Skip to main content
The Journal of Neuroscience logoLink to The Journal of Neuroscience
. 2000 Nov 15;20(22):8533–8541. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-22-08533.2000

Spectral Integration in the Inferior Colliculus of the Mustached Bat

Scott A Leroy 1, Jeffrey J Wenstrup 1
PMCID: PMC6773173  PMID: 11069961

Abstract

Acoustic behaviors including orientation and social communication depend on neural integration of information across the sound spectrum. In many species, spectral integration is performed by combination-sensitive neurons, responding best when distinct spectral elements in sounds are combined. These are generally considered a feature of information processing in the auditory forebrain. In the mustached bat's inferior colliculus (IC), they are common in frequency representations associated with sonar signals but have not been reported elsewhere in this bat's IC or the IC of other species. We examined the presence of combination-sensitive neurons in frequency representations of the mustached bat's IC not associated with biosonar. Seventy-five single-unit responses were recorded with the best frequencies in 10–23 or 32–47 kHz bands. Twenty-six displayed single excitatory tuning curves in one band with no additional responsiveness to a second signal in another band. The remaining 49 responded to sounds in both 10–23 and 32–47 kHz bands, but response types varied. Sounds in the higher band were usually excitatory, whereas sounds in the lower band either facilitated or inhibited responses to the higher frequency signal. Interactions were usually strongest when the higher and lower frequency stimuli were presented simultaneously, but the strength of interactions varied. Over one-third of the neurons formed a distinct subset; they responded most sensitively to bandpass noise, and all were combination sensitive. We suggest that these combination-sensitive interactions are activated by elements of mustached bat social vocalizations. If so, neuronal integration characterizing analysis of social vocalizations in many species occurs in the IC.

Keywords: auditory pathways, bat, combination sensitive, complex sounds, frequency integration, inferior colliculus, mustached bat, spectral integration


Acoustically guided behavior requires analyses of spectrally and temporally complex signals. The auditory system first decomposes sounds into their spectral components at the cochlea and then transmits the results of these analyses through frequency-tuned neurons of the auditory nerve. Further information processing of complex sounds uses the reverse of this spectral analysis, involving neuronal integration across spectral elements in sounds. For complex vocal signals, such neuronal integration is characterized by temporally sensitive facilitatory or inhibitory interactions between responses to distinct spectral elements. Sometimes called combination sensitive, these responses have been described in a variety of vertebrates from frogs to birds to mammals and are thought to contribute to selective responses to complex vocalizations (Suga et al., 1978, 1983; Fuzessery and Feng, 1983;Schuller et al., 1991b; Margoliash and Fortune, 1992; Olsen, 1992; Rauschecker et al., 1995; Ohlemiller et al., 1996; Doupe, 1997). It has been further suggested that combination-sensitive neurons underlie the encoding of phonemic elements of speech sounds (Suga, 1996; Sussman et al., 1998). Neural interactions that create these response properties are generally thought to originate in the auditory forebrain (Olsen, 1992; Winer et al., 1995; Rauschecker, 1998).

In the auditory cortex of the mustached bat (Pteronotus parnellii), combination-sensitive responses occur commonly among neurons tuned to frequency ranges of the bat's sonar vocalizations (see Fig. 1A). Most display facilitated responses to combinations of elements in the sonar call and returning echoes that may extract information about target features such as distance and movement (O'Neill and Suga, 1979; Suga et al., 1983). Recently, it has become clear that such neurons are abundant in the mustached bat's inferior colliculus (IC) (Mittmann and Wenstrup, 1995; Yan and Suga, 1996; Portfors and Wenstrup, 1999a). Their presence in the auditory midbrain may be understandable in the context of sonar behavior, in which flight adjustments in response to target movement must be made within the 1–2 sec duration of an interception sequence. Projections of the IC to premotor centers could provide highly processed information useful for rapid changes in vocalization or flight (Schweizer, 1981; Frisina et al., 1989; Schuller et al., 1991a; Wenstrup et al., 1994; Casseday and Covey, 1996).

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

A, Schematic sound spectrogram of the mustached bat's biosonar call. The frequency bands labeled by thebrackets (10–23 and 32–47 kHz) are analyzed by neurons in the present report. B, C, Schematic illustration of the organization of the mustached bat's central nucleus of the inferior colliculus (ICC) in transverse sections.Numbers at the bottom right of the sections indicate fractions of the caudal-to-rostral dimension of the IC; the section in B is more caudal than the section inC. Numbers belowabbreviations indicate the frequency representation (in kiloHertz) of each ICC subdivision. In the present study, single units were recorded from the anterolateral division (ALD) of the ICC (blackened region). In ALD, tonotopic representation advances from ∼10 kHz at the rostral tip to frequencies up to 59 kHz more caudally (Zook et al., 1985; O'Neill et al., 1989). bic, Brachium of the inferior colliculus;CG, central gray; D, dorsal;DC, dorsal cortex of the inferior colliculus;DPD, dorsoposterior division or the ICC;Ex, external nucleus of the inferior colliculus;M, medial; MD, medial division of the ICC;SC, superior colliculus.

Is the presence of these sonar-related neurons in the IC an exception, or does similar spectral integration occur among neurons analyzing other types of complex acoustic signals? This report examines whether combination-sensitive responses occur in frequency representations of the mustached bat's IC outside those used in biosonar (see Fig.1A). We found that the majority of neurons responding to these frequency bands (10–23 and 32–47 kHz) displayed combination-sensitive responses. This indicates that the integration of information from distinct spectral elements in sounds is performed by a wide range of IC neurons and is not an exclusive property of neurons analyzing biosonar information. We suggest that many of these response properties are well suited for analyses of the mustached bat's social vocalizations (Kanwal et al., 1994). If so, neuronal integration characterizing analysis of social vocalizations in many species occurs in the auditory midbrain, not just in the auditory cortex as is commonly supposed.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Single-unit recordings were obtained from the IC in awake greater mustached bats (P. parnellii parnellii). Thirteen bats, captured in Jamaica, West Indies, provided the data used in this report. All procedures on the bats followed protocols approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the Northeastern Ohio Universities College of Medicine.

Surgical preparation. At least 1 d before surgery, bats were placed in a holding cage with tetracycline in their drinking water. For surgery, the bats were anesthetized with methoxyflurane (Metofane; Pitman-Moore, Inc., Mundelein, IL) in combination with sodium pentobarbital (5 mg/kg, i.p.; Nembutal; Abbott Labs, Irving, TX) and acepromazine (2 mg/kg, i.p.; Med-Tech, Inc., Buffalo, NY). A midline incision was made over the dorsal surface of the skull, and the skin and muscles were retracted laterally. A tungsten ground electrode was then cemented into the skull over the cerebral cortex on the side opposite to the exposed IC. A metal pin was glued to the skull to hold the head in the stereotaxic apparatus and ensure the proper position of the brain during experiments. After a topical antibiotic and local anesthetic (lidocaine; Elkins-Sinns, Cherry Hill, NJ) were applied to the wound, the bat was returned to the holding cage and allowed to recover for at least 1 d before physiological recording.

Acoustic stimulation and physiological recording. On the first day of recording, bats were anesthetized with methoxyflurane, and a small piece of the skull (<0.5 mm in diameter) was removed to expose the IC. During recording sessions, the animals were placed in a stereotaxic apparatus to orient the head in a standard position. The apparatus was housed in a heated and humidified sound-attenuating chamber, lined with polyurethane foam to reduce echoes. In addition, the surface of the stereotaxic apparatus was covered with cotton to reduce echoes. If a bat struggled or showed other signs of discomfort, it was returned to its holding cage. Between electrode penetrations, the bats were given water, and the exposed brain tissue was covered with petroleum jelly to keep it moist. Recording sessions generally lasted 4–6 hr.

Stimulus generation and data acquisition were computer-controlled. Two different tone or noise bursts (duration varied; 0.5 msec rise–fall time; 3–4/sec) were separately generated (WaveTek model 395), switched (Tucker-Davis Technologies model SW2), and attenuated (Tucker-Davis Technologies model PA4). The signals were combined (Tucker-Davis Technologies model SM3) and then sent to a power amplifier (Parasound model HCA-800II) and a speaker (Technics leaf tweeter) placed 10 cm away from the bat and 25° into the sound field contralateral to the recording electrode.

The acoustic system was calibrated several times over the course of the experiments. A calibrated microphone (Brüel and Kjaer model 4135) was placed in the position of the bat's head during experiments. There was a smooth, consistent decrease of 2.7 dB per 10 kHz from 10 to 120 kHz. The microphone output was digitized (National Instruments model NB-A2000), and a fast Fourier transform was computed. Distortion components were not detectable 60 dB below the peak signal level.

Single-unit activity was recorded with micropipette electrodes filled with 3 m KCl and having resistances of 8–15 MΩ. In many experiments, multiunit responses were also recorded to compare the single-unit responses with the surrounding population. Electrodes were positioned for recording in the IC by the use of surface landmarks and advanced with a hydraulic micropositioner (David Kopf Instruments model 650). Extracellular action potentials were amplified and then sent through a bandpass filter (500–6000 Hz) and a window discriminator (Frederick Haer and Company model 74-60-3). The output of the discriminator was then digitized at 10 kHz for analysis of spike times (National Instruments model NB-MIO-16X). The laboratory software generated histograms, raster displays, and statistics on neural responses within a 100 msec peristimulus window for 32 stimulus presentations. Usually, spike counts were based on the entire 100 msec window. The duration of the window was reduced to 50 or 60 msec if there was high spontaneous activity that obscured differences in response magnitude or for display purposes (e.g., see Figs. 3, 7). In no case did shortening the window duration eliminate an observable response. The output of the window discriminator was also sent to an oscilloscope and a speaker for audiovisual display and estimates of response thresholds (see below).

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3.

Combinatorial interactions of single units responsive to 10–23 and 32–47 kHz frequency bands.A–C, Responses of a facilitated combination-sensitive unit. A, Frequency tuning in response to single tones and combinations of tones. Dashed lines andunfilled circles indicate responses to single-tone bursts. Solid lines and filled circlesindicate tuning curves obtained in the presence of a second tone burst, the frequency and intensity of which are indicated by theX placed within the other tuning curve. EachX also indicates the frequencies and intensities of signals used in the tests shown in B andC. The unit did not respond to frequencies in the fundamental sonar harmonic (24–31 kHz) at any sound level tested.B, Peristimulus time histograms (PSTHs; 32 stimulus presentations) showing a facilitated response when signals were presented simultaneously. Signal timing and duration (3 msec) are indicated by the unfilled bars (higher frequency signals) and solid bars (lower frequency signals).C, Temporal sensitivity of facilitation. Facilitation only occurred when the two signals were presented near the 0 msec delay. Error bars indicate 95% confidence limits.Separate data points indicate responses of single tones at the indicated frequencies. D–F, Responses of an inhibited combination-sensitive unit. D, Frequency tuning in response to single tones and combinations of tones. Theblackened curve indicates tuning of an inhibitory response that suppresses the excitatory response to the higher frequency signal marked by the X. The X symbols also mark the frequencies and intensities of test signals used in the tests shown in E andF. E, PSTHs demonstrating moderate suppression of the higher frequency response by the lower frequency signal presented 4 msec before the higher frequency signal. The signal duration was 30 msec. F, Temporal sensitivity of inhibition.

Fig. 7.

Fig. 7.

Sharp temporal sensitivity of low-frequency inhibition. In contrast to the inhibitory unit in Figure3F, this single unit displayed strong inhibition with sharp temporal selectivity. Inhibition was eliminated when the relative timing of tone bursts was changed by 2 msec. See Figure 3 for an explanation of figure conventions andsymbols.

Neurons were stimulated with tone bursts or bandpass noise bursts (roll-offs exceeded 100 dB per octave). Signal duration was typically 30 msec but was changed for neurons that responded better to shorter (as short as 3 msec) or longer (up to 70 msec) stimuli. When a single unit was isolated, we obtained its best frequency, threshold, and tuning curve by monitoring the oscilloscope and audio monitor, i.e., audiovisually. We defined best frequency as the frequency requiring the lowest intensity to elicit stimulus-locked spikes and threshold as the lowest intensity required to elicit a consistent spike response. For units that were excited by sounds within two different frequency bands, we tuned the response in each frequency band and refer to a best high-frequency response and a best low-frequency response. For some neurons that responded poorly to tones, frequency tuning was tested with 5-kHz-wide noise bands.

By the use of a two-stimulus paradigm, single units were tested for sensitivity to combinations of tone or noise bursts as described previously (Portfors and Wenstrup, 1999a; Wenstrup, 1999). Sensitivity to delay between the low- and high-frequency signals was initially assessed audiovisually by varying the delay, usually in steps of 2 msec. If we noted a detectable change in response as a function of delay, quantitative data were collected at the delay that evoked the largest change in response. This was compared with the unit's response magnitude to the two stimuli presented separately. Quantitative data were then obtained as a function of delay. The range of delays tested included those in which the low-frequency signal was presented first and the high-frequency signal was delayed and also those in which the high-frequency signal was presented first and the low-frequency signal was delayed. The delay between the low- and high-frequency signals that elicited the greatest response (or the least response in the case of an inhibited combination-sensitive effect) was defined as the neuron's best delay.

We then tuned the facilitated or inhibited response. For facilitated neurons, we tuned both the low- and high-frequency sounds. To assess the low frequencies that elicited a facilitated response, the high-frequency signal was held constant, and the low frequencies eliciting a facilitated response (a detectable change in the response rate evaluated audiovisually) at various intensities were recorded. Then the low-frequency signal was held constant, and the responses to the high frequencies were tuned across intensities. For inhibited neurons, we determined the range of low frequencies that inhibited the high-frequency response while the high-frequency sound was held constant. For both facilitated and inhibited responses, the sound held constant was typically presented at 10 dB above threshold.

Single-unit responses were considered to be combination sensitive if they met three criteria: (1) the response to the two signals presented together was 20% more (for facilitation) or 20% less (for inhibition) than the sum of responses to the signals presented separately, (2) the facilitatory or inhibitory interactions were tuned to clearly distinct frequency bands (separated by a half octave or more), and (3) the facilitatory or inhibitory interaction displayed temporal sensitivity as revealed in delay tests. The degree of facilitation or inhibition was quantified as the index of interaction (I), according to the following formula:I = (Rc − Rl − Rh)/(Rc + Rl + Rh), where Rc, Rl, and Rh are the neuron's responses to the combination of the low- and high-frequency signals, the low-frequency signal alone, and the high-frequency signal alone, respectively. An interaction index value of 0.09 corresponds to 20% facilitation, the criterion for combination-sensitive facilitation. Negative numbers indicate combination-sensitive inhibition. Interaction index values of 1 and −1 indicate maximum facilitation and inhibition, respectively. These criteria are identical to those used in similar studies of sonar-related combination-sensitive neurons in the IC (Portfors and Wenstrup, 1999a) and medial geniculate body (MGB) (Wenstrup, 1999).

To test for combination sensitivity unrelated to the processing of sonar signals, we recorded from the 10–23 and 32–47 kHz representations of the IC. Among Jamaican mustached bats, the frequency range of the sonar fundamental emitted by resting bats is ∼24–31 kHz (see Fig. 1A), differing slightly among individuals (Kobler et al., 1985; Suga et al., 1987). In flight, sonar echoes are shifted upward in frequency as a function of the bat's flight speed toward the echo source. Mustached bats compensate for upward Doppler frequency shifts by lowering the frequency of emitted signals (Schnitzler, 1970; Keating et al., 1994). In the laboratory, flight velocities are <5 m/sec (Schnitzler, 1970; Lancaster et al., 1992). Although these bats may fly faster in natural habitats, speeds >10 m/sec are unlikely (Norberg, 1987). Even if mustached bats compensated completely for frequency shifts introduced by a flight velocity of 10 m/sec, the lower frequency of the emitted signal would extend down only to 22 kHz for the fundamental and 44 kHz for the second harmonic. This suggests that neural responses tuned <22 kHz and between 33 and 44 kHz are not involved in the analysis of sonar signals.

Histological procedures. In some electrode penetrations, a tracer was deposited to mark recording sites. Electrodes were filled with dextran conjugates (dextran–rhodamine or biotin dextran amine; Molecular Probes, Eugene, OR) in 0.9% NaCl, iontophoresed with pulsed current (+5 μA; 7 sec on and 7 sec off) for at least 6 min, or 1% Fluoro-Gold (Fluorochrome, Inc., Englewood, CO) in 0.9% NaCl, iontophoresed with pulsed current (+1 μA; 7 sec on and 7 sec off) for 5 min. In all but one animal, each marked penetration used a different tracer.

The animals were perfused within 12 d of deposits. After the bat was deeply anesthetized with Nembutal (75 mg/kg, i.p.) and reflexes were eliminated, the chest was opened, and the bat was perfused through the heart with phosphate buffer and 10% Formalin. The head was removed, blocked, and refrigerated overnight in 30% sucrose. The brain was sectioned with a freezing microtome, usually at 40 μm. The sections were collected in chilled 0.1 m phosphate buffer and rinsed in 0.05 m phosphate buffer before mounting on slides. At least one of the three series was stained with cresyl violet to reveal cytoarchitecture.

The majority of single units described in this report responded to sounds in both the 10–23 and 32–47 kHz bands. Because most units were not histologically localized, we used physiological and depth criteria to determine whether a unit was located within the 10–23 or 32–47 kHz frequency band representations of the tonotopically organized IC. This analysis is based on previous studies in mustached bats showing that frequencies in the ∼10–59 kHz range are represented in the anterolateral division of the IC (Zook et al., 1985; O'Neill et al., 1989). Here, anatomically defined fibrodendritic laminae (Zook et al., 1985) and physiologically defined frequency-band laminae (O'Neill et al., 1989) extend dorsoventrally and mediolaterally, their edges curving rostrally. There is an orderly progression from the highest frequencies located more caudally (59 kHz) to lower frequencies represented more rostrally. Our penetrations, which were angled 5–25° dorsocaudal to ventrorostral, are expected to encounter decreasing best frequencies with increasing depth, as have other studies using similar electrode orientations (Wenstrup et al., 1994;Wenstrup and Grose, 1995). All single units in the present study were recorded at depths of 125–1975 μm, consistent with IC recordings of 10–50 kHz responses in these previous studies using similar electrode angles.

To localize a single-unit response within the physiological tonotopic organization, we required the penetration to show a descending progression of best frequencies combined with the presence of singly tuned responses (single unit or multiunit) to 24–31 kHz, the frequency representation that intervenes between the 10–23 and 32–47 kHz bands. Thus, if a combinatorial unit responding to both the 10–23 and 32–47 kHz bands occurred more superficially in the penetration than did a 24–31 kHz response, we concluded that the combinatorial unit was in the 32–47 kHz representation. If it had occurred deeper, we would conclude that the combinatorial unit was in the 10–23 kHz representation. A similar analysis was performed for neurons responding to only one of the 10–23 or 32–47 kHz frequency bands. By the use of these criteria, 38 of 75 single-unit responses were localized within the physiological tonotopic organization of the IC.

RESULTS

The anterolateral division of the mustached bat's IC represents frequencies in the 10–59 kHz range (Fig.1B,C). This range includes frequency bands used in biosonar (24–31 kHz, first sonar harmonic; 48–59 kHz, second harmonic of the frequency-modulated sonar component) as well as frequency bands below and above the first sonar harmonic (10–23 and 32–47 kHz, respectively) (Fig.1A). This report describes single-unit recordings of 75 neurons tuned to signals in either the 10–23 or 32–47 kHz frequency bands; we examined tuning to multiple frequency bands and sensitivity to combinations of tonal or noise stimuli. Multiunit responses and single units tuned to other frequency bands were also examined to establish the location of the single-unit responses described here within the tonotopic map of the IC.

Of the 75 single units, 26 (35%) displayed a single excitatory tuning curve with their best frequencies in either the 10–23 kHz (n = 6) or 32–47 kHz (n = 20) frequency bands (Fig. 2). Furthermore, in the presence of a sound at their best excitatory frequency, these neurons showed no additional tuned responsiveness, either facilitatory or inhibitory, to sounds over a wide range of frequencies, even when tested over a range of delays between the two sounds. On the basis of our definition (see Materials and Methods), these neurons were not combination sensitive. In contrast, the majority of neurons in our sample (49 of 75, 65%) had excitatory responses tuned to one or both of the 10–23 or 32–47 kHz bands but in addition showed facilitatory or inhibitory interactions when stimuli in the two frequency bands were presented together (Fig. 2). Single units with facilitatory interactions (57%) were slightly more common than were those with inhibitory interactions (43%).

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2.

Categories of response to combinations of tone or noise bursts among single units tuned to 10–23 and 32–47 kHz frequency bands. Singly tuned neurons had an excitatory response tuned to only one frequency band. Facilitatoryneurons responded to the combination of two signals from the 10–23 and 32–47 kHz bands at a level higher than the sum of responses to the separate signals (see Materials and Methods). Inhibitoryneurons showed an excitatory response to one of these frequency bands that was inhibited by signals in the other band.Tone-responsive and Noise-responsiverefer to single units that displayed better responses (lower threshold or more spikes) to tone bursts or bandpass noise bursts, respectively.

Facilitatory and inhibitory combination-sensitive interactions

Responses of a single unit in Figure3A–C illustrate fundamental properties of combination-sensitive facilitation. In response to single tone burst stimuli, this unit displayed a sensitive excitatory tuning curve, centered at 40.4 kHz with the threshold at 19 dB sound pressure level (SPL). Its responses to signals in the 10–23 kHz band were weak and insensitive, with threshold responses near 20 kHz obtained only at levels exceeding 80 dB SPL (Fig. 3A, unfilled circles and dashed lines). However, the combination of high- and low-frequency stimuli revealed a low-threshold, facilitating input that was sharply tuned to 19.2 kHz (Fig. 3A,filled circles and solid lines). The drop in the threshold response to the lower frequency signal, when presented together with the facilitating higher frequency signal, was substantial, 61 dB for the neuron in Figure 3A. The facilitated response to the high-frequency signal, also sharply tuned, decreased in threshold but only by 10 dB. The unit did not respond to signals in the 24–31 kHz band, the frequency of the first sonar harmonic, in either single- or two-tone presentations. In addition to its effect on response threshold, the facilitation also increased response magnitude. The response to combined tone bursts was 123% greater than the sum of responses to the higher and lower frequency signals presented separately (Fig. 3B), corresponding to a facilitation index value of 0.38. This facilitation was also dependent on the relative timing of the two signals. Strong facilitation was only obtained when the higher frequency signal was presented simultaneously with or 2 msec after the lower frequency signal (Fig. 3C). The frequency- and time-dependent facilitation shown for the neuron in Figure 3A–C satisfies the criteria for combination sensitivity described in Materials and Methods.

For 21 units, the effect of signals in one of the 10–23 or 32–47 kHz frequency bands was to inhibit responses to signals in the other band (Fig. 2). The unit in Figure 3D–F had a sensitive excitatory tuning curve in the higher frequency band, tuned to 34.9 kHz (Fig. 3D). Excitatory responses to the 10–23 kHz band were tuned near 13 kHz but only obtained at intensities >70 dB SPL. The combination of low-intensity signals in both frequency bands revealed a low-frequency inhibitory effect, sharply tuned at 13.2 kHz with a low threshold, which reduced the excitatory response to the high-frequency stimulus. At an intensity 9 dB above the threshold for inhibition, the lower frequency signal suppressed the response to the higher frequency signal by 41%, with an index of inhibition of −0.37 (Fig.3E). As with facilitatory interactions, the inhibitory interactions were sensitive to the timing of the two signals. In the unit in Figure 3D–F, the inhibitory effect was maximal when the high-frequency signal followed the lower frequency signal by 4 msec (Fig. 3F). However, most inhibitory units showed the greatest suppression when the inhibitory stimulus was presented simultaneously with the excitatory stimulus (see Temporal sensitivity).

A substantial number of single units (29 of 75, 39%) responded well to bandpass noise (5–20 kHz). For these neurons, responses to tonal stimuli either alone or in combination were poor. Among 15 neurons for which tone and noise thresholds were compared, thresholds to tones were on average >38 dB higher than thresholds for noise stimuli, on the basis of decibel attenuation values. The average difference in the threshold was probably higher, because no tone threshold could be obtained in 11 of the 15 tested neurons. Responses to noise stimuli and combinations of noise stimuli could nonetheless be tuned in frequency. Figure 4 shows the tuning properties of two such neurons, obtained by presenting 5-kHz-wide noise bands at different center frequencies. As with combination-sensitive neurons that responded well to tones, these neurons showed either facilitatory (Fig. 4A) or inhibitory (Fig. 4B) effects of the lower frequency stimulus. For the facilitated neuron in Figure 4A, the facilitatory interaction reduced the threshold to the low-frequency stimulus by 46 dB, whereas the decrease in threshold for the high-frequency response was smaller (8 dB). A striking feature of noise-responsive neurons is that all displayed combination-sensitive interactions between signals in the 10–23 and 32–47 kHz bands; 18 units showed facilitatory interactions, and 11 units showed inhibitory interactions (Fig. 2).

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4.

Tuning curves for combination-sensitive single units that respond best to noise stimuli. Thresholds were obtained with 5-kHz-wide noise bands, indicated in the figure by thecircles (center frequency) and horizontal lines (bandwidth). Dashed lines andunfilled circles indicate responses to single noise bursts. Solid lines and filled circlesindicate tuning curves obtained in the presence of a second noise burst, the center frequency and intensity of which are indicated by theX placed within the other tuning curve. Theblackened curve shows tuning of an inhibitory response as described in Figure 3. A, Single unit facilitated by signals in the 10–23 and 32–47 kHz frequency bands. B, Single unit excited by signals in the higher frequency band but inhibited by signals in the lower frequency band. For both units, combination effects were documented when signals were presented simultaneously (0 msec delay). The units did not respond to tonal stimuli in the 10–50 kHz range at levels as high as 80 dB SPL, the highest tested.

Frequency sensitivity

Tuning curves in Figures 3 and 4 show that these neurons displayed two distinct frequency sensitivities that did not overlap, except possibly at high sound levels. Figure5A shows the best low and high frequencies for neurons responding to signals in both frequency bands. Low best frequencies ranged from 12 to 23 kHz, whereas high best frequencies ranged from 32 to 47 kHz. Only one neuron was tuned to exact harmonics, although several were tuned very close to a harmonic relationship. For the majority, the best high-frequency response was tuned more than an octave above the best low-frequency response. One implication of these results is that few of these combination-sensitive interactions would be activated by signals within the frequency bands of biosonar signals.

Fig. 5.

Fig. 5.

Aspects of combinatorial interactions among the recorded population of single units. A, Frequency tuning of combination-sensitive interactions. Tone indicates tone-responsive units, whereas Noise indicates units responding better to bandpass noise. The line plots an exact fundamental-second harmonic relationship. Most noise-sensitive units are not included because their best frequencies were not measured. These were stimulated with noise bursts including most of the 10–23 or 32–47 kHz bands. B, Strength of interaction, as measured by the interaction index. Only units with index values of 0.09 and greater (facilitation) or −0.11 or less (inhibition) are shown. C, Best delays among single units showing combination-sensitive facilitation (top) or inhibition (bottom).

In most neurons, the response to sounds in the higher frequency band was greater than that to sounds in the lower frequency band. At the sound levels tested, in which the lower frequency signal was usually more intense, 21 of 28 facilitated neurons responded more strongly to the higher frequency signal, 4 neurons responded to the lower and higher frequencies at the same magnitude, and only 3 facilitated neurons responded more strongly to the lower frequency signal. For inhibited neurons, 18 of 21 neurons were excited by the higher frequency signal and suppressed by the lower frequency signal. The greater excitatory responses to the higher frequency signal may be related to the probable location of these neurons in the ICC representation of the higher frequency band (see below).

Strength of interactions

The strength of combination-sensitive interactions, as expressed by facilitation or inhibition indexes, varied among the population (Fig. 5B). For 28 facilitatory neurons, the average facilitation index was 0.32 (SD, 0.24), corresponding to a facilitated response that was 95% greater than the sum of responses to the low- and high-frequency stimuli presented separately. Strengths of facilitation ranged from 20 to 1200% (facilitation index values of 0.09–0.98). For 21 inhibitory neurons, the average inhibitory index was −0.36 (SD, 0.21), corresponding to suppression of the excitatory response by 53%. Strengths of inhibition ranged from 20 to 97% (inhibitory index values of −0.11 to −0.94). However, inhibitory index values do not indicate the maximum effect of inhibition on these neurons, because the inhibiting stimulus was typically presented at 10 dB above the threshold for inhibition. Comparing the noise- and tone-responsive units, there was no significant difference in the mean facilitatory index (unpaired t test; df = 26;p > 0.5) or the mean inhibitory index (unpairedt test; df = 19; p > 0.5). However, all noise-responsive neurons showed combination sensitivity, whereas many tone-responsive units were singly tuned (Fig. 2).

Temporal sensitivity

The facilitatory interactions in responses to the two frequency bands showed clear selectivity for the timing of the two signals (Figs.3C, 6). Most facilitatory responses were best when the two signals were presented simultaneously, e.g., 0 msec delay (Fig. 5C). However, the sharpness of this temporal selectivity, or delay tuning, was variable. Figure 6 offers a particularly sharp contrast. The unit in Figure 6Aresponded well only when the two 50 msec, narrowband noise bursts were presented within 2 msec of each other. In contrast, the unit in Figure6B, likewise stimulated with relatively long-duration (30 msec) signals, responded well as long as some overlap occurred between the high- and low-frequency signals. Such results suggest a range of temporal integration properties across the population of facilitated units.

Fig. 6.

Fig. 6.

Differences in temporal sensitivity among facilitated combination-sensitive single units. A, Strongly facilitated neuron with best delay at 0 msec. Note the narrow temporal selectivity despite the use of long-duration signals (50 msec). This noise-responsive unit was stimulated with 10–23 and 35–45 kHz noise bands. B, Facilitated unit with very broad delay sensitivity. This noise-responsive unit was stimulated with 12–22 and 35–45 kHz noise bands. See Figure 3 for an explanation of figure conventions and symbols.

Inhibitory combination-sensitive interactions were also sensitive to timing (Figs. 3F, 7). Most inhibitory units showed the greatest suppression when the inhibitory stimulus was presented simultaneously with the excitatory stimulus (Fig. 5C). As with facilitated units, there was variability in the width of the inhibitory delay function. The unit in Figure 7showed very sharp temporal sensitivity of inhibition, whereas that in Figure 3F was broader.

Topographic features

Tracer deposits placed in some penetrations showed that both tone- and noise-responsive combination-sensitive neurons were located in the ICC (Fig. 8). For 38 of the 75 single units in our sample, there were sufficient data from other responses in the same penetration to determine the unit's location within the tonotopic organization of the IC (see Materials and Methods). To make this determination, we required the penetration to show a descending progression of best frequencies in the dorsocaudal to ventrorostral penetration, combined with the presence of singly tuned responses (single unit or multiunit) to 24–31 kHz, the frequency representation that intervenes between the 10–23 and 32–47 kHz bands (Fig. 8). For singly tuned neurons, 17 of 18 localized single units were in the tonotopic representation of 32–47 kHz (e.g., Fig.8A). For combination-sensitive responses, all 20 localized units were in the 32–47 kHz representation (e.g., Fig. 8). These results show that combination-sensitive responses are common in the 32–47 kHz representation. Because so few neurons were localized to the 10–23 kHz representation, it is unclear whether it too contains combination-sensitive response properties. All combination-sensitive neurons localized to the 32–47 kHz tonotopic representation in the IC had higher magnitudes of response to signals in that frequency band than to signals in the 10–23 kHz frequency band, at the sound intensities used to assess combinatorial properties.

Fig. 8.

Fig. 8.

Locations of tone-responsive and noise-responsive recording sites in the ALD of the ICC. All deposits of tracer shown here were made at single-unit recording sites indicated byfilled circles in this figure and had frequency responses indicated in bold numbers to theleft of the section. Most other responses illustrated in this figure were multiunit responses. For combination-sensitive responses, both the higher and lower best frequencies are reported. Thenumber in the bottom right corner of each section indicates the location of the section along the caudal-to-rostral dimension of the IC. A, In the more caudal section, neurons tuned to the 32–47 kHz band were tone responsive (T). B, In the more rostral section, neurons tuned to this frequency band were noise responsive (N) and combination sensitive.PAG, Periaqueductal gray.

DISCUSSION

This study describes integrative neurons in the IC of mustached bats responding to combinations of acoustic signals in two frequency bands, 10–23 and 32–47 kHz. Previous studies showed that combination-sensitive neurons are common in IC frequency representations analyzing biosonar vocalizations (Mittmann and Wenstrup, 1995; Yan and Suga, 1996; Portfors and Wenstrup, 1999a). The present results show that the type of frequency–time integration performed by combination-sensitive neurons is widespread throughout the mustached bat's IC and is not a feature unique to the analysis of biosonar signals. We suggest that some combination-sensitive response properties in the IC are well suited for analyses of social vocalizations.

Spectral integration in ascending auditory pathways

The spectral integration documented here differs from other forms of integration described previously in the auditory brainstem and midbrain. For instance, in comparison with auditory nerve fibers, some cochlear nucleus neurons display narrower frequency tuning, whereas others show broader frequency tuning, noise sensitivity, or facilitatory–inhibitory frequency interactions (Rhode and Greenberg, 1992a,b; Young et al., 1992; Jiang et al., 1996). In the IC, most neurons have frequency tuning curves that differ significantly in shape from those of auditory nerve fibers (Ehret and Moffat, 1985a; Ehret and Merzenich, 1988; Ramachandran et al., 1999). Among IC neurons, local application of antagonists to inhibitory neurotransmitters alters frequency tuning and selectivity for frequency sweeps (Vater et al., 1992; Yang et al., 1992; Fuzessery and Hall, 1996), suggesting that mechanisms of spectral integration act in the IC as well as the cochlear nucleus. These studies generally demonstrate frequency integration that involves either integration of excitatory and/or inhibitory inputs tuned to relatively closely related frequencies or broad integration across a continuous band of frequencies that supplies inhibition on either side of an excitatory tuning curve. One exception is the multipeaked tuning curve, which may reflect dual excitatory inputs. However, such tuning curves are quite rare in the IC (Ehret and Moffat, 1985a; Casseday and Covey, 1992).

In contrast, the integration documented here is specific for widely separated frequency bands, usually an octave or more, suggesting input from two neural populations tuned to widely separated frequencies. These combinatorial properties, characterized by excitation, facilitation, or inhibition activated by distinct frequency bands of sounds, have typically been described in areas of the auditory forebrain (cortex or thalamus) outside of tonotopically organized regions (Suga et al., 1978, 1983; Fuzessery and Feng, 1983; Margoliash and Fortune, 1992; Olsen, 1992; Rauschecker et al., 1995; Ohlemiller et al., 1996; Doupe, 1997). This has supported the view that higher-order processing of complex sounds performed by combinatorial neurons is characteristic of forebrain auditory centers, particularly nontonotopic regions (Olsen, 1992; Winer et al., 1995; Rauschecker, 1998).

Although spectral combinatorial responses may be more common or more highly organized in nontonotopic auditory cortical areas, they also occur in tonotopically organized auditory cortex. In mustached bats, combination-sensitive responses are common in primary auditory cortex (Fitzpatrick et al., 1993; Kanwal et al., 1999). Increasingly, similar spectral combinatorial properties involving facilitation or inhibition have been described in primary auditory cortices of cats and primates. These properties include multipeaked excitatory tuning curves (Sutter and Schreiner, 1991), multiple inhibitory tuning curves (Sutter et al., 1999), and time-dependant facilitatory and inhibitory interactions between different spectral elements (Brosch et al., 1999; Kadia et al., 2000). Viewed across species, the data suggest that many neurons in the primary auditory cortex use facilitatory and/or inhibitory interactions between distinct spectral sensitivities to analyze complex acoustic signals.

Do spectral combinatorial properties originate in the primary auditory cortex? In most species, observations of cortical combinatorial responses are not matched by similar observations in the auditory midbrain or thalamus. However, these latter regions have not been systematically examined using unanesthetized or lightly anesthetized preparations, test paradigms, or natural stimuli that may be related to recent observations of combinatorial properties in the primary auditory cortex. Application of these methods may reveal additional spectral combinatorial responses. For example, previous studies of cat and monkey MGB found that the complexity of frequency tuning curves and frequency organization increased when obtained from unanesthetized or lightly anesthetized animals (Allon et al., 1981; Morel et al., 1987). Recent studies of the ventral division of the MGB support the presence of complex inhibitory frequency tuning (Imig et al., 1997).

In the mustached bat, previous studies and the current results show that combinatorial response properties are created within the tonotopically organized ascending pathway below the auditory forebrain. In IC representations of frequencies within higher harmonics of the bat's biosonar signal, ∼75% of neurons display combination-sensitive response properties (Portfors and Wenstrup, 1999a). There are many similarities in combination-sensitive responses between those populations and the neurons described here. All are characterized by neural interactions activated by sound in two distinct frequency bands. These interactions are sensitive to the relative timing of two signals in the range of milliseconds or tens of milliseconds. Finally, each of the IC populations of combination-sensitive neurons displays facilitatory and inhibitory interactions between two frequency inputs, with a range in the strength of interactions.

Most combination-sensitive responses in the mustached bat appear to originate in one nucleus, the ICC. Thus, patterns of inputs to combination-sensitive IC neurons from the cochlear and lateral lemniscal nuclei, the lack of combination-sensitive responses in the lateral lemniscal nuclei, and the ability of strychnine to eliminate combination-sensitive facilitation in IC neurons all favor the hypothesis that most sonar-related combination-sensitive properties are constructed in the ICC (Leroy and Wenstrup, 1999; Portfors and Wenstrup, 1999b; Wenstrup et al., 1999, 2000). On the basis of the similarities among combination-sensitive response properties, we hypothesize that nonsonar neurons also originate in the ICC and depend on similar neuronal mechanisms.

Despite physiological and possible mechanistic similarities with sonar-related combination-sensitive neurons, we believe the present results have broader significance. Although sonar-related combination-sensitive neurons may function in other behavioral contexts such as social communication (Ohlemiller et al., 1996; Esser et al., 1997), their functional roles are closely linked to analyses of information about sonar objects, e.g., distance and movement (O'Neill and Suga, 1982; Suga et al., 1983; Olsen and Suga, 1991a,b). Their presence in the IC might be regarded as a specialized neural feature unique to a highly specialized behavior. The combination-sensitive response properties described here do not function in biosonar behavior. We conclude that the presence of combination-sensitive properties in the IC is not a sonar-related specialization. Instead, such response properties may be a general feature of analyses of complex sounds in the IC.

Possible roles of combination-sensitive response properties

The nonsonar neurons described here do not analyze sonar echoes but may instead analyze other complex acoustic signals of interest to mustached bats. These signals include social vocalizations of mustached bats (Kanwal et al., 1994), sonar and social vocalizations of other bat species (Goodwin, 1970), clicks produced by moths to deter bat predation (Dunning and Roeder, 1965; Goldman and Henson, 1977), and other animal-generated sounds. The complex spectral responses of nonsonar neurons could participate in both identification and localization of these signals. However, this discussion focuses on their potential role in discriminating among the mustached bat's social vocalizations because there is a striking correspondence between the dual frequency sensitivities of the neurons and spectral peaks occurring in several mustached bat social vocalizations [Kanwal et al. (1994), their Fig. 13].

We predict that spectral and temporal response properties of nonsonar combination-sensitive neurons should provide selectivity among the mustached bat's repertoire of social vocalizations. In this regard, the most obvious response property is dual frequency selectivity, which will restrict strong responses to those calls with significant energy in the 10–23 and 32–47 kHz bands. Additional selectivity could result from the harmonic relationship between lower and higher best frequencies of nonsonar neurons. For example, in some neurons the two best frequencies are tuned near a fundamental-second harmonic (1:2) relationship, so these neurons would respond well to constant frequency (CF) signals with their fundamental at the neuron's lower best frequency. Other neurons, not tuned in a 1:2 harmonic relationship, should respond better to social vocalizations that have low-frequency fundamentals with many harmonics [e.g., short, quasi CF signals (Kanwal et al., 1994)] or have broad bands. Finally, a major source of variation among social vocalizations of the same category is a call's fundamental frequency (Kanwal et al., 1994). A population of such neurons, tuned to different frequency combinations, could distinguish among different versions of the same call, possibly emitted by different individuals.

A distinctive feature of many nonsonar combination-sensitive neurons is their requirement for bandpass noise in each of the 10–23 and 32–47 kHz bands. These neurons may respond preferentially to a class of mustached bat vocalizations containing noise bursts (Kanwal et al., 1994). One such call, a rectangular broadband noise burst used in agonistic interactions among individual mustached bats (Gupta et al., 1998), should effectively stimulate noise-sensitive facilitated neurons. These communication signals may elicit fixed behavioral responses and thus on theoretical grounds may be most appropriate for processing within the IC (Casseday and Covey, 1996).

The above considerations suggest that nonsonar neurons may show selectivity among social vocalizations, something that has been examined rarely in the IC (Ehret and Moffat, 1985b; Aitkin et al., 1994). Direct studies of responses of nonsonar, combination-sensitive neurons to the mustached bat's repertoire of social vocalizations will be required to test this prediction.

Footnotes

This work was supported by the National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders, National Institutes of Health Grant 5 R01 DC 00937. We thank C. V. Portfors for helpful comments on this manuscript, C. D. Grose for technical assistance, F.-M. Chen for software, and the Natural Resources Conservation Authority of Jamaica for permission to collect bats.

Correspondence should be addressed to Dr. Jeffrey J. Wenstrup, Department of Neurobiology and Pharmacology, Northeastern OH Universities College of Medicine, 4209 State Route 44, Rootstown, OH 44272-0095. E-mail: jjw@neoucom.edu.

Dr. Leroy's present address: Abbott Laboratories, Building AP32-LL, 200 Abbott Park Road, Abbott Park, IL 60064.

REFERENCES

  • 1.Aitkin LM, Tran L, Syka J. The responses of neurons in subdivisions of the inferior colliculus of cats to tonal, noise and vocal stimuli. Exp Brain Res. 1994;98:53–64. doi: 10.1007/BF00229109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Allon N, Yeshurun Y, Wollberg Z. Responses of single cells in the medial geniculate body of awake squirrel monkeys. Exp Brain Res. 1981;41:222–232. doi: 10.1007/BF00238879. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Brosch M, Schulz A, Scheich H. Processing of sound sequences in macaque auditory cortex: response enhancement. J Neurophysiol. 1999;82:1542–1559. doi: 10.1152/jn.1999.82.3.1542. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Casseday JH, Covey E. Frequency tuning properties of neurons in the inferior colliculus of an FM bat. J Comp Neurol. 1992;319:34–50. doi: 10.1002/cne.903190106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Casseday JH, Covey E. A neuroethological theory of the operation of the inferior colliculus. Brain Behav Evol. 1996;47:311–336. doi: 10.1159/000113249. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Doupe AJ. Song- and order-selective neurons in the songbird anterior forebrain and their emergence during vocal development. J Neurosci. 1997;17:1147–1167. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-03-01147.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Dunning DC, Roeder KD. Moth sounds and the insect-catching behavior of bats. Science. 1965;147:173–174. doi: 10.1126/science.147.3654.173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Ehret G, Merzenich MM. Complex sound analysis (frequency resolution, filtering and spectral integration) by single units of the inferior colliculus of the cat. Brain Res. 1988;472:139–163. doi: 10.1016/0165-0173(88)90018-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Ehret G, Moffat AJM. Inferior colliculus of the house mouse. II. Single unit responses to tones, noise and tone-noise combinations as a function of sound intensity. J Comp Physiol [A] 1985a;156:619–635. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Ehret G, Moffat AJM. Inferior colliculus of the house mouse. III. Response probabilities and thresholds of single units to synthesized mouse calls compared to tone and noise bursts. J Comp Physiol [A] 1985b;156:637–644. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Esser KH, Condon CJ, Suga N, Kanwal JS. Syntax processing by auditory cortical neurons in the FM-FM area of the mustached bat Pteronotus parnellii. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1997;94:14019–14024. doi: 10.1073/pnas.94.25.14019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Fitzpatrick DC, Kanwal JS, Butman JA, Suga N. Combination-sensitive neurons in the primary auditory cortex of the mustached bat. J Neurosci. 1993;13:931–940. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-03-00931.1993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Frisina RD, O'Neill WE, Zettel ML. Functional organization of mustached bat inferior colliculus. II. Connections of the FM2 region. J Comp Neurol. 1989;284:85–107. doi: 10.1002/cne.902840107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Fuzessery ZM, Feng AS. Mating call selectivity in the thalamus and midbrain of the leopard frog (Rana p. pipiens): single and multiunit analyses. J Comp Physiol [A] 1983;150:333–344. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Fuzessery ZM, Hall JC. Role of GABA in shaping frequency tuning and creating FM sweep selectivity in the inferior colliculus. J Neurophysiol. 1996;76:1059–1073. doi: 10.1152/jn.1996.76.2.1059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Goldman LJ, Henson OW., Jr Prey recognition and selection by the constant frequency bat, Pteronotus p. parnellii. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1977;2:411–419. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Goodwin RE. The ecology of Jamaican bats. J Mammal. 1970;51:571–579. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Gupta P, Dietz N, Kanwal JS. Vocal communication and stereotypic behavior patterns in the mustached bat, Pteronotus parnellii. Assoc Res Otolaryngol Abstr. 1998;21:141. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Imig TJ, Poirier P, Irons WA, Samson FK. Monaural spectral contrast mechanism for neural sensitivity to sound direction in the medial geniculate body of the cat. J Neurophysiol. 1997;78:2754–2771. doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.78.5.2754. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Jiang D, Palmer AR, Winter IM. Frequency extent of two-tone facilitation in onset units in the ventral cochlear nucleus. J Neurophysiol. 1996;75:380–395. doi: 10.1152/jn.1996.75.1.380. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Kadia S, Snider R, Wang X. Influence of stimulus components placed outside classical receptive field reveals harmonic structure of the auditory system. Assoc Res Otolaryngol Abstr. 2000;23:54. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Kanwal JS, Matsumura S, Ohlemiller K, Suga N. Analysis of acoustic elements and syntax in communication sounds emitted by mustached bats. J Acoust Soc Am. 1994;96:1229–1254. doi: 10.1121/1.410273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Kanwal JS, Fitzpatrick DC, Suga N. Facilitatory and inhibitory frequency tuning of combination-sensitive neurons in the primary auditory cortex of mustached bats. J Neurophysiol. 1999;82:2327–2345. doi: 10.1152/jn.1999.82.5.2327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Keating AW, Henson OW, Jr, Henson MM, Lancaster WC, Xie DH. Doppler-shift compensation by the mustached bat: quantitative data. J Exp Biol. 1994;188:115–129. doi: 10.1242/jeb.188.1.115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Kobler JB, Wilson BS, Henson OW, Jr, Bishop AL. Echo intensity compensation by echolocating bats. Hear Res. 1985;20:99–108. doi: 10.1016/0378-5955(85)90161-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Lancaster WC, Keating AW, Henson OW., Jr Ultrasonic vocalizations of flying bats monitored by radiotelemetry. J Exp Biol. 1992;173:43–58. doi: 10.1242/jeb.173.1.43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Leroy SA, Wenstrup JJ. Role of inhibitory neurotransmitters in responses of combination-sensitive neurons of the mustached bat's inferior colliculus. Assoc Res Otolaryngol Abstr. 1999;22:219. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Margoliash D, Fortune ES. Temporal and harmonic combination-sensitive neurons in the zebra finch's HVc. J Neurosci. 1992;12:4309–4326. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.12-11-04309.1992. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Mittmann DH, Wenstrup JJ. Combination-sensitive neurons in the inferior colliculus. Hear Res. 1995;90:185–191. doi: 10.1016/0378-5955(95)00164-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Morel A, Rouiller E, de Ribaupierre Y, de Ribaupierre F. Tonotopic organization in the medial geniculate body (MGB) of lightly anesthetized cats. Exp Brain Res. 1987;69:24–42. doi: 10.1007/BF00247026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Norberg UM. Wing form and flight mode in bats. In: Fenton MB, Racey P, Rayner JMV, editors. Recent advances in the study of bats. Cambridge UP; Cambridge, UK: 1987. pp. 43–56. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Ohlemiller KK, Kanwal JS, Suga N. Facilitative responses to species-specific calls in cortical FM-FM neurons of the mustached bat. NeuroReport. 1996;7:1749–1755. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199607290-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Olsen JF. High-order auditory filters. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1992;2:489–497. doi: 10.1016/0959-4388(92)90185-n. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Olsen JF, Suga N. Combination-sensitive neurons in the medial geniculate body of the mustached bat: encoding of relative velocity information. J Neurophysiol. 1991a;65:1254–1274. doi: 10.1152/jn.1991.65.6.1254. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Olsen JF, Suga N. Combination-sensitive neurons in the medial geniculate body of the mustached bat: encoding of target range information. J Neurophysiol. 1991b;65:1275–1296. doi: 10.1152/jn.1991.65.6.1275. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.O'Neill WE, Suga N. Target range-sensitive neurons in the auditory cortex of the mustache bat. Science. 1979;203:69–73. doi: 10.1126/science.758681. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.O'Neill WE, Suga N. Encoding of target range and its representation in the auditory cortex of the mustached bat. J Neurosci. 1982;2:17–31. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.02-01-00017.1982. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.O'Neill WE, Frisina RD, Gooler DM. Functional organization of mustached bat inferior colliculus. I. Representation of FM frequency bands important for target ranging revealed by 14C-2-deoxyglucose autoradiography and single unit mapping. J Comp Neurol. 1989;284:60–84. doi: 10.1002/cne.902840106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Portfors CV, Wenstrup JJ. Delay-tuned neurons in the inferior colliculus of the mustached bat: implications for analyses of target distance. J Neurophysiol. 1999a;82:1326–1338. doi: 10.1152/jn.1999.82.3.1326. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Portfors CV, Wenstrup JJ. Origin of combination-sensitive neurons in the mustached bat: evidence from the nuclei of the lateral lemniscus. Soc Neurosci Abstr. 1999b;25:396. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Ramachandran R, Davis KA, May BJ. Single-unit responses in the inferior colliculus of decerebrate cats. I. Classification based on frequency response maps. J Neurophysiol. 1999;82:152–163. doi: 10.1152/jn.1999.82.1.152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Rauschecker JP. Cortical processing of complex sounds. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1998;8:516–521. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(98)80040-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Rauschecker JP, Tian B, Hauser M. Processing of complex sounds in the macaque nonprimary auditory cortex. Science. 1995;268:111–114. doi: 10.1126/science.7701330. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Rhode WS, Greenberg S. Physiology of the cochlear nuclei. In: Popper AN, Fay RR, editors. The mammalian auditory pathway: neurophysiology. Springer; New York: 1992a. pp. 94–152. [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Rhode WS, Greenberg S. Lateral suppression and inhibition in the cochlear nucleus of the cat. J Neurophysiol. 1992b;71:493–514. doi: 10.1152/jn.1994.71.2.493. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Schnitzler H-U. Echoortung bei der Fledermaus Chilonycteris rubiginosa. Z Vergl Physiolog. 1970;68:25–38. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Schuller G, Covey E, Casseday JH. Auditory pontine gray: connections and response properties in the horseshoe bat. Eur J Neurosci. 1991a;3:648–662. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1991.tb00851.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Schuller G, O'Neill WE, Radtke-Schuller S. Facilitation and delay sensitivity of auditory cortex neurons in CF-FM bats, Rhinolophus rouxi and Pteronotus p. parnellii. Eur J Neurosci. 1991b;3:1165–1181. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1991.tb00051.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Schweizer H. The connections of the inferior colliculus and the organization of the brainstem auditory system in the greater horseshoe bat (Rhinolophus ferrumequinum). J Comp Neurol. 1981;201:25–49. doi: 10.1002/cne.902010104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Suga N. Basic acoustic patterns and neural mechanisms shared by humans and animals for auditory perception: a neuroethologists's view. In: Ainsworth A, Greenberg S, editors. Auditory basis of speech perception. Keele University; Staffordshire, UK: 1996. pp. 31–38. [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Suga N, O'Neill WE, Manabe T. Cortical neurons sensitive to combinations of information-bearing elements of biosonar signals in the mustache bat. Science. 1978;200:778–781. doi: 10.1126/science.644320. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Suga N, O'Neill WE, Kujirai K, Manabe T. Specificity of combination-sensitive neurons for processing of complex biosonar signals in auditory cortex of the mustached bat. J Neurophysiol. 1983;49:1573–1626. doi: 10.1152/jn.1983.49.6.1573. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Suga N, Niwa H, Taniguchi I, Margoliash D. The personalized auditory cortex of the mustached bat: adaptation for echolocation. J Neurophysiol. 1987;58:643–654. doi: 10.1152/jn.1987.58.4.643. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Sussman HM, Frutcher D, Hilbert J, Sirosh J. Linear correlates in the speech signal: the orderly output constraint. Behav Brain Sci. 1998;21:241–299. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x98001174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Sutter ML, Schreiner CE. Physiology and topography of neurons with multipeaked tuning curves in cat primary auditory cortex. J Neurophysiol. 1991;65:1207–1226. doi: 10.1152/jn.1991.65.5.1207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Sutter ML, Schreiner CE, McLean M, O'Connor KN, Loftus WC. Organization of inhibitory frequency receptive fields in cat primary auditory cortex. J Neurophysiol. 1999;82:2358–2371. doi: 10.1152/jn.1999.82.5.2358. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Vater M, Habbicht H, Kossl M, Grothe B. The functional role of GABA and glycine in monaural and binaural processing in the inferior colliculus of horseshoe bats. J Comp Physiol [A] 1992;171:541–553. doi: 10.1007/BF00194587. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Wenstrup JJ. Frequency organization and responses to complex sounds in the medial geniculate body of the mustached bat. J Neurophysiol. 1999;82:2528–2544. doi: 10.1152/jn.1999.82.5.2528. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Wenstrup JJ, Grose CD. Inputs to combination-sensitive neurons in the medial geniculate body of the mustached bat: the missing fundamental. J Neurosci. 1995;15:4693–4711. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-06-04693.1995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Wenstrup JJ, Larue DT, Winer JA. Projections of physiologically defined subdivisions of the inferior colliculus in the mustached bat: targets in the medial geniculate body and extrathalamic nuclei. J Comp Neurol. 1994;346:207–236. doi: 10.1002/cne.903460204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Wenstrup JJ, Mittmann DH, Grose CD. Inputs to combination-sensitive neurons in the inferior colliculus. J Comp Neurol. 1999;409:509–528. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1096-9861(19990712)409:4<509::aid-cne1>3.0.co;2-s. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62. Wenstrup JJ, Leroy SA, Portfors C, Grose CD. Neural mechanisms underlying the analysis of target distance. Echolocation in bats and dolphins Thomas J, Moss C, Vater M. 2000. University of Chicago; Chicago, in press. [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Winer JA, Larue DT, Pollak GD. GABA and glycine in the central auditory system of the mustache bat: structural substrates for inhibitory neuronal organization. J Comp Neurol. 1995;355:317–353. doi: 10.1002/cne.903550302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Yan J, Suga N. The midbrain creates and the thalamus sharpens echo-delay tuning for the cortical representation of target-distance information in the mustached bat. Hear Res. 1996;93:102–110. doi: 10.1016/0378-5955(95)00209-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Yang L, Pollak GD, Resler C. GABAergic circuits sharpen tuning curves and modify response properties in the mustache bat inferior colliculus. J Neurophysiol. 1992;68:1760–1774. doi: 10.1152/jn.1992.68.5.1760. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Young ED, Spirou GA, Rice JJ, Voigt HF. Neural organization and responses to complex stimuli in the dorsal cochlear nucleus. Philos Trans R Soc Lond [Biol] 1992;336:407–413. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1992.0076. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Zook JM, Winer JA, Pollak GD, Bodenhamer RD. Topology of the central nucleus of the mustache bat's inferior colliculus: correlation of single unit response properties and neuronal architecture. J Comp Neurol. 1985;231:530–546. doi: 10.1002/cne.902310410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from The Journal of Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Society for Neuroscience

RESOURCES