Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2020 Oct 1.
Published in final edited form as: Am Psychol. 2019 Jun 20;74(7):764–777. doi: 10.1037/amp0000481

Scientific Imperatives Vis-à-vis Growing Inequality in America

Julie A Kirsch 1, Gayle D Love 1, Barry T Radler 1, Carol D Ryff 1
PMCID: PMC6776687  NIHMSID: NIHMS1042590  PMID: 31219260

Abstract

A landmark paper published in the American Psychologist (Adler et al., 1994) encouraged psychologists to engage in research on socioeconomic inequality and health. Numerous contributions followed to fill in psychosocial and behavioral pathways. Specifically, we review advances on health inequalities research from a large public-use study (Midlife in the U.S., MIDUS). The Great Recession of 2007-2009 and its lingering effects are then reviewed to underscore widening inequality in access to education, employment, and income. Two MIDUS national samples of same-aged adults recruited two decades apart are then compared to assess historical changes in socioeconomic, physical health, and well-being profiles from the 1990’s to post-Recession. Despite historical gains in educational attainment over time, we show that indicators of SES, health, and well-being are more compromised in the post-recession sample relative to the 1990’s sample. Building on these preliminary findings, we elaborate opportunities for further inquiry by the scientific community to examine whether widening socioeconomic inequalities exacerbated by the Great Recession translate to widening health inequalities.

Keywords: socioeconomic status, health inequalities, psychosocial factors, Great Recession


Over two decades ago, an article in the American Psychologist (Adler, Boyce, Chesney et al., 1994) advised psychologists to contribute to research on socioeconomic inequality and health. The authors described the socioeconomic gradient in health, which showed that those with lower standing in socioeconomic status (SES) hierarchies tended to have poorer health (mental and physical) as well as reduced length of life. These SES disparities in health are now commonly referred to as health inequalities (Marmot, 2015). In addition to describing SES and health relationships, Adler et al. called for psychologists to explicate mechanisms such as psychosocial and behavioral processes. Health behaviors (smoking, physical activity) and psychological characteristics (psychological stress, negative emotions) as well as physiological processes were proposed as promising avenues to advance the science of health inequalities. The article became a highly cited classic in research on health inequalities.

Psychologists then began to fill in psychosocial and behavioral pathways that link SES to diverse health outcomes. One pathway pertained to stress and related links to psychological distress (Matthews & Gallo, 2011). Socioeconomically disadvantaged were found to experience more negative events and chronic strains (Hatch & Dohrenwend, 2007) as well as greater emotional distress (Businelle, Mills, Chartier et al., 2014). Another pathway pertained to the presence or absence of social and psychological resources (Adler, 2009). Less social integration partially explained the relationship between low education and excess stroke risk (Avendano, Kawachi, Van Lenthe et al., 2006), while low sense of control accounted for a proportion of the relationship between SES and cardiovascular disease (Bosma et al., 2005). Nonetheless, psychosocial resources can offset the stresses of socioeconomic disadvantage (Chen, Miller, Lachman et al., 2012; Donnellan, Conger, McAdams & Neppl, 2009).

To this literature, we bring four key objectives. First, we illustrate select findings on health inequalities from a public-use national longitudinal study, known as MIDUS (Midlife in the U.S.) that has attracted researchers seeking to explicate psychosocial, behavioral, and biological factors that mediate or moderate links between SES and health, thereby responding to recommendations of Adler et al. (1994). Second, we bring into high relief the changing historical context within which health inequalities are studied, drawing on recent evidence from economists showing that socioeconomic inequality in America is getting worse over time, in part due to lingering effects of the Great Recession disproportionately borne by SES disadvantaged. This changing societal context calls for new science that compares representative samples of Americans situated at different points in historical time. Third, we provide a window on this changing historical stage by comparing two cross-sectional MIDUS samples of same-aged adults recruited 20 years apart, from the mid 1990’s to post-Recession. Preliminary findings show more compromised SES, physical health, and psychological profiles at post-Recession. Building on these outcomes, our fourth objective is to put forward new scientific imperatives to test whether and how growing socioeconomic inequality is translating to worsening health inequalities. The overarching aim is to renew the call of Adler et al. (1994), but with a sense of urgency, given ever more diminished life opportunities among disadvantaged segments of American society.

MIDUS Contributions to the Science of Health Inequalities

The MIDUS (Midlife in the U.S.) national study (www.midus.wisc.edu) was launched in 1995 with a national sample of over 7,000 adults aged 25 to 74 (Brim, Ryff, & Kessler, 2004) on whom detailed information was collected on sociodemographic and psychosocial factors, life stresses, and health. Funded by the John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation, the study brought together psychologists with demographers, epidemiologists, and sociologists, thereby affording a unique look at the interplay of factors involved in health inequalities. A decade later, with funding from the National Institute on Aging, a longitudinal follow-up was launched and included new data collection on cognition, biomarkers, and affective neuroscience. We focus below on select findings (about 20% of 135+ publications) on health inequalities, giving primary attention to investigations that included psychosocial, behavioral, or biological factors, thereby responding to earlier calls to incorporate these influences (Adler et al., 1994; Matthews & Gallo, 2011; Taylor & Seeman, 1999). Further, we chose recent findings from high visibility journals in psychology or behavioral medicine, given that biomarkers were obtained in MIDUS at the 2nd wave.

The first column in Table 1 lists multiple indices of SES. This diversity reflects the larger area of literature on SES and health inequalities. Adler et al. (1994) has formulated SES as a multi-dimensional construct that incorporates facets of work status (occupation), economic status (income), and social status (education). In MIDUS, education was the most commonly studied indicator of individual-level SES. As described in previous literature (Marmot et al., 1998), education is a desirable indicator of SES because it is considered the most basic component of SES that sets the stage for future occupational and income opportunities, and it is less prone than income or wealth to missing data. Alternatively, Adler et al. noted limitations of restricting SES to single individual-level indicators and called for consideration of multiple sources of SES disadvantage. Accordingly, MIDUS researchers have incorporated composite indicators of SES that include education, income, occupation, and subjective financial well-being (e.g., Zilioli et al., 2015). Other studies have taken a life-course approach to SES and health through assessments of early childhood SES compared to adult SES (e.g., Chen et al., 2012; Grunewald et al., 2012). Researchers also attended to contextual-level influences, such as neighborhood SES (Robinette et al., 2017). Comparisons of subjective ratings of social status (SSS) with objective SES indicators (Kan et al., 2012) also gained in prominence. Adler et al. (1994) also recommended emphasis on how both race and SES operate to influence health. Several studies using MIDUS have also been attentive to racial disparities in health (e.g., Fuller-Rowell et al., 2016; Karlamangla et al., 2013).

Table 1.

Illustrative Findings on Socioeconomic Inequality and Health from MIDUS

Primary Outcome: Physical Health, Health Behaviors and/or Mortality
Indicator of Inequality Key Findings
Parent occupation (Chapman, et al., 2009a) • Low parent occupation higher adult BMI and obesity (mediated by adult SES and personality traits)
Education (Chapman et al., 2009b) • Low education higher smoking risk (conscientiousness buffered smoking risk in low educated)
Childhood SES (Ferraro et al., 2016) • Low childhood SES & greater childhood abusemore chronic conditions, higher obesity and smoking risk, fewer psychosocial resources
Race, neighborhood SES (Fuller-Rowell et al., 2016) • Minority statuspoorer sleep quality (neighborhood SES accounted for race differences in sleep)
Education, daily stress (Grzywacz & Almeida, 2008) • Low education →higher cumulative stress exposure higher levels of binge drinking and negative affect
Income (Johnson & Krueger, 2005) • Low incomemore chronic conditions, higher BMI (Larger genetic influence in lower income and low control)
Education, Subjective Social Status (SSS, Kan et al., 2012) • Education, SSS more chronic conditions, lower self-rated health (mediated by sense of control, neuroticism)
Childhood SES, gender (Lee & Ryff, 2016) • Low childhood SES higher incidence of heart problems (mediated by maternal early transition to parenthood)
Neighborhood SES (Robinette et al., 2017) • Low neighborhood income lower incidence chronic conditions
Education (Turiano et al., 2014) • Low education higher mortality risk (sense of control buffered mortality risk)
Primary Outcome: Psychological Health
Education (Almeida et al. 2005) • Low education greater distress and physical health symptoms (mediated by daily stress exposure and appraisal)
Education (Lachman et al., 2010) • Low education Low cognitive functioning (frequent cognitive activity buffered cognitive declines)
Adult SES (Park et al., 2013) • Low SES More anger expression (mediated by frustration)
Income (South & Krueger, 2011) • Low income more anxiety, depression, and panic attacks
Parent education (Ward, 2013) • Low maternal education & adult SES low sense of control
Primary Outcome: Biomarkers
Salivary Cortisol
Race (Fuller-Rowell et al., 2012) • Minority status flatter diurnal (less healthy) cortisol slope (Moderated by perceived discrimination)
Education, Race (Karlamangla et al., 2013) • Low education and minority status lower daily cortisol peak and higher nadir (flatter, less healthy, diurnal profile)
Adult SES (Zilioli et al, 2015) • Low SES flatter diurnal cortisol slope (Life satisfaction steepened cortisol slope in lower SES)
Adult SES composite (Zilioli et al., 2017a) • Low SES flatter diurnal cortisol slopes greater health symptom severity and frequency, especially at low control
Inflammation
Education (Boylan & Ryff, 2013) • Low education greater inflammation (association amplified by higher anger)
Education, Race/ethnicity (Boylan, et al., 2015) • Minority status, low education greater inflammation (moderated by anger expression in higher educated Blacks)
Adult SES, gender (Elliot & Chapman, 2016) • Low SES greater inflammation (psychological resources buffered inflammation in men)
Adult SES (Elliot et al., 2017) • Low SES greater inflammation (buffered by high conscientiousness and low neuroticism)
Education (Morozink et al., 2010) • Low education greater inflammation (psychological well being reduced inflammation risk)
Race, education (Stepanikova et al., 2017) • Low education, Black racial status greater inflammation (lifetime discrimination further predicted inflammation)
Cardiovascular and Metabolic Function
Race (Curtis et al., 2017) • Minority status greater cardiometabolic risk (mediated by sleep time and efficiency)
Parent education (Miller et al., 2011) • Low parent education higher metabolic syndrome risk (maternal nurturance protected against heightened risk)
Race, gender (Tsenkova et al., 2012) • For women, Minority status poorer glucose metabolism function (mediated by anxiety)
Childhood SES, adult SES (Tsenkova et al., 2014) • Low childhood SES & adult SES greater prediabetes and diabetes risk (mediated by depression and waist circ.)
Allostatic Load
Parental education (Chen et al., 2012) • Low parental education greater allostatic load (shift-and-persist buffered allostatic load risk in low SES)
Childhood SES, adult SES (Gruenewald et al., 2012) • Lower SES at each phase of and cumulatively across the life course greater allostatic load
Childhood SES, gender, race (Lee, Coe & Ryff, 2017) • Low childhood SES, women, African American status greater physiological dysregulation (Mediated by childhood abuse)
Race (Ong et al., 2017) • Chronic Bias/unfair treatment greater allostatic load (Relationship was not moderated by educational status)
Adult SES (Zilioli et al., 2017b) • Low SES greater allostatic load (Mediated by discrimination and anger control)

Table 1 focuses on three primary outcomes: physical health, psychological health, and biomarkers. Within each category, studies (organized alphabetically by author) show the indicator of socioeconomic inequality that was examined and key findings obtained, including whether mediating or moderating influences were identified. Details of each study (e.g., covariates employed, cross-sectional vs. longitudinal analyses) and strength of relationships are available in the published studies.

Different indicators of SES predicted numerous physical health outcomes (e.g., chronic conditions, functional limitations, obesity, mortality) and health behaviors (e.g., smoking, sleep), and many studies included mediating or moderating influences. Personality traits (conscientiousness, agreeableness, neuroticism) mediated the link between low parental occupation and obesity (Chapman et al., 2009a). Conscientiousness buffered against the risk of smoking among those with lower educational attainment (Chapman et al., 2009b). Low childhood SES and greater childhood abuse predicted more chronic conditions, higher obesity, and smoking risk in adulthood (Ferraro et al., 2016). Poorer neighborhood SES explained poorer sleep quality among minorities (Fuller-Rowell et al., 2016). According to daily diary data, low education was associated with higher cumulative daily stress exposure and excess levels of binge drinking (Grcywacz & Almeida, 2008). Genetic factors were stronger predictors of chronic conditions and BMI among individuals with lower sense of control and lower income (Johnson & Krueger, 2005). Among women, low childhood SES predicted higher incidence of heart problems in adulthood, with effects mediated by an early maternal transition to parenthood (Lee & Ryff, 2016). Sense of control buffered against the risk of mortality among adults with low education (Turiano et al., 2014).

Turning to psychological health, low educational status predicted greater distress and physical health symptoms, with effects mediated by daily stress exposure and appraisal (Almeida et al., 2005). Low educational status also predicted poorer cognitive functioning, although frequent cognitive activity buffered against cognitive decline (Lachman et al., 2010). Low SES predicted more frequent anger expression, with effects mediated by frustration (Park et al., 2013). Low income predicted greater anxiety, depression and panic attacks (South & Krueger, 2011). Both low maternal education and adult SES independently contributed to low sense of control (Ward et al., 2013).

Biological measures were collected on over 1,200 respondents at MIDUS 2. Minority status and lower adult SES predicted flatter (less healthy) diurnal cortisol slopes, with such links modified by perceived discrimination (Fuller-Rowell et al.2012), life satisfaction (Zilioli et al., 2015) and sense of control (Zilioli et al., 2017a). Low education and minority status predicted elevated inflammation. Higher levels of psychological well-being (Morozink et al., 2010), higher conscientiousness/low neuroticism (Elliot et al., 2017), and higher levels of optimism, perceived control, and self-esteem (Elliot & Chapman, 2016) buffered the effects of lower adult SES on elevated inflammation. Higher levels of anger amplified the association between low education and elevated inflammation (Boylan & Ryff, 2013). Among African Americans, high anger expression predicted elevated inflammation among those with higher educational status (Boylan et al., 2015). High levels of lifetime discrimination predicted elevated systemic inflammation (Stepanikova et al., 2017).

For cardiometabolic health, sleep mediated the effects of minority status on elevated cardiometabolic risk, (Curtis et al., 2017). Among women, minority status predicted poorer glucose metabolism, with the effects mediated by anxiety (Tsenkova et al., 2012). Lower childhood SES and adult SES increased diabetes risk, with effects mediated by waist circumference and depression (Tsenkova et al., 2014). Low parental educational standing also increased metabolic risk, with maternal nurturance serving as a protection against such heightened risk (Miller et al., 2011).

Five studies measured allostatic load, a summary index of wear and tear on multiple physiological systems. Low parental education predicted higher allostatic load, with effects buffered by a shift and persist appraisal style (Chen et al., 2012). Low childhood and low adulthood SES combined predicted the highest risk of allostatic load (Gruenewald et al., 2012). Among women, low childhood SES and minority status predicted greater physiological dysregulation across multiple systems, with effects mediated by childhood abuse (Lee et al.,2017. Discrimination contributed to elevated allostatic load in African Americans (Ong et al.,2018). Lower adult SES predicted greater allostatic load, with effects mediated by perceived discrimination and anger control (Zilioli et al., 2017b).

Taken together, the above results illustrate multiple advances linking indicators of SES in childhood and adulthood to diverse health outcomes in adulthood. Guided by multiple conceptual frameworks, researchers have illuminated psychosocial, behavioral, and biological pathways. Life-course pathway models emphasize how early life experiences shape socioeconomic, psychological and behavioral trajectories that have downstream consequences for health (e.g., Power & Hertzman, 1997). MIDUS findings for childhood SES indicated that early life disadvantage had long-lasting impacts on adult health (e.g., Chapman et al., 2009a; Gruenewald et al., 2012). The stress proliferation life course model posits that early life adversity increases exposure to subsequent stressors (e.g., Pearlin et al., 2005). Cumulative exposure to life stressors then leads to declines in emotional and physical health through stress-mediated biological disturbances (Cohen, Gianaros, & Manuck, 2016). Consistent with these frameworks, childhood abuse explained relationships between childhood SES and adulthood physiological dysregulation (Lee et al., 2017).

The reserve capacity model (Matthews & Gallo, 2011) underscores the importance of the presence or absence of protective psychosocial resources in addition to experiences of stress and negative emotions. In line with the reserve capacity model, early life protective factors were found to dampen the influence of early life adversity on health. High levels of maternal nurturance protected against higher risk of metabolic syndrome associated with low childhood SES (Miller et al., 2012). In adulthood, psychosocial resources mitigated the negative impacts of SES on health. Higher levels of conscientiousness, sense of control, and well-being predicted better biological functioning among SES disadvantaged (e.g., Elliot et al., 2017; Morozink et al., 2010, Zilioli et al., 2015; 2017a). In further support of the reserve capacity framework, negative emotions (anxiety, depression, and anger) contributed to SES and race disparities in health (Boylan & Ryff, 2013; Tsenkova et al., 2012; 2014; Zilioli et al., 2017b).

MIDUS findings also underscore the importance of including both race and SES in health inequalities research. Stress exposures, including interpersonal (discrimination) and environmental (poor neighborhood environment) were linked to a wide range of health outcomes that were patterned by both race and SES. For example, low education and minority status were both independent predictors of dysregulation in diurnal cortisol (Karlamangla et al., 2013). Among African Americans, unfair treatment predicted greater allostatic load, regardless of educational attainment (Ong, et al., 2017). Poor neighborhood environment was also linked with poorer sleep quality in African Americans. Sleep is increasingly recognized as a behavior fundamental to health, with MIDUS findings showing that sleep time and efficiency accounted for race disparities in cardiometabolic risk (Curtis et al., 2017). Further, psychological factors such as anger expression were shown to exacerbate health risk among high SES African Americans but not high SES Whites (Boylan, et al., 2015). In summary, race and SES represent distinct processes of social stratification and health inequalities. The next section highlights the changing historical context surrounding research on health inequalities.

Deepening Inequality in America

The Great Recession of 2007-2009 produced radical change in the U.S. economy, with poverty rates rising from 33 million in 2005 to more than 48 million in 2012 (Bishaw, 2013). Health correlates of job loss, unemployment, financial strain, and Recession-related hardships were documented (Burgard & Kalousova, 2015). Importantly, less advantaged groups (low SES and minorities) have experienced the largest percentage declines in wealth following the Great Recession (Pfeffer, Danziger, & Schoeni, 2013). National studies have shown that less educated adults experienced more economic hardships and had more difficulty recovering from the Recession than higher educated adults (Hoynes, Miller, & Schaller, 2012; Carnevale, Javasundera, & Gulish, 2016).

Historically, it is also worth noting that a dramatic drop in income inequality in the U.S. and in Europe was observed in the first half of the 20th century (Piketty & Saez, 2014), but since the 1970s, income inequality in the U.S. has surged back, making the U.S. notably more unequal today than Europe. Reeves (2017) has described “hoarding” of the American Dream, with evidence showing that the top 20% of income earners have privileged access to better educations, jobs, income, and wealth as well as greater likelihood of stable marriages to successful partners, thriving neighborhoods, and healthier lifestyles. Graham (2017) linked such economic and life opportunity discrepancies to ever more compromised levels of optimism, life satisfaction, and happiness among disadvantaged segments of society. Taken together, recent economic evidence underscores that socioeconomic inequality is an ever more salient societal problem in contemporary America. This historical shift calls for new research. The next section uses MIDUS to compare life in the U.S. from two different historical periods characterized by different levels of socioeconomic inequality and access to life opportunities.

A Look Pre- and Post-Recession America

The findings described in Table 1 were derived from the MIDUS baseline 1995 (prerecession) sample. Many of these individuals came of age during a historical period characterized by socioeconomic mobility and economic stability (Hatch & Clinton, 2000). In this section, we compare the baseline MIDUS sample with a new national sample of adults, known as the MIDUS refresher, recruited with the purpose of evaluating impacts of the Great Recession. In contrast to the pre-recession sample, the MIDUS refresher (post-recession) sample was recruited during a period of increasing socioeconomic inequality made worse by the Great Recession. Inequality in the distribution of SES indicators increased in the post-recession sample as a result of declines in income, wealth (objective SES) and financial stability (subjective SES) for those at the bottom and increases in wealth among those at the top (Glei, Goldman, & Weinstein, 2018). The comparison between the two samples affords a unique look at same-aged adults from two different temporal periods (i.e., a time-lag design), thereby offering a window on historical change in America.

Data from the pre-recession baseline sample were collected in 1995 to 1996, while data from the post-recession refresher sample were collected in 2011 to 2014. Both the pre-recession baseline sample (N = 3487, aged 25-74) and the post-recession refresher sample (N = 3577, aged 25-74) were probability samples recruited through random digit dialing of phone numbers within the coterminous United States. Participation included a phone interview and a self-administered questionnaire (SAQ). Response rates were lower for the post-recession (59%) compared to the pre-recession sample (70%). Similarly, of those who completed the phone interview, 73% of post-recession sample compared to 87% of the pre-recession sample completed the SAQ. Such declining response rates parallel changes observed for other national telephone surveys over the same period. Nonetheless, even with declining response rates, telephone surveys that are weighted to match the demographic composition of the population continue to provide accurate data on most social and economic measures (Kohut, Keeter, Doherty et al., 2012).

U.S. census current population survey (CPS) data from 1995 and 2012 provide benchmarks to compare the pre- and post-recession samples to the larger U.S. population. Both samples represented their respective population in gender, race, and marital status. However, both samples over-represented college-educated adults – by approximately 5% for the pre-recession sample and 15% for the post-recession sample. CPS data also document historical gains in educational attainment from 1995 to 2012: over this period, the percent of college educated increased from 24.8% to 33.2%, and the percent of less than high school educated declined from 15.3% to 11.3%. Multivariate post-stratification weights (using 1995 and 2012 CPS data) are employed in the comparisons below to maximize population-level representativeness. See Supplemental Material Table S1 for more information on sociodemographic representation of un-weighted and weighted samples.

Pre- versus post-recession sample differences were examined for three categories of outcomes. Socioeconomic standing included three indicators that had the fewest missing responses and had been tested in the prior literature: household income1 (log10 transformed to adjust for positive skew), educational status (1 = no school/some grade school to 12 = doctorate or professional degree), and a composite measure of financial stability rated across five subjective measures (current and future financial situation, control over financial situation, money to meet needs, difficulty paying bills; Glei et al., 2018). To account for population shifts in educational achievement, a cohort-standardized index of the number of years spent in formal schooling was computed. Education was standardized by 10-year age cohorts (e.g., 25-34 in pre-recession: M = 13.72, SD = 2.28; 24-34 in post-recession: M = 15.04, SD = 2.53). Physical health included five continuous indicators based on self-report: general assessment of health (0 = best, 10= worst), total chronic conditions (0 to 26), body mass index (BMI, range = 16 to 80), functional health (e.g., difficulty walking a few blocks, climbing stairs; range = 7 to 28) and physical symptoms in past 30 days (e.g., frequency of backaches, joint pain, trouble sleeping; range = 8-48). Psychological factors included continuous measures of negative and positive affect (Mroczek & Kolarz, 1998), satisfaction with life overall (0 = worst to 10 = best), six dimensions of eudaimonic well-being (Ryff, 1989), a composite measure of sense of control (Lachman & Weaver, 1998), and composite measures of the Big Five personality traits (neuroticism, openness, extraversion, agreeableness, and conscientiousness; Goldberg, 1992).

Analyses are based on 3,034 pre-recession and 2,599 post-recession respondents who completed both the phone interview and the SAQ. Missing data were imputed; analyses with and without imputed data yielded similar findings. General linear regression models tested pre-vs. post-recession mean differences in outcomes. The sample was entered as a dummy coded predictor (0 = pre-recession sample, 1 = post-recession sample). Analyses adjusted for age, gender, marital status, and race. The mean difference estimates (M(Post-recession Sample)M(Pre-recession sample) and 95% confidence intervals were standardized with the compute.es package in R.

Figure 1 displays the effect size differences between pre- and post-recession America after adjusting for covariates. Exact values are available in Table S2 in Supplementary Material. As expected, post-recession respondents had significantly higher educational attainment compared to pre-recession respondents. However, post-recession respondents reported less total household income (after adjusting for inflation) than pre-recession respondents. Financial stability was also significantly lower in the post-recession sample than the pre-recession sample. Thus, despite having higher levels of education, the post-recession sample showed more compromised outcomes in income and financial stability relative to the pre-recession sample.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Standardized estimates of pre-recession vs. post-recession sample differences in socioeconomic, physical health, and psychological outcomes. The dots represent the estimate (Cohen’s d for continuous outcomes). The whiskers represent the 95% confidence interval. Effects are statistically non-significant where the whiskers intersect with 0 (vertical line).

The physical health status of the post-recession sample was also generally worse than pre-recession sample. Specifically, (a) post-recession respondents had significantly worse ratings of general health; (b) more chronic conditions; (c) higher BMI; (d) more functional limitations; and (e) more frequent physical health symptoms.

For hedonic well-being, indicators of positive affect and life satisfaction were significantly lower in the post-recession sample, although the samples did not differ in negative affect. For eudaimonic well-being, ratings of autonomy, self-acceptance, and personal growth were significantly lower in the post-recession sample. Environmental mastery was the only indicator of well-being that was significantly higher in the post-recession sample, but this effect was reduced to non-significance when education was included in the model. Purpose in life and positive relations with others were not significantly different. Sense of control was significantly lower in the post-recession sample. For personality traits, the post-recession sample was significantly lower on ratings of openness, extraversion, and agreeableness, but not conscientiousness. Ratings of neuroticism were significantly lower in the post-recession sample, but differences were attenuated with the inclusion of education as a covariate.

The preceding differences between pre- and post-recession adults may reflect other historical changes that have occurred over time, such as globalization of the economy, obesity trends, or political factors. The post-recession sample may have also been exposed to more environmental pollutants and endocrine disruptors at critical developmental time points, thus having lasting effects on physical health2. Because the Great Recession disproportionately impacted younger adults (Taylor, Morin, & Wang, 2010), follow-up analyses tested age (continuous) as a moderator of sample-period differences in physical health and psychological outcomes (see Supplementary Material Table S3 for physical health). Age significantly moderated the sample difference for BMI and sense of control only. Pre-to post-recession differences in BMI and sense of control were greater for younger compared to older adults.

As a preliminary window on whether the above differences might reflect growing inequality, additional analyses examined educational gradients in physical health in the two samples. We focus on educational status because it is the most commonly used indicator of SES in findings summarized in Table 1, and it has the advantage of applying to working and non-working adults. Both unstandardized education and cohort-standardized education were significant negative predictors of each physical health outcome. Interactions between education and sample period were significant for all five physical health outcomes: general health, chronic conditions, BMI, functional limitations, and physical health symptoms (controlling for age, gender, race, and marital status). Figure 2 illustrates this pattern for BMI, showing a steeper educational gradient in the post- compared to pre-recession sample. Similar patterns were observed for other health outcomes (see Table S3 and Figures S1-S4). Overall, the figure shows that the steepening of the educational gradient was due to more pronounced pre- to post-Recession declines in health among the educationally disadvantaged, rather than improvements in health among educationally advantaged. To test the robustness of the interaction, the indicators of physical health were included as covariates. The interaction effect held for BMI, but adding the health covariates reduced the size of the interaction effect to non-significance for general health, chronic conditions, functional limitations, and physical health symptoms. Nonetheless, sample period main effects remained significant.

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Displayed is a significant interaction between educational status (1 = junior high or lower completed; 12 = professional degree completed), and MIDUS pre- and post-recession sample in predicting body mass index. Lines represent general linear model (GLM) predictions of chronic conditions by sample. Confidence interval bars (+/− 1 standard error of point estimates from GLM) are provided to indicate the precision of the point estimates of the population means for the pre-recession sample and the post-recession sample.

In sum, this preliminary descriptive comparison of two national samples situated at two different time points show more compromised profiles among same-aged U.S. adults from a decade before to a few years after the Great Recession. Despite gains in educational status, household income levels were lower and there was less financial stability. Post-Recession adults showed more health problems and poorer psychological profiles. The overall picture at the population-level suggests life has gotten worse on multiple, but not all, fronts across time, and that educational gradients in health appear to have widened. More targeted analyses are nonetheless needed to determine whether health inequalities are worsening over time. What psychologists have to contribute to this tale of societal change are more penetrating analyses that illuminate individual-level profiles beneath these patterns – i.e., for whom has life become more compromised across historical time, why, and with what consequences for health?

Advancing Historically Situated Inquiry on Health Inequalities

Economists, sociologists, and population health scientists have been at the forefront of research on the health consequences of widening socioeconomic inequality and the Great Recession. Adler et al. (1994) demonstrated that research on health inequalities requires knowledge of health and human behavior across multiple dimensions and must attend to the larger social context, thus calling for integrative approaches. Our aim in this paper has been to illustrate how the Midlife in the U.S. Study (MIDUS) has been uniquely valuable in advancing the science of health inequalities in ways that are attentive to Adler et al.’s recommendations.

We show how researchers using the MIDUS data have filled in psychological, behavioral, and biological pathways to health inequalities. Further, in comparing two MIDUS samples situated at different historical periods, we documented that person-level indicators of SES, physical health, and psychological health look more compromised in the post-recession sample. Building on this previous research guided by multiple conceptual frameworks (e.g., life-course pathway models, reserve capacity model), our final section calls for new science to advance historically situated research on health inequalities.

Linking Widening Socioeconomic Inequalities to Widening Health Inequalities

The findings summarized in Table 1 link multiple indicators of SES to health inequalities, with all findings coming from assessments prior to the Great Recession. Whether historical shifts toward greater socioeconomic inequality may change the strength of relationships between SES and health is a topic ripe for future inquiry using the MIDUS post-Recession data. Emerging research indicates that the physical and mental health toll of low SES has worsened over time. Case and Deaton (2015) documented widening inequalities in mortality across educational status over the past three decades. Using data from MIDUS, Goldman, Glei, and Weinstein (2018) have, in turn, shown declining mental health among recent cohorts of socioeconomically disadvantaged relative to their more advantaged counterpart. In some instances, more SES advantaged showed improvements in mental health over time. Such findings demonstrate the need to consider historical changes in health across the entire range of the SES gradient. Findings in the initial comparison of the pre- and post-recession samples offer preliminary evidence that education-based inequalities in self-reported physical health appear worse in the post-recession sample. This was largely driven by declines in physical health among educationally disadvantaged. Such analyses need to be extended to objective indicators of physical health.

Revisiting Mediators and Moderators of Health Inequalities in the Post-Recession MIDUS Sample

Previous MIDUS findings (Table 1) have expanded on diverse psychological, behavioral and biological outcomes theorized to contribute to health inequalities. According to the reserve capacity model, exposure to stress and negative emotions in combination with a limited reserve of psychosocial resources (social support, psychological well-being) undermines the health of low SES (see Matthews & Gallo, 2011). In pre-Recession findings, lower SES was tied to more acute and chronic stress exposures (Almeida et al., 2005; Grzywacz & Almeida, 2008), and more dysregulated diurnal cortisol (Zilioli et al., 2015). Historical shifts toward growing socioeconomic inequality and hardships of the Great Recession call for re-examination of the conceptual linkages between SES and health proposed in the reserve capacity model. National studies have shown that socioeconomically disadvantaged adults experienced greater financial losses and job strain in the aftermath of the Great Recession (Hoynes et al., 2012; Carnevale et al., 2016; Pfeffer et al., 2013). Such findings suggest that the relationships between SES and stress-mediated biological dysregulation may be more pronounced in the post-recession sample.

Turning to psychosocial moderators of health inequalities, prior findings in Table 1 showed that psychosocial resources such as conscientiousness, sense of control, and well-being were health protective in the face of socioeconomic inequality (e.g., Chapman et al., 2009a; Elliot & Chapman, 2016; Elliot et al., 2017; Morozink et al., 2010; Turiano et al., 2014). Future research needs to examine whether such resources remain protective under conditions of heightened socioeconomic inequality. Findings from the post-recession sample showed that recession hardships (e.g. job loss, home foreclosure, financial loss) were disproportionately borne by the less educated, and further that aspects of well-being were not protective against poorer health (Kirsch & Ryff, 2016). These results suggest that protective resources may be undone (disabled) under notably difficult life circumstances (Shanahan, Hill, Roberts, et al., 2014). If this is the case, it is critical to assess whether prior findings showing that psychosocial resources prospectively predict better health and longer lives among SES disadvantaged remain protective in post-Recession America.

Everyday discrimination, a chronic stressor, has been linked with health among racial minorities, across all SES levels (Friedman et al., 2009; Ong et al., 2017). Economic insecurity is a highly stigmatizing experience, especially in the American cultural context where self-sufficiency and independence are prominent ideals (Markus & Kitayama, 2003). It is important to underscore that the black-white economic divide in the U.S. has grown more stark because higher income and educational attainment among African Americans are not leading to accumulation of wealth to the same degree as they do for whites (Shapiro et al., 2013). Additionally, racial minorities are known to have lost disproportionately more wealth in the aftermath of the Great Recession (Pfeffer et al., 2013). Such added burdens of economic insecurity may thus be widening racial inequalities in health via heightened experiences of economic stress and discrimination. Future work needs to examine these possibilities.

Life Course Issues and Multi-level Contributions to Health Inequalities

Health Inequalities Across the Life Course.

Understanding how socioeconomic inequality and losses linked to the Great Recession matter for American lives needs life course perspectives. The inclusion of childhood measures of SES in MIDUS offers opportunities to assess SES at each phase of the life course and to test models of cumulative SES disadvantage and health. According to the stress proliferation model, early life adversity increases risk for future stress exposures that have downstream consequences for health. In the MIDUS-core sample, early childhood SES has been linked with adverse health in adulthood, such as higher incidence of heart problems (Lee & Ryff, 2016), more chronic conditions (Ferraro et al., 2016), and greater allostatic load (Chen et al., 2012). Cumulative exposure to low SES at both childhood and adulthood predicted elevated allostatic load relative to those reporting low SES at only a single time point (Gruenewald et al., 2012). Growing socioeconomic inequality and the Great Recession may thus amplify health inequality because individuals exposed to early life SES adversity may be more vulnerable to experiences of Recession hardship and declining income and occupation opportunities. Whether the effects of early life adversity on health inequalities will be amplified in the post-Recession period is an important question to address in future research.

Most findings summarized in Table 1 have been limited to cross-sectional data. However, a key feature of MIDUS is its longitudinal design. The baseline (pre-recession) sample has been followed for over 20 years, and it is likely there will be follow-ups for the refresher sample. Such data will permit tests of claims put forth at the beginning of the Great Recession, namely, that it was going to change the life course of some Americans (Peck, 2010). This prediction harkens back to Elder’s classic work on Children of the Great Depression (Elder, 1998; Elder, Pavalko, & Hastings, 1991), and importantly, age differences in how their lives unfolded. In parallel fashion, it will be critical to track long-term biopsychosocial health profiles of adults of the Great Recession. Future waves of the post-recession (refresher) sample, compared to the longitudinal data available on the MIDUS core sample, will clarify whether the life course of some Americans was, in fact, changed relative to those lives played out in less economically difficult times.

With the assessment of longitudinal data, further questions pertain to the nature of cumulative adversity in the lives of Americans. For some, the stresses of socioeconomic inequality may become a ubiquitous feature of life that takes a toll mentally and physically. The accumulation of risks model posits that repeated exposures to SES disadvantage throughout the life course could amplify the adverse effects over time (e.g., Ferraro & Shippee, 2009). Age at exposure to the Great Recession and widening socioeconomic inequality may therefore be an especially salient marker of sub-group differences in health inequalities. Younger adults did experience disproportionate losses (Taylor, Morin, & Wang, 2010), which could fuel problems for health and well-being, compared to older adults who had more established incomes and wealth prior to the onset of the Great Recession. Longitudinal assessments need to clarify whether historical changes in health inequalities are more pronounced in younger adults.

Multi-level Approaches to Health Inequalities.

According to the sociological model of cumulative dis(advantage), the consequences of inequality may be embedded in multiple levels of social context (micro-level, meso-level, macro-level; Dannefer, 2018). At the macro-level, inequality emerges from social and economic forces while at the meso-level it emerges from the stratification of social roles (e.g., occupation, marriage). At the micro-level, interpersonal interactions and daily experiences reinforce one’s position in the SES hierarchy. Future research can fruitfully compare pre- and post-recession MIDUS samples to test how macro-level forces, like the Great Recession, influence meso-level and micro-level processes that exacerbate health inequalities. For example, MIDUS daily diary data can be leveraged to test SES differences in the consequences of the Great Recession on daily stress exposures and work and family life. Previous studies have shown that financial loss and job strain have downstream consequences on marital and family strain (Kahn & Pearlin, 2006; Sturgeon et al., 2016) and social isolation (Brand & Burgard, 2008). Additionally, prior work in MIDUS has shown that contextual factors like neighborhood SES matter for individual health (e.g., Fuller-Rowell et al. 2016; Robinette et al., 2017). Future work should consider neighborhood SES in light of post-Recession home foreclosure rates and increases in poverty in vulnerable communities (Owens & Sampson, 2013).

Comparing SES Indicators and their Relevance to Health

A unique strength of MIDUS is the inclusion of multiple indicators of SES and health. Most studies, however, have focused primarily on relating a single indicator of SES to a single health outcome. Population health scientists have suggested that objective indicators may be limited representations of SES (Braveman, et al., 2005; Shavers, 2007). Future integrative work could usefully examine the unique contributions of more diverse indicators of SES and their relationship with health, particularly as tied to widening socioeconomic inequality. Researchers have noted that in the last two decades disparities in subjective financial standing have increased to a greater extent than in objective indicators (Glei et al., 2018). According to the status anxiety hypothesis, heightened socioeconomic inequality increases the salience of one’s relative position within the socioeconomic hierarchy (Layte, 2011). This heightened awareness may increase perceptions of relative disadvantage, producing negative consequences for stress and health (Adler & Tan, 2017). Sommet et al. (2018) found that perceptions of financial scarcity, but not income level, strengthened the association between increasing income inequality and poorer psychological health. Whether these findings replicate for physical health outcomes needs further investigation. In sum, subjective experiences of SES warrant greater attention. MIDUS has assessments of relative disadvantage (ladder of subjective socioeconomic status), but few studies have tested its association with objective indicators of health.

Conclusion: Pursuing the Science of Inequality on a Changing National Stage

The discipline of psychology is critical for understanding how socioeconomic inequality matters for health. This article highlights evidence from a large public-use study that has attracted widespread engagement from the scientific community. Numerous findings have shown how behavioral and psychosocial factors mediate or moderate linkages between SES and health outcomes, including biological risk factors. Novel opportunities for future inquiries follow from two national samples of U.S. adults situated on either side of the Great Recession. Preliminary findings suggest that life for many individuals has gotten worse over time, relative to same-aged adults from pre-recession America, but future inquiries using individual-level analyses are needed to evaluate for whom health inequalities are getting worse and why. Ultimately, high quality science on multiple fronts is needed to address societal challenges of inequality that are now unfolding. Our intent is to nurture such inquiries.

Supplementary Material

Supplemental Material

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the National Institution on Aging grant (P01-AG020166; U19-AGO51426) to conduct a refresher sample data collection and longitudinal follow-up of the MIDUS (Midlife in the United States) investigation. Biomarker data collection was further supported by the NIH National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences (NCATS) Clinical and Translational Science Award (CTSA) program as follows UL1TR001409 (Georgetown), UL1TR001881 (UCLA) and UL1RR025011 (UW-Madison). The original study was supported by the John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation Research Network on Successful Midlife Development. Support was also provided by a National Institute on Aging training grant (2 T32 AG 129-29).

Biography

graphic file with name nihms-1042590-b0001.gif

graphic file with name nihms-1042590-b0002.gif

graphic file with name nihms-1042590-b0003.gif

graphic file with name nihms-1042590-b0004.gif

Footnotes

1

Mean household income was based on wages, pensions, and social security. To reduce deductive disclosure of respondent identity, household income was top-coded at 300,000 USD. Using the Bureau of Labor Statistics (BLS) inflation calculator, income reported in the pre-recession sample was adjusted for the inflation rate from 1995 to 2012.

2

Using the MIDUS 2 follow-up (2005-2006) would reduce the time-window between pre-and post-recession and aid in ruling out alternative explanations. However, use of the MIDUS-2 follow-up results in analysis of adults of a more restricted age-range (35-75) who are also more selective, given attrition over time (Radler & Ryff, 2010). For these reasons, we compare the original MIDUS-baseline sample to the MIDUS-Refresher baseline sample. Nonetheless, follow-up analyses that substituted the baseline sample with the follow-up sample did not alter the direction or strength of the majority sample-period differences reported in Figure 1.

References

  1. Adler NE, Boyce T, Chesney MA, Cohen S, Folkman S, Kahn RL, & Syme SL (1994). Socioeconomic status and health: the challenge of the gradient. American Psychologist, 49(1), 15. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.49.1.15 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Adler NE (2009). Health disparities through a psychological lens. American Psychologist, 64(8), 663. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.64.8.663 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Almeida DM, Neupert SD, Banks SR, & Serido J (2005). Do daily stress processes account for socioeconomic health disparities?. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 60(Special_Issue_2), S34–S39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Avendano M, Kawachi I, Van Lenthe F, Boshuizen HC, Mackenbach JP, Van den Bos GAM, … &Berkman LF (2006). Socioeconomic status and stroke incidence in the US elderly. Stroke, 37, 1368–1373. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000221702.75002.66 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bishaw A (2013). Poverty: 2000 to 2012. American Community Survey Briefs, 1–16. [Google Scholar]
  6. Bosma H, Van Jaarsveld CHM, Tuinstra J, Sanderman R, Ranchor AV, Van Eijk JTM, & Kempen GIJM (2005). Low control beliefs, classical coronary risk factors, and socio-economic differences in heart disease in older persons. Social Science & Medicine, 60, 737–745. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2004.06.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Boylan JM, Lewis TT, Coe CL, & Ryff CD (2015). Educational status, anger, and inflammation in the MIDUS national sample: Does race matter?. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 49(4), 570–578. doi: 10.1007/s12160-015-9687-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Boylan JM, & Ryff CD (2013). Varieties of anger and the inverse link between education and inflammation: Toward an integrative framework. Psychosomatic medicine, 75(6), 566. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31829683bd [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Brand JE, & Burgard SA (2008). Job displacement and social participation over the lifecourse: Findings for a cohort of joiners. Social Forces, 87, 211–242. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Braveman PA Cubbin C, Egerter S, Chideya S, Marchi KS, Metzler M, & Posner S (2005). Socioeconomic status in health research: One size does not fit all. Journal of the American Medical Association, 294(22), 2879–2888. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Brim OG, Ryff CD, & Kessler RC (2004). The MIDUS National Survey: An Overview. University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
  12. Burgard SA, & Kalousova L (2015). Effects of the Great Recession: Health and well-being. Annual Review of Sociology, 41, 181–201. [Google Scholar]
  13. Businelle MS, Mills BA, Chartier KG, Kendzor DE, Reingle JM, & Shuval K (2014). Do stressful events account for the link between socioeconomic status and mental health?. Journal of Public Health, 36(2), 205–212. doi: 10.1093/pubmed/fdt060 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Carnevale AP, Jayasundera T, & Gulish A (2016). America’s Divided Recovery: College Haves and Have-Nots. Center on Education and the Workforce, Georgetown Public Policy Institute, Georgetown University. [Google Scholar]
  15. Case A, & Deaton A (2015). Rising morbidity and mortality in midlife among white non-Hispanic Americans in the 21st century. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112, 15078–15083. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1518393112 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Chapman BP, Fiscella K, Duberstein P, Coletta M, & Kawachi I (2009). Can the influence of childhood socioeconomic status on men’s and women’s adult body mass be explained by adult socioeconomic status or personality? Findings from a national sample. Health Psychology, 28(4), 419. doi: 10.1037/a0015212 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Chapman B, Fiscella K, Duberstein P, & Kawachi I (2009). Education and smoking: Confounding or effect modification by phenotypic personality traits? Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 38(3), 237–248. doi: 10.1007/s12160-009-9142-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Chen E, Miller GE, Lachman ME, Gruenewald TL, & Seeman TE (2012). Protective factors for adults from low-childhood socioeconomic circumstances: The benefits of shift-and-persist for allostatic load. Psychosomatic Medicine, 74(2), 178–186. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Cohen S, Gianaros PJ, & Manuck SB (2016). A stage model of stress and disease. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(4), 456–463. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Curtis DS, Fuller-Rowell TE, El-Sheikh M, Carnethon MR, & Ryff CD (2017). Habitual sleep as a contributor to racial differences in cardiometabolic risk. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 201618167. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Dannefer D (2018). Systemic and reflexive: Foundations of cumulative dis/advantage and life- course processes. J Gerontol B Psychol SCi Soc Sci, XX (XX), 1–15. doi: 10.1093/geron/gby118 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Donnellan MB, Conger KJ, McAdams KK, & Neppl TK (2009). Personal characteristics and resilience to economic hardship and its consequences: Conceptual issues and empirical illustrations. Journal of Personality, 77, 1645–1676. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Elder GH Jr, (1998). Children of the great depression. Westview Press. [Google Scholar]
  24. Elder GH Jr, Pavalko EK, & Hastings TJ (1991). Talent, history, and the fulfillment of promise. Psychiatry, 54(3), 251–267. http://dx.doi.org.ezproxy.library.wisc.edu/10.1080/00332747.1991.11024555 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Elliot AJ, & Chapman BP (2016). Socioeconomic status, psychological resources, and inflammatory markers: Results from the MIDUS study. Health Psychology, 35, 1205. doi: 10.1037/hea0000392 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Elliot AJ, Turiano NA, & Chapman BP (2017). Socioeconomic status interacts with conscientiousness and neuroticism to predict circulating concentrations of inflammatory markers. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 51(2), 240–250. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Ferraro KF, Schafer MH, & Wilkinson LR (2016). Childhood disadvantage and health problems in middle and later life: Early imprints on physical health? American Sociological Review, 81(1), 107–133. doi: 10.1177/0003122415619617 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Ferraro KF, and Shippee TP 2009. Aging and cumulative inequality: How does inequality get under the skin? Gerontologist 49 (3):333–43. doi: 10.1093/geront/gnp034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Friedman EM, Williams DR, Singer BH, & Ryff CD (2009). Chronic discrimination predicts higher circulating levels of E-selectin in a national sample: The MIDUS study. Brain, Behavior & Immunity, 23, 684–692. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2009.01.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Fuller-Rowell TE, Curtis DS, El-Sheikh M, Chae DH, Boylan JM, & Ryff CD (2016). Racial disparities in sleep: The role of neighborhood disadvantage. Sleep Medicine, 27-28, 1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2016.10.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Fuller-Rowell TE, Doan SN, & Eccles JS (2012). Differential effects of perceived discrimination on the diurnal cortisol rhythm of African Americans and Whites. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 37(1), 107–118. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.05.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Glei DA, Goldman N, & Weinstein M (2018). Perception has its own reality: Subjective versus objective measures of economic distress. Population and Development Review. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Goldberg LR (1992). The development of markers for the big-five factor structure. Psychological Assessment, 4, 26–42. [Google Scholar]
  34. Goldman N, Glei D & Weinstein B (2018). Declining mental health among disadvantaged Americans, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, advanced online copy, www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1722023115 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Graham C (2017). Happiness for All?: Unequal Hopes and Lives in Pursuit of the American Dream. Princeton University Press. [Google Scholar]
  36. Gruenewald TL, Karlamangla AS, Hu P, Stein-Merkin S, Crandall C, Koretz B, & Seeman TE (2012). History of socioeconomic disadvantage and allostatic load in later life. Social Science & Medicine, 74(1), 75–83. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.09.037 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Grzywacz JG, & Almeida DM (2008). Stress and binge drinking: A daily process examination of stressor pile-up and socioeconomic status in affect regulation. International Journal of Stress Management, 15(4), 364. doi: 10.1037/a0013368 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Hatch J, & Clinton A (2000). Job growth in the 1990s: a retrospect. Monthly Lab. Rev., 123, 3. [Google Scholar]
  39. Hatch SL, & Dohrenwend BP (2007). Distribution of traumatic and other stressful life events by race/ethnicity, gender, SES and age: a review of the research. American Journal of Community Psychology, 40(3-4), 313–332. doi: 10.1007/s10464-007-9134-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Hoynes H, Miller DL, & Schaller J (2012). Who suffers during recessions?. The Journal of Economic Perspectives, 26(3), 27–47. doi: 10.1257/jep.26.3.27 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  41. Johnson W, & Krueger RF (2005). Higher perceived life control decreases genetic variance in physical health: evidence from a national twin study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(1), 165–173. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.88.1.165 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Kahn JR, & Pearlin LI (2006). Financial strain over the life course and health among older adults. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 47(1), 17–31. doi: 10.1177/002214650604700102 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Kan C, Kawakami N, Karasawa M, Love GD, Coe CL, Miyamoto Y, … & Markus HR (2014). Psychological resources as mediators of the association between social class and health: Comparative findings from Japan and the USA. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 21(1), 53–65. DOI: 10.1007/s12529-012-9249-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Karlamangla AS, Friedman EM, Seeman TE, Stawksi RS, & Almeida DM (2013). Daytime trajectories of cortisol: demographic and socioeconomic differences—findings from the National Study of Daily Experiences. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 38(11), 2585–2597. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.06.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Kirsch JA, & Ryff CD (2016). Hardships of the Great Recession and health: Understanding varieties of vulnerability. Health Psychology Open, 3(1), 2055102916652390. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Kohut A, Keeter S, Doherty C, Dimock M, & Christian L (2012). Assessing the representativeness of public opinion surveys. Pew Research Center, Washington, DC: Retrieved from http://www.people-press.org/2012/05/15/assessing-the-representativeness-of-public-opinion-surveys/ [Google Scholar]
  47. Lachman ME, & Weaver SL (1998). The sense of control as a moderato of social class differences in health and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 763–773. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Lachman ME, Agrigoroaei S, Murphy C, & Tun PA (2010). Frequent cognitive activity compensates for education differences in episodic memory. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 18(1), 4–10. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181ab8b62 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Layte R (2011). The association between income inequality and mental health: Testing status anxiety, social capital, and neo-materialist explanations. European Sociological Review, 28, 498–511. [Google Scholar]
  50. Lee C, & Ryff CD (2016). Early parenthood as a link between childhood disadvantage and adult heart problems: A gender-based approach. Social Science & Medicine, 171, 58–66. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Lee C, Coe CL, & Ryff CD (2017). Social disadvantage, severe child abuse, and biological profiles in adulthood. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 0022146516685370. doi: 10.1177/0022146516685370 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Markus HR, & Kitayama S (2003). Models of agency: Sociocultural diversity in the construction of action In Murphy-Berman V & Berman JJ (Eds.), Nebraska symposium on motivation: Vol. 49. Cross-cultural differences in perspectives on self (pp. 1–57). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Marmot MG, Fuhrer R, Ettner SL, Marks NF, Bumpass LL, & Ryff CD (1998). Contribution of psychosocial factors to socioeconomic differences in health. Milbank Quarterly, 76(3), 403–448. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Marmot M (2015). The health gap: the challenge of an unequal world. The Lancet, 386, 2442–44. 10.1016/S0140-6736(15)00150-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Matthews KA, & Gallo LC (2011). Psychological perspectives on pathways linking socioeconomic status and physical health. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 501–530. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Miller GE, Lachman ME, Chen E, Gruenewald TL, Karlamangla AS, & Seeman TE (2011). Pathways to resilience: Maternal nurturance as a buffer against the effects of childhood poverty on metabolic syndrome at midlife. Psychological science, 22(12), 1591–1599. doi: 10.1177/0956797611419170 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Morozink JA, Friedman EM, Coe CL, & Ryff CD (2010). Socioeconomic and psychosocial predictors of interleukin-6 in the MIDUS national sample. Health Psychology, 29(6), 626. doi: 10.1037/a0021360 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Mroczek DK, & Kolarz CM (1998). The effect of age on positive and negative affect: A developmental perspective on happiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75,1333–1349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Ong AD, Williams DR, Nwizu U, & Gruenewald TL (2017). Everyday unfair treatment and multisystem biological dysregulation in African American adults. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 23(1), 27–35. doi: 10.1037/cdp0000087 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Owens A & Sampson RJ (2012). Community well-being and the Great Recession. Stanford, CA: Stanford Center on Poverty and Inequality. [Google Scholar]
  61. Park J, Kitayama S, Markus HR, Coe CL, Miyamoto Y, Karasawa M,… & Ryff CD (2013). Social status and anger expression: The cultural moderation hypothesis. Emotion, 13(6), 1122–1131. doi: 10.1037/a0034273 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Peck D (2010). How a new jobless era will transform America. The Atlantic, 305(2), 42–56. [Google Scholar]
  63. Power C, & Hertzman C (1997). Social and biological pathways linking early life and adult disease. British Medical Bulletin, 53(1), 210–221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Pearlin LI, Schieman S, Fazio EM, & Meersman SC (2005). Stress, health, and the life course: Some conceptual perspectives. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 46(2), 205–219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Pfeffer FT, Danziger S, & Schoeni RF (2013). Wealth disparities before and after the Great Recession. The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 650(1), 98–123. doi: 10.1177/0002716213497452 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Piketty T, & Saez E (2014). Inequality in the long run. Science, 344(6186), 838–843. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Radler BT, & Ryff CD (2010). Who participates? Accounting for longitudinal retention in the MIDUS national study of health and well-being. Journal of aging and health, 22(3), 307–331. https://doi-org.ezproxy.library.wisc.edu/10.1177/0898264309358617 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Reeves RV (2018). Dream Hoarders: How the American Upper Middle Class is Leaving Everyone Else in the Dust, why that is a Problem, and what to Do about it. Brookings Institution Press. [Google Scholar]
  69. Robinette JW, Charles ST, & Gruenewald TL (2017). Neighborhood socioeconomic status and health: a longitudinal analysis. Journal of Community Health, 42(5), 865–871. doi: 10.1007/s10900-017-0327-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Ryff CD (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological wellbeing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 57, 1069–1081. [Google Scholar]
  71. Shanahan MJ, Hill PL, Roberts BW, Eccles J, & Friedman HS (2014). Conscientiousness, health, and aging: the life course of personality model. Developmental Psychology, 50(5), 1407 10.1037/a0031130 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Shapiro T, Meschede T, & Osoro S (2013). The roots of the widening racial wealth gap: Explaining the black-white economic divide. Research and Policy Brief. [Google Scholar]
  73. Shavers VL (2007). Measurement of socioeconomic status in health disparities research. Journal of the National Medical Association, 99(9), 1013. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Sommet N, Morselli D, & Spini D (2018). Income Inequality Affects the Psychological Health of Only the People Facing Scarcity. Psychological Science, 0956797618798620. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. South SC, & Krueger RF (2011). Genetic and environmental influences on internalizing psychopathology vary as a function of economic status. Psychological Medicine, 41(1), 107. doi: 10.1017/S0033291710000279 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Stepanikova I, Bateman LB, & Oates GR (2017). Systemic inflammation in midlife: Race, socioeconomic status, and perceived discrimination. American journal of preventive medicine, 52(1), S63–S76. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2016.09.026 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Sturgeon JA, Arewasikporn A, Okun MA, Davis MC, Ong AD, & Zautra AJ (2016). The psychosocial context of financial stress: Implications for inflammation and psychological health. Psychosomatic medicine, 78(2), 134–143. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000276 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Taylor P, Morin R, & Wang W (2010). One recession, two Americas: Those who lost ground slightly outnumber those who held their own. Pew Research Center’s Social & Demographic Trends Project. Retrieved from: http://pewsocialtrends.org [Google Scholar]
  79. Taylor SE, & Seeman TE (1999). Psychosocial resources and the SES-health relationship. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 896(1), 210–225. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb08117.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Tsenkova VK, Albert MA, Georgiades A, & Ryff CD (2012). Trait anxiety and glucose metabolism in people without diabetes: vulnerabilities among black women. Diabetic medicine, 29(6), 803–806. doi: 10.1111/j.1464-5491.2011.03534.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Tsenkova V, Pudrovska T, & Karlamangla A (2014). Childhood Socioeconomic Disadvantage and Pre-diabetes and Diabetes in Later Life: A Study of Biopsychosocial Pathways. Psychosomatic medicine, 76(8), 622. doi: 10.1097/psy.0000000000000106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Turiano NA, Chapman BP, Agrigoroaei S, Infurna FJ, & Lachman M (2014). Perceived control reduces mortality risk at low, not high, education levels. Health Psychology, 33(8), 883–890. doi: 10.1037/hea0000022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Ward MM (2013). Parental educational attainment and sense of control in mid- and late-adulthood. Developmental Psychology, 49(7), 1407–1412. doi: 10.1037/a0029557 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Zilioli S, Imami L, Ong AD, Lumley MA, & Gruenewald T (2017). Discrimination and anger control as pathways linking socioeconomic disadvantage to allostatic load in midlife. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 103, 83–90. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2017.10.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Zilioli S, Imami L, & Slatcher RB (2015). Life satisfaction moderates the impact of socioeconomic status on diurnal cortisol slope. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 60, 91–95. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.06.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Zilioli S, Imami L, & Slatcher RB (2017). Socioeconomic status, perceived control, diurnal cortisol, and physical symptoms: A moderated mediation model. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 75, 36–43. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.09.025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplemental Material

RESOURCES