Abstract
Background
Low vision in childhood is a significant barrier to learning and development, particularly for reading and education. Optical low vision aids may be used to maximise the child's functional vision. The World Health Organization (WHO) has previously highlighted the importance of the use of low vision aids in managing children with visual impairment across the world.
Objectives
To assess the effect of optical low vision aids on reading in children and young people with low vision.
Search methods
We searched CENTRAL (which contains the Cochrane Eyes and Vision Group Trials Register) (2014, Issue 12), Ovid MEDLINE, Ovid MEDLINE In‐Process and Other Non‐Indexed Citations, Ovid MEDLINE Daily, Ovid OLDMEDLINE (January 1946 to January 2015), EMBASE (January 1980 to January 2015), the Health Technology Assessment Programme (HTA) (www.hta.ac.uk/), the ISRCTN registry (www.isrctn.com/editAdvancedSearch), ClinicalTrials.gov (www.clinicaltrials.gov) and the World Health Organization (WHO) International Clinical Trials Registry Platform (ICTRP) (www.who.int/ictrp/search/en). We did not use any date or language restrictions in the electronic searches for trials. We last searched the electronic databases on 8 January 2015.
We also used manual searching to check the references listed in retrieved articles. Manufacturers of low vision aids were contacted to request any information about studies or research regarding their products.
Selection criteria
We planned to include randomised controlled trials (RCTs) and quasi‐RCTs where any optical low vision aid was compared to standard refractive correction in children and young people aged between 5 and 16 years of age with low vision as defined by the WHO. We planned to include within‐person design studies where the order of presentation of devices was randomised.
Data collection and analysis
Two authors independently reviewed the search results for eligibility .
Main results
No studies met the inclusion criteria for this review.
Authors' conclusions
There is a lack of good quality evidence regarding the use of optical low vision aids in children and young people. As such, no implications for practice can be drawn. We believe future research should include functional outcome measures such as reading speed, accuracy and comprehension, as well as the effect of low vision aids on quality of life, in order to truly assess and compare the effect of these devices on a child's life and development.
Keywords: Humans; Reading; Optical Devices; Vision, Low; Vision, Low/rehabilitation
Plain language summary
Magnifying reading aids for children and teenagers with low vision
Background Low vision in children and teenagers not only affects reading, learning and education but is also thought to have a significant effect on a child's general development. Magnifying reading aids can assist a child or teenager to make the best use of the vision they have.
We reviewed the current evidence on the use of magnifying reading aids in children between the ages of 5 and 16 years of age with low vision, when compared to the use of glasses alone. We included magnifying aids such as hand‐ or stand‐held magnifying glasses, telescopes or binoculars but we excluded electronic reading aids which will be the subject of a separate review.
Search date The electronic databases were last searched on 8 January 2015.
We found no studies which met our criteria for inclusion in this review. We recommend that future studies in this area consider the effect of magnifying reading aids on reading speed and accuracy as well as simply the size of text correctly seen, in order to assess more fully the effect of the reading aid on a child's normal function. We also recommend that the effect of the reading aid on the child's quality of life is investigated as this may have significant implications on its regular and/or long‐term use.
Background
Description of the condition
In 2004, the World Health Organization (WHO) reported that more than 161 million people worldwide were visually impaired, with 124 million classified as having low vision and 37 million classified as blind (defined as visual acuity less than 3/60 in the better‐seeing eye (World Health Organization 2004). In children, the prevalence of blindness varies from 0.3/1000 in high‐income countries to over 1.0/1000 in low‐ and middle‐income countries, equating to around 1.4 million blind children worldwide (Gilbert 2001; World Health Organization 2000). Low vision is about twice as common as childhood blindness, and might affect almost 3 million children worldwide (Gilbert 2008a; Gilbert 2008b).
The leading causes of low vision in children worldwide are retinal conditions, corneal scarring (caused by vitamin A deficiency, measles, harmful traditional practices), globe anomalies, cataract, optic nerve anomalies, glaucoma, and central nervous system disorders (Gilbert 2001). A recent study in Nepal identified corneal disease as the leading cause of visual impairment, followed by retinal disease and lens pathology. In 46% of children, however, the cause of visual loss could not be identified (Shrestha 2012). In high‐income countries, brain damage sustained around the time of birth has become the leading cause of severe visual impairment (Bodeau‐Livinec 2007; Mitry 2013; Rahi 2003). In England and Wales, the commonest conditions in children with impaired, but not severely impaired sight, are hereditary retinal conditions or congenital globe abnormalities (Mitry 2013). In the United Kingdom (UK), there are an estimated 25,000 children with vision impairment (VI) or severe vision impairment/blindness (SVI/BL) (Morris 2008). The cumulative incidence of SVI/BL by 16 years of age is 5.9, and that of VI around 7 per 10,000 live births (Bodeau‐Livinec 2007; Rahi 2003). About 950 new cases of VI or SVI/BL are diagnosed each year (Bodeau‐Livinec 2007).
Children are considered to have 'low vision' when the corrected visual acuity (VA) is between less than 6/18 and light perception in their better eye, or their visual field is less than 10 degrees from the point of fixation, but they use, or are potentially able to use, vision for the planning or execution, or both, of a task (World Health Organization 1992). There is an overlap between the definitions of VI and SVI/BL. The exact definition of childhood blindness is variable, but usually ranges between a best‐corrected visual acuity of less than 6/60 to 3/60 in the better‐seeing eye in a young person under the age of 15 years (Gilbert 2001; World Health Organization 2004).
Visual impairment can result in developmental delay by reducing the range of experiences to which the child is exposed. Early assessment with provision and training of low vision aids (LVAs) is essential to improve functional vision and adaptation to visual impairment, so allowing most children to enter and remain in mainstream schools (Ducrey 1998; Massof 1998; Silver 1976). In the UK, approximately 70% of children with VI are educated in mainstream schools where the use of LVAs to enable use of printed educational materials is essential (Morris 2008). In the developing world, access to enlarged print, or methods to enlarge text (i.e. computers or photocopiers) is more spartan, and magnifiers can be provided as a cheaper and more transportable option for children with low vision. Epidemiological studies in Pakistan have demonstrated that the provision of basic magnification aids would permit at least 11% of children currently educated in schools for the blind to be moved to mainstream schooling (Sight Savers International 2003). This estimate, however, was based on a sample of 1000 children in schools for the blind and was subject to selection bias due to the small percentage of children with low vision currently being educated in special schools in low‐ and middle‐income countries; the overall potential for improvement is significantly higher. In Nepal, optical intervention provided a significant improvement to the vision of 48.2% of children in schools for the blind, enabling those learning braille to learn to read visually, or visually in conjunction with braille (Gnyawali 2012). Despite this improvement however, only 34.8% of children were still using their LVA one year later. Damage or loss was the most common reason reported for cessation of use; however, inadequate instruction and inappropriate setting/lighting were also reported, both of which highlight the vital importance of maintenance of equipment ‐ however basic ‐ and instruction to enable its use (Gnyawali 2012).
Description of the intervention
An LVA can be defined as any device that enables a person with low vision to improve visual performance. LVAs can be classified into optical aids (magnifiers) and electronic 'assistive technologies' (AT). Non‐optical aids (filters, tinted lenses and coloured overlays) are also sometimes used to enhance vision, but are less frequently used in children with VI and will, therefore, not be included in this review.
Commonly used optical aids include:
magnifiers: hand and stand magnifiers, with and without illumination; in general, the higher the magnification the greater the restriction of visual field
high dioptric power reading glasses or near adds in bifocal glasses (above +4.00 DS and up to +20.00 DS)
distance telescopes or binoculars: a hand‐held or spectacle‐mounted lens system that provides magnification at greater distance
electronic magnification: the longest established form of electronic magnification uses closed‐circuit television (CCTV).
Other devices increasingly used in educational settings include screen‐magnifying and screen‐reading software operated on computers (desktops, laptops, tablets). For the purpose of this review, we will exclude devices that include monitors to display enlarged text. The present review will focus on optical LVAs; assistive technologies including CCTV will be the topic of a second review. No review is planned on non‐optical visual aids.
A different magnification strategy used in educational settings is the enlargement of hardcopy printed material. Decisions about which strategy is superior, i.e. LVAs or text modification, depend on the outcomes selected for evaluation. Any form of visual support, i.e. LVAs or enlarged print, can be expected to facilitate access to the educational curriculum and to enable a child to develop better reading and literacy skills. Compared with text enlargement, LVAs may have the additional advantage of providing children and young people with greater independence of access to printed material (Corn 2002; Douglas 2011). However, peer pressure and the fear of 'standing out' may reduce usage of LVAs by children and young people (Mason 1999).
Why it is important to do this review
Improving functional vision in children with VI is important for enabling education and personal development, and for improving vision‐related quality of life. The previously held belief that children with low vision should be treated as children with no vision may, in the past, have hampered the study and use of LVAs. The WHO identified and highlighted the provision, education and use of LVAs in children as a priority in managing children with VI (World Health Organization 1992).
The use and benefit of LVAs in adults is well documented, although the need for further research into the comparative benefits of different types of visual aids was highlighted by a previous Cochrane review (Margrain 2000; Virgili 2013). Multiple studies document the use and subjective benefit of LVAs in children (de Carvalho 1998; Haddad 2006; Haddad 2009), and training in the use of magnifiers has been shown to improve the beneficial effects of their use (Cox 2009). There appears to be, however, a lack of agreement and comparative data on relevant outcomes and benefits of LVAs in children and young people. LVA users, i.e. children, their families and carers, as well as healthcare providers or commissioners, require high‐quality evidence to make informed choices about allocation of personal, institutional and public resources. Facilitating reading and literacy in children and young people not only optimises individuals' access to education and employment, but also benefits society. The rationale for this review is, therefore, to provide critical evaluation of information that is already available from high‐quality trials, and to delineate a framework for future research and practice policies in low‐income, middle‐income, and high‐income countries.
Objectives
To assess the effect of optical low vision aids on reading in children and young people with low vision.
Methods
Criteria for considering studies for this review
Types of studies
We planned to include randomised controlled trials (RCTs) and quasi‐RCTs in this review. We considered within‐person design studies, in which the order of presentation of devices was randomised, as quasi‐RCTs. Within‐person studies are similar in design to conventional cross‐over studies, but instead of offering interventions sequentially, LVA studies frequently offer these simultaneously and measure outcomes sequentially, in the same session.
Non‐randomised studies were excluded from the review, although if sufficient relevant studies are identified in future updates of this review, they might be included in the discussion.
Types of participants
We planned to include trials involving children between the ages of 5 and 16 years with low vision as defined by, or equivalent to, the WHO 1992 definition (World Health Organization 1992):
“A person with low vision is one who has impairment of visual functioning even after treatment and/or standard refractive correction, and has a visual acuity of less than 6/18 to light perception, or a visual field of less than 10° from the point of fixation, but who uses, or is potentially able to use, vision for the planning and/or execution of a task”.
We excluded children of pre‐school age, as young children tend to hold objects close to their face to achieve magnification, and LVAs are not usually prescribed for this age group. When LVAs are issued to children under the age of five years, the aim is to introduce children to the concept of magnifying devices in a playful manner, and not actually to improve access to visual information.
Types of interventions
We planned to include studies that assess optical visual aids. These included non‐electronic magnifiers of all types. We did not include CCTV, which, although it magnifies, is an electronic device. A separate Cochrane review will explore the effects of assistive technologies in the same population. The motivation for splitting the topic into two reviews has its basis in the different objectives of these technologies. Optical aids are prescribed to facilitate reading and access to printed material by providing magnification. Electronic assistive technologies have a broader aim: facilitating access to education, but also to social media and real‐time information available via the internet, for example maps/directions, educational or leisure activities offered in the vicinity etc. As such, a comparison of optical aids with assistive technologies would be difficult, as trial outcomes would be limited to the smallest common denominator, i.e. reading‐related outcomes. We planned to include studies in which both mediums were used, however, if it was possible to isolate the data relating to the use of optical aids alone.
We aimed to compare the use of optical aids with standard practice, which consists of standard refractive correction including any required near add up to +4.00 DS in aphakic (lacking a lens) or pseudophakic patients. We also would have included studies that compared different types of optical aids with each other, as we aimed to compare optical aids provided with, or without, instructions for use.
Types of outcome measures
Outcome measures for objective outcomes include near visual acuity, distance visual acuity, and reading accuracy, comprehension and speed (Binns 2012). A range of questionnaires is available to measure functional outcomes relating to activities of daily living (ADL), psychological status, and quality of life (both voice‐related (VRQoL) and health‐related (HRQoL)), such as the Manchester Low Vision Questionnaire (MLVQ) (Harper 1999), the Low Vision Quality of Life Questionnaire (LVQoL) (Wolffsohn 2000), the National Eye Institute Visual Function Questionnaire (NEI‐VFQ) (Mangione 1998; Mangione 2001), and the Impact of Vision Impairment profile (IVI) (Hassell 2000; Weih 2002). Only a few tools have been developed and validated for use in children and young people, and even fewer have been developed with focus groups of children and young people. Examples include the Impact of Vision Impairment Profile for Children (IVI_C) (Cochrane 2011), the Cardiff Visual Ability Questionnaire for Children (CVAQL) to assess VRQoL (Khadka 2010), the Functional Vision Questionnaire for Children and Young People with Visual Impairment (FVQ CYP) (Tadic 2013), and the general health‐related Pediatric Quality of Life Inventory (Varni 2001; Varni 2002). Some tools gather the views of parents/carers about their own as well as the child's quality of life (Gothwal 2015; Varni 2001; Varni 2002).
Usage of LVAs is an additional important outcome measure, as it may reflect pragmatic and emotional difficulties with using devices. Peer pressure and the fear of 'standing out' may lead to optical aids being used infrequently or abandoned (Mason 1999). Usage of the LVA is likely to be a more specific outcome measure than VRQoL and HRQoL tools.
Whilst the main aim of LVAs is to magnify visual information, near visual acuity, reflecting successful enlargement of text, is of limited value as a main outcome measure. Reading speed may be the most appropriate primary outcome, as it evaluates the functional visual effect of the aid. Reading performance has been found to be one of the best predictors of patient‐reported visual ability and VRQoL (Hazel 2000; McClure 2000). Reading is an important function in daily life. It is a standard outcome in studies monitoring conditions causing VI and in clinical trials evaluating the effectiveness of interventions (Rubin 2013). Maximum reading speed may be the most commonly used outcome in assessing the effect of reading aids, and is the primary outcome explored in a Cochrane review on reading aids for adults with low vision (Virgili 2013). It is typically stable across a range of print sizes over a certain threshold (critical print size), whereas at smaller print sizes, below the critical print size, the reading speed slows and the reading acuity limit is reached (Ahn 1995a; Ahn 1995b; Bailey 2003). Using standardised reading charts such as those in the Minnesota Low‐Vision Reading test (MNREAD), a plot of reading speed against font size (adjusted for reading distance and expressed in logMar) can be obtained (Legge 2007). Typically, reading speed also slows above a certain magnification due to the restricted field of view and a lack of a proportional increase in the size of saccades (fast movements of the eyes) (Dickinson 2000).
The use of different font sizes in various studies is a methodological problem for meta‐analysis. The most recent update of the Cochrane review on reading aids for adults with low vision included only studies assessing reading speed "when reading ordinary print size", i.e. 10 to 14 points (Virgili 2013). However, there is no universal agreement on ordinary print size for children. Books for young readers frequently use a large font size, i.e. 14 points or larger. School textbooks frequently reduce font size as their target audience matures, but there are no standards, and no recommendations as to when 'standard adult font size' (usually 9 to 14 points) should be used.
The type of reading material also influences reading speed. Research studies often use standardised reading charts such as the MNREAD and, more recently, the International Reading Speed Texts (IReST). Repeated, standardised assessment of reading performance requires a collection of texts of similar difficulty. Whilst the MNREAD chart contains single short sentences, IReST consists of 10 paragraphs of texts (around 130 words each) and offers the advantage of a longer paragraph, which facilitates more accurate measurement of reading speed and judgement of fluency and mistakes (Trauzettel‐Klosinski 2012). IReST has been evaluated in a cohort of normal sighted young adults and in patients with age‐related macular degeneration, but has not been validated in children and young people.
In addition to reading performance, literacy outcomes such as reading accuracy and comprehension can give additional functional information. A measure of reading ability used in children with VI is the Neale Analysis of Reading Ability (NARA), currently available in its second edition (NARA II) (Neale 1997). This is a comprehensive assessment of reading ability aimed for use with pupils aged 6 to 12 years, and is also recommended for use beyond the age of 12 years in children with sensory impairment. The test material consists of 6 paragraphs that increase in length from 26 to 140 words, and increase in difficulty.The test is designed to assess oral reading ability in terms of reading rate, accuracy and comprehension. Validation data are available for normally sighted individuals, and also for children and young people with visual impairment (Douglas 2002; Hill 2005). There are two parallel versions of the test, which permits the same child to be re‐tested without remembering a previous test and thereby altering the score. The child’s scores are converted into reading ages for accuracy, comprehension and speed. Accuracy is determined by noting reading errors such as mispronunciations, substitutions, refusals, additions, omissions and reversals. Comprehension is measured by asking the child a number of set questions concerning the passage he/she has just read. Reading speed is measured by timing the passages read and converting this into words per minute over the total number of passages read. Results can be plotted as graphs comparing the performance of VI‐students with normal‐sighted age‐matched peers (Douglas 2002; Hill 2005).
All literacy evaluations need to take into account that children are learning to read, i.e. are developing a skill. Children with low vision often read print more slowly and less accurately than normally sighted peers (Douglas 2004; Gompel 2004). Comprehension may also be delayed; this may be linked to general delay in reading development (Douglas 2002). Other literacy tests used in educational settings, such as the National Foundation for Educational Research (NFER) and Access Reading Test (ART), include access features for children with low vision (enlarged print, braille, extended time), but no data from children with low vision are available.
Primary outcomes
Maximum reading speed in words per minute using MNREAD, IRest, NARA, NARA II or equivalent test in another language.
Secondary outcomes
Any of the following outcomes assessed using a standardised chart such as MNRead or IReST, or a standardised literacy test such as NARA.
Reading accuracy as errors per words read.
Reading comprehension as number of correctly answered set questions concerning the text read.
Reading acuity in logMar, defined as the smallest print that the child/young person can read without making significant errors.
Critical print size, defined as the smallest print that the child/young person can read with maximum speed.
Fatigue‐free reading duration in minutes.
Secondary outcomes with different means of assessment (i.e. not standardised chart or literacy test).
Acceptance of the LVA, as reflected in usage (days per week, hours per day, at home and at school).
Independent learning, i.e. ability to access the curriculum independently, as assessed by questionnaires.
VRQoL, evaluated using any validated VRQoL scale for children.
HRQoL evaluated using any validated HRQoL scale for children.
Cost effectiveness.
Adverse outcomes, for example loss of motivation to use the device.
With regard to the time points of evaluation, general child development and, particularly, the development of reading and literacy skills will affect the effect size of interventions at given time points. One would expect an increase in reading speed with time as a younger child learns to read, regardless of LVA use, but using an aid may allow faster development of reading skills. On the other hand, a child’s ability may have improved to a degree over that period of time, just as his/her general development has progressed.
For this review, we considered the following time points: Primary outcome: 3 and 12 months (+/‐ 3 months) after the intervention and relevant instructions, if any, have been issued, where 3 months was a proof of concept. Secondary outcomes: 12 months (+/‐ 3 months). Ultimate outcomes such as educational attainment, as measured in educational progress, would be desirable, but due to the length of follow‐up required, these are unlikely to be captured in research studies.
Search methods for identification of studies
Electronic searches
We searched CENTRAL (which contains the Cochrane Eyes and Vision Group Trials Register) (2014, Issue 12), Ovid MEDLINE, Ovid MEDLINE In‐Process and Other Non‐Indexed Citations, Ovid MEDLINE Daily, Ovid OLDMEDLINE (January 1946 to January 2015), EMBASE (January 1980 to January 2015), the Health Technology Assessment Programme (HTA) (www.hta.ac.uk/), the ISRCTN registry (www.isrctn.com/editAdvancedSearch), ClinicalTrials.gov (www.clinicaltrials.gov) and the World Health Organization (WHO) International Clinical Trials Registry Platform (ICTRP) (www.who.int/ictrp/search/en). We did not use any date or language restrictions in the electronic searches for trials. We last searched the electronic databases on 8 January 2015.
See: Appendices for details of search strategies for CENTRAL (Appendix 1), MEDLINE (Appendix 2), EMBASE (Appendix 3), HTA (Appendix 4), ISRCTN (Appendix 5), ClinicalTrials.gov (Appendix 6) and the ICTRP (Appendix 7).
Searching other resources
We also used manual searching to check the references listed in retrieved articles. Manufacturers of LVAs were contacted to request any information of which they are aware about studies or research regarding their products.
Data collection and analysis
Selection of studies
Independently, two review authors (LB and AHDN) assessed titles and abstracts for eligibility. Studies were divided into categories to 'definitely include', 'definitely exclude' and 'possibly include', and final judgements about inclusion/exclusion were made by obtaining full‐text copies of the studies in the 'possibly include' category. Abstracts and, where necessary, full‐text articles were translated into English before a final decision was made regarding inclusion/exclusion. Disagreements between the two review authors was to be resolved by discussion or a designated third author, or both, but no disagreement occurred.
Data extraction and management
We had planned for two authors to independently extract data using the data extraction form (Appendix 8) developed in conjunction with the Cochrane Eyes and Vision Group using Chapter 7 of the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions for guidance (Higgins 2011a). Data were to be entered into Review Manager (RevMan 2014) software by one author and to be independently reviewed and cross‐checked by a second.
For continuous data, data on the mean and standard deviation (SD) in each group were to be extracted. RevMan was to be used to calculate the mean difference and 95% confidence intervals (CI). When dealing with cross‐over studies we planned to use the generic inverse variance method as described in Chapter 16 of the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions for guidance (Higgins 2011b).
Assessment of risk of bias in included studies
Two authors were to work independently to review the risk of bias of included studies using the Cochrane Collaboration's 'Risk of Bias' assessment tool, detailed in Chapter 8 of the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Review of Interventions (Higgins 2011c), as guidance. The five main domains of the tool include:
Selection bias
Studies were to be graded by review authors as 'high risk', 'low risk' or 'unclear risk' based on the method of randomisation (sequence generation) and allocation concealment. If an 'unclear risk' assessment is made, study authors are contacted to provide further information to enable a more detailed risk assessment to be made.
Performance bias
Masking of participants is not possible given the nature of the intervention in question. We did not, therefore, plan to grade studies on the basis of masking alone. A judgement regarding performance bias was to be made by the review authors, taking into consideration the instruction and education given to participants for each visual aid and the 'learning‐effect' time allocated before the final assessment was made.
Attrition bias
Incomplete outcome data was to be recorded and attempts were to be made to contact the study authors in order to obtain complete data. A judgement of 'high risk', 'low risk' or 'unclear risk' of attrition bias was to be made by review authors with regard to the completeness of the data and the handling of incomplete data in the studies.
Detection bias
Masking of study investigators and personnel is not possible due to the nature of the intervention in question. Detection bias would occur if the allocated intervention, i.e. use of the optical aid, was visible to the outcome assessor. One way of reducing this risk would be to record reading on audiotape, or as audiofile, and later to have masked evaluation by a masked observer. We planned to judge studies on use of masking strategies. Detection bias may, in turn, affect reporting, if assessors then chose to exclude some participants or type of devices based on performance, thereby introducing reporting bias.
Reporting bias
Where a study protocol was available, the review authors were to compare the published protocol with the final outcomes reported to assess the risk of selective outcome reporting as 'high risk', 'low risk' or 'unclear risk'. Where no protocol was available, the full‐text article was to be studied to make this judgement.
Other bias
The review authors planned to judge whether each study design was subject to any other risks for the introduction of bias that are not detailed above. In particular, we expected studies with within‐person design to be commonly used in assessing the effects of LVAs, as a previous Cochrane review found this to be the case in studies investigating the use of LVAs in adults (Virgili 2013). A particular problem with this design in paediatric studies is that by the time participants start using a second intervention they may have matured and acquired more skills, which may influence the effect size of the second intervention. Chapter 16 of the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions recommends that when cross‐over studies are evaluated, review authors should take into account whether the condition is chronic and stable, whether the intervention provides temporary relief and not permanent change, whether the outcome can be repeated in the second period if it occurs in the first, whether the effect of the first intervention lasts into the second treatment period, and whether trial length is sufficient to allow appropriate use of the intervention (Higgins 2011b).
Within‐person studies can provide randomisation by including independent sequence generation and allocation concealment. We planned to grade both sequence generation and allocation concealment as factors carrying a 'low risk' of bias in these studies, as in a previous review focussing on LVA use in adults (Virgili 2013). We planned to ask two questions to rate the quality of randomisation and allocation in this type of study: 1. Does knowledge of the first LVA selected affect recruitment into the trial, and 2. Does the order in which the LVAs are used affect the results?
For within‐subject studies testing several devices within the same research session, knowledge of the first LVA should not affect recruitment. In such situations we planned to consider two additional items for question 2: 1) period effect, that is, whether the condition might change during subsequent phases of testing of each device; and 2) carry‐over effect and period‐by‐treatment interaction, that is, whether the effect on performance of using a specific device affects the performance of the devices assessed afterwards (Virgili 2013).
Review authors were not masked to any aspect of the study design and any disagreement was to be settled by discussion or a third designated author, or both.
Measures of treatment effect
The primary outcome (reading speed) is a continuous variable, as are reading accuracy, comprehension, acuity, print size, duration, acceptance/usage. Validated VRQoL and HRQoL tools also deliver continuous scores.
Non‐continuous variables were not to be included in the meta‐analysis. These might have included data assessed by non‐validated questionnaires, such as 'independent learning', and data such as 'cost‐effectiveness' and 'adverse outcomes'.
Unit of analysis issues
Individual participants/children, rather than individual eyes were to be used as the unit of randomisation, as the use of LVAs is most commonly binocular.
Although near and distance visual acuity is commonly measured for individual eyes in a clinical setting, reading speeds and educational assessments are routinely obtained with both eyes open. This allows a more functional assessment, based on the better seeing eye and excluding artefacts such as, for example, an increase in nystagmus amplitude by covering one eye. Studies that measured outcome in the better eye were to be included.
As the main outcome is measured at the person level, we did not expect any unit of analysis issues.
Dealing with missing data
Study authors were to be contacted to obtain missing data where necessary. Where not available or forthcoming, the details of the missing data and the handling of this in the outcome reporting was investigated and reported in detail.
Assessment of heterogeneity
We planned to examine the characteristics of the included studies to identify clinical, methodological and statistical heterogeneity. Methodological heterogeneity may arise from differences in interventions, masking, allocation concealment, outcomes and their measurement.
If sufficient studies are identified in future updates of this review, we will use the I2 statistic with CIs along with inspection of forest plots (poor overlap of CIs) to assess heterogeneity as detailed in Chapter 9 of the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions (Deeks 2011). The interpretation of this statistic would be in line with the guidance in the Handbook, where an I2 value of over 50% was considered to represent 'substantial' (50% to 90%), or 'considerable' (75% to 100%) heterogeneity.
Assessment of reporting biases
Assessment of reporting bias was to be carried out as detailed in the Assessment of risk of bias in included studies section above. In future updates of this review, if 10 or more studies are included in analysis, we plan on constructing a funnel plot and examining it for asymmetry in order to assess reporting bias.
Data synthesis
Meta‐analysis would have been carried out if more than one RCT had been identified and there had been sufficient homogeneity in study design to yield a meaningful analysis. In future updates of this review, if sufficient studies are included, a meta‐analysis will be conducted according to Chapter 9 of the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions (Deeks 2011).
Since a within‐person design is common in research on the effectiveness of LVAs (Virgili 2013), these studies were to be included if the devices were presented in randomised or quasi‐randomised order. This study design leads to specific issues, such as within‐subject correlation and multiplicity of testing. We proposed to deal with these issues using methods suggested in Elbourne 2002 and in Chapter 16 of the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions (Higgins 2011b).
Subgroup analysis and investigation of heterogeneity
No subgroup analysis was planned.
Sensitivity analysis
If sufficient studies are identified in subsequent updates of this review, sensitivity analysis will be conducted to establish the effect of:
assumptions made when dealing with missing data;
excluding studies at 'high risk' of bias.
'Summary of findings' table
We planned to summarise the results in a 'Summary of findings' table using relative and absolute measures of effect. The overall quality of the evidence will be assessed using GRADE (GRADEpro 2014).
Results
Description of studies
Results of the search
The electronic searches yielded a total of 431 references (Figure 1). After duplicates were removed we assessed 368 references for potential inclusion in the review. We discarded 364 reports as they were not relevant to the scope of the review. We assessed the full‐text reports of four studies (Greene 1993; Huurneman 2013; John 2006; NCT00366392) as they potentially met the inclusion criteria; two were reviewed in full, while for the third, only the abstract was available. None of the four studies met the inclusion criteria for this review.
Contacting device manufacturers did not yield any ongoing or recent studies.
Included studies
No studies met the inclusion criteria for this review.
Excluded studies
Four studies (Greene 1993; Huurneman 2013; John 2006; NCT00366392) were initially placed in the 'possibly include' category but ultimately excluded as they did not meet the inclusion criteria:
Risk of bias in included studies
No studies met the inclusion criteria for this review and therefore no comment on risk of bias in included studies can be made.
Effects of interventions
No studies met the inclusion criteria for this review and therefore no comment on the effects of interventions can be made.
Discussion
Summary of main results
The main question posed by this review is whether optical low vision aids (LVAs) have a beneficial effect on reading in children and young people with low vision. No studies were identified which could be included in this review, due to a lack of randomisation and/or a variability of outcome measures, and therefore our main conclusions in this review must be that:
High‐quality evidence on the use of LVAs by children and young people is lacking.
For future studies, greater uniformity of outcome measures is important to facilitate comparison and meta‐analysis of findings.
Overall completeness and applicability of evidence
We identified only two studies and one non‐randomised clinical trial which recruited children. The difficulty in assessing LVA use in young children has already been discussed with regard to the additional confounding factor that these children are learning to read at the same time. This may contribute to the paucity of studies on LVA use in children. A similar review performed in adult participants however, also identified a lack of high‐quality evidence from which to draw conclusions (Virgili 2013).
The variability of outcome measures should also be mentioned. In the three studies detailed above on which we have sufficient information, Greene 1993 used a subjective questionnaire to assess patient use and acceptance of LVAs, Huurneman 2013 used near visual acuity and response time and John 2006 used distance acuity, reading speed and comprehension assessments as outcome measures. The disparity in reported outcomes prohibits comparison, even if the study designs had been more favourable. A standardised approach to assessing the effect of LVA interventions would allow more accurate and evidence‐based comparison in future studies.
Quality of the evidence
The lack of randomisation and the favoured use of cross‐over or within‐person design studies in this subject has also been discussed above and in a previous review on LVA use in adults (Virgili 2013). Whilst we elected to include within‐person design studies where the order in which the devices were presented was randomised, the limitations of this type of methodology remain, particularly when used in developing children (period effect) or progressive pathology (worsening of baseline condition). Ideally, parallel‐armed studies at multiple age groups would provide comparable and extrapolatable evidence, however the large numbers required for this type of design, particularly if it was to include all age groups, likely make this type of study design prohibitive.
Authors' conclusions
Implications for practice.
Due to the lack of high‐quality evidence from randomised controlled trials (RCTs), we cannot discuss the implications for practice.
Implications for research.
Low vision aids (LVAs) can allow children and young people with sight impairment independent access to learning. Education, and particularly the ability to read, is an important component of children’s life and development. Educational success can have a major economic and social impact, as it may help a sight‐impaired person to lead an independent life. As such, research on low vision and its treatment and management in children and young people is vital. High‐quality evidence is needed to assess the efficacy and cost‐effectiveness of LVAs and the training in their use.
Stringent inclusion criteria may mean that not all children and young people with low vision can be included in an RCT, as all participants would need to be able to use the devices to which they are randomised, and to carry out the outcome assessments. However, data from such trials would allow comparison of devices and techniques. Trials have to ensure that adequate training in the use of each device is included in the study design.
This review details outcomes deemed relevant not only by researchers, but also by children and young people with low vision, their families and teachers. It is important to move away from visual acuity as a principal outcome measure, as it merely reflects the optical/geometrical relationship between angle of resolution and magnification. Functional outcomes such as reading speed, accuracy and comprehension are more informative and relevant to users and providers. In addition, assessing the wider impact of devices on quality of life is increasingly important for decisions about allocation of resources. Several tools to measure the impact of interventions on the vision‐related and general health‐related quality of life and functional vision of sight‐impaired children/young people as well as the quality of life of their families are now available for use (Cochrane 2011; Gothwal 2015; Khadka 2010; Tadic 2013; Varni 2001; Varni 2002).
As device usage may be adversely affected by practicalities such as device weight and transportability, comfort, fit and cosmesis, future studies should assess and record these factors, possibly as adverse events.
Acknowledgements
The Cochrane Eyes and Vision Group (CEVG) created and executed the electronic search strategies. We thank ‐ Catey Bunce for comments on the protocol; ‐ Hua Andrew Law for comments on the review; ‐ Gianfrancesco Villani for comments on the protocol and review; ‐ Gianni Virgili, Jennifer Evans and Anupa Shah for general guidance throughout on the review process.
Appendices
Appendix 1. CENTRAL search strategy
#1 MeSH descriptor: [Vision, Low] explode all trees #2 low near/2 vision* #3 MeSH descriptor: [Visually Impaired Persons] explode all trees #4 (vision* or visual*) near/2 impair* #5 MeSH descriptor: [Blindness] explode all trees #6 MeSH descriptor: [Hemianopsia] explode all trees #7 hemianop* #8 quadrantanop* #9 amauros* #10 (handicap* or disabil* or disabl*) near/3 (visual*) #11 (handicap* or disabil* or disabl*) near/3 (vision) #12 #1 or #2 or #3 or #4 or #5 or #6 or #7 or #8 or #9 or #10 or #11 #13 MeSH descriptor: [Sensory Aids] explode all trees #14 MeSH descriptor: [Lenses] this term only #15 MeSH descriptor: [Optics and Photonics] this term only #16 MeSH descriptor: [Optical Devices] this term only #17 "low vision aid" #18 MeSH descriptor: [Reading] explode all trees #19 (aid* or device* or instrument* or equipment or apparatus) near (read*) #20 (aid* or device* or instrument* or equipment or apparatus) near (optic*) #21 telescop* or magnifi* or binocular* #22 #13 or #18 or #20 or #21 #23 MeSH descriptor: [Child] explode all trees #24 MeSH descriptor: [Infant] explode all trees #25 MeSH descriptor: [Adolescent] explode all trees #26 MeSH descriptor: [Pediatrics] explode all trees #27 boy* or girl* or child* or minor* or offspring or prepubescen* or pubescen* #28 adolescen* or juvenile* or teen or teens or teenage* or youth or youths or underage #29 paediatric* or pediatric* #30 (primary or elementary or high or secondary) near/1 school* #31 nurser* or kindergarten* or preschool* or pre school* or school* #32 schoolchild* or schoolage or highschool* or daycare #33 #23 or #24 or #25 or #26 or #27 or #28 or #29 or #30 or #31 or #32 #34 #12 and #22 and #33
Appendix 2. MEDLINE (OvidSP) search strategy
1. randomized controlled trial.pt. 2. (randomized or randomised).ab,ti. 3. placebo.ab,ti. 4. dt.fs. 5. randomly.ab,ti. 6. trial.ab,ti. 7. groups.ab,ti. 8. or/1‐7 9. exp animals/ 10. exp humans/ 11. 9 not (9 and 10) 12. 8 not 11 13. exp vision low/ 14. (low adj2 vision$).tw. 15. exp visually impaired persons/ 16. ((visual$ or vision$) adj3 impair$).tw. 17. exp blindness/ 18. exp hemianopsia/ 19. hemianop$.tw. 20. exp quadrantanopsia/ 21. quadrantanop$.tw. 22. amauros$.tw. 23. ((handicap$ or disabil$ or disabl$) adj3 vision$).tw. 24. ((handicap$ or disabil$ or disabl$) adj3 visual$).tw. 25. or/13‐24 26. sensory aids/ 27. Lenses/ 28. "Optics and Photonics"/ 29. Optical Devices/ 30. "low vision aid$".tw. 31. exp reading/ 32. ((aid$ or device$ or instrument$ or equipment or apparatus) adj3 read$).tw. 33. ((aid$ or device$ or instrument$ or equipment or apparatus) adj3 optic$).tw. 34. (telescop$ or magnifi$ or binocular$).tw. 35. or/26‐34 36. exp child/ 37. exp adolescent/ 38. exp pediatrics/ 39. (boy$ or girl$ or child$ or minor$ or offspring or prepubescen$ or pubescen$).tw. 40. (adolescen$ or juvenile$ or teen or teens or teenage$ or youth or youths or underage).tw. 41. (paediatric$ or pediatric$).tw. 42. ((primary or elementary or high or secondary) adj1 school$).tw. 43. (schoolchild$ or schoolage or highschool$ or daycare).tw. 44. or/36‐43 45. 25 and 35 and 44 46. 12 and 45
The search filter for trials at the beginning of the MEDLINE strategy is from the published paper by Glanville et al (Glanville 2006).
Appendix 3. EMBASE (OvidSP) search strategy
1. exp randomized controlled trial/ 2. exp randomization/ 3. exp double blind procedure/ 4. exp single blind procedure/ 5. random$.tw. 6. or/1‐5 7. (animal or animal experiment).sh. 8. human.sh. 9. 7 and 8 10. 7 not 9 11. 6 not 10 12. exp clinical trial/ 13. (clin$ adj3 trial$).tw. 14. ((singl$ or doubl$ or trebl$ or tripl$) adj3 (blind$ or mask$)).tw. 15. exp placebo/ 16. placebo$.tw. 17. random$.tw. 18. exp experimental design/ 19. exp crossover procedure/ 20. exp control group/ 21. exp latin square design/ 22. or/12‐21 23. 22 not 10 24. 23 not 11 25. exp comparative study/ 26. exp evaluation/ 27. exp prospective study/ 28. (control$ or prospectiv$ or volunteer$).tw. 29. or/25‐28 30. 29 not 10 31. 30 not (11 or 23) 32. 11 or 24 or 31 33. exp visual disorder/ 34. exp visual impairment/ 35. exp blindness/ 36. (low adj2 vision$).tw. 37. ((visual$ or vision$) adj3 impair$).tw. 38. exp hemianopia/ 39. hemianop$.tw. 40. quadrantanop$.tw. 41. amauros$.tw. 42. ((handicap$ or disabil$ or disabl$) adj3 vision$).tw. 43. ((handicap$ or disabil$ or disabl$) adj3 visual$).tw. 44. or/33‐43 45. exp visual aid/ 46. exp general medical aids/ 47. optical instrumentation/ 48. exp reading/ 49. ((aid$ or device$ or instrument$ or equipment or apparatus) adj3 read$).tw. 50. ((aid$ or device$ or instrument$ or equipment or apparatus) adj3 optic$).tw. 51. (telescop$ or magnifi$ or binocular$).tw. 52. or/45‐51 53. exp child/ 54. exp adolescent/ 55. exp pediatrics/ 56. (boy$ or girl$ or child$ or minor$ or offspring or prepubescen$ or pubescen$).tw. 57. (adolescen$ or juvenile$ or teen or teens or teenage$ or youth or youths or underage).tw. 58. (paediatric$ or pediatric$).tw. 59. ((primary or elementary or high or secondary) adj1 school$).tw. 60. (schoolchild$ or schoolage or highschool$ or daycare).tw. 61. or/53‐60 62. 44 and 52 and 61 63. 32 and 62
Appendix 4. HTA Programme search strategy
low vision
Appendix 5. ISRCTN search strategy
low vision
Appendix 6. ClinicalTrials.gov search strategy
Reading Aids AND Low Vision
Appendix 7. ICTRP search strategy
Low Vision = Condition AND Reading Aids = Intervention
Appendix 8. Data extraction form
Review author | ||||||
Study ID | ||||||
Dates when study was conducted | If not available, comment "dates not available" | |||||
Funding source(s) | ||||||
Declarations of interest by researchers | ||||||
Methods | Study design, e.g. parallel group randomised trial, cluster‐randomised trial, controlled before and after study, within‐person Duration Within‐person design: method of intervention allocation |
|||||
Participants | Total number, number in each group (sample size) Comparability Setting |
|||||
Risk of bias | Assessed using 'Risk of bias' tool (see Handbook) | |||||
Outcomes (as defined in study) Please specify which |
Primary outcome
Secondary outcomes
all assessed using a standardised chart such as MNRead or IReST or a standardised literacy test such as NARA.
|
|||||
Interventions compared | Intervention 1 = Standard care (baseline refractive correction), or LVA (specify type) Intervention 2 = LVA (specify type) additional interventions: LVA ‐ specify type |
|||||
PRIMARY OUTCOME: Maximum reading speed | Intervention 1 | Intervention 2 | ||||
Time point | Total number of participants | Mean | Standard deviation* | Total number of participants | Mean | Standard deviation* |
Baseline | ||||||
Immediately after intervention issued | ||||||
3 months | ||||||
6 months | ||||||
12 months | ||||||
Intervention 3 | Intervention 4 | |||||
Time point | Total number of participants | Mean | Standard deviation* | Total number of participants | Mean | Standard deviation* |
Baseline | ||||||
Immediately after intervention issued | ||||||
3 months | ||||||
6 months | ||||||
12 months | ||||||
SECONDARY OUTCOMES: Copy table for each secondary outcome | Intervention 1 | Intervention 2 | ||||
Time point | Total number of participants | Mean | Standard deviation* | Total number of participants | Mean | Standard deviation* |
Baseline | ||||||
Immediately after intervention issued | ||||||
3 months | ||||||
6 months | ||||||
12 months | ||||||
Intervention 3 | Intervention 4 | |||||
Time point | Total number of participants | Mean | Standard deviation* | Total number of participants | Mean | Standard deviation* |
Baseline | ||||||
Immediately after intervention issued | ||||||
3 months | ||||||
6 months | ||||||
12 months |
Characteristics of studies
Characteristics of excluded studies [ordered by study ID]
Study | Reason for exclusion |
---|---|
Greene 1993 | Described a randomised cross‐over trial of spectacle‐mounted telescope low vision aids (new technology versus older models), but all participants were adults. In addition, the outcome measures were subjectively reported based on the type (when the telescopes were used in day‐to‐day life) and amount (how often the telescopes were used) of usage. |
Huurneman 2013 | Compared magnifiers versus large print in children aged 4 to 8 years. The article did not specify whether children were allocated to treatment groups by randomisation. The first author was contacted and confirmed that allocation was not randomised but carried out by an independent observer who allocated children to either study arm based on age and near visual acuity to ensure matching. |
John 2006 | Presented a comparison of low vision telescopes (LVT) and standard‐correction glasses (SCG) in paediatric glaucoma patients as a conference proceeding. The abstract did not give details regarding how many children had low vision and whether children were randomly allocated to interventions. We contacted the senior and first authors for further details of the study, but did not receive a reply. |
NCT00366392 | This clinical trial compared large print versus optical aids for visually impaired students aged 5 to 17 years. The study was closed in 2007. It did not meet the inclusion criteria for this review, as allocation was not randomised and no masking took place. |
Contributions of authors
Lucy Barker: design and co‐ordination of review, writing of protocol and review. Annegret Dahlmann‐Noor: guarantor of review; design and writing of protocol and review. Rachel Thomas, Gary Rubin: design and writing of protocol and review.
Sources of support
Internal sources
-
National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Biomedical Research Centre (BRC), UK.
Annegret Dahlmann‐Noor and Catey Bunce acknowledge support from the Department of Health through the award made by the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Biomedical Research Centre based at Moorfields Eye Hospital National Health Service (NHS) Foundation Trust and University College London Institute of Ophthalmology. The views expressed are those of the author(s) and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health.
External sources
-
National Institute for Health Research, UK.
- Richard Wormald, Co‐ordinating Editor for the Cochrane Eyes and Vision Group (CEVG) acknowledges financial support for his CEVG research sessions from the Department of Health through the award made by the National Institute for Health Research to Moorfields Eye Hospital NHS Foundation Trust and UCL Institute of Ophthalmology for a Specialist Biomedical Research Centre for Ophthalmology.
- The NIHR also funds the CEVG Editorial Base in London.
The views expressed in this publication are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NIHR, NHS, or the Department of Health.
Declarations of interest
Lucy Barker: No commercial or proprietary interests to declare. Rachel Thomas: No commercial or proprietary interests to declare. Gary Rubin: No commercial or proprietary interests to declare. Annagret Dahlmann‐Noor: No commercial or proprietary interests to declare.
New
References
References to studies excluded from this review
Greene 1993 {published data only}
- Greene HA, Pekar J, Brilliant R, Freeman PB, Lewis HT, Siwoff R, et al. Use of spectacle mounted telescope systems by the visually impaired. Journal of the American Optometric Association 1993;64(7):507‐13. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Huurneman 2013 {published data only}
- Huurneman B, Boonstra FN, Verezen CA, Cillessen AH, Rens G, Cox RF. Crowded task performance in visually impaired children: magnifier versus large print. Graefe's Archive for Clinical and Experimental Ophthalmology 2013;251(7):1813‐9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
John 2006 {published data only}
- John D, McRedmond B, Corn A, Sonsino J, Joos K. The effect of low vision optical devices on distance visual acuity and reading speeds in pediatric glaucoma patients. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science 2006;47:ARVO E‐abstract 5831. [Google Scholar]
NCT00366392 {published data only}
- NCT00366392. Project magnify ‐ a comparison of two strategies (large print versus optical aids) for helping visually impaired students improve reading abilities. clinicaltrials.gov/show/NCT00366392 (accessed 2 July 2014).
Additional references
Ahn 1995a
- Ahn SJ, Legge GE. Psychophysics of reading‐XIII. Predictors of magnifier‐aided reading speed in low vision. Vision Research 1995;35(13):1931‐8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Ahn 1995b
- Ahn SJ, Legge GE, Luebker A. Printed cards for measuring low‐vision reading speed. Vision Research 1995;35(13):1939‐44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Bailey 2003
- Bailey IL, Hall‐Lueck A, Greer RB, Mon‐Tuan K, Bailey VM, Dornbusch HG. Understanding the relationship between print size and reading in low vision. Journal of Visual Impairment and Blindness 2003;97(6):325‐34. [Google Scholar]
Binns 2012
- Binns AM, Bunce C, Dickinson C, Harper R, Tudor‐Edwards R, Woodhouse M, et al. How effective is low vision service provision? A systematic review. Survey of Ophthalmology 2012;57(1):34‐65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Bodeau‐Livinec 2007
- Bodeau‐Livinec F, Surman G, Kaminski M, Wilkinson AR, Ancel PY, Kurinczuk JJ. Recent trends in visual impairment and blindness in the UK. Archives of Disease in Children 2007;92(12):1099‐104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Cochrane 2011
- Cochrane GM, Marella M, Keeffe JE, Lamoureux EL. The Impact of Vision Impairment for Children (IVI_C): validation of a vision‐specific pediatric quality‐of‐life questionnaire using Rasch analysis. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science 2011;52(3):1632‐40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Corn 2002
- Corn AL, Koenig AJ. Literacy for students with low vision: A framework for delivering instruction. Journal of Visual Impairment and Blindness 2002;96(5):305‐21. [Google Scholar]
Cox 2009
- Cox RF, Reimer AM, Verezen CA, Smitsman AW, Vervloed MP, Boonstra NF. Young children's use of a visual aid: an experimental study of the effectiveness of training. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 2009;51(6):460‐7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
de Carvalho 1998
- Carvalho KM, Minguini N, Moreira Filho DC, Kara‐Jose N. Characteristics of a pediatric low‐vision population. Journal of Pediatric Ophthalmology and Strabismus 1998;35(3):162‐5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Deeks 2011
- Deeks JJ, Higgins JPT, Altman DG (editors). Chapter 9: Analysing data and undertaking meta‐analyses. In: Higgins JPT, Green S (editors). Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions Version 5.1.0 (updated March 2011). The Cochrane Collaboration, 2011. Available from www.cochrane‐handbook.org.
Dickinson 2000
- Dickinson CM, Fotinakis V. The limitations imposed on reading by low vision aids. Optometry and Vision Science 2000;77(7):364‐72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Douglas 2002
- Douglas G, Grimley M, Hill E, Long R, Tobin M. The use of the NARA for assessing the reading ability of children with low vision. British Journal of Visual Impairment 2002;20(2):69‐75. [Google Scholar]
Douglas 2004
- Douglas G, Grimley M, McLinden M, Watson L. Reading errors made by children with low vision. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics 2004;24(4):319‐22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Douglas 2011
- Douglas G, McLinden M, McCall S, Pavey S, Ware J, Farrell AM. Access to print literacy for children and young people with visual impairment: findings from a review of the literature. European Journal of Special Needs Education 2011;26(1):25‐38. [Google Scholar]
Ducrey 1998
- Ducrey N, Mathis M, Goldschmidt M, Figueiredo V. Management of children with low vision [La prise en charge des enfants malvoyants]. Klinische Monatsblatter fur Augenheilkunde 1998;212(5):372‐5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Elbourne 2002
- Elbourne DR, Altman DG, Higgins JPT, Curtin F, Worthington HV, Vaillancourt JM. Meta‐analyses involving cross‐over trials: methodological issues. International Journal of Epidemiology 2002;31(1):140‐9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Gilbert 2001
- Gilbert C, Foster A. Blindness in children: control priorities and research opportunities. British Journal of Ophthalmology 2001;85(9):1025‐7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Gilbert 2008a
- Gilbert C, Muhit M. Twenty years of childhood blindness: what have we learnt?. Community Eye Health 2008;21(67):46‐7. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Gilbert 2008b
- Gilbert CE, Ellwein LB. Prevalence and causes of functional low vision in school‐age children: results from standardized population surveys in Asia, Africa, and Latin America. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science 2008;49(3):877‐81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Glanville 2006
- Glanville JM, Lefebvre C, Miles JN, Camosso‐Stefinovic J. How to identify randomized controlled trials in MEDLINE: ten years on. Journal of the Medical Library Association 2006;94(2):130‐6. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Gnyawali 2012
- Gnyawali S, Shrestha JB, Bhattarai D, Upadhyay M. Optical needs of students with low vision in integrated schools of Nepal. Optometry and Vision Science 2012;89(12):1752‐6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Gompel 2004
- Gompel M, vanBon WHJ, Schreuder R. Reading by children with low vision. Journal of Visual Impairment and Blindness 2004;98(2):77‐89. [Google Scholar]
Gothwal 2015
- Gothwal VK, Bharani S, Mandal AK. Quality of life of caregivers of children with congenital glaucoma: development and validation of a novel questionnaire (CarCGQoL). Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science 2015;56(2):770‐7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
GRADEpro 2014
- [Computer program on www.gradepro.org]. McMasters University, 2014:McMaster University.
Haddad 2006
- Haddad MA, Lobato FJ, Sampaio MW, Kara‐Jose N. Pediatric and adolescent population with visual impairment: study of 385 cases. Clinics 2006;61(3):239‐46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Haddad 2009
- Haddad MA, Sampaio MW, Oltrogge EW, Kara‐Jose N, Betinjane AJ. Visual impairment secondary to congenital glaucoma in children: visual responses, optical correction and use of low vision AIDS. Clinics 2009;64(8):725‐30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Harper 1999
- Harper R, Doorduyn K, Reeves B, Slater L. Evaluating the outcomes of low vision rehabilitation. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics 1999;19(1):3‐11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Hassell 2000
- Hassell JB, Weih LM, Keeffe JE. A measure of handicap for low vision rehabilitation: the impact of vision impairment profile. Clinical and Experimental Ophthalmology 2000;28(3):156‐61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Hazel 2000
- Hazel CA, Petre KL, Armstrong RA, Benson MT, Frost NA. Visual function and subjective quality of life compared in subjects with acquired macular disease. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science 2000;41(6):1309‐15. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Higgins 2011a
- Higgins JPT, Deeks JJ (editors). Chapter 7: Selecting studies and collecting data. In: Higgins JPT, Green S (editors), Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions Version 5.1.0 (updated March 2011). The Cochrane Collaboration, 2011. Available from www.cochrane‐handbook.org.
Higgins 2011b
- Higgins JPT, Deeks JJ, Altman DG (editors). Chapter 16: Special topics in statistics. In: Higgins JPT, Green S (editors), Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions Version 5.1.0 (updated March 2011). The Cochrane Collaboration, 2011. Available from www.cochrane‐handbook.org.
Higgins 2011c
- Higgins JPT, Altman DG, Sterne JAC (editors). Chapter 8: Assessing risk of bias in included studies. In: Higgins JPT, Green S (editors). Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions Version 5.1.0 (updated March 2011). The Cochrane Collaboration, 2011. Available from www.cochrane‐handbook.org.
Hill 2005
- Hill E, Long R, Douglas G, Tobin M, Grimley M. Neale Analysis of Reading Ability for Readers with Low Vision. A Supplementary Manual to Aid the Assessment of Partially Sighted Pupils' Reading Using the Neale Analysis of Reading Ability (NARA). Birmingham, UK: Visual Impairment Centre for Teaching and Research; School of Education; University of Birmingham, 2005. [Google Scholar]
Khadka 2010
- Khadka J, Ryan B, Margrain TH, Court H, Woodhouse JM. Development of the 25‐item Cardiff Visual Ability Questionnaire for Children (CVAQC). British Journal of Ophthalmology 2010;94(6):730‐5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Legge 2007
- Legge GE. Psychophysics of Reading in Normal and Low Vision. NJ and London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 2007. [Google Scholar]
Mangione 1998
- Mangione CM, Berry S, Spritzer K, Janz NK, Klein R, Owsley C, et al. Identifying the content area for the 51‐item National Eye Institute Visual Function Questionnaire: results from focus groups with visually impaired persons. Archives of Ophthalmology 1998;116(2):227‐33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Mangione 2001
- Mangione CM, Lee PP, Gutierrez PR, Spritzer K, Berry S, Hays RD. Development of the 25‐item National Eye Institute Visual Function Questionnaire. Archives of Ophthalmology 2001;119(7):1050‐8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Margrain 2000
- Margrain TH. Helping blind and partially sighted people to read: the effectiveness of low vision aids. British Journal of Ophthalmology 2000;84(8):919‐21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Mason 1999
- Mason H. Blurred vision: a study of the use of low vision aids by visually impaired secondary school pupils. British Journal of Visual Impairment 1999;17(3):94‐7. [Google Scholar]
Massof 1998
- Massof RW. A systems model for low vision rehabilitation. II. Measurement of vision disabilities. Optometry and Vision Science 1998;75(5):349‐73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
McClure 2000
- McClure ME, Hart PM, Jackson AJ, Stevenson MR, Chakravarthy U. Macular degeneration: do conventional measurements of impaired visual function equate with visual disability?. British Journal of Ophthalmology 2000;84(3):244‐50. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Mitry 2013
- Mitry D, Bunce C, Wormald R, Leamon S, Simkiss P, Cumberland P, et al. Causes of certifications for severe sight impairment (blind) and sight impairment (partial sight) in children in England and Wales. British Journal of Ophthalmology 2013;97(11):1431‐6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Morris 2008
- Morris M, Smith P. Educational provision for blind and partially sighted children and young people in Britain. www.rnib.org.uk/aboutus/Research/reports/education/Pages/educational_provision.aspx. London: National Foundation for Educational Research (NFER) for RNIB, 2008 (accessed 20 February 2013).
Neale 1997
- Neale MD. Neale Analysis of Reading Ability ‐ Revised: Manual for Schools. Windsor: NFER‐Nelson, 1997. [Google Scholar]
Rahi 2003
- Rahi JS, Cable N. Severe visual impairment and blindness in children in the UK. Lancet 2003;362(9393):1359‐65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
RevMan 2014 [Computer program]
- The Nordic Cochrane Centre, The Cochrane Collaboration. Review Manager (RevMan). Version 5.3. Copenhagen: The Nordic Cochrane Centre, The Cochrane Collaboration, 2014.
Rubin 2013
- Rubin GS. Measuring reading performance. Vision Research 2013;90:43‐51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Shrestha 2012
- Shrestha JB, Gnyawali S, Upadhyay MP. Causes of blindness and visual impairment among students in integrated schools for the blind in Nepal. Ophthalmic Epidemiology 2012;19(6):401‐6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Sight Savers International 2003
- Sight Savers International. Current Status of the aetiology, prevalence and distribution of childhood blindness in Pakistan. pk.sightsavers.org/in_depth/policy_and_research/14313_Current%20status%20Childhood%20Blindness%20in%20Pakistan.pdf 2003 (accessed 22 April 2013).
Silver 1976
- Silver J, Gould E. A study of some factors concerned in the schooling of visually handicapped children. Child: care, health and development 1976;2(3):145‐53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Tadic 2013
- Tadic V, Cooper A, Cumberland P, Lewando‐Hundt G, Rahi JS. Development of the Functional Vision Questionnaire for Children and Young People with Visual Impairment: The FVQ_CYP. Ophthalmology 2013;120(12):2725‐32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Trauzettel‐Klosinski 2012
- Trauzettel‐Klosinski S, Dietz K. Standardized assessment of reading performance: the New International Reading Speed Texts IReST. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science 2012;53(9):5452‐61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Varni 2001
- Varni JW, Seid M, Kurtin PS. PedsQL 4.0: reliability and validity of the Pediatric Quality of Life Inventory version 4.0 Generic Core Scales in healthy and patient populations. Medical Care 2001;39(8):800‐12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Varni 2002
- Varni JW, Seid M, Knight TS, Uzark K, Szer IS. The PedsQL 4.0 Generic Core Scales: sensitivity, responsiveness, and impact on clinical decision‐making. Journal of Behavioral Medicine 2002;25(2):175‐93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Virgili 2013
- Virgili G, Acosta R, Grover LL, Bentley SA, Giacomelli G. Reading aids for adults with low vision. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2013, Issue 10. [DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD003303.pub3] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Weih 2002
- Weih LM, Hassell JB, Keeffe J. Assessment of the impact of vision impairment. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science 2002;43(4):927‐35. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Wolffsohn 2000
- Wolffsohn JS, Cochrane AL. Design of the low vision quality‐of‐life questionnaire (LVQOL) and measuring the outcome of low‐vision rehabilitation. American Journal of Ophthalmology 2000;130(6):793‐802. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
World Health Organization 1992
- World Health Organization. Management of low vision in children. www.iceh.org.uk/display/WEB/Management+of+low+vision+in+children. Bangkok, 1992 (accessed 29 March 2013).
World Health Organization 2000
- World Health Organization. Preventing blindness in children. Report of a WHO/IAPB scientific meeting. www.who.int/ncd/vision2020_actionplan/documents/WHO_PBL_00.77.pdf. Geneva: WHO, 2000 (accessed 29 March 2013).
World Health Organization 2004
- World Health Organization. Magnitude and causes of visual impairment: fact sheet 282. www.who.int/mediacentre/factsheets/fs282/en/ 2004 (accessed 29 March 2013).
References to other published versions of this review
Barker 2014
- Barker L, Thomas R, Rubin G, Dahlmann‐Noor A. Optical reading aids for children and young people with low vision. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2014, Issue 2. [DOI: 10.1002/14651858.CD010987] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]