Abstract
Interaural time differences (ITDs) are a major cue for localizing the azimuthal position of sounds. The dominant models for processing ITDs are based on the Jeffress model and predict neurons that fire maximally at a common ITD across their responsive frequency range. Such neurons are indeed found in the binaural pathways and are referred to as “peak-type.” However, other neurons discharge minimally at a common ITD (trough-type), and others do not display a common ITD at the maxima or minima (intermediate-type). From recordings of neurons in the auditory cortex of the unanesthetized rabbit to low-frequency tones and envelopes of high-frequency sounds, we show that the different response types combine to form a continuous axis of best ITD. This axis extends to ITDs well beyond that allowed by the head width. In Jeffress-type models, sensitivity to large ITDs would require neural delay lines with large differences in path lengths between the two ears. Our results suggest instead that sensitivity to large ITDs is created with short delay lines, using neurons that display intermediate- and trough-type responses. We demonstrate that a neuron's best ITD can be predicted from (1) its characteristic delay, a rough measure of the delay line, (2) its characteristic phase, which defines the response type, and (3) its best frequency for ITD sensitivity. The intermediate- and trough-type neurons that have large best ITDs are predicted to be most active when sounds at the two ears are decorrelated and may transmit information about auditory space other than sound localization.
Keywords: auditory neurophysiology, auditory pathways, interaural temporal disparities, sound localization, low-frequency hearing, low-frequency signals
The difference in the time of arrival of low-frequency sounds at the two ears is a major cue for sound localization along the azimuth. The Jeffress (1948) model is the dominant scheme for how sensitivity to these interaural time differences (ITDs) arises. In his model, neurons at the primary site of binaural interaction act as coincidence detectors or equivalently as cross-correlators. Each neuron receives excitatory, phase-locked inputs from each ear, and the path lengths of the inputs vary from neuron to neuron. For each neuron, there is an ITD that offsets the difference in path lengths such that the inputs arrive in coincidence and the neuron fires maximally. By assuming that the path lengths are systematically arrayed, a neural place code for azimuthal sound location is created.
Although there is considerable behavioral, physiological, and anatomical support for the Jeffress model (for review, see Carr, 1993; Colburn, 1995; Kuwada et al., 1997; Stern and Trahiotis, 1997;Yin et al., 1997; Joris et al., 1998), it is insufficient to explain the variety of neural responses encountered in the auditory system. Some neurons discharge maximally at a common ITD across frequency (i.e., peak-type neurons), as predicted by the Jeffress model, but many do not. Instead, some neurons discharge minimally at a common ITD (trough-type neurons), whereas others do not display a common ITD at which the response is maximal or minimal (intermediate-type neurons) (cf. Rose et al., 1966; Yin and Kuwada, 1983; Kuwada et al., 1987;Reale and Brugge, 1990; Stanford et al., 1992; Batra et al., 1997a,b). Trough- and intermediate-type neurons are not predicted by the Jeffress model and are not part of later models based on his original scheme (Colburn, 1973; Stern and Colburn, 1978;Shackelton et al., 1992; Stern and Trahiotis, 1992).
The Jeffress model also does not consider ITD sensitivity to high-frequency signals. In the Jeffress model, sensitivity to ITDs arises from phase-locked inputs to low-frequency sounds. However, phase-locked responses also occur to the envelopes of high-frequency sounds, and many neurons display sensitivity to ITDs in envelopes (Crow et al., 1980; Yin et al., 1984; Batra et al., 1989; Joris and Yin, 1995). As with low-frequency sounds, there are peak-, trough-, and intermediate-type neurons sensitive to ITDs in the envelopes of high-frequency sounds.
The Jeffress model is an attempt to explain ITD sensitivity at the primary site of binaural interaction, which is in the superior olivary complex (SOC). In this report, we examined ITD sensitivity to low-frequency sounds and to envelopes of high-frequency sounds near the endpoint of ITD processing, in the primary auditory cortex. We demonstrate that peak-, trough-, and intermediate-type responses to ITDs in low-frequency sounds and envelopes can create a single, functionally continuous axis of ITD representation. This axis extends to values of ITD much greater than that normally considered. The presence of this continuous and extended axis indicates that Jeffress-type models capture only a part of the ITD representation in the brain.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Experimental animal. Single and multiunit recording was performed in five female Dutch-Belted rabbits (1.5–2.5 kg). Surgical and experimental procedures have been described previously (Kuwada et al., 1987; Batra et al., 1989) and will be outlined here briefly.
Surgical procedures. All surgery was performed using aseptic techniques on rabbits with clean external ears. Under anesthesia (ketamine, 35 mg/kg, and xylazine, 5 mg/kg, i.m.), a square brass rod was anchored to the skull using screws and dental acrylic. Several days later, the animal was reanesthetized, and a small rectangular hole was made in the skull overlying the dorsal part of the auditory cortex, extending from ∼2 to 5 mm posterior to bregma and from ∼10 to 12 mm lateral to the midline. The hole was covered with sterilized medical elastopolymer (Smith and Nephew Rolyan). At this time, custom ear molds were made for sound delivery.
In one animal, during the initial surgery a catheter for injection of anesthetics during recording was inserted ∼6 cm into the external jugular vein and led subdermally to the skull. A blunted hypodermic (24 gauge) was inserted into the catheter and cemented to the head bar assembly. Heparinized saline was injected daily to keep the catheter patent.
Recording procedures and data collection. All recordings were conducted in a double-walled, sound-insulated chamber. The unanesthetized rabbit was placed in a body stocking from which its head protruded, seated in a padded cradle, and further restrained using nylon straps. The stocking and straps provided only mild restraint, their primary purpose being to discourage movements that might cause injury to the rabbit. The rabbit's head was fixed in a constant position by clamping to the surgically implanted rod. After the rabbit was secured, the elastopolymer covering was removed to expose the opening in the skull. To eliminate pain during the penetration of the electrode, a topical anesthetic (lidocaine) was applied to the dura for ∼5 min and then removed by aspiration. With these procedures, rabbits remained still for a period of 2 or more hours, an important criterion for single-neuron recording. Typically, a rabbit participated in daily recording sessions over a period of 2–6 months. A session was terminated if the rabbit showed any signs of discomfort. The rabbit's comfort was a priority both for ethical reasons and because movements made it difficult to record from single neurons.
Extracellular recordings were made with glass-coated, platinum–tungsten microelectrodes (tip diameter of 1–2 μm; impedances of 10–30 MΣ). The action potentials of single neurons or small clusters of neurons were isolated with the aid of a time/amplitude window discriminator (BAK Electronics, Germantown, MD) and timed relative to the stimulus onset with an accuracy of 10 μsec.
The rabbit has a lissencephalic cerebral cortex, and the primary auditory cortex (AI) is situated on its lateral surface. The vertical electrode penetrations were tangential to the cortical surface and approximately perpendicular to the isofrequency contours (McMullen and Glaser, 1982). Because of the lissencephalic cortex, long penetrations (>2 mm) could be made within the auditory cortex. By exploring the medial–lateral dimension until the most robust responses were encountered, the electrodes could be oriented toward the middle layers of the AI.
Acoustic stimulation. Stimuli were generated by a two-channel digital stimulation system (Rhode, 1976) and delivered independently to the two ears through Beyer DT-48 earphones coupled to the custom-fitted ear molds to form a sealed system. Stimuli were pure tones, sinusoidally amplitude-modulated (SAM) tones or noise presented either monaurally or binaurally. The stimuli were gated on and off with linear rise and fall times of 4.0 msec. Amplitudes and phases of tones (60 Hz to 40 kHz in 20 Hz steps) were calibrated before the first recording session in each animal, by means of a probe tube that extended ∼1 mm from the end of the sound tube. The sound tube extended to within ∼2.5 cm of the tympanum. This calibration was used to deliver tones of specified amplitude and phase.
Sensitivity to ITDs was primarily assessed using binaural-beat stimuli. For low-frequency sounds (less than ∼2 kHz), the binaural-beat stimulus was created by delivering tones to the two ears that differed by 1 Hz, which resulted in a continuously varying ITD with a period of 1 sec (Kuwada et al., 1980). For high-frequency sounds (>2.5 kHz), the binaural-beat stimulus was created by delivering SAM tones to the two ears that had the same carrier frequency (at or near the best frequency of the neuron) but had modulation frequencies that differed by 1 Hz. In some neurons, ITD sensitivity was assessed using noise as well as binaural-beat stimuli. The noises were either low-pass (cutoff frequencies of up to 10 kHz) or bandpass (1/2 or 1 octave wide).
Determination of best frequencies. Two types of best frequency were determined. The first was the best frequency (BF) to monaural (usually contralateral) or diotic tones. This determination was made using isointensity tone bursts, 75 msec long and repeated every 300–400 msec, in 1/2-octave steps from 35,000 to 243 Hz. This frequency range was taken at one or a few suprathreshold levels (usually 40–70 dB sound pressure level). In neurons with narrow-frequency tuning, finer steps of frequency were taken within the responsive range. The centroid of the response range that exceeded 50% of the maximum response was considered the neuron's best frequency. If responses at more than one intensity were recorded, the lowest intensity above threshold was used.
The second determination was the best frequency for ITD sensitivity (BFITD). This was taken as the centroid of the responses over the range of frequencies or modulation frequencies at which a neuron showed significant synchrony to the binaural-beat stimulus (Rayleigh test of uniformity, p < 0.001) (Mardia, 1972). Note that many neurons sensitive to ITDs in envelopes were low-pass, so that the BFITD in these neurons represents an average modulation frequency for ITD sensitivity.
RESULTS
Our results are based on recordings from single (n= 303) and multiple (n = 233) neurons from the primary auditory cortex of the unanesthetized rabbit that were sensitive to ITDs. The response types and distributions were similar between single neuron and multineuron recordings. In the following, all of the illustrated responses are from single neurons, whereas the distributions are pooled single and multiple neurons. Of the total neurons, 410 were sensitive to ITDs in low-frequency sounds (less than ∼2 kHz; 219 single neurons), and 126 were sensitive to ITDs in the envelopes of high-frequency sounds (84 single neurons). All of these met the Yin and Kuwada (1983) criterion for linearity in the change of mean response phase with changes in the stimulus frequency (see below).
Effect of anesthesia
We used an unanesthetized preparation, because anesthesia is known to alter ITD sensitivity in the inferior colliculus (IC) (Kuwada et al., 1989). In the auditory cortex of barbiturate-anesthetized cats, most neurons (74%) sensitive to static ITDs were insensitive to dynamic ITDs created by the binaural-beat stimulus (Reale and Brugge, 1990). However, in the unanesthetized rabbit almost all neurons sensitive to ITDs responded vigorously to binaural beats. To determine whether this difference could be caused by anesthesia, we tested the responses of neurons to static and dynamic ITDs in low-frequency sounds before and after an intravenous injection of sodium pentobarbital. In the example shown in Figure 1, before anesthesia the neuron responded vigorously to both static (Fig.1A) and dynamic (Fig. 1B) ITDs. After anesthesia, the same neuron retained tuning to static ITDs, although the response was greatly reduced (Fig. 1C). The neuron lost its sensitivity to dynamic ITDs (Fig.1D). Most neurons (7/12) lost sensitivity to dynamic ITDs while remaining tuned to static ITDs. Thus, it seems that the reported inability of cortical neurons to follow dynamic ITDs (Reale and Brugge, 1990) was caused by anesthesia. We therefore regard the use of unanesthetized animals as critical for investigating ITD sensitivity.
Types of ITD-sensitive responses
Jeffress-type models predict that all neurons have peak-type responses; i.e., the peak response occurs at or near the same ITD across frequencies. Peak-type responses are common at all levels of the auditory system, but they are not the only response type encountered. In this section, we will show examples of peak-, trough-, and intermediate-type responses for both low-frequency sounds and for envelopes and demonstrate how parameters that describe the responses are determined. Subsequent sections will show how each parameter affects the tuning to ITDs and describe the distributions of the parameters among cortical neurons.
Examples of different response types are shown in Figures2 and 3 for neurons sensitive to ITDs in low-frequency tones and in envelopes, respectively. For each neuron, the family of curves in theleft column shows the tuning to ITDs derived from the responses to binaural-beat stimuli with different tone or envelope frequencies. For the peak-type neurons (Figs. 2A,3A) the ITD functions at different frequencies align (dashed lines) at or near the maximal discharge for each frequency. In contrast, for the trough-type neurons (Figs.2B, 3B) the ITD functions align at or near the minimal discharge for each frequency. For the intermediate-type neurons (Figs. 2C, 3C) the curves do not align at the peaks or the troughs but rather somewhere in-between.
Two measures are commonly used to assess the ITD to which a neuron is tuned. The first of these is the ITD at which the ITD functions at each frequency best align (Figs. 2, dashed lines, 3,dashed lines) and is called the characteristic delay (CD) (Rose et al., 1966). The CD can be determined by a least squares, linear fit to a plot of the mean interaural phase of the response versus the stimulating frequency (Figs. 2, middle column, 3, middle column) (Yin and Kuwada, 1983). The slope of the fit is the CD. Yin and Kuwada (1983)devised a measure of the significance of the fit based on the mean square error. Only a few cortical neurons failed this test and were not included. The phase intercept at 0 Hz is called the characteristic phase (CP). The CP is a measure of whether alignment occurs near the peaks of the ITD functions (CP near 0 cycles), near the troughs (CP near ±0.5 cycles), or intermediate between the peak and trough (CP near ±0.25 cycles). Thus, the CP can be used to distinguish peak-, trough-, and intermediate-type responses. In Jeffress-type models, the CDs for peak-type neurons correspond to the difference in conduction delay for the excitatory inputs from each ear to reach the binaural coincidence detector. The models do not consider CDs of trough- and intermediate-type neurons.
The second measure used to assess the ITD to which a neuron is tuned is the best ITD of the composite curve (Yin and Kuwada, 1983). The composite curve is obtained by averaging the ITD functions at each frequency. The peak of a parabola fit to the upper 50% of the composite curve is considered the neuron's best ITD (Figs. 2,arrows, 3, arrows). As expected from the Jeffress model, for the peak-type neurons the best ITD and the CD are essentially equal (Figs. 2A, right column, 3A, right column). However, they are not equal for trough- and intermediate-type neurons (Figs. 2B,C, right column, 3B,C, right column).
The composite curve is expected to be representative of a neuron's ITD sensitivity to a broad-band sound. Figure4 displays the correspondence between a neuron's composite curve and its response to ITDs of a noise stimulus for several neurons sensitive to ITDs in low-frequency sounds: a peak-type neuron (Fig. 4A), two intermediate-type neurons (Fig. 4B,C), and a trough-type neuron (Fig.4D). In each case, the ITD tuning of the composite curves was similar to that from the noise. Across our sample (Fig.4E), there was a good correspondence between the best ITDs derived from the composite curve and from noise (r= 0.86; slope = 0.93). Thus, among cortical neurons, as in neurons from the IC (Yin et al., 1986; Palmer et al., 1990), the composite curves reasonably predict the responses to a broad-band sound, suggesting that integration across frequency is relatively linear.
Relationships among parameters important for ITD sensitivity
In Jeffress-type models, the range of best ITDs is governed by the range of CDs. This range is shown in Figure5A using as examples five peak-type neurons (CPs near 0) tuned to ipsilateral delays, or contralateral space. In each case, the CDs (arrows) were similar to the best ITDs. Thus, when only peak-type neurons are considered, the range of best ITDs is equivalent to the range of CDs.
In intermediate- and trough-type neurons, which are not considered in Jeffress-type models, the range of best ITDs is not limited by the range of CDs. The five neurons in Figure 5B had a narrow range of CDs (from −30 to −140 μsec) but covered a wide range of best ITDs (from −60 to −680 μsec), because they differed in their CP. As the CP changed from ∼0 to −0.5 cycles, i.e., from peak-type through intermediate-type to trough-type, the best ITDs became progressively larger.
The effect of CP on the best ITD is dependent on frequency tuning. Figure 5C shows five intermediate-type neurons (CPs near −0.25 cycles) that had progressively lower-frequency tuning. This lower-frequency tuning resulted in progressively larger best ITDs. This figure also demonstrates a continuum between the ITD tuning to low-frequency sounds and envelopes. The two intermediate-type neurons that were sensitive to ITDs in low-frequency sounds (solid lines) had BFITD values (see Materials and Methods) of 1000 and 450 Hz and were sensitive to relatively small ITDs (<1 msec). The remaining three intermediate-type neurons (dashed lines) were sensitive to ITDs in envelopes and had much lower BFITD values (50–200 Hz). The best ITDs of these neurons extended to several milliseconds (Fig. 5, note the difference in scale betweenC, A,B). Thus, sensitivity to lower frequencies greatly increased the shift in best ITD from the CD for a given CP. This effect of frequency is also not considered in Jeffress-type models.
The net effects of variations in the CD, CP, and frequency tuning on a neuron's best ITD (ITDbest) can be described by the equation:
Equation 1 |
By the use of this equation, the best ITD is equal to the CD when the CP is 0, as is the case for peak-type neurons. As the CP moves away from 0, the best ITD differs from the CD by an amount dependent on the BFITD, with lower-frequency tuning yielding a larger difference. Figure 6 compares the best ITDs of neurons in the cortex with the best ITDs predicted from Equation 1. For neurons sensitive to low-frequency sounds (Fig.6A) or envelopes (Fig. 6B), most comparisons fell on or near the line of equality, supporting the relationship described by Equation 1.
The distributions of parameters important for ITD sensitivity
The previous section described the physical relationships among parameters important for ITD sensitivity. In this section, we will show how these parameters are distributed among neurons in the auditory cortex and the resultant representation of ITDs that is achieved.
Frequency tuning
Before turning to the best frequencies for ITD sensitivity used in Equation 1, we will first consider the more traditional best frequency measured to monaural or diotic tones. The prevailing view is that ITD sensitivity to low-frequency sounds occurs in neurons with low BFs and that ITD sensitivity to envelopes occurs in neurons with high BFs. However, we found no such simple dichotomy. Instead, many neurons sensitive to ITDs in low-frequency sounds (less than ∼2 kHz) had high BFs (more than ∼2 kHz) (Fig.7A; 94/293 neurons or 32% of the sample). The range of BFs of neurons sensitive to ITDs in low-frequency sounds extended from ∼300 Hz to ∼8 kHz. The range of BFs for neurons sensitive to ITDs in envelopes extended from ∼2 to 40 kHz. Sensitivity to ITDs in envelopes was only tested in neurons with high BFs but could also be expected to occur in neurons with low BFs (see Joris and Yin, 1995). Thus, sensitivity to both types of ITDs extends over a wide range of the tonotopic axis in the auditory cortex.
In contrast to the BFs to monaural or diotic tones (Fig.7A), the BFITD values to envelope modulations were generally lower than the BFITDvalues for low-frequency sounds. Figure 7B shows the distributions of BFITD values for neurons sensitive to both signal types (median = 172 Hz modulation frequency for envelopes and 863 Hz for low-frequency sounds). In Figure7B, the distributions of frequency representation for the two signal types appear primarily separate and to drop off below ∼200 Hz for envelopes. However, this view is misleading, because many of the envelope-sensitive neurons were low-pass to modulation frequency. When the proportion of neurons sensitive to ITDs at each frequency was considered (Fig. 7C), the distributions of the two signal types overlapped such that all frequencies from <50 Hz to nearly 2 kHz were represented by a large proportion of neurons. Sensitivity to envelopes therefore serves to extend the range of sensitivity to ITDs to lower frequencies than can be extracted from the fine structure of signals.
CP
For neurons sensitive to ITDs in low-frequency tones, there was a nearly uniform distribution of CPs (Fig.8A); i.e., there were many intermediate-type neurons that filled the range between peak- and trough-type. For neurons sensitive to ITDs in envelopes (Fig.8B), there was a large group of neurons with CPs near 0, but also many intermediate- and trough-type neurons. Thus, in both cases there was a continuum of CP, such that it was not possible to draw clear dividing lines between different response types. For both tones and envelopes, there were more neurons (58% for tones and 73% for envelopes) with negative CPs. Neurons with negative CPs have best ITDs that are shifted toward ipsilateral delays relative to the CDs. Ipsilateral delays correspond to those created by sounds in the contralateral field.
CD
The distribution of CDs was similar between different types of neurons and across sensitivity to tones and envelopes. For illustrative purposes we divided our sample into two groups: peak- and trough-like. The division was made at a CP of ±0.25 cycles, and consequently both groups contain numerous intermediate neurons. The distributions of CDs (Fig. 8C,D) for peak- and trough-like neurons were similar, suggesting that the different response types encode a similar range of differences in the conduction delays from the two sides. For low-frequency sounds, almost all neurons (90%) had CDs within ±500 μsec. The range of CDs was somewhat wider for envelope ITD sensitivity, but still most neurons (61%) had CDs within ±500 μsec. For both low-frequency sounds and envelopes, more neurons had CDs to ipsilateral than to contralateral delays (68 and 63%, respectively).
Best ITD
Because of the effects of CP and frequency tuning described by Equation 1, the best ITDs of peak- and trough-like neurons became separated (Fig. 8E,F). The peak-like neurons represented relatively small ITDs, whereas trough-like neurons represented larger ITDs. For both signal types, trough-like neurons were separated into two groups, one on either side of the peak-like neurons, because of differences in the sign of the CP. The range of best ITDs represented by envelopes was especially large because of the low frequencies involved (Fig. 8E,F, note change of scale of x-axis). There were more neurons with negative best ITDs associated with sounds in contralateral space (65% for low-frequency sounds and 75% for envelopes).
The distributions of best ITDs for the two types of signals were combined in Figure 8G to display the pattern and range of best ITD on a single axis. The range within the head width [Fig.8G, horizontal bar abovethe histogram, ±300 μsec (from Heffner and Masterton, 1980)] was strongly represented, primarily by neurons sensitive to low-frequency sounds but also by neurons sensitive to envelopes. The largest ITDs (>1 msec) were predominantly represented via envelope ITD sensitivity. Thus, neurons with different CPs and neurons sensitive to both types of signals combine to form a single representation that extends to very large values of ITD.
Comparison with other brain levels
In accord with previous studies of the cortex and other brain levels, the representation of best ITDs in the auditory cortex of the rabbit was primarily to ipsilateral delays associated with sounds in contralateral space. In Figure 9, we compare the bias for contralateral space of the best ITDs in the cortex with that of other brain levels, including the SOC, IC, and auditory thalamus. The bars show the proportion of neurons with best ITDs to ipsilateral delays (contralateral space) divided by the proportion of neurons with best ITDs to contralateral delays (ipsilateral space). Above the level of the SOC, the contralateral representation of ITDs predominated for both peak- and trough-like neurons (Fig. 9A,B, respectively) and for ITD sensitivity to both low-frequency sounds and envelopes (closed andopen bars, respectively). For peak-like neurons, there was also a large contralateral bias in the SOC. In contrast, for trough-like neurons the bias for contralateral space in the SOC was quite small for ITD sensitivity to low-frequency sounds, and there was an ipsilateral bias for sensitivity to envelopes. Thus, the bias for contralateral space in the best ITDs for trough-like neurons seen in the cortex seems to depend on a transformation that occurs between the SOC and IC.
DISCUSSION
Our main result is that the distribution of best ITDs was continuous across peak-, trough-, and intermediate-type neurons and across sensitivity to ITDs in low-frequency sounds and envelopes. Thus, Jeffress-type models that depend exclusively on delay lines and peak-type neurons to encode ITDs capture only a part of the ITD representation in the brain. In the following, we will first compare our results with those from previous studies, then discuss how subcortical processing may contribute to the cortical representation described, and end by considering the functional significance of the results.
Comparison with previous studies
Cortical studies of ITD sensitivity are remarkably few (Brugge et al., 1969; Brugge and Merzenich, 1973; Benson and Teas, 1976; Reale and Brugge, 1990), and none have considered ITDs in envelopes. The previous studies examined few neurons and few frequencies for each neuron. On the basis of responses to approximately three frequencies per neuron,Benson and Teas (1976) reported that most neurons did not show sufficient alignment of their delay curves to have a CD. Reale and Brugge (1990) recorded over a wider range of frequencies and reported that most cortical neurons showed linear changes in mean interaural phase with frequency according to the criterion defined by Yin and Kuwada (1983) and thus had a CD. In agreement with our results, they reported the presence of peak-, trough-, and intermediate-type responses.
Construction of a continuous representation of ITDs
Much evidence indicates that the initial site of ITD processing is in the SOC, including both the medial superior olive (MSO) and the lateral superior olive (LSO). However, there has been no coherent view of how the peak-like neurons most common in the MSO and the trough-like neurons most common in the LSO may be related. The continuum among response types reported here indicates that both nuclei work together to provide a single representation of auditory space. This view is supported by the physiology at higher levels and by the anatomy of the output pathways from the SOC. At levels above the SOC, peak- and trough-like responses occur in the same rather than in separate nuclei. Anatomically, the projections from the MSO and LSO to the IC overlap to a considerable extent (Henkel and Brunso-Bechtold, 1993; Grothe et al., 1994; Oliver et al., 1995; Kelly et al., 1998). Interactions among putative MSO and LSO inputs can be demonstrated in responses of IC neurons (Batra et al., 1993; McAlpine et al., 1998).
Above the level of the SOC, a contralateral representation of ITDs predominated for both peak- and trough-like neurons (Fig. 9). In the SOC, in contrast, the bias for contralateral space for trough-like neurons was small for low-frequency sounds, and for envelopes the bias was for ipsilateral space. This transformation between the SOC and higher levels is consistent with the principal excitatory projection of the LSO, which is crossed (Saint Marie et al., 1989; Glendenning et al., 1992; Oliver et al., 1995). That is, the crossed LSO projection appears to transform a representation of space in the SOC that is discontinuous between the MSO and LSO to one that is continuous and predominately contralateral at higher levels. This view is supported by unilateral lesions of the SOC, which yield bilateral deficits in sound localization, whereas unilateral lesions at higher levels cause predominantly contralateral deficits (Kavanagh and Kelly, 1992).
Other pathways also support the physiological results observed. The consistent bias for contralateral space across levels for peak-type neurons is presumably based on the predominantly ipsilateral projections starting from the MSO and continuing to the cortex. The LSO has a small ipsilateral excitatory projection, which should transmit trough-type responses with best ITDs primarily to ipsilateral space. Taken together, these projections can account for the distribution of best ITDs seen in the cortex, which consists of a central representation of peak-like neurons flanked on either side by trough-like neurons in which the contralateral representation predominates (Fig. 8E,F).
In addition to peak- and trough-type responses, the proposed continuum emphasizes the presence of intermediate-type responses. The SOC has been shown recently to contain many neurons with intermediate-type responses (Batra et al., 1997a,b). Such neurons could be derived from mismatches of frequency inputs to the two sides (Schroeder, 1977;Shamma et al., 1989; Bonham and Lewis, 1999) or via inhibition (Batra et al., 1997a; Grothe and Park, 1998). The distribution of CPs in the SOC is less flat than is that in the auditory cortex (Yin and Chan, 1990; Spitzer and Semple, 1995; Joris, 1996; Batra et al., 1997a), suggesting that the numbers of intermediate-type neurons increase at higher levels in the pathway. It has been shown that intermediate-type responses can be created by a convergence of peak- and trough-type inputs in the IC (McAlpine et al., 1998). Similarly, computations indicate that increasing the strength of ITD-tuned inhibition onto IC neurons can cause a shift from peak- to trough-type responses (Cai et al., 1998). Thus, mechanisms for creating the continuum observed in the cortex start at the most peripheral level and accumulate as ITD information ascends through the auditory pathways.
Functional significance of the proposed continuum
The continuum proposed assumes that the signal encoded is the best ITD, or peak of firing. In the following, we will first consider other possible interpretations. We will then consider the functional implications of the proposed continuum.
A contrasting view would be that while peak-type neurons encode the ITD by the peak of response, trough-type neurons use the trough. Such an organization would resemble on- and off-center cells in the visual system. However, if peak- and trough-type neurons were providing information that differed only in sign, there would be no need for a continuum between them. The large numbers of intermediate neurons at all levels make a simple dichotomy between peak- and trough-type neurons less credible.
Another difficulty is that if both peak- and trough-type neurons provide information about relatively small ITDs, then the distributions should be governed by the size of the head. However, distributions of CDs from all animals are similar, despite wide variation in head size (Palmer et al., 1990; Grothe and Park, 1998). Thus, there is little reason to assume that trough-type neurons signal the ITD of the trough simply because it occurs within the head width in most neurons.
It is also possible that neither the peaks nor the troughs are the salient response features, but that the slope between them is (Skottun, 1998; McAlpine et al., 1999). However, although the slopes are typically to smaller ITDs than are the peaks, there are still many neurons, especially those sensitive to envelopes, with slopes that lie well outside the head width, and there is a continuum between neurons with slopes at small and large ITDs.
In humans wearing headphones, the side with a leading ITD can be discerned for ITDs >10 times the head width (e.g., >10 msec) (Blodgett et al., 1956; Mossup and Culling, 1998). Experiments to probe the ability of humans to compensate for external delays with internal processing indicate that sensitivity to large ITDs cannot occur via the use of elements tuned only to short delays but that internal representations of delay up to at least several milliseconds must exist (van der Heijden and Trahiotis, 1998). If peak- and trough-type neurons were each signaling a similar range of ITDs, delay lines up to these extreme ITDs would seem necessary. Alternatively, the amplification of the ITD representation by variations in CP and frequency tuning can allow for sensitivity to very long ITDs without the need for comparably large delay lines.
In our scheme, large ITDs are principally encoded by trough-like neurons, which will be suppressed by ITDs that occur within the head width. This suggests that the signal that drives these neurons is not large ITDs per se but rather the absence of a small ITD, a condition that arises when sounds at the two ears have a low correlation. A relationship between ITD sensitivity and the correlation of signals at the ears has long been known (Sayers and Cherry, 1957; Blauert, 1978;Colburn and Durlach, 1978; Bernstein and Trahiotis, 1996). Highly correlated signals that have an ITD within the head width yield a fused and compact sound image with a strong perception of azimuthal location. As the correlation declines, the image broadens, and the localization strength diminishes (Jeffress et al., 1962; Blauert and Lindemann, 1986). Physiologically, neurons sensitive to ITDs are also sensitive to interaural correlation (Yin et al., 1987; Albeck and Konishi, 1995;Saberi et al., 1998). Extending the representation to large ITDs may therefore serve to create a continuous representation of the binaural correlation.
In a sound field, the interaural correlation is affected by many factors, including the size of the space, the number of sources, and the number and position of reflective surfaces. Because most sounds have dynamically changing envelopes, the interaural correlation is dynamically changing. Periods of low correlation may contain regularities related to the patterns of reverberation defined by the room acoustics. We therefore suggest that the large axis of ITD sensitivity may be useful not only for sound localization but also for gaining information about auditory space.
Footnotes
This study was supported by National Institutes of Health Grants DC03948 and DC01366 and by National Science Foundation Grant IBN9807872. We thank Lisa M. Fitzpatrick for technical assistance and Drs. Constantine Trahiotis and Leslie R. Bernstein for lively discussions of this work.
Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Douglas C. Fitzpatrick, Department of Otolaryngology, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, 610 Burnett-Womack Building, CB# 7070, Chapel Hill, NC 27510. E-mail: dcf@med.unc.edu.
Dr. Batra's present address: Department of Anatomy, University of Mississippi Medical Center, 2500 North State Street, Jackson, MS 39216.
REFERENCES
- 1.Albeck Y, Konishi M. Responses of neurons in the auditory pathway of the barn owl to partially correlated binaural signals. J Neurophysiol. 1995;74:1689–1700. doi: 10.1152/jn.1995.74.4.1689. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Batra R, Kuwada S, Stanford TR. Temporal coding of envelopes and their interaural delays in the inferior colliculus of the unanesthetized rabbit. J Neurophysiol. 1989;61:257–268. doi: 10.1152/jn.1989.61.2.257. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Batra R, Kuwada S, Stanford TS. High-frequency neurons in the inferior colliculus that are sensitive to interaural delays of amplitude-modulated tones: evidence for dual binaural influences. J Neurophysiol. 1993;70:64–80. doi: 10.1152/jn.1993.70.1.64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Batra R, Kuwada S, Fitzpatrick DC. Sensitivity to interaural temporal disparities of low and high frequency neurons in the superior olivary complex. I. Heterogeneity of responses. J Neurophysiol. 1997a;78:1222–1236. doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.78.3.1222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Batra R, Kuwada S, Fitzpatrick DC. Sensitivity to interaural temporal disparities of low and high frequency neurons in the superior olivary complex. II. Coincidence detection. J Neurophysiol. 1997b;78:1237–1247. doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.78.3.1237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Benson DA, Teas DC. Single unit study of binaural interactions in the auditory cortex of the chinchilla. Brain Res. 1976;103:313–338. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(76)90801-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Bernstein LR, Trahiotis C. The normalized correlation: accounting for binaural detection across center frequency. J Acoust Soc Am. 1996;100:3774–3784. doi: 10.1121/1.417237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Blauert J. Some consideration of binaural cross correlation analysis. Acustica. 1978;39:96–104. [Google Scholar]
- 9.Blauert J, Lindemann W. Spatial mapping of intracranial auditory events for various degrees of interaural coherence. J Acoust Soc Am. 1986;79:806–813. doi: 10.1121/1.393471. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Blodgett HC, Wilbanks WA, Jeffress LA. Effect of large interaural time differences upon the judgment of sidedness. J Acoust Soc Am. 1956;28:639–643. [Google Scholar]
- 11.Bonham BH, Lewis ER. Localization by interaural time difference (ITD): effects of interaural frequency mismatch. J Acoust Soc Am. 1999;106:281–290. doi: 10.1121/1.427056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Brugge JF, Merzenich MM. Responses of neurons in auditory cortex of the macaque monkey to monaural and binaural stimulation. J Neurophysiol. 1973;36:1138–1158. doi: 10.1152/jn.1973.36.6.1138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Brugge JF, Dubrovsky NA, Aitkin LM, Anderson DJ. Sensitivity of single neurons in auditory cortex of cat to binaural tonal stimulation: effects of varying interaural time and intensity. J Neurophysiol. 1969;32:1005–1024. doi: 10.1152/jn.1969.32.6.1005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Cai H, Carney LH, Colburn HS. A model for binaural response properties of inferior colliculus neurons. I. A model with interaural time difference-sensitive excitatory and inhibitory inputs. J Acoust Soc Am. 1998;103:475–493. doi: 10.1121/1.421100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Carr CE. Processing of temporal information in the brain. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1993;16:223–243. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.16.030193.001255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Colburn HS. Theory of binaural interaction based on auditory-nerve data. I. General strategy and preliminary results on interaural discrimination. J Acoust Soc Am. 1973;54:1458–1470. doi: 10.1121/1.1914445. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Colburn HS. Computational models of binaural processing. In: Hawkins H, McMullen T, editors. Auditory computation. Springer; New York: 1995. pp. 332–400. [Google Scholar]
- 18.Colburn SH, Durlach NI. Models of binaural interaction. In: Carterette EC, Friedman MP, editors. Hearing. Academic; New York: 1978. pp. 365–466. [Google Scholar]
- 19.Crow G, Langford TL, Moushegian G. Coding of interaural time differences by some high-frequency neurons of the inferior colliculus: responses to noise bands and two-tone complexes. Hear Res. 1980;3:147–153. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Glendenning KK, Baker BN, Hutson KA, Masterton RB. Acoustic chiasm V: inhibition and excitation in the ipsilateral and contralateral projections of LSO. J Comp Neurol. 1992;319:100–122. doi: 10.1002/cne.903190110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Grothe B, Park TJ. Sensitivity to interaural time differences in the medial superior olive of a small mammal, the Mexican free-tailed bat. J Neurosci. 1998;18:6608–6622. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-16-06608.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Grothe B, Schweizer H, Pollak GD, Schuller G, Rosemann C. Anatomy and projection patterns of the superior olivary complex in the Mexican free-tailed bat, Tadarida brasiliensis mexicana. J Comp Neurol. 1994;343:630–646. doi: 10.1002/cne.903430412. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Heffner H, Masterton B. Hearing in glires: domestic rabbit, cotton rat, feral house mouse, and kangaroo rat. J Acoust Soc Am. 1980;68:1584–1599. [Google Scholar]
- 24.Henkel CK, Brunso-Bechtold JK. Laterality of superior olive projections to the inferior colliculus in adult and developing ferret. J Comp Neurol. 1993;331:458–468. doi: 10.1002/cne.903310403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Jeffress LA. A place theory of sound localization. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1948;41:35–39. doi: 10.1037/h0061495. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Jeffress LA, Blodgett HC, Deatherage BH. Effect of interaural correlation on the precision of centering a noise. J Acoust Soc Am. 1962;32:1122–1123. [Google Scholar]
- 27.Joris PX. Envelope coding in the lateral superior olive. II. Characteristic delays and comparison with responses in the medial superior olive. J Neurophysiol. 1996;76:2137–2156. doi: 10.1152/jn.1996.76.4.2137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Joris PX, Yin TCT. Envelope coding in the lateral superior olive. I. Sensitivity to interaural time differences. J Neurophysiol. 1995;73:1043–1062. doi: 10.1152/jn.1995.73.3.1043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Joris PX, Smith PH, Yin TCT. Coincidence detection in the auditory system: 50 years after Jeffress. Neuron. 1998;21:1235–1238. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80643-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Kavanagh GL, Kelly JB. Midline and lateral field sound localization in the ferret (Mustela putorius): contribution of the superior olivary complex. J Neurophysiol. 1992;67:1643–1656. doi: 10.1152/jn.1992.67.6.1643. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Kelly JB, Liscum A, van Adel B, Ito M. Projections from the superior olive and lateral lemniscus to tonotopic regions of the rat's inferior colliculus. Hear Res. 1998;116:43–54. doi: 10.1016/s0378-5955(97)00195-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Kuwada S, Yin TCT, Wickesberg RE. Response of cat inferior colliculus neurons to binaural beat stimuli: possible mechanisms for sound localization. Science. 1980;206:586–588. doi: 10.1126/science.493964. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Kuwada S, Stanford TR, Batra R. Interaural phase sensitive units in the inferior colliculus of the unanesthetized rabbit. Effects of changing frequency. J Neurophysiol. 1987;57:1338–1360. doi: 10.1152/jn.1987.57.5.1338. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Kuwada S, Batra R, Stanford TR. Monaural and binaural response properties of neurons in the inferior colliculus of the rabbit: effects of sodium pentobarbital. J Neurophysiol. 1989;61:269–282. doi: 10.1152/jn.1989.61.2.269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Kuwada S, Batra R, Fitzpatrick DC. Neural processing of binaural temporal cues. In: Gilkey RH, Anderson TR, editors. Binaural and spatial hearing in real and virtual environments. Erlbaum; Mahwah, NJ: 1997. pp. 399–426. [Google Scholar]
- 36.Mardia KV. Statistics of directional data. Academic; New York: 1972. [Google Scholar]
- 37.McAlpine D, Jiang D, Shackleton TM, Palmer AR. Convergent input from brainstem coincidence detectors onto delay-sensitive neurons in the inferior colliculus. J Neurosci. 1998;18:6026–6039. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-15-06026.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.McAlpine D, Jiang D, Palmer AR. Neural coding of ITDs: mechanisms for enhanced acuity. Assoc Res Otolaryngol. 1999;21:180. [Google Scholar]
- 39.McMullen NT, Glaser EM. Tonotopic organization of rabbit auditory cortex. Exp Neurol. 1982;75:208–220. doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(82)90019-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Mossup JE, Culling JF. Lateralization of large interaural delays. J Acoust Soc Am. 1998;104:1574–1579. doi: 10.1121/1.424369. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Oliver DL, Beckius GE, Schneiderman A. Axonal projections from the lateral and medial superior olive to the inferior colliculus of the cat. A study using electron microscopic autoradiography. J Comp Neurol. 1995;360:17–32. doi: 10.1002/cne.903600103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Palmer AR, Rees A, Caird D. Interaural delay sensitivity to tones and broad band signals in the guinea-pig inferior colliculus. Hear Res. 1990;50:71–86. doi: 10.1016/0378-5955(90)90034-m. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Reale RA, Brugge JF. Auditory cortical neurons are sensitive to static and continuously changing interaural phase cues. J Neurophysiol. 1990;64:1247–1260. doi: 10.1152/jn.1990.64.4.1247. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Rhode WS. A digital system for auditory neurophysiological research. In: Brown P, editor. Current computer technology in neurobiology. Hemisphere; Washington, DC: 1976. pp. 543–567. [Google Scholar]
- 45.Rose JE, Gross NB, Geisler CD, Hind JE. Some neural mechanisms in the inferior colliculus of the cat which may be relevant to localization of a sound source. J Neurophysiol. 1966;29:288–314. doi: 10.1152/jn.1966.29.2.288. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Saberi K, Takahashi Y, Konishi M, Albeck Y, Arthur BJ, Farahbod H. Effects of interaural decorrelation on neural and behavioral detection of spatial cues. Neuron. 1998;21:789–798. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80595-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Saint Marie RL, Ostapoff E-M, Morest DK, Wenthold RJ. Glycine-immunoreactive projection of the cat lateral superior olive: possible role in midbrain ear dominance. J Comp Neurol. 1989;279:382–396. doi: 10.1002/cne.902790305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Sayers B, Cherry EC. Mechanism of binaural fusion in the hearing of speech. J Acoust Soc Am. 1957;29:973–986. [Google Scholar]
- 49.Schroeder MR. New viewpoints in binaural interactions. In: Evans EF, Wilson JP, editors. Psychophysics and physiology of hearing. Academic; New York: 1977. pp. 455–467. [Google Scholar]
- 50.Shackelton TM, Meddis R, Hewitt MJ. Across frequency integration in a model of lateralization. J Acoust Soc Am. 1992;91:2276–2279. [Google Scholar]
- 51.Shamma SA, Shen N, Gopalaswamy P. Sterausis: binaural processing without neural delays. J Acoust Soc Am. 1989;86:989–1006. doi: 10.1121/1.398734. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Skottun BC. Sound localization and neurons. Nature. 1998;393:531. doi: 10.1038/31134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Spitzer MW, Semple MN. Neurons sensitive to interaural phase disparity in gerbil superior olive: diverse monaural and temporal response properties. J Neurophysiol. 1995;73:1668–1690. doi: 10.1152/jn.1995.73.4.1668. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Stanford TR, Batra R, Kuwada SK. A comparison of the interaural time sensitivity of neurons in the inferior colliculus and thalamus of the unanesthetized rabbit. J Neurosci. 1992;12:3200–3216. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.12-08-03200.1992. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Stern RM, Colburn HS. Theory of binaural interaction based on auditory-nerve data. IV. A model for subjective lateral position. J Acoust Soc Am. 1978;64:127–140. doi: 10.1121/1.381978. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Stern RM, Trahiotis C. The role of consistency of interaural timing over frequency in binaural lateralization. In: Cazals Y, Demany L, Horner K, editors. Auditory physiology and perception. Pergamon; New York: 1992. pp. 547–554. [Google Scholar]
- 57.Stern RM, Trahiotis CT. Models of binaural perception. In: Gilkey RH, Anderson TR, editors. Binaural and spatial hearing in real and virtual environments. Erlbaum; Mahwah, NJ: 1997. pp. 499–531. [Google Scholar]
- 58.van der Heijden M, Trahiotis C. Binaural detection as a function of interaural correlation and bandwidth of masking noise: implications for estimates of spectral resolution. J Acoust Soc Am. 1998;103:1609–1614. doi: 10.1121/1.421295. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Yin TCT, Chan JCK. Interaural time sensitivity in medial superior olive of cat. J Neurophysiol. 1990;64:465–488. doi: 10.1152/jn.1990.64.2.465. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Yin TCT, Kuwada S. Binaural interaction in low-frequency neurons in inferior colliculus of the cat. III. Effects of changing frequency. J Neurophysiol. 1983;50:1020–1042. doi: 10.1152/jn.1983.50.4.1020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Yin TCT, Kuwada S, Sujaku Y. Interaural time sensitivity of high-frequency neurons in the inferior colliculus. J Acoust Soc Am. 1984;76:1401–1410. doi: 10.1121/1.391457. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Yin TCT, Chan JCK, Irvine DRF. Effects of interaural time delays of noise stimuli on low-frequency cells in the cat's inferior colliculus. I. Responses to wideband noise. J Neurophysiol. 1986;55:280–300. doi: 10.1152/jn.1986.55.2.280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Yin TCT, Chan JCK, Carney LH. Effects of interaural time delays of noise stimuli of low-frequency cells in the cat's inferior colliculus. III. Evidence for cross-correlation. J Neurophysiol. 1987;58:562–583. doi: 10.1152/jn.1987.58.3.562. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Yin TCT, Joris PX, Smith PH, Chan JCK. Neural processing for interaural temporal disparities. In: Gilkey RH, Anderson TR, editors. Binaural and spatial hearing in real and virtual environments. Erlbaum; Mahwah, NJ: 1997. pp. 427–445. [Google Scholar]