Abstract
Objectives:
Determine the impact of classroom-based physically active lessons on time-on-task. Secondarily, determine the relationship of time-on-task with academic achievement controlling for key demographic variables.
Methods:
Seventeen elementary schools were cluster randomized to receive classroom physical activity (A+PAAC, N=9) or control (i.e., no physical activity, N=8) for a 3-year trial. Teachers were trained to deliver physically active lessons with moderate-to-vigorous intensity targeting 100 minutes per week. Outcome measures included academic achievement (Weschler Individual Achievement Test-III), administered at baseline and repeated each spring for 3 years, time spent in moderate-to-vigorous physical activity (MVPA), and time spent on task (TOT) pre- and post-physical activity. Multilevel modeling was utilized to estimate the impacts of percent time spent in MVPA and percent TOT post-lesson on academic achievement and the change in these impacts over 3 years, accounting for dependency among observations and covariates including age, gender, race, free or reduced lunch, BMI, and fitness.
Results:
A greater percentage of time performing MVPA were each significantly associated with higher math scores (p=.034) and spelling scores (p<.001), but not reading scores. Academic achievement was not associated with TOT.
Conclusion:
Findings suggest that a greater percentage of time spent participating in MVPA results in higher math scores and spelling scores, but not reading scores, independent of TOT. Future studies should continue to evaluate the influence of physically active classroom lessons on time spent on task and its impact on academic achievement. Furthermore, strategies are needed to maximize impact and to determine the sustainability of physical activity’s effect on classroom behavior and academic achievement.
Keywords: Children, Time-On-Task, Physical Activity, Academic Achievement
Introduction
Physical activity lessons within the classroom have been shown to be a viable means of aiding in the accumulation of the daily recommended 60 minutes of physical activity for children (1, 2). Physical Activity Across the Curriculum (PAAC) (3–5) and “brain break” type physical activity opportunities including Energizers (6, 7) and FUNtervals (8) have been shown to result in greater than 75 minutes of physical activity each week. In addition to increasing physical activity time, several cross-sectional reports have demonstrated that school-based physical activity sessions may have effects on a child’s attention to classroom instruction (i.e., attention-to-task) following the activity (2). Similarly, classroom physical activity may also influence academic performance, however, this has yet to be established (9).
Several examples of how classroom physical activity has been utilized within the classroom exist. For example, Energizers, 10-minute classroom-based physical activities, resulted in a mean percentage increase of on-task behavior by more than 8% from pre- to post-Energizer break (6, 7). Similarly, Ma et al. (8) found that off-task behavior decreased following FUNtervals consisting of 4-minute-high-intensity interval exercises, in both 2nd and 4th graders when compared to a no-activity break. Additionally, Ma et al. (8) found that FUNtervals resulted in fewer errors during the performance of the d2 Test of Attention in 3rd-5th-grade students when compared to rest. Greico et al. (10) found that time-on-task (TOT) following an inactive control lesson significantly decreased while time-on-task remained unchanged following the physical activity lesson. In similar fashion, Szabo-Reed et al. (5) assessed physical activity (i.e., moderate to vigorous physical activity, MVPA) and TOT pre and post active and non-active academic lessons in elementary aged students over the course of 3-years. Results showed that those students receiving the active lessons not only did more MVPA than those not receiving the lessons, but they also had improved TOT. Together these studies indicate that participation in short bouts of physical activity in the classroom can help to increase attention and TOT; however, the association between TOT with academic achievement has not been studied.
Several studies have explored the effect of classroom physical activity on cognitive outcomes including executive control, reaction time and attention. However, the results have mostly been inconclusive producing no immediate change in performance(9). Howie, Schatz, and Pate (11) did find that math scores were higher in 9 to 12-year-old children after 10 and 20-minute classroom physical activity breaks as compared to sedentary activity time. Acute studies, outside of the classroom, have previously examined the influence of confined bouts of physical activity on measures of immediate academic achievement and concentration to find positive results. For example, Caterino and Polak (12) found that concentration in fourth graders improved post-recess participation. Tine (13) found that 12-minute bouts of aerobic exercise improved selective attention in children, but did not affect reading comprehension. Similarly, Hillman et al. (14) compared a physically active condition (i.e., brisk walking on a treadmill) to an inactive condition (i.e., sitting) in the laboratory and found significant benefits for performance in reading, but not math or spelling. Duncan and Johnson (15) compared a rest condition to cycling at both moderate and vigorous intensities and found that spelling and reading were significantly higher after moderate-intensity physical activity, while math scores were significantly lower. Overall, acute bouts of physical activity appear to influence performance on standardized academic achievement tests and concentration, however, due to the conflicting findings, and lack of evidence, additional research is needed to determine the effect of long-term classroom-based physical activity participation on TOT and academic outcomes. Long-term physical activity interventions in elementary aged students have also shown positive improvements in academic performance, however, have not explored the effect of MVPA on TOT and academic performance changes (16–19).
Therefore, the objectives of the present investigation were to extend the findings of Szabo-Reed et al. (5) utilizing data from the Academic Achievement and Physical Activity Across the Curriculum (A+PAAC, NCT01699295) study to determine the long-term effects of MVPA classroom physical activity breaks and TOT on academic achievement over the course of the 3 year intervention.
Methods
Participants
A detailed description of the rationale, design, and methods of A+PAAC (20), as well as primary outcomes (4), has been previously published. A+PAAC was a 3-year, adequately powered, cluster randomized controlled trial designed to evaluate the influence of classroom-based physical activity lessons on academic achievement. The trial incorporated 17 elementary schools in northeast Kansas from 4 school districts. Schools were stratified by school district (N=3) to receive A+PAAC (9 schools) or serve as controls (8 schools). A+PAAC provided academic lessons delivered by classroom teachers using MVPA (100 minutes/week, >3 METs) to increase total daily MVPA while maintaining academic instruction time. A+PAAC instruction time began when the students were in 2nd or 3rd grade and ended in 5th or 6th grade, respectively. The primary outcome was academic achievement measured by Weschler Individual Achievement Test-III, WIAT-III (21). Parents and students provided consent and assent, respectively, prior to initiation of A+PAAC. The investigation was approved by The University of Kansas Human Subjects Committee.
Intervention procedures
Teachers were asked to deliver two, 10-minute A+PAAC lessons per day, one lesson in the morning and one in the afternoon, 5 days per week, for a weekly total of school-based physical activity of ~100 min/week. A+PAAC lessons were also encouraged outside the classroom during alternative learning, such as field trips. A+PAAC lessons provided MVPA with physical activity intensity of 4–5 METs (22, 23) and used in a variety of academic disciplines including math, language arts, geography, history, spelling, science, and health.
Classroom teachers participating in the intervention schools were trained to deliver A+PAAC during two, 4-hour in-service sessions conducted at the school prior to implementation of the intervention and were compensated. Teachers were also provided a written guide and access to a study website which included a variety of sample A+PAAC lessons with each lesson’s estimated MET values (See Videos for examples, SDC1.mp4, SDC1, Language arts example of A+PAAC, 55sec, 32.6MB, SDC2.mp4, SDC2, Reading comprehension example of A+PAAC delivery, 3min 23sec, 107MB, SDC3.mp4, SDC3, Mathematics example of A+PAAC delivery, 2min 16sec, 73.1MB). Teachers in the control schools were asked to continue using traditional classroom instruction and students in both the intervention and control schools continued with their typical physical education schedule (i.e., 2, 30-minute classes/week). Evaluation of external or competing factors was completed by each school’s principal at the end of each year to ensure that no new physical activity or nutritional programs were adopted by the school that could pose a threat to the internal validity of the intervention. No programs were identified.
System for Observing Fitness Instruction Time (SOFIT)
which is a validated time-moment sampling technique (24), was used to assess intervention fidelity and the amount of A+PAAC lessons delivered. Classroom activities were coded on a 5-point Likert scale (1=lying down, 2=sitting, 3=standing, 4=walking, 5=jogging/running). MVPA was defined as the fourth or fifth activity code. Schools were selected at random for observation on a specific day each week. Within the school, the classroom to be observed was randomly selected. Finally, the specific students to be observed were randomly selected from those who provided consent/assent. Students were observed individually and activity was recorded in 20-second intervals. Approximately five students were observed consecutively, yielding ~6 observations per child over a 10-minute period. The total percentage of time spent participating in MVPA during each observation for each student observed was calculated (i.e., 10 minutes observed with 6 minutes spent in MVPA is 60% MVPA). The same procedure was also performed within the control schools. Inter-observer reliability for SOFIT across the 3-year period was 92%.
Time-on-task (TOT)
was assessed by trained research staff on the same random schedule for recording classroom activity using a modification of the procedures from Mahar et al. (6). During each assessment, ~5 students were individually observed between 5–20 minutes before and 5–20 minutes after the delivery of an academic lesson (i.e., A+PAAC or traditional). Observation time fluctuated due to varying class activities (i.e., students leaving classroom to go to art). Each child was observed individually for 10 seconds, after which the appropriate behavior code was recorded. This procedure was repeated over the 5-minute interval to result in a total of ~20 observations per child per session and subsequently, the appropriate behavior code was recorded. Coding included on-task, motor off-task, noise off-task, or passive/other off-task (25, 26). A score for percent TOT, percent motor of-task, etc. was computed for each individual student based on the number of minutes of observation pre- and post-lesson. Inter-observer reliability for TOT across the 3-year period was 92%.
Demographics.
Age, race/ethnicity, sex, free/reduced lunch etc. for both participants and their parent(s) were collected at baseline via questionnaire.
Anthropometrics (Height/weight/waist circumference).
Children were weighed in duplicate wearing school clothes without shoes during the first period of the school day on a calibrated scale to the nearest 0.1 kg (Model #PS6600, Befour, Saukville, WI). Standing height was measured with a portable stadiometer (Model #IP0955, Invicta Plastics Limited, Leicester, UK). BMI was calculated as weight (kg)/height (m2) and BMI percentile was calculated using the CDC growth charts (27).
Cardiorespiratory Fitness.
The PACER test was administered at each school in small groups (8–12 students) with a 1:4 staff to children ratio. During testing, children were instructed to run back and forth between two lines, spaced 20 meters apart, as they were paced by a tone signaling the time at which students must reach the opposite side. The pace began slowly and became progressively faster until any child was unable to traverse the 20-meter distance on two consecutive trials, at which point the test ended for that child (28, 29). A higher level of fitness was denoted by a greater number of completed laps. There has been considerable work in adults and children utilizing the PACER and it has been shown to be a useful school-based test that not only provides an accurate and viable method for identifying childhood health risks but also greatly increases the feasibility of collecting aerobic fitness information in large samples of children(30–33).
Academic achievement
Academic achievement was assessed using the Weschsler Individual Achievement Test-Third Edition (WIAT-III; (21)). The WIAT-III was individually administered by research staff trained and supervised by a qualified co-investigator. Five WIAT-III subtests (reading comprehension, oral reading fluency, spelling, mathematics problem solving, and numerical operations) were administered over 40–50 minute sessions (34). The WIAT-III was scored by the examiner and all protocols were checked for accuracy by a trained investigator. Any discrepancy in scores between examiner and the research team member were evaluated and resolved by a co-investigator. All scores were entered into the WIAT-III computerized scoring assistant, which automatically disallows out-of-range values and computes subtest and composite scores.
Statistical analysis
Descriptive statistics and bivariate tests (i.e., t-test, chi-square test) were utilized to summarize baseline demographic and anthropometric characteristics of the sample. Table 1 provides a description of the number of students available and included in each analysis. Independent-samples t-test (with Satterthwaite approximation if necessary) was performed to compare percent time spent in MVPA and percent TOT behavior before and after academic lesson delivery (A+PAAC or traditional) between students in the control schools and those in the intervention schools.
Table 1.
Total (N = 633) | |||
---|---|---|---|
Variable | n | M | SD |
Math score | |||
Year 1 | 562 | 103.18 | 12.00 |
Year 2 | 545 | 107.14 | 13.19 |
Year 3 | 516 | 107.57 | 12.03 |
Reading score | |||
Year 1 | 557 | 100.75 | 13.35 |
Year 2 | 541 | 105.98 | 14.93 |
Year 3 | 510 | 108.05 | 13.08 |
Spelling score | |||
Year 1 | 563 | 99.81 | 11.34 |
Year 2 | 545 | 102.14 | 12.74 |
Year 3 | 516 | 104.29 | 13.31 |
Spelling score | |||
Year 1 | 563 | 99.81 | 11.34 |
Year 2 | 545 | 102.14 | 12.74 |
Year 3 | 516 | 104.29 | 13.31 |
% time in on-task behavior: post-PA | |||
Year 1 | 544 | 86.84 | 15.45 |
Year 2 | 591 | 88.86 | 14.26 |
Year 3 | 525 | 91.35 | 16.14 |
% time in Sofit MVPA | |||
Year 1 | 569 | 25.66 | 31.62 |
Year 2 | 625 | 25.46 | 33.04 |
Year 3 | 524 | 19.37 | 30.39 |
Multilevel models were fitted separately for WIAT-III math, reading, and spelling scores of students from the control and intervention schools, accounting for dependency among observations — i.e., repeated measurements (level 1) of students (level 2) who were nested within schools (level 3). In fact, schools were disaggregated in the analysis because the clustering at the school level (level 3) was negligible (intraclass correlation=.01–.05) and the number of schools was small (N=17; i.e., low power for 3-level modeling). Each fitted 2-level model examined the change in the WIAT score over the study period, the impact of percent time spent in MVPA or percent TOT post-lesson, and their interaction. Models also included covariates including age, gender, race, free or reduced lunch, BMI, and fitness (laps). All analyses were conducted using SAS 9.4 (SAS Institute, 2002–2012).
Results
Sample characteristics
Table 2 contains baseline demographics and anthropometrics adapted from Szabo-Reed et al. (5). Participants were 7.57±0.58 years old at baseline and age did not significantly differ between the intervention and control schools. BMI, height, weight, and fitness at baseline also did not significantly differ between the two groups. Ethnicity (p=.002), race (p=.012), and free or reduced meals (p=.001) did significantly differ between the intervention and controls schools, with the control schools having more minorities and a greater percentage of students enrolled in free or reduced meal plans than the intervention schools.
Table 2.
Total (N = 633) | A+PAAC Intervention (N = 319) | Control (N = 314) | Group difference | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Variable | n | M / % | SD | n | M / % | SD | n | M / % | SD | t / χ2 | df | p |
Age (year) | 633 | 7.57 | 0.58 | 319 | 7.58 | 0.58 | 314 | 7.56 | 0.59 | 0.35 | 631 | 0.726 |
Gender | 3.80 | 1 | 0.051 | |||||||||
Male | 313 | 49.4% | 170 | 53.3% | 143 | 45.5% | ||||||
Female | 320 | 50.6% | 149 | 46.7% | 171 | 54.5% | ||||||
Ethnicity | 16.90 | 4 | 0.002 | |||||||||
Not Hispanic/Latino | 536 | 84.7% | 283 | 88.7% | 253 | 80.6% | ||||||
Hispanic/Latino | 68 | 10.7% | 23 | 7.2% | 45 | 14.3% | ||||||
Unknown | 14 | 2.2% | 10 | 3.1% | 4 | 1.3% | ||||||
Refused | 0 | 0.0% | 0 | 0.0% | 0 | 0.0% | ||||||
Missing | 15 | 2.4% | 3 | 0.9% | 12 | 3.8% | ||||||
Race | 16.31 | 6 | 0.012 | |||||||||
White | 515 | 81.4% | 273 | 85.6% | 242 | 77.1% | ||||||
Black/African American | 21 | 3.3% | 8 | 2.5% | 13 | 4.1% | ||||||
Native Hawaiian/Pacific Islander | 0 | 0.0% | 0 | 0.0% | 0 | 0.0% | ||||||
Asian | 10 | 1.6% | 5 | 1.6% | 5 | 1.6% | ||||||
American Indian /Alaska Native | 6 | 0.9% | 4 | 1.3% | 2 | 0.6% | ||||||
Two or more races | 70 | 11.1% | 26 | 8.2% | 44 | 14.0% | ||||||
Unknown | 4 | 0.6% | 3 | 0.9% | 1 | 0.3% | ||||||
Refused | 0 | 0.0% | 0 | 0.0% | 0 | 0.0% | ||||||
Missing | 7 | 1.1% | 0 | 0.0% | 7 | 2.2% | ||||||
Free/reduced meals | 15.89 | 3 | 0.001 | |||||||||
Yes | 180 | 28.4% | 77 | 24.1% | 103 | 32.8% | ||||||
No | 438 | 69.2% | 240 | 75.2% | 198 | 63.1% | ||||||
Refused | 1 | 0.2% | 0 | 0.0% | 1 | 0.3% | ||||||
Missing | 14 | 2.2% | 2 | 0.6% | 12 | 3.8% | ||||||
Height (cm) | 625 | 130.89 | 6.94 | 315 | 130.99 | 6.68 | 310 | 130.79 | 7.22 | 0.36 | 623 | 0.722 |
Weight (kg) | 625 | 30.57 | 7.83 | 315 | 30.62 | 7.45 | 310 | 30.53 | 8.20 | 0.14 | 623 | 0.885 |
BMI (kg/m2) | 625 | 17.65 | 3.24 | 315 | 17.68 | 3.24 | 310 | 17.62 | 3.24 | 0.22 | 623 | 0.829 |
BMI percentile | 625 | 62.28 | 29.27 | 315 | 62.56 | 29.10 | 310 | 61.99 | 29.49 | 0.24 | 623 | 0.807 |
Laps | 560 | 17.29 | 9.36 | 277 | 17.99 | 9.73 | 283 | 16.61 | 8.96 | 1.74 | 558 | 0.082 |
Values are presented as Mean (M) or percent of total (%) and standard deviation (SD) Table adapted from Szabo-Reed et al. (2017).
As previously indicated in Szabo-Reed et al. (4), the participants who received A+PAAC lessons completed significantly more MVPA during the observed academic lessons than those in the control schools overall years of the intervention (p<.001). The intervention group performed MVPA 52.7%, 58.7%, and 60.1% of observed lesson time each year for 3 years, as compared to 1.6%, 3.6%, and 2.3% in the control group. Also, the greater time spent participating in MVPA was associated with a greater amount of TOT in the classroom (p<.01).
Influence of amount of MVPA on academic achievement
A series of 2-level models were fitted to examine the effect of percent time spent in MVPA on students’ math, reading, and spelling scores (N=546 each). Results showed that math score significantly increased in a linear pattern during the 3-year period (p<.001, see Table 3). Greater time spent in MVPA was significantly associated with greater math achievement (p=.034); however, this effect became smaller in the later years (interaction p=.001) — Cohen’s f2=.01. Boys had significantly higher math scores than girls (p=.036) and students receiving free/reduced meals had significantly lower math scores than students who did not (p<.001). Finally, greater fitness was also significantly associated with higher math scores (p<.001). There was no significant effect of age, race, or BMI. The covariates together explained 16% of the variance in math score (Cohen’s f2=.16).
Table 3.
Fixed effect | Estimate | SE | z | p | F | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Intercept | 105.47 | 6.35 | 16.61 | 0.000 | ||
Year (linear change) | 1.93 | 0.31 | 6.20 | 0.000 | 38.39 | 0.000 |
% time in Sofit MPVA | 0.02 | 0.01 | 2.13 | 0.034 | 4.52 | 0.034 |
Year * % time in Sofit MPVA | −0.02 | 0.01 | −3.20 | 0.001 | 10.22 | 0.001 |
Age | −0.81 | 0.83 | −0.97 | 0.331 | 0.95 | 0.331 |
Gender | 4.42 | 0.036 | ||||
Male | 2.08 | 0.99 | 2.10 | 0.036 | ||
Female (reference) | - | - | - | - | ||
Race | 1.28 | 0.273 | ||||
White (reference) | - | - | - | - | ||
Black/African American | −4.80 | 2.51 | −1.91 | 0.056 | ||
Asian | 4.86 | 4.00 | 1.21 | 0.225 | ||
American Indian /Alaska Native | −4.67 | 5.57 | −0.84 | 0.402 | ||
Two or more races | −0.76 | 1.63 | −0.47 | 0.640 | ||
Unknown | −5.60 | 8.04 | −0.70 | 0.487 | ||
Free/reduced meals | 17.48 | 0.000 | ||||
Yes | −4.63 | 1.11 | −4.18 | 0.000 | ||
No (reference) | - | - | - | - | ||
BMI percentile | 0.01 | 0.02 | 0.42 | 0.672 | 0.18 | 0.672 |
PACER Laps | 0.23 | 0.04 | 6.56 | 0.000 | 43.09 | 0.000 |
Random effect | Estimate | SE | z | p | ||
Student | 111.61 | 7.78 | 14.35 | 0.000 | ||
Residual | 26.89 | 1.32 | 20.34 | 0.000 |
Similar to math, reading score also linearly increased throughout the 3-year period (p<.001, see Table 4), however, there was no significant effect of percent time spent in MVPA on reading achievement (Cohen’s f2=.002). Reading score was also significantly lower among students who received free/reduced meals as compared to those who did not (p<.001). There was no significant effect of age, gender, race, BMI, or fitness on reading score. The covariates together explained 27% of the variance in math score (Cohen’s f2=.27).
Table 4.
Fixed effect | Estimate | SE | z | p | F | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Intercept | 91.31 | 7.75 | 11.78 | 0.000 | ||
Year (linear change) | 3.49 | 0.31 | 11.16 | 0.000 | 124.55 | 0.000 |
% time in Sofit MPVA | 0.01 | 0.01 | 0.80 | 0.424 | 0.64 | 0.424 |
Year * % time in Sofit MPVA | 0.00 | 0.01 | −0.37 | 0.713 | 0.14 | 0.713 |
Age | 1.69 | 1.01 | 1.67 | 0.095 | 2.79 | 0.095 |
Gender | 0.22 | 0.640 | ||||
Male | −0.56 | 1.20 | −0.47 | 0.640 | ||
Female (reference) | - | - | - | - | ||
Race | 0.30 | 0.910 | ||||
White (reference) | - | - | - | - | ||
Black/African American | −2.92 | 3.06 | −0.96 | 0.339 | ||
Asian | 1.38 | 4.87 | 0.28 | 0.777 | ||
American Indian /Alaska Native | −0.81 | 6.80 | −0.12 | 0.905 | ||
Two or more races | −1.58 | 1.99 | −0.79 | 0.428 | ||
Unknown | 0.02 | 9.75 | 0.00 | 0.999 | ||
Free/reduced meals | 11.28 | 0.001 | ||||
Yes | −4.53 | 1.35 | −3.36 | 0.001 | ||
No (reference) | - | - | - | - | ||
BMI percentile | −0.01 | 0.02 | −0.51 | 0.613 | 0.26 | 0.613 |
PACER Laps | −0.02 | 0.04 | −0.55 | 0.583 | 0.30 | 0.583 |
Random effect | Estimate | SE | z | p | ||
Student | 171.85 | 11.48 | 14.97 | 0.000 | ||
Residual | 25.87 | 1.28 | 20.21 | 0.000 |
Spelling score also increased in a linear pattern throughout the 3-year period (p<.001, see Table 5). More time spent in MVPA was significantly associated with higher spelling score (p<.001), and the benefit was maintained over the years (interaction p=.168) — Cohen’s f2=.02. Students receiving free/reduced meals had significantly lower spelling scores than those who did not receive free/reduced meals (p=.009), while those with higher fitness had significantly higher spelling scores (p=.045). There was no significant effect of age, gender, race, or BMI on spelling score. The covariates together explained 8% of the variance in spelling score (Cohen’s f2=.08).
Table 5.
Fixed effect | Estimate | SE | z | p | F | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Intercept | 95.39 | 6.90 | 13.83 | 0.000 | ||
Year (linear change) | 1.01 | 0.26 | 3.80 | 0.000 | 14.45 | 0.000 |
% time in Sofit MPVA | 0.04 | 0.01 | 3.88 | 0.000 | 15.04 | 0.000 |
Year * % time in Sofit MPVA | −0.01 | 0.01 | −1.38 | 0.168 | 1.91 | 0.168 |
Age | 0.48 | 0.90 | 0.53 | 0.597 | 0.28 | 0.597 |
Gender | 0.50 | 0.479 | ||||
Male | −0.76 | 1.07 | −0.71 | 0.479 | ||
Female (reference) | - | - | - | - | ||
Race | 0.33 | 0.896 | ||||
White (reference) | - | - | - | - | ||
Black/African American | 0.41 | 2.74 | 0.15 | 0.881 | ||
Asian | 4.05 | 4.36 | 0.93 | 0.354 | ||
American Indian /Alaska Native | −4.14 | 6.10 | −0.68 | 0.498 | ||
Two or more races | −0.73 | 1.77 | −0.41 | 0.680 | ||
Unknown | 2.76 | 8.73 | 0.32 | 0.752 | ||
Free/reduced meals | 6.78 | 0.009 | ||||
Yes | −3.13 | 1.20 | −2.60 | 0.009 | ||
No (reference) | - | - | - | - | ||
BMI percentile | 0.01 | 0.02 | 0.74 | 0.457 | 0.55 | 0.457 |
PACER Laps | 0.07 | 0.03 | 2.01 | 0.045 | 4.04 | 0.045 |
Random effect | Estimate | SE | z | p | ||
Student | 139.27 | 9.20 | 15.14 | 0.000 | ||
Residual | 18.60 | 0.91 | 20.34 | 0.000 |
Effect of time-on-task after PA on academic achievement
A second series of 2-level models were fitted to examine the effect of percent TOT post-lesson on academic achievement (N=539 for each score). Results showed that math score did not change significantly throughout the 3-year period (p=.191, see Table 6). There was no significant effect of percent TOT post-lesson on math achievement (Cohen’s f2=.001). Students receiving free/reduced meals had significantly lower math scores than students who did not receive free/reduced meals (p<.001). Finally, greater fitness was also significantly associated with higher math scores (p<.001). There was no significant effect of age, race, or BMI.
Table 6.
Fixed effect | Estimate | SE | z | p | F | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Intercept | 106.80 | 6.64 | 16.09 | 0.000 | ||
Year (linear change) | 1.81 | 1.38 | 1.31 | 0.191 | 1.72 | 0.191 |
% TOT post-lesson | 0.00 | 0.02 | −0.13 | 0.898 | 0.02 | 0.898 |
Year * % TOT post-lesson | −0.01 | 0.02 | −0.38 | 0.704 | 0.14 | 0.704 |
Age | −0.90 | 0.84 | −1.07 | 0.287 | 1.14 | 0.287 |
Gender | 3.62 | 0.057 | ||||
Male | 1.91 | 1.01 | 1.90 | 0.057 | ||
Female (reference) | - | - | - | - | ||
Race | 1.24 | 0.290 | ||||
White (reference) | - | - | - | - | ||
Black/African American | −4.79 | 2.53 | −1.90 | 0.058 | ||
Asian | 4.61 | 4.03 | 1.14 | 0.254 | ||
American Indian /Alaska Native | −4.79 | 5.61 | −0.85 | 0.393 | ||
Two or more races | −0.85 | 1.64 | −0.52 | 0.603 | ||
Unknown | −5.57 | 8.10 | −0.69 | 0.492 | ||
Free/reduced meals | 17.53 | 0.000 | ||||
Yes | −4.71 | 1.13 | −4.19 | 0.000 | ||
No (reference) | - | - | - | - | ||
BMI Percentile | 0.01 | 0.02 | 0.51 | 0.614 | 0.26 | 0.614 |
PACER Laps | 0.25 | 0.04 | 6.87 | 0.000 | 47.22 | 0.000 |
Random effect | Estimate | SE | z | p | ||
Student | 113.19 | 7.98 | 14.18 | 0.000 | ||
Residual | 27.13 | 1.38 | 19.73 | 0.000 |
Reading score linearly increased throughout the 3-year period (p<.001, see Table 7), however, there was no significant effect of percent TOT post-lesson on reading achievement (Cohen’s f2=.01). Reading score was also significantly lower among students who received free/reduced meals as compared to those who did not (p<.001). There was no significant effect of age, gender, race, BMI, or fitness on reading score.
Table 7.
Fixed effect | Estimate | SE | z | p | F | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Intercept | 90.69 | 7.91 | 11.47 | 0.000 | ||
Year (linear change) | 3.90 | 1.37 | 2.85 | 0.005 | 8.12 | 0.005 |
% TOT post-lesson | 0.00 | 0.02 | 0.15 | 0.883 | 0.02 | 0.883 |
Year * % TOT post-lesson | −0.01 | 0.01 | −0.35 | 0.730 | 0.12 | 0.730 |
Age | 1.77 | 1.01 | 1.74 | 0.082 | 3.04 | 0.082 |
Gender | 0.38 | 0.539 | ||||
Male | −0.74 | 1.20 | −0.61 | 0.539 | ||
Female (reference) | - | - | - | - | ||
Race | 0.36 | 0.874 | ||||
White (reference) | - | - | - | - | ||
Black/African American | −3.07 | 3.02 | −1.02 | 0.310 | ||
Asian | 1.63 | 4.82 | 0.34 | 0.735 | ||
American Indian /Alaska Native | −1.02 | 6.73 | −0.15 | 0.880 | ||
Two or more races | −1.74 | 1.97 | −0.88 | 0.377 | ||
Unknown | −0.20 | 9.65 | −0.02 | 0.983 | ||
Free/reduced meals | 11.75 | 0.001 | ||||
Yes | −4.62 | 1.35 | −3.43 | 0.001 | ||
No (reference) | - | - | - | - | ||
BMI Percentile | 0.00 | 0.02 | −0.19 | 0.850 | 0.04 | 0.850 |
PACER Laps | −0.02 | 0.04 | −0.61 | 0.540 | 0.38 | 0.540 |
Random effect | Estimate | SE | z | p | ||
Student | 167.98 | 11.34 | 14.81 | 0.000 | ||
Residual | 26.03 | 1.32 | 19.67 | 0.000 |
Spelling score did not change significantly during the 3-year period (p=.584, see Table 8). There was no significant effect of percent TOT post-lesson on spelling achievement (Cohen’s f2=.02). Students receiving free/reduced meals had significantly lower spelling scores than those who did not receive free/reduced meals (p=.007), while those with higher fitness had significantly higher spelling scores (p=.040). There was no significant effect of age, gender, race, or BMI on spelling score.
Table 8.
Fixed effect | Estimate | SE | z | p | F | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Intercept | 96.42 | 7.12 | 13.55 | 0.000 | ||
Year (linear change) | 0.65 | 1.18 | 0.55 | 0.584 | 0.30 | 0.584 |
% TOT post-lesson | −0.01 | 0.02 | −0.44 | 0.660 | 0.19 | 0.660 |
Year * % TOT post-lesson | 0.00 | 0.01 | 0.21 | 0.834 | 0.04 | 0.834 |
Age | 0.55 | 0.91 | 0.60 | 0.549 | 0.36 | 0.549 |
Gender | 0.49 | 0.486 | ||||
Male | −0.76 | 1.09 | −0.70 | 0.486 | ||
Female (reference) | - | - | - | - | ||
Race | 0.38 | 0.864 | ||||
White (reference) | - | - | - | - | ||
Black/African American | 0.04 | 2.75 | 0.02 | 0.987 | ||
Asian | 4.29 | 4.38 | 0.98 | 0.328 | ||
American Indian /Alaska Native | −4.06 | 6.12 | −0.66 | 0.508 | ||
Two or more races | −1.07 | 1.78 | −0.60 | 0.547 | ||
Unknown | 2.74 | 8.77 | 0.31 | 0.755 | ||
Free/reduced meals | 7.29 | 0.007 | ||||
Yes | −3.29 | 1.22 | −2.70 | 0.007 | ||
No (reference) | - | - | - | - | ||
BMI Percentile | 0.01 | 0.02 | 0.84 | 0.403 | 0.70 | 0.403 |
PACER Laps | 0.07 | 0.03 | 2.06 | 0.040 | 4.25 | 0.040 |
Random effect | Estimate | SE | z | p | ||
Student | 139.90 | 9.34 | 14.98 | 0.000 | ||
Residual | 19.39 | 0.98 | 19.77 | 0.000 |
Discussion
This study expanded upon Szabo-Reed et al.’s (5) findings to suggest that a greater percentage of time spent participating in MVPA results in higher math scores and spelling scores, but not reading scores, independent of TOT. These findings were also significantly associated with higher fitness and not receiving free/reduced lunches.
The present findings suggest that physically active classroom lessons have a small positive influence on academic achievement, specifically, math and spelling as compared to participating in less classroom physical activity. In our previous 3-year cluster randomized controlled trial (16), PAAC, we found significant improvements in reading, math, spelling, and composite scores from baseline to 3 years. However, this investigation was not appropriately powered to test this exploratory aim. Other researchers have also found physically active classroom lessons to be positively associated with improved academic achievement, but the findings as to which academic subjects are not as clear. Previous research by Erwin and colleagues (17) found that a 20-week intervention of 20+ minutes of activity per day resulted in significantly higher reading fluency and mathematics scores on a validated curriculum-based measure but no differences were seen on standardized test scores. In a 2-year study of a school-based physical activity program that included physically active academic lessons (Healthier Options for Public Schoolchildren), Hollar et al. (18) found significantly higher math and reading scores for intervention participants. Reed and colleagues (35) integrated physical activity into the elementary school curriculum for four months and found significant improvements in social studies, but no differences in math, language arts, or science. Finally, Mullender-Wijnsma et al. (19) compared performance on speeded tests of math and reading following a 1-year intervention of physically active academic lessons or a control condition. The study showed that math and reading scores improved in third graders when compared to controls, but math scores of second graders were significantly lower than the controls. Thus, three out of the four studies (16–18) on physically active academic lessons showed improvements in mathematics scores. The present investigation, as well as others (16) have also found classroom physical activity lessons to have a positive influence on spelling as well.
The present study suggests that participation in MVPA is important for improvements in academic achievement. We hypothesized that these improvements may be driven by an interaction between MVPA and increased TOT. However, this was not observed. Previous findings from Syväoja and colleagues (36) found that after controlling for gender, parental education, and remedial education, MVPA collected by accelerometry, had a positive association with attention. These findings suggest that MVPA may be beneficial for classroom attention (37). However, this was a “snapshot”, one-week evaluation of MVPA and single evaluation of attention, not a 3-year investigation, like the present study. In addition, a recent review (2) suggests that the effect of MVPA on attention/concentration may be acute (immediately after PA) and not chronic or its effect may dissipate over time. It has been hypothesized that exercise-induced arousal may result in a heightened level of arousal during physical activity which facilitates gains in attention/concentration (38). However, this effect of physical activity on attention requires further research for confirmation. Such research studies should measure both the long-term and immediate effect of physical activity on TOT and/or attention. This would help to determine if there are long-term gains in attention associated with classroom physical activity bouts, as well as the short-term gains that have been observed in acute studies (39–43).
In general, physically active children have greater cardiovascular fitness compared to individuals who are sedentary and cardiovascular fitness has been associated with better cognition and academic achievement (44). The present study suggests that fitness is associated with more time spent in MVPA and TOT as well as math and spelling performance. A variety of cross-sectional studies have found a positive relationship between fitness and cognitive performance (39–43). Two prospective studies have been published that report the changes in cognitive performance observed over time relative to baseline measures of aerobic fitness and suggest that higher aerobic fitness at baseline is associated with better cognitive performance at both baseline and follow-up (45, 46). Similarly, randomized controlled trials using MVPA in children (47–53) that measure fitness, have consistently shown significant improvements in fitness as well as cognition, specifically executive control (i.e., working memory, the ability plan, and perform goal-oriented tasks), which appears to be important for succeeding in the classroom (37).
The association among gender, socioeconomic status, and academic achievement has not been clearly established. In the present study, we observed that boys had significantly higher math scores than girls. Additionally, students receiving free/reduced meals had significantly lower math, reading and spelling scores than students who did not. The impact of the covariates within each model was substantial with 16% of the variance associated with math performance, 27% for reading and 8% for spelling. This suggests that the covariates on academic achievement including gender, socioeconomic status need to be defined. In many studies, the impact of such variables is either not explored or the reason for the association is spurious in nature (44). Thus, future studies should be designed and powered to explore the association between demographic covariates, such as gender and socioeconomic status, and academic achievement.
Limitations
A pre-intervention value or true baseline value of TOT or SOFIT was not collected prior to the students participating in the A+PAAC physical activity lessons as the intervention began when the school year started. Thus, future studies may endeavor to collect baseline assessment of classroom TOT and physical activity behavior prior to starting the intervention. In addition, students were only observed for a short time (5 to 20 minutes) following academic lessons (A+PAAC or control), therefore we are unable to determine if the length of impact physical activity participation impacts TOT in the classroom. Finally, the authors are assuming that classroom PA coded as a 4 (walking) or 5 (jogging/running) are activities that represent MVPA. This assumption was based on previous work where SOFIT and energy expenditure were collected during A+PAAC activities in the classroom (23).
Conclusion
In conclusion, the present study found that more time spent in MVPA and participation in on-task behavior after a physically active lesson was significantly associated with higher math scores and spelling scores, but not reading scores. Overall, these findings provide support that physically active classroom lessons may have a positive impact on academic achievement in elementary aged children. Future studies should continue to evaluate the influence of physically active classroom lesson on time spent on-task and their impact on academic achievement to determine the sustainability of physical activity’s effect on classroom behavior. In addition, these findings could have positive implications for the translation and further dissemination and use of MVPA in the classroom to improve TOT and academic outcomes. Policymakers should consider the role of MVPA on academic TOT and academic achievement when formulating educational recommendations and regulations.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
The results of the study are presented clearly, honestly, and without fabrication, falsification, or inappropriate data manipulation. The authors would like to thank their funding sources from the Nation Institutes of Health R01-DK85317 (Donnelly) and KL2TR002367 (Szabo-Reed). The results of the present study do not constitute endorsement by ACSM.
Disclosure of funding: This study was supported by the National Institutes of Health grant R01-DK085317 (Donnelly) and KL2TR002367 (Szabo-Reed). Trial registration: US NIH Clinical Trials, NCT01699295.
References
- 1.Promotion USDoHaHSOoDPaH. Healthy People 2020; In. Washington, DC: Available at https://www.healthypeople.gov/2020/topics-objectives Accessed July 3, 2018; 2014. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Watson A, Timperio A, Brown H, Best K, Hesketh KD. Effect of classroom-based physical activity interventions on academic and physical activity outcomes: a systematic review and meta-analysis. The international journal of behavioral nutrition and physical activity. 2017;14(1):114-. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Donnelly JE, Sullivan DK, Gibson C et al. Physical activity across the curriculum: Effects for BMI and academic achievement. Obesity. 2007;15(9):A18. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Donnelly JE, Hillman CH, Greene JL et al. Physical activity and academic achievement across the curriculum: Results from a 3-year cluster-randomized trial. Prev Med. 2017;99:140–5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Szabo-Reed AN, Willis EA, Lee J, Hillman CH, Washburn RA, Donnelly JE. Impact of 3 Years of Classroom Physical Activity Bouts on Time-on-Task Behavior. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Mahar MT, Murphy SK, Rowe DA, Golden J, Shields AT, Raedeke TD. Effects of a classroom-based program on physical activity and on-task behavior. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2006;38(12):2086–94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Mahar MT. Impact of short bouts of physical activity on attention-to-task in elementary school children. Prev Med. 2011;52 Suppl 1:S60–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Ma JK, Le Mare L, Gurd BJ. Four minutes of in-class high-intensity interval activity improves selective attention in 9- to 11-year olds. Appl Physiol Nutr Metab. 2015;40(3):238–44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Daly-Smith AJ, Zwolinsky S, McKenna J, Tomporowski PD, Defeyter MA, Manley A. Systematic review of acute physically active learning and classroom movement breaks on children’s physical activity, cognition, academic performance and classroom behaviour: understanding critical design features. BMJ Open Sport & Exercise Medicine. 2018;4(1). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Grieco LA, Jowers EM, Bartholomew JB. Physically active academic lessons and time on task: the moderating effect of body mass index. Medicine and science in sports and exercise. 2009;41(10):1921–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Howie EK, Schatz J, Pate RR. Acute Effects of Classroom Exercise Breaks on Executive Function and Math Performance: A Dose–Response Study. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport. 2015;86(3):217–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Caterino MC, Polak ED. Effects of Two Types of Activity on the Performance of Second-, Third-, and Fourth-Grade Students on a Test of Concentration. Perceptual and Motor Skills. 1999;89(1):245–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Tine M Acute aerobic exercise: an intervention for the selective visual attention and reading comprehension of low-income adolescents. Frontiers in Psychology. 2014;5:575. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Hillman CH, Pontifex MB, Raine LB, Castelli DM, Hall EE, Kramer AF. The effect of acute treadmill walking on cognitive control and academic achievement in preadolescent children. Neuroscience. 2009;159(3):1044–54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Duncan M, Johnson A. The effect of differing intensities of acute cycling on preadolescent academic achievement. European journal of sport science. 2013;14(3):279–86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Donnelly JE, Greene JL, Gibson CA et al. Physical Activity Across the Curriculum (PAAC): a randomized controlled trial to promote physical activity and diminish overweight and obesity in elementary school children. Preventive medicine. 2009;49(4):336–41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Erwin H, Fedewa A, Ahn S. Student academic performance outcomes of a classroom physical activity intervention: A pilot study. International Electronic Journal of Elementary Education. 2013;5(2):109–24. [Google Scholar]
- 18.Hollar D, Messiah SE, Lopez-Mitnik G, Hollar TL, Almon M, Agatston AS. Effect of a two-year obesity prevention intervention on percentile changes in body mass index and academic performance in low-income elementary school children. Am J Public Health. 2010;100(4):646–53. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Mullender-Wijnsma MJ, Hartman E, de Greeff JW, Bosker RJ, Doolaard S, Visscher C. Improving academic performance of school-age children by physical activity in the classroom: 1-year program evaluation. J Sch Health. 2015;85(6):365–71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Donnelly JE, Greene JL, Gibson CA et al. Physical activity and academic achievement across the curriculum (A + PAAC): rationale and design of a 3-year, cluster-randomized trial. BMC Public Health. 2013;13:307. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Wechsler D Wechsler Individual Achievement Test 3rd Edition (WIAT III). London: The Psychological Corp.; 2009. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Honas JJ, Washburn RA, Smith BK, Greene JL, Donnelly JE. Energy expenditure of the physical activity across the curriculum intervention. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2008;40(8):1501–5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Honas JJ, Willis EA, Herrmann SD, Greene JL, Washburn RA, Donnelly JE. Energy Expenditure and Intensity of Classroom Physical Activity in Elementary School Children. Journal of physical activity & health. 2016;13(6 Suppl 1):S53–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.McKenzie TL. Observational measures of children’s physical activity. J School Health. 1991;61(5):224–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Katz RC, Singh NN. Increasing recreational behavior in mentally retarded children. Behavior modification. 1986;10(4):508–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Shimabukuro SM, Prater MA, Jenkins A, Edelen-Smith P. The effects of self-monitoring of academic performance on students with learning disabilities with ADD/ADHD. Educ Treatment Child. 1999;22:397–414. [Google Scholar]
- 27.Kuczmarski RJ, Ogden CL, Guo SS et al. 2000 CDC Growth Charts for the United States: methods and development. Vital Health Stat. 2002;11(246):1–190. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Welk GJ, Meredith MD. Fitnessgram/Activitygram Reference Guide. Dallas TX: The Cooper Institute; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- 29.Carrel AL, Bowser J, White D et al. Standardized Childhood Fitness Percentiles Derived from School-Based Testing. The Journal of Pediatrics. 2012;161(1):120–4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Leger LA, Lambert J. A maximal multistage 20-m shuttle run test to predict VO2 max. Eur J Appl Physiol Occup Physiol. 1982;49(1):1–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Leger LA, Mercier D, Gadoury C, Lambert J. The multistage 20 metre shuttle run test for aerobic fitness. J Sports Sci. 1988;6(2):93–101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Mahar MT, Guerieri AM, Hanna MS, Kemble CD. Estimation of aerobic fitness from 20-m multistage shuttle run test performance. Am J Prev Med. 2011;41(4 Suppl 2):S117–23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Olds T, Tomkinson G, Léger L, Cazorla G. Worldwide variation in the performance of children and adolescents: An analysis of 109 studies of the 20-m shuttle run test in 37 countries. Journal of Sports Sciences. 2006;24(10):1025–38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Pearson. The Wechsler Individual Achievement Test-Third Edition (WIAT III). In. San Antonio, TX: Pearson; 2009. [Google Scholar]
- 35.Reed JA, Einstein G, Hahn E, Hooker SP, Gross VP, Kravitz J. Examining the impact of integrating physical activity on fluid intelligence and academic performance in an elementary school setting: a preliminary investigation. Journal of physical activity & health. 2010;7(3):343–51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Syväoja HJ, Tammelin TH, Ahonen T, Kankaanpää A, Kantomaa MT. The Associations of Objectively Measured Physical Activity and Sedentary Time with Cognitive Functions in School-Aged Children. PLoS ONE. 2014;9(7):e103559. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Raine LB, Lee HK, Saliba BJ, Chaddock-Heyman L, Hillman CH, Kramer AF. The influence of childhood aerobic fitness on learning and memory. PloS one. 2013;8(9):e72666. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Lambourne K, Tomporowski P. The effect of exercise-induced arousal on cognitive task performance: a meta-regression analysis. Brain research. 2010;1341:12–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Jacob A, D’Souza CD, Sumithra S, Avadhani S, Subramanya CM, Srinivasan K. Aerobic Fitness and Cognitive Functions in Economically Underprivileged Children Aged 7–9 Years: A preliminary Study from South India. International Journal of Biomedical Science. 2011;7(1):51–4. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Scudder MR, Khan NA, Drollette ES et al. Cognitive Control in Preadolescent Children With Risk Factors for Metabolic Syndrome. Health Psychology. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Scudder MR, Lambourne K, Drollette ES et al. Aerobic capacity and cognitive control in elementary school-age children. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2014;46(5):1025–35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Drollette ES, Scudder MR, Raine LB et al. The sexual dimorphic association of cardiorespiratory fitness to working memory in children. Dev Sci. 2015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Buck SM, Hillman CH, Castelli DM. The relation of aerobic fitness to stroop task performance in preadolescent children. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2008;40(1):166–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Donnelly JE, Hillman CH, Castelli D et al. Physical Activity, Fitness, Cognitive Function, and Academic Achievement in Children: A Systematic Review. Medicine and science in sports and exercise. 2016;48(6):1223–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Chaddock L, Hillman CH, Pontifex MB, Johnson CR, Raine LB, Kramer AF. Childhood aerobic fitness predicts cognitive performance one year later. J Sports Sci. 2012;30(5):421–30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Niederer I, Kriemler S, Gut J et al. Relationship of aerobic fitness and motor skills with memory and attention in preschoolers (Ballabeina): A cross-sectional and longitudinal study. BMC Pediatrics. 2011;11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Kamijo K, Pontifex MB, O’Leary KC et al. The Effects of an Afterschool Physical Activity Program on Working Memory in Preadolescent Children. Developmental Science. 2011;14(5):1046–58. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Chaddock-Heyman L, Erickson KI, Voss MW et al. The effects of physical activity on functional MRI activation associated with cognitive control in children: a randomized controlled intervention. Frontiers in human neuroscience. 2013;7:72. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Monti JM, Hillman CH, Cohen NJ. Aerobic fitness enhances relational memory in preadolescent children: the FITKids randomized control trial. Hippocampus. 2012;22(9):1876–82. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Hillman CH, Pontifex MB, Castelli DM et al. Effects of the FITKids Randomized Controlled Trial on Executive Control and Brain Function. Pediatrics. 2014;134(4):e1063–71. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Krafft CE, Pierce JE, Schwarz NF et al. An eight month randomized controlled exercise intervention alters resting state synchrony in overweight children. Neuroscience. 2014;256:445–55. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Krafft CE, Schaeffer DJ, Schwarz NF et al. Improved frontoparietal white matter integrity in overweight children is associated with attendance at an after-school exercise program. Dev Neurosci. 2014;36(1):1–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Krafft CE, Schwarz NF, Chi L et al. An 8-month randomized controlled exercise trial alters brain activation during cognitive tasks in overweight children. Obesity (Silver Spring, Md ) 2014;22(1):232–42. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.