overall cognitive function score also helps to minimize floor and ceiling effects across cognitive function tasks in a population of older adults (39).

Study covariates

Covariates included age, sex, race/ethnicity (non-Hispanic white, non-Hispanic black, Hispanic, or non-Hispanic other race/ethnicities), highest education level (<12 y, high school graduate or General Education Diploma equivalent, or any college), marital status (married or living with partner, and never married/widowed/divorced/separated), poverty income ratio (0–100% FPL, 100.1–200% FPL, 200.1–300% FPL), and smoking status (never smoker, and former smoker/current smoker). Age was modeled as a continuous and quadratic (i.e. squared) term to account for a potential curvilinear relation with cognitive functioning. These variables were selected because it was hypothesized that they were related to both food insecurity and cognitive functioning among older adults.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses were conducted using Stata SE 12.1 (StataCorp LLC) with the SVY commands to account for the complex survey design of NHANES. Survey weights from the MEC were recalculated to reflect the probability of being selected across the 4-y period, and were used in all analyses to account for differential patterns of selection and nonresponse. First, differences in covariates of interest were examined by food insecurity levels and the significance of these differences were examined by chi-squared tests. Next, differences in mean raw scores and mean z scores of the 4 cognitive assessments by food insecurity groups were examined and significance was determined by univariate linear regression models. Lastly, multivariate linear regression models were fit for each of the standardized cognitive assessment outcomes and the overall cognitive function score to examine adjusted differences by food insecurity. Models were first adjusted for age and sex, and then all covariates were included in the final model. Statistical tests were 2-sided and statistical significance was considered at P < 0.05.

Results

In our analytic sample of 1823 older adults, 23.7% reported food insecurity during the past 12 mo. Sociodemographic and health characteristics of study participants by food insecurity levels are shown in Table 1. Food insecurity was more prevalent among adults aged 60–69 y, of minority racial/ethnic backgrounds, with lower levels of education, not married or living with a partner, and with lower household incomes (P < 0.05). There was no significant difference in food insecurity by participant's sex.

TABLE 1.

Characteristics of food-secure and food-insecure older adults with household incomes ≤300% of the federal poverty level: NHANES 2011–2014

Food secure (n = 1255) Food insecure (n = 568)
n % n % P value1
Age, y <0.001
 60–69 569 43.4 355 59.7
 70–79 392 32.4 159 30.9
 ≥80 294 24.1 54 9.4
Sex 0.51
 Male 585 39.9 254 38.7
 Female 670 60.1 314 61.3
Race/ethnicity <0.001
 Non-Hispanic white 616 76.1 163 55.2
 Non-Hispanic black 235 8.3 182 20.3
 Hispanic 283 9.5 187 19.9
 Non-Hispanic other 121 6.2 36 4.7
Education level <0.001
 <12 y 406 22.7 262 41.5
 High school graduate 334 30.4 149 25.3
 Any college 515 46.9 157 33.2
Marital status 0.02
 Married or living with partner 653 48.4 322 57.2
 Single or formerly partnered 602 51.6 246 42.8
Household income (as ratio to federal poverty line) <0.001
 <100% 268 13.6 267 41.5
 100–200% 628 48.7 240 45.5
 >200–300% 359 37.7 61 13.1
Smoking status 0.047
 Never smoker 624 48.8 246 42.1
 Current or former smoker 631 51.2 322 57.9

1From chi-squared tests.

Table 2 describes the mean scores of the cognitive assessments by food insecurity status. Across all cognitive assessments, the mean raw and standardized scores among food-insecure adults were significantly lower than the scores among food-secure adults. With respect to overall cognitive function z score, food-insecure adults had a 0.19-point lower unadjusted mean z score than food-secure adults (P = 0.002).

TABLE 2.

Mean scores on cognitive function assessments by food insecurity status among 1823 older adults with household incomes ≤300% of the federal poverty level: NHANES 2011–20141

Raw score (SE) z score (SE)
Food secure (n = 1255) Food insecure (n = 568) Food secure (n = 1255) Food insecure (n = 568)
CERAD word learning subtest 6.2 ± 0.09 6.0 ± 0.07* 0.02 ± 0.05 −0.13 ± 0.04*
CERAD delayed word recall 5.8 ± 0.11 5.6 ± 0.12 0.004 ± 0.05 −0.07 ± 0.05
Animal Fluency Test 16.7 ± 0.28 15.7 ± 0.41* 0.07 ± 0.05 −0.11 ± 0.07*
Digit Symbol Substitution Test 47.0 ± 0.69 40.0 ± 1.17* 0.09 ± 0.04 −0.31 ± 0.07*
Overall cognitive function score 0.04 ± 0.04 −0.15 ± 0.04*

1Values are means ± SEs. *Different from food secure, P < 0.05. CERAD, Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease.

Multivariate-adjusted associations between food insecurity and cognitive assessment z scores are shown in Table 3. After adjusting for age and sex, food insecurity was significantly associated with lower z scores for each individual cognitive test and the overall cognitive function score. After adjusting for all sociodemographic and health characteristics, food insecurity was associated with a lower overall cognitive function score (β = −0.15, 95% CI: −0.26, −0.05). Associations were also observed between food insecurity and the DSST z score (β = −0.24, 95% CI: −0.33, −0.15), AFT z score (β = −0.13, 95% CI: −0.25, −0.002), and the CERAD word learning subtest z score (β = −0.14, 95% CI: −0.26, −0.01). The association between food insecurity and CERAD delayed word recall was no longer significant after further adjustment of additional covariates.

TABLE 3.

Associations between food insecurity status and cognitive function assessment z scores among 1823 older adults with household incomes ≤300% of the federal poverty level: NHANES 2011–20141

β (95% CI)
Age and sex adjusted Multivariate adjusted2
CERAD word learning subtest −0.28 (−0.40, −0.17) −0.14 (−0.26, −0.01)
CERAD delayed word recall −0.22 (−0.36, −0.08) −0.10 (−0.25, 0.05)
Animal Fluency Test −0.31 (−0.49, −0.13) −0.13 (−0.25, −0.002)
Digit Symbol Substitution Test −0.55 (−0.68, −0.42) −0.24 (−0.33, −0.15)
Overall cognitive function score −0.36 (−0.48, −0.24) −0.15 (−0.26, −0.05)

1CERAD, Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease.

2Models adjusted for age, age as a quadratic term, sex, race/ethnicity, highest education level, marital status, household income (as a ratio to poverty guidelines), and smoking status.

Discussion

In this national sample of older adults, food insecurity was inversely associated with cognitive function, independent of sociodemographic factors. The cognitive skills most strongly associated with food insecurity were processing speed, sustained attention, verbal fluency, working memory, and immediate learning ability—all processes related to executive function. This study builds on the previous findings from the Boston Puerto Rican Health Study (23, 24), demonstrating that the associations between food insecurity and cognitive function are observed with varying measures of executive functions and extending the association to individuals beyond one particular ethnic group or geographic region. This study also corroborates a prior analysis by Frith and Loprinzi of food insecurity and cognitive function (40). Using data from 1999–2002 NHANES, the authors found that food insecurity was associated with lower DSST scores among older adults, and that the magnitude of the association was greater at more extreme levels of food insecurity. Results of the present study are consistent with studies that have focused on variations in adult cognitive function by poverty (41) or educational attainment (42–45). In the present study, the associations with food insecurity and cognitive function were observed even after the adjustment of education and income, suggesting food insecurity is another social determinant that may contribute to cognitive impairment in older adults.

Proposed mechanisms for the observed associations include inadequate dietary intake and increased psychological stress. Previous research has found that food-insecure adults have lower intakes of important food groups (e.g. fruits, vegetables) (21), key nutrients (46), and lower diet quality than food-secure adults (47). Consuming a healthy diet has been shown to be important for maintaining cognitive health in later life (48, 49). A second mechanism is the role of stress and allostatic load. Food insecurity is an inherently stressful experience that has been associated with poor mental health (13, 50). In a prior study of older adults, food insecurity was associated with dysregulation of the allostatic load system (22). This repeated “wear and tear” on the body's systems has been associated with brain changes that dampen one's ability to cognitively respond to psychological stress, subsequently leading to cognitive decline (51, 52). The evidence to date suggests that the direction of association is likely food insecurity influencing cognitive function. This prospective finding has been observed in 2 studies. One of these was the Boston Puerto Rican Health Study, which showed that food insecurity among older adults was associated with cognitive decline over a 2-y period (24). The other study was conducted among Malaysian older adults and found that food insufficiency during childhood was predictive of dementia in later life (53). No studies have shown the reverse association—that cognitive impairment contributes to food insecurity among older adults. However, the study of food insecurity among older populations is sparse and it is plausible that cognitive impairment could exacerbate food insecurity through a strain on one's finances, inability to adopt healthy behaviors, and a lack of social support. Although more research is needed to establish the direction and magnitude of the observed association, the present study also highlights the need for programs and policies that address the concurrent burden of food insecurity and cognitive impairment among older adults.

This study has several strengths: 1) a large and recent national sample of adults, 2) the creation of an overall cognitive function z score to avoid floor or ceiling effects of individual cognitive tasks, and 3) the ability to control for the effects of multiple sociodemographic and health confounders in the model. In addition to the cross-sectional design of the study, another limitation is the potential for unmeasured confounding by psychosocial factors (e.g. social support), environmental factors (e.g. urban compared with rural neighborhood, neighborhood socioeconomic status), or other unknown factors that could bias the association, as these data were not available from NHANES. The sociodemographic covariates included in the present analysis were considered the strongest known confounders for the association between food insecurity and cognitive function. There may also be independent differential measurement error if individuals with undiagnosed mild cognitive impairment or Alzheimer's disease misreport their food security status and have poorer performance on the cognitive tasks, which could have biased the observed results. Further research is needed with populations across broader age ranges and with repeated measures of food insecurity and cognitive function to better understand the nature of these associations over time and life stages, and to better account for undiagnosed cognitive impairment over time. This study was also not designed to test potential mediating pathways, primarily due to the cross-sectional nature of the data. Prospective cohort studies are needed to better understand the extent to which health behaviors, psychological stress, and chronic disease burden mediate the pathways between food insecurity and cognitive function in older adults.

In conclusion, this study shows that food insecurity is associated with lower cognitive function among older adults, which may translate into higher risk of cognitive impairment over time. Programs and policies that improve the availability of nutritious food are needed to alleviate both food insecurity and maintain cognitive health in this vulnerable population.

Acknowledgments

The authors’ responsibilities were as follows—ETP-P and CWL: designed the research study; ETP-P: analyzed data and prepared the first draft of the manuscript; CWL: critically revised the manuscript and had primary responsibility for the final content; both authors: read and approved the final manuscript.

Notes

CWL was supported by grant 1K99 HD084758 from the NIH.

Author disclosures: ETP-P and CWL, no conflicts of interest.

Abbreviations used: AFT, Animal Fluency Test; CERAD, Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease; DSST, Digit Symbol Substitution Test; FPL, federal poverty level; MEC, mobile examination center.

References

  • 1. Ziliak JP, Gundersen C. The State of Senior Hunger in America 2016: an Annual Report. Feeding America and the National Foundation to End Senior Hunger. 2018. [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Perez-Escamilla R, Vianna RP. Food insecurity and the behavioral and intellectual development of children: a review of the evidence. [Internet] J Appl Res Child. 2012;3(1) Available from: https://eric.ed.gov/?id=EJ1189045. [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Seligman HK, Laraia BA, Kushel MB. Food insecurity is associated with chronic disease among low-income NHANES participants. J Nutr. 2010;140(2):304–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Berkowitz SA, Berkowitz TSZ, Meigs JB, Wexler DJ. Trends in food insecurity for adults with cardiometabolic disease in the United States: 2005–2012. PloS One. 2017;12(6):e0179172. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Mendy VL, Vargas R, Cannon-Smith G, Payton M, Enkhmaa B, Zhang L. Food insecurity and cardiovascular disease risk factors among Mississippi adults. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2018;15(9):E2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Seligman HK, Bindman AB, Vittinghoff E, Kanaya AM, Kushel MB. Food insecurity is associated with diabetes mellitus: results from the National Health Examination and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES) 1999–2002. J Gen Int Med. 2007;22(7):1018–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Ford ES. Food security and cardiovascular disease risk among adults in the United States: findings from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey, 2003–2008. [Internet]. Prev Chron Dis. 2013;10:E202.Available from: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24309090. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. De Nardi M, French E, Jones JB, McCauley J. Medical Spending of the U.S. Elderly (Working Paper 21270). NBER Working Paper Series. Cambridge (MA): National Bureau of Economic Research; 2015. [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Jih J, Stijacic-Cenzer I, Seligman HK, Boscardin WJ, Nguyen TT, Ritchie CS. Chronic disease burden predicts food insecurity among older adults. Public Health Nutr. 2018;21(9):1737–42. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Sattler EL, Lee JS. Persistent food insecurity is associated with higher levels of cost-related medication nonadherence in low-income older adults. J Nutr Gerontol Geriatr. 2013;32(1):41–58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Afulani P, Herman D, Coleman-Jensen A, Harrison GG. Food insecurity and health outcomes among older adults: the role of cost-related medication underuse. J Nutr Gerontol Geriatr. 2015;34(3):319–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Garcia SP, Haddix A, Barnett K. Incremental health care costs associated with food insecurity and chronic conditions among older adults. [Internet]. Prev Chron Dis. 2018;15:180058 Available from: https://www.cdc.gov/pcd/issues/2018/18_0058.htm. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Leung CW, Epel ES, Willett WC, Rimm EB, Laraia BA. Household food insecurity is positively associated with depression among low-income Supplemental Nutrition Assistance Program participants and income-eligible nonparticipants. J Nutr. 2015;145(3):622–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Whitaker RC, Phillips SM, Orzol SM. Food insecurity and the risks of depression and anxiety in mothers and behavior problems in their preschool-aged children. Pediatrics. 2006;118(3):e859–68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Alaimo K, Olson CM, Frongillo EA. Family food insufficiency, but not low family income, is positively associated with dysthymia and suicide symptoms in adolescents. J Nutr. 2002;132(4):719–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Bronte-Tinkew J, Zaslow M, Capps R, Horowitz A, McNamara M. Food insecurity works through depression, parenting, and infant feeding to influence overweight and health in toddlers. J Nutr. 2007;137(9):2160–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. McIntyre L, Williams JV, Lavorato DH, Patten S. Depression and suicide ideation in late adolescence and early adulthood are an outcome of child hunger. J Affect Disord. 2013;150(1):123–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Melchior M, Caspi A, Howard LM, Ambler AP, Bolton H, Mountain N, Moffitt TE. Mental health context of food insecurity: a representative cohort of families with young children. Pediatrics. 2009;124(4):e564–72. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Alaimo K, Olson CM, Frongillo EA Jr.. Food insufficiency and American school-aged children's cognitive, academic, and psychosocial development. Pediatrics. 2001;108(1):44–53. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Jyoti DF, Frongillo EA, Jones SJ. Food insecurity affects school children's academic performance, weight gain, and social skills. J Nutr. 2005;135(12):2831–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Hanson KL, Connor LM. Food insecurity and dietary quality in US adults and children: a systematic review. Am J Clin Nutr. 2014;100(2):684–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. McClain AC, Xiao RS, Gao X, Tucker KL, Falcon LM, Mattei J. Food insecurity and odds of high allostatic load in Puerto Rican adults: the role of participation in the Supplemental Nutrition Assistance Program during 5 years of follow-up. Psychosom Med. 2018;80(8):733–41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Gao X, Scott T, Falcon LM, Wilde PE, Tucker KL. Food insecurity and cognitive function in Puerto Rican adults. Am J Clin Nutr. 2009;89(4):1197–203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Wong JC, Scott T, Wilde P, Li YG, Tucker KL, Gao X. Food insecurity is associated with subsequent cognitive decline in the Boston Puerto Rican Health Study. J Nutr. 2016;146(9):1740–5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. National Center for Health Statistics (NCHS), Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. NHANES—National Health and Nutrition Surveys. 2019; [Internet][accessed 2017 Oct 4]. Available from: http://www.cdc.gov/nchs/nhanes.htm. [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Morrissey TW, Jacknowitz A, Vinopal K. Local food prices and their associations with children's weight and food security. Pediatrics. 2014;133(3):422–30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Kimbro RT, Denney JT. Transitions into food insecurity associated with behavioral problems and worse overall health among children. Health Aff (Millwood). 2015;34(11):1949–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Gamba R, Leung CW, Guendelman S, Lahiff M, Laraia B. Household food insecurity is not associated with overall diet quality among pregnant women in NHANES 1999–2008. Mat Child Health J. 2016;20(11):2348–56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Ziliak JP, Gundersen C, Haist M. The causes, consequences, and future of senior hunger in America. Lexington (KY): University of Kentucky, Center for Poverty Research; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Gundersen C, Ziliak JP. Food insecurity and health outcomes. Health Aff (Millwood). 2015;34(11):1830–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. National Center for Health Statistics (NCHS), Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. NHANES 2011–2012: Cognitive Functioning Data Documentation, Codebook, and Frequencies. 2017. [Internet]. [accessed 2017 Oct 4] Available from: https://wwwn.cdc.gov/Nchs/Nhanes/2011-2012/CFQ_G.htm. [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Morris JC, Heyman A, Mohs RC, Hughes JP, van Belle G, Fillenbaum G, Mellits ED, Clark C. The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease (CERAD). Part I. Clinical and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer's disease. Neurology. 1989;39(9):1159–65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Strauss E, Sherman EMS, Spreen O. A compendium of neuropsychological tests: administration, norms, and commentary(3rd edition). New York: Oxford University Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Weschler D. WAIS Manual. Third Edition New York: Psychological Corporation; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Matarazzo JD, Herman DO. Base rate data for the WAIS-R: test-retest stability and VIQ-PIQ differences. J Clin Neuropsychol. 1984;6(4):351–66. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Afrdila A, Ostrosky-Solis F, Bernal B. Cognitive testing toward the future: the example of Semantic Verbal Fluency (ANIMALS). Int J Psychol. 2007;41(5):324–32. [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Wolfsgruber S, Jessen F, Wiese B, Stein J, Bickel H, Mosch E, Weyerer S, Werle J, Pentzek M, Fuchs A et al.. The CERAD neuropsychological assessment battery total score detects and predicts Alzheimer disease dementia with high diagnostic accuracy. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2014;22(10):1017–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Karrasch M, Sinerva E, Gronholm P, Rinne J, Laine M. CERAD test performances in amnestic mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease. Acta Neurol Scand. 2005;111(3):172–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Li H, Wang Z, Fu Z, Yan M, Wu N, Wu H, Yin P. Associations between blood cadmium levels and cognitive function in a cross-sectional study of US adults aged 60 years or older. BMJ Open. 2018;8(4):e020533. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Frith E, Loprinzi PD. Food insecurity and cognitive function in older adults: brief report. Clin Nutr. 2018;37(5):1765–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Mani A, Mullainathan S, Shafir E, Zhao J. Poverty impedes cognitive function. Science. 2013;341(6149):976–80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Manly JJ, Touradji P, Tang MX, Stern Y. Literacy and memory decline among ethnically diverse elders. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2003;25(5):680–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Alvarado BE, Zunzunegui MV, Del Ser T, Beland F. Cognitive decline is related to education and occupation in a Spanish elderly cohort. Aging Clin Exp Res. 2002;14(2):132–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Cagney KA, Lauderdale DS. Education, wealth, and cognitive function in later life. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2002;57(2):P163–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Wilson RS, Hebert LE, Scherr PA, Barnes LL, Mendes de Leon CF, Evans DA. Educational attainment and cognitive decline in old age. Neurology. 2009;72(5):460–5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Lee JS, Frongillo EA Jr.. Nutritional and health consequences are associated with food insecurity among U.S. elderly persons. J Nutr. 2001;131(5):1503–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Leung CW, Epel ES, Ritchie LD, Crawford PB, Laraia BA. Food insecurity is inversely associated with diet quality of lower-income adults. J Acad Nutr Diet. 2014;114(12):1943–53. e2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Gardener SL, Rainey-Smith SR. The role of nutrition in cognitive function and brain ageing in the elderly. Curr Nutr Rep. 2018;7(3):139–49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Solfrizzi V, Custodero C, Lozupone M, Imbimbo BP, Valiani V, Agosti P, Schilardi A, D'Introno A, La Montagna M, Calvani M et al.. Relationships of dietary patterns, foods, and micro- and macronutrients with Alzheimer's disease and late-life cognitive disorders: a systematic review. J Alzheimers Dis. 2017;59(3):815–49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Bruening M, Dinour LM, Chavez JBR. Food insecurity and emotional health in the USA: a systematic narrative review of longitudinal research. Public Health Nutr. 2017;20(17):3200–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Juster RP, McEwen BS, Lupien SJ. Allostatic load biomarkers of chronic stress and impact on health and cognition. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2010;35(1):2–16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Seeman TE, Singer BH, Rowe JW, Horwitz RI, McEwen BS. Price of adaptation–allostatic load and its health consequences. MacArthur studies of successful aging. Arch Intern Med. 1997;157(19):2259–68. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Momtaz YA, Haron SA, Hamid TA, Ibrahim R, Masud J. Does food insufficiency in childhood contribute to dementia in later life?. Clin Interv Aging. 2015;10:49–53. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from The Journal of Nutrition are provided here courtesy of American Society for Nutrition

RESOURCES