Abstract
Background
Frailty and cognitive impairment (CI) are associated and often coexist in older adults. Whether temporal patterns of occurrence reflect different etiologies remain unknown.
Methods
Participants from the National Health and Aging Trends Study were assessed annually (2011–2016) for frailty (Fried’s criteria) and CI (bottom quintile of clock drawing test or immediate and delayed recall; proxy-report of dementia diagnosis or AD8 ≥ 2). We used the Fine & Gray model to identify correlates of frailty onset before CI, CI onset before frailty, and frailty-CI co-occurrence, accounting for death as a competing risk.
Results
Of 3,848 free of frailty, CI, and dementia at baseline, 2,183 (61.2%) developed neither frailty nor CI during the 5-year follow-up; 343 (8.3%) developed frailty first; 1,014 (24.4%) developed CI first; and 308 (6.0%) developed frailty-CI co-occurrence. Incident dementia, as a marker of underlying neuropathologies, was associated with greater likelihood of CI onset first (subdistribution hazard ratios [SHR] = 2.60, 95% confidence interval [ci] 2.09 to 3.24), and frailty-CI co-occurrence (SHR = 8.77, 95% ci 5.79 to 13.28), but lower likelihood of frailty onset first (SHR = 0.38, 95% ci 0.21 to 0.68). Number of comorbidities was only associated with frailty occurrence first (1 comorbidity: SHR = 2.51, 95% ci 1.15 to 5.47; 4+ comorbidities: SHR = 6.48, 95% ci 2.78 to 15.48).
Conclusions
Different patterns of frailty and CI occurrence exist, and dementia-related pathologies and comorbidities may be important correlates of order of emergence, potentially reflecting different etiologies. Future investigation into relationships between these patterns and dementia subtypes and related pathologies is needed to elucidate etiologic pathways and to provide new targets for prevention, intervention, and risk screening.
Keywords: Frailty, Cognition, Dementia, Comorbidities
Frailty and cognitive impairment (CI), among the most common geriatric conditions, have contributed to growing economic, medical, and social burdens affecting older adults worldwide. CI occurs in about 16% to 20% of older adults in the United States (1) and clinically manifests as a decline in cognitive functioning in at least one cognitive domain (memory, executive functioning, attention, language, and/or visuospatial ability). It is increasingly acknowledged as a prodromal phase of many types of later life dementias (1–4). Frailty is a syndrome (5–7) distinct from comorbidity and disability (8), occurring in about 10% to 15% of community-living older adults (7,9). It is described by many as a deterioration in physiologic reserve and manifests as an inability to recover efficiently from chronic and acute stressors (5–7). Frailty and CI appear to be distinct but related conditions with shared antecedents, and associations with adverse outcomes (10,11). Unlike most dementias, frailty is potentially preventable or reversible as demonstrated by prior randomized clinical trials and cohort studies (12).
Epidemiologic and clinical studies have found that frailty is associated with poorer cognitive performance and steeper cognitive decline in older adults (10,11,13), and that the association may be stronger for executive function compared to memory (14). However, while some studies have found that greater frailty severity was predictive of cognitive decline (15–17), incident CI (18), and incident dementia, including non-Alzheimer’s Disease (AD) (19) and AD Dementia (20), other studies have found that baseline cognitive performance was associated with incident frailty (21,22). Collectively, evidence suggests that many of the aging processes catalyzing frailty may also be responsible for brain aging and cognitive decline (10,11). However, the causal mechanisms underlying this association remain unclear.
Using data from the National Health and Aging Trends Study (NHATS), a nationally-representative sample of older adult U.S. Medicare beneficiaries, we (a) described the patterns of frailty and CI onset; and (b) investigated whether patterns of occurrence in frailty and CI are associated with different baseline socio-demographic factors, disease characteristics, and health events. Characterizing the profiles of individual-level characteristics associated with the different patterns of frailty and CI occurrence could inform whether temporal ordering matters with respect to identifying distinct underlying etiologies and pathways.
Method
Study Design
Data are from NHATS, a nationally-representative sample of U.S. Medicare beneficiaries aged 65 years and older. In 2011, the cohort was drawn from the Medicare enrollment file, with a response rate of 71% (n = 8,245) (23). All participants were followed annually for a maximum of 5 years (2011–2016), and data were collected via 2-hour, in-person interviews (23). Mortality was ascertained via reports by informants during attempts to contact the participant for their annual interview. NHATS is ongoing and approved by the Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health Institutional Review Board.
Study Population
The study population was restricted to older adults residing in community-dwelling or in non-nursing home, residential care settings (n = 7,609). We further dropped 112 individuals with insufficient data on frailty (<3 of 5 measures as described below). At baseline, we excluded 444 cases with prevalent frailty (5.8%), 2,302 cases with prevalent CI or dementia (30.3%), and 903 cases with both frailty and CI or dementia (11.9%). Among those with CI at baseline (n = 3,139), with or without frailty, 1,894 (57.2%) had prevalent dementia. Our final analytic sample comprised of 3,848 community-dwelling older adults free of frailty, CI, and dementia.
Measures
Frailty
In this study, frailty was measured using the physical frailty phenotype (PFP), which was previously operationalized in NHATS (9) using validated interview and performance measures of functioning (24) and guidance from prior studies (5,6). It is based on five criteria: exhaustion, low activity, weakness, slowness, and shrinking (Table 1) (5).
Table 1.
Variables of Interest in the National Health and Aging Trends Study
Physical frailty | • Exhaustion: Self-reported low energy/easily exhausted, enough to limit activities • Low Physical Activity: Self-reported in the last month: (a) never walked for exercise and (b) never engaged in vigorous activities • Unintentional weight loss: Self-reported having lost 10+ lbs within prior year or had a BMI <18.5 kg/m2 • Slow Gait: ≤20th percentile of time to walk 3 meters within four sex-by-height categories • Weakness: ≤ 20th percentile within 8 sex-by-BMI categories |
Cognitive impairment | • Test performance in bottom quintile in at least 1 of 2 cognitive domains ○ Executive functioning (clock drawing test score <3 on a 0–5 scale) ○ Memory (summed scores of 10-item immediate and delayed recall batteries ≤ 5 • Proxy-reported doctor’s diagnosis of dementia • AD8 administered to proxy not reporting a diagnosis (score ≥ 2) |
Dementia | • Probable Dementia ○ Proxy-reported doctor’s diagnosis of dementia ○ AD8 administered to proxy not reporting a diagnosis (score ≥ 2) ○ Self- and proxy-respondents not reporting a diagnosis with test performance score of <1.5 SD below the mean in at least two domains ▪ Executive functioning: clock drawing test (score range 0–5, cutoff ≤ 1) ▪ Memory: summed scores of 10-item immediate word recall and 10-item delayed word recall batteries (score range 0–20, cutoff ≤ 3) ▪ Orientation to Date and President and Vice-President naming (score range 0–8, cutoff ≤ 3) • Possible Dementia ○ Self- and proxy-respondents not reporting a diagnosis with test performance scores of <1.5 SD below the mean in 1 domain |
Each criterion was scored as 0 or 1 representing the absence or presence of the component, respectively. Consistent with recommended practice, participants were scored “0” for a criterion if they were not tested because of safety concerns, if they were ineligible due to recent surgery or pain, or if they were unable to complete a task (25,26). Criteria were summed to create a score ranging from 0 to 5; participants were defined as frail if they had a score of 3 or higher.
Cognitive impairment
CI was assessed in two ways: cognitive performance testing or by proxy-reports (Table 1). Participants were classified with CI if at least one of the three criteria were met: (a) scored in the bottom quintile in at least one of the two cognitive domains: executive functioning (scoring <3 on a clock drawing test with a 0–5 scale, where lower values indicate poorer cognitive scores) and memory (scoring ≤ 5 on a summed score of the 10-item immediate and delayed recall batteries, where lower values indicating poorer cognitive scores); (b) self- or proxy-reported doctor’s diagnosis of dementia; or (c) scored ≥ 2 on the AD8, an 8-item instrument administered to proxy respondents not reporting a diagnosis, which assesses memory, temporal orientation, judgment, and function (27). Self-respondents who refused a test, answered “do not know,” or were unable to do a test were scored as 0 to indicate CI. A majority were self-respondents (98.8%).
Dementia
Dementia status was considered as a potential marker for the latent period of disease that manifests after decades of underlying progression (2). Dementia was treated as time-independent covariate that was binary coded as 1 if dementia onset was observed anytime during the follow-up, and 0 otherwise. The purpose of including dementia was to distinguish neurodegenerative pathologies from others underlying CI, based on the fact that the majority of dementia cases are AD-dementia, using the same logic from the definition of “cognitive frailty” given by the International Academy on Nutrition and Aging (IANA) and the International Association of Gerontology and Geriatrics (IAGG) in 2013, which requires the absence of dementia to try to exclude neurodegenerative etiologies (28). Participants were classified into three groups—probable dementia, possible dementia, and no dementia based on previously defined criteria in NHATS (29) (Table 1). “Probable dementia” was defined as any one of the three criteria derived from cognitive performance testing and self- or proxy- reports: (a) self- or proxy-report of doctor’s diagnosis of dementia or AD, (b) a score of ≥ 2 on the AD8 administered to proxy-respondents not reporting a diagnosis, or (c) self- and proxy-respondents not reporting a diagnosis with test performance scores > 1.5 SD below the mean in at least two of three domains (memory, orientation, and executive functioning). “Possible dementia” was defined for self- and proxy-respondents not reporting a diagnosis with test performance scores > 1.5 SD below the mean in one domain. Self-respondents who refused a test, were unable to do a test, or answered “do not know,” were scored 0.
Participant characteristics
Demographic factors including age, sex, race/ethnicity, highest level of educational attainment, residence, and annual household income were considered (30). History of medical conditions was self-reported, which has been shown to be reasonably accurate against medical records (31); participants were asked whether a doctor had ever told them they had: arthritis, diabetes, heart disease, high blood pressure, lung disease, osteoporosis, and stroke. Healthcare utilization, including ever having an overnight hospitalization stay or surgery (back, heart, knee, hip) in the past year was also based on self-reports. Subjective well-being (32), depressive symptoms (33–35), activities of daily living (ADLs) (24), and instrumental activities of daily living (IADLs) (24) were also considered. Additionally, measures of gait speed, memory function, and executive function at baseline were also considered as markers for distinct underlying latent disease processes, such as Alzheimer’s Disease and Vascular Dementia (36,37).
Statistical analysis
Nationally-representative descriptive statistics were generated by different individual-level patterns of onset: (a) frailty onset 1 year or more before CI, (b) CI onset 1 year or more before frailty, (c) CI-frailty co-occurrence (within the same year), and (d) neither CI nor frailty anytime during follow-up.
We used the Fine & Gray competing risks model (38) to estimate subdistribution hazard ratios (SHR) and identify correlates of the patterns of onset, namely: frailty onset before CI, CI onset before frailty, or CI-frailty co-occurrence. The model treated these patterns as mutually exclusive outcomes while accounting for death and other respective patterns of occurrence as competing risks (38). Follow-up time was calculated as time (in years) since baseline. In Model I, we only included baseline age, sex, race, income, education, and residence. To better control for age, we used natural splines with knots at 75, 85, and 95 years. In Model II, we explored all correlates included in Model I, as well as ADLs, depression, well-being, number of comorbidities, slow gait (<0.8 m/s), history of hospitalization in the past year, history of surgery in the past year, current smoking status, clock drawing test score, and immediate and delayed word recall sum score measured at baseline, as well as incidence of dementia anytime during follow-up. In Model III, we explored all above-mentioned correlates, except instead of number of comorbidities, we incorporated history of specific medical conditions. NHATS sampling weights of the sample design were incorporated in all models (39).
We conducted a series of sensitivity analyses to determine whether coefficients remained similarly associated with the different patterns of occurrence. First, we excluded individuals with incident probable and possible dementia, and secondly, we excluded individuals who were severely depressed (score PHQ-2 score of ≥5); by conducting these analyses, we aimed to assess whether associations remained robust independently of related etiologies. Thirdly, we conducted analyses stratified by white and black race to assess whether associations remained the same by race; associations among Hispanics were not examined due to insufficient power. All analyses were performed using Stata version 14.0.
Results
Patterns of Frailty and CI Occurrence
Of 7,497 community-dwelling participants, 3,848 (59.4%) were free of frailty and CI/dementia at baseline with a median follow-up of 5 years (IQR = 4). Among those without frailty and CI/dementia at baseline, 2,183 (61.2%) developed neither during the 5-year follow-up, of which 167 (6.2%) died during follow-up before any of the outcomes could occur; 343 (8.3%) developed frailty first, of which 58 (15.8%) developed subsequent CI; 1,014 (24.4%) developed CI first, of which 84 (7.4%) developed subsequent frailty; and 308 (6.0%) developed frailty-CI co-occurrence (Table 2). A majority of individuals for whom CI and frailty co-occurred had developed dementia sometime during the follow-up period (63.0%), and over a third (35.0%) of those who developed CI first developed dementia sometime during the follow-up period. However, only 9.1% of those who developed frailty first developed dementia sometime during the follow-up period (Table 2).
Table 2.
Nationally-Representative Estimates of Baseline Population Characteristics by Order of Cognitive Impairment (CI) or Frailty Onset (n = 3,848)a
Overall 3,848 (100) |
Neither CI nor Frailty 2,183 (61.2) |
Incident CI Before Frailty 1,014 (24.4) |
Incident Frailty Before CI 343 (8.3) |
CI-Frailty Co-occurrence 308 (6.0) |
|
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age | 73.2 (6.8) | 72.2 (6.2) | 74.3 (6.9) | 74.9 (6.8) | 77.7 (7.2) |
Female | 2,178 (55.5) | 1,210 (54.1) | 555 (53.9) | 228 (66.6) | 185 (60.1) |
Race | |||||
White, non-Hispanic | 2,963 (86.9) | 1,722 (88.3) | 755 (84.4) | 273 (87.7) | 213 (81.7) |
Black, non-Hispanic | 638 (5.9) | 332 (5.3) | 183 (6.7) | 51 (5.8) | 72 (9.8) |
Hispanic | 144 (4.2) | 66 (3.4) | 51 (6.0) | 11 (4.1) | 16 (6.0) |
Income | |||||
1st quartile | 719 (17.7) | 386 (15.2) | 232 (20.0) | 69 (19.9) | 105 (30.4) |
4th quartile | 974 (29.9) | 617 (32.9) | 237 (27.1) | 67 (21.9) | 53 (21.6) |
Incident Dementia | 619 (13.4) | 11 (0.5) | 378 (35.0) | 34 (9.1) | 196 (63.0) |
Depression | 350 (8.6) | 158 (6.6) | 93 (9.2) | 48 (15.0) | 51 (18.3) |
Activities of daily living | |||||
Moderate disability | 737 (18.2) | 200 (15.0) | 347 (19.3) | 100 (31.2) | 90 (27.9) |
Severe disability | 207 (4.5) | 40 (3.1) | 80 (3.5) | 36 (10.0) | 51 (15.7) |
Current smoker | 318 (16.0) | 189 (16.4) | 64 (12.0) | 39 (21.5) | 26 (20.3) |
Gait speed < 0.8 m/s | 1,845 (42.5) | 930 (37.9) | 499 (43.3) | 212 (61.0) | 204 (61.3) |
History of surgery | 1,438 (37.1) | 741 (33.9) | 375 (37.3) | 172 (50.4) | 150 (51.4) |
Hospitalization | 620 (14.9) | 290 (12.5) | 172 (15.9) | 84 (23.8) | 74 (;22.7) |
Number of comorbidities | |||||
0 | 759 (21.3) | 516 (25.1) | 187 (19.5) | 25 (7.0) | 31 (10.1) |
1 | 1,251 (32.8) | 743 (34.2) | 350 (34.7) | 83 (24.0) | 75 (23.6) |
2 | 1,040 (26.0) | 551 (24.5) | 282 (26.5) | 119 (34.5) | 88 (27.9) |
3 | 519 (13.1) | 259 (11.6) | 126 (12.5) | 61 (18.1) | 73 (23.5) |
4 or more | 279 (6.8) | 114 (5.5) | 69 (6.7) | 55 (16.4) | 41 (15.0) |
History of cancer | 1,034 (26.5) | 558 (25.1) | 272 (27.3) | 107 (29.7) | 97 (33.1) |
History of hip fracture | 117 (2.5) | 41 (1.5) | 28 (3.5) | 14 (3.0) | 24 (7.6) |
History of heart disease | 586 (14.6) | 300 (13.4) | 143 (13.5) | 74 (21.5) | 69 (21.9) |
History of high blood pressure | 2,495 (61.6) | 1,258 (59.2) | 651 (61.0) | 253 (72.4) | 233 (73.4) |
History of arthritis | 1,967 (49.9) | 1,014 (45.5) | 508 (49.3) | 247 (73.5) | 198 (65.4) |
History of osteoporosis | 735 (19.5) | 380 (17.7) | 192 (19.8) | 94 (29.6) | 69 (23.3) |
History of diabetes | 828 (19.9) | 410 (17.0) | 228 (21.9) | 102 (29.7) | 88 (28.4) |
History of lung disease | 537 (13.9) | 290 (13.0) | 120 (11.8) | 73 (22.6) | 54 (18.7) |
History of stroke | 288 (6.6) | 114 (4.6) | 85 (7.4) | 40 (11.4) | 49 (16.1) |
Subjective well-being (range 0–22) | 17.9 (2.9) | 18.0 (2.8) | 17.9 (2.8) | 16.9 (3.3) | 17.3 (3.3) |
Years of follow-up | 3.4 (2.0) | 2.8 (2.2) | 4.3 (1.4) | 4.3 (1.2) | 4.4 (1.1) |
Note: aRaw numbers and weighted percentages (%) for categorical characteristics, as well as weighted means and raw standard deviations (SD) for continuous characteristics are presented, except for years of follow-up, where median and interquartile range is provided.
Determinants of Distinct Patterns of Frailty and CI Occurrence
In Model I, females were at greater risk of developing frailty first (SHR = 1.52, 95% confidence interval [ci] 1.19 to 1.95), but had lower risk of developing CI first (SHR = 0.85, 95% ci 0.74 to 0.98). Both Hispanics (SHR = 1.64, 95% ci 1.21 to 2.21) and blacks (SHR = 1.33, 95% ci 1.12 to 1.60) were at higher risk of developing CI before frailty than whites; blacks were also at higher risk of CI-frailty co-occurrence than whites (SHR = 1.95, 95% ci 1.43 to 2.67). Higher educational attainment had a dose-response, protective association with developing CI first and CI-frailty co-occurrence. Income and residence were not strongly associated with any patterns (Table 3).
Table 3.
Model I: Demographic Correlates of Different Patterns of Incident Frailty-Cognitive Impairment (CI) Occurrence in Community-Dwelling Older Adults (n = 3,848)a
Outcome 1: CI first Competing Risks: Frailty First, Co-occurrence, Death | Outcome 2: Frailty first Competing Risks: CI First, Co-occurrence, Death | Outcome 3: Co-occurrence Competing Risks: CI First, Frailty First, Death | |
---|---|---|---|
Baseline age (knots at 75, 85, and 95 y) | |||
Age spline covariate 1 | 1.028 (1.013, 1.042)** | 1.044 (1.019, 1.070)** | 1.108 (1.076, 1.141)** |
Age spline covariate 2 | 0.976 (0.935, 1.019) | 0.948 (0.876, 1.026) | 0.950 (0.891, 1.013) |
Female | 0.848 (0.738, 0.975)** | 1.519 (1.185, 1.948)** | 1.009 (0.773, 1.316) |
Race | |||
White | Ref | Ref | Ref |
Black | 1.333 (1.120, 1.587)** | 1.078 (0.778, 1.492) | 1.954 (1.431, 2.667)** |
Hispanic | 1.636 (1.210, 2.213)** | 1.054 (0.531, 2.091) | 1.333 (0.742, 2.394) |
Other minority | 1.423 (0.942, 2.150) | 1.175 (0.530, 2.601) | 1.358 (0.590, 3.126) |
Education | |||
8th Grade or Less | Ref | Ref | Ref |
9th–12th Grade (no diploma) | 0.914 (0.659, 1.267) | 0.872 (0.452, 1.683) | 0.705 (0.423, 1.175) |
High School Diploma or Equivalent | 0.895 (0.670, 1.196) | 1.138 (0.630, 2.057) | 0.499 (0.317, 0.785)** |
Some College but no Degree | 0.757 (0.562, 1.020) | 0.876 (0.477, 1.608) | 0.583 (0.365, 0.931)** |
Associates or Bachelor’s Degree | 0.693 (0.505, 0.952)** | 0.776 (0.403, 1.492) | 0.343 (0.199, 0.590)** |
Graduate Degree | 0.507 (0.356, 0.722)** | 0.684 (0.338, 1.381) | 0.353 (0.188, 0.665)** |
Residence (Residential Care, non-nursing home vs. Community) | 0.828 (0.546, 1.255) | 0.623 (0.310, 1.256) | 1.199 (0.668, 2.152) |
Income | 0.961 (0.894, 1.033) | 0.963 (0.846, 1.095) | 0.870 (0.751, 1.008) |
Note: aSubdistribution hazard ratios (95% confidence intervals) are presented from a Fine & Gray competing risks model, which treated each of the patterns of incident frailty/CI occurrence as mutually exclusive outcomes, accounting for mortality and other respective patterns of occurrence, where ** p < .05.
In Model II, being female remained strongly associated with developing frailty first (SHR = 1.45, 95% ci 1.05 to 2.03); however, was no longer associated with developing CI first (SHR = 1.00, 95% ci 0.82 to 1.21). Race and education lost strong significance; however, higher income was associated with a lower risk of developing CI-frailty co-occurrence (SHR = 0.73, 95% ci 0.59 to 0.89). Current smokers had greater risk of developing frailty first (SHR = 1.98, 95% ci 1.30 to 2.99) and CI-frailty co-occurrence (SHR = 1.99, 1.25 to 3.18), and borderline lower risk of developing CI first (SHR = 0.74, 95% ci 0.55 to 1.01). Interestingly, persons who developed possible or probable dementia by the end of follow-up were at 2.60 times greater risk of developing CI before frailty (95% ci 2.09 to 3.24) and were at 8.77 times greater risk of CI-frailty co-occurrence compared to those without dementia (95% ci 5.79 to 13.28). Conversely, those with incident dementia were about 62% less likely to develop frailty before CI (SHR = 0.38, 95% ci 0.21 to 0.68). Additionally, number of comorbidities was positively associated with frailty occurrence before CI, but neither CI before frailty nor CI-frailty co-occurrence; specifically, those with one comorbidity were at 2.51 times greater risk (SHR = 2.51, 95% ci 1.15 to 5.47), and those with 4+ comorbidities were at 6.48 times greater risk of developing frailty first (SHR = 6.48, 95% ci 2.71 to 15.48) (Table 4).
Table 4.
Model II: Health Correlates of Different Patterns of Incident Frailty-Cognitive Impairment (CI) Occurrence in Community-dwelling Older Adults (n = 3,848)a
Outcome 1: CI first Competing Risks: Frailty first, Co-occurrence, Death | Outcome 2: Frailty first Competing Risks: CI first, Co-occurrence, Death | Outcome 3: Co-occurrence Competing Risks: CI first, Frailty first, Death | |
---|---|---|---|
Incident dementiab | 2.604 (2.094, 3.240)** | 0.375 (0.208, 0.677)** | 8.770 (5.794, 13.275)** |
Memory (Immediate & Delayed Word Recall Sum Score) | 0.906 (0.865, 0.950)** | 0.997 (0.930, 1.069) | 0.970 (0.885, 1.063) |
Executive functioning (Clock Drawing Score) | 0.766 (0.665, 0.882)** | 1.535 (1.204, 1.957)** | 0.893 (0.693, 1.151) |
Ever had surgery | 0.914 (0.752, 1.111) | 1.210 (0.873, 1.675) | 1.441 (1.002, 2.071)** |
Current smoker | 0.742 (0.547, 1.006) | 1.974 (1.304, 2.988)** | 1.992 (1.248, 3.179)** |
Depression | 0.979 (0.687, 1.395) | 0.976 (0.593, 1.607) | 1.443 (0.902, 2.309) |
ADL | |||
No disability | Ref | Ref | Ref |
Moderate disability | 0.996 (0.782, 1.268) | 1.766 (1.235, 2.525)** | 1.309 (0.828, 2.067) |
Severe disability | 0.642 (0.380, 1.084) | 1.527 (0.806, 2.895) | 2.231 (1.267, 3.928)** |
Gait Speed ≤ 0.8 | 0.889 (0.734, 1.077) | 1.275 (0.892, 1.824) | 1.158 (0.801, 1.673) |
History of hospitalization | 1.004 (0.769, 1.311) | 1.291 (0.900, 1.853) | 0.725 (0.437, 1.204) |
Number of comorbidities | |||
0 | Ref | Ref | Ref |
1 | 0.914 (0.688, 1.215) | 2.508 (1.151, 5.468)** | 0.990 (0.535, 1.834) |
2 | 0.889 (0.665, 1.188) | 4.575 (2.116, 9.890)** | 1.094 (0.595, 2.011) |
3 | 1.002 (0.710, 1.414) | 2.390 (0.994, 5.747) | 1.616 (0.851, 3.070) |
4 or more | 0.746 (0.474, 1.175) | 6.480 (2.712, 15.481)** | 1.409 (0.675, 2.939) |
Subjective well-being | 1.006 (0.970, 1.042) | 0.935 (0.883, 0.990)** | 0.991 (0.933, 1.052) |
Notes: ADL = activities of daily living.
aSubdistribution hazard ratios (95% confidence intervals) are presented from a Fine & Gray competing risks model, which treated each of the patterns of incident frailty/CI occurrence as mutually exclusive outcomes, accounting for mortality and other respective patterns of occurrence, where ** p < .05. Model is adjusted for age, sex, race, education, income, and residence.
bIncident dementia comprises of both “Probable” and “Possible” dementia developed anytime during the follow-up period after baseline (2012–2016).
In Model III, being female was no longer associated with elevated risk of developing frailty first (SHR = 1.42, 95% ci 0.98 to 2.04). As in Model II, race and education were not strongly associated with any patterns of occurrence, though higher income was associated with a lower risk of CI-frailty co-occurrence (SHR = 0.74, 95% ci 0.60 to 0.90). Consistent throughout Model III, incident dementia was strongly associated with higher risk of developing CI first (SHR = 2.62, 95% ci 2.10 to 3.25), and CI-frailty co-occurrence (SHR = 9.00, 95% ci 5.98 to 13.54), and was strongly associated with lower risk of developing frailty first (SHR = 0.37, 95% ci 0.20 to 0.67) (Table 5). A history of some conditions was associated with frailty first, including arthritis (SHR = 1.86, 95% ci 1.28 to 1.70), diabetes (SHR = 1.46, 95% ci 1.01 to 2.13), and lung disease (SHR=1.43, 95% ci 0.96 to 2.11); none were associated with CI first or CI-frailty co-occurrence (Table 5).
Table 5.
Model III: Health Correlates of Different Patterns of Incident Frailty-Cognitive Impairment (CI) Occurrence by Specific Comorbidities (n = 3,848)a
Outcome 1: CI first Competing Risks: Frailty first, Co-occurrence, Death | Outcome 2: Frailty first Competing Risks: CI first, Co-occurrence, Death | Outcome 3: Co-occurrence Competing Risks: CI first, Frailty first, Death | |
---|---|---|---|
Incident dementia | 2.616 (2.103, 3.254)** | 0.366 (0.202, 0.665)** | 8.997 (5.979, 13.538)** |
Memory (Immediate & Delayed Word Recall Sum Score) | 0.909 (0.868, 0.952)** | 1.000 (0.932, 1.074) | 0.963 (0.878, 1.056) |
Executive functioning (Clock Drawing Score) | 0.775 (0.673, 0.892)** | 1.506 (1.174, 1.932)** | 0.900 (0.685, 1.183) |
Ever had surgery | 0.931 (0.758, 1.142) | 1.179 (0.833, 1.668) | 1.422 (0.982, 2.060) |
Current smoker | 0.744 (0.550, 1.007) | 2.115 (1.371, 3.262)** | 1.867 (1.144, 3.049)** |
Depression | 0.969 (0.673, 1.394) | 0.988 (0.590, 1.656) | 1.350 (0.805, 2.266) |
ADL | |||
No disability | Ref | Ref | Ref |
Moderate disability | 0.990 (0.777, 1.262) | 1.713 (1.188, 2.468)** | 1.312 (0.830, 2.074) |
Severe disability | 0.604 (0.353, 1.034) | 1.329 (0.687, 2.571) | 2.450 (1.373, 4.372)** |
Gait Speed ≤ 0.8 | 0.891 (0.734, 1.083) | 1.269 (0.884, 1.820) | 1.164 (0.805, 1.681) |
History of hospitalization | 1.033 (0.791, 1.350) | 1.303 (0.888, 1.914) | 0.683 (0.411, 1.136) |
History of cancer | 1.114 (0.906, 1.369) | 1.104 (0.772, 1.579) | 1.270 (0.880, 1.832) |
History of hip fracture | 1.431 (0.861, 2.380) | 0.830 (0.325, 2.118) | 1.712 (0.786, 3.729) |
History of heart disease | 0.848 (0.646, 1.113) | 1.074 (0.702, 1.642) | 1.093 (0.690, 1.730) |
History of high blood pressure | 0.858 (0.705, 1.044) | 1.354 (0.939, 1.953) | 1.047 (0.706, 1.554) |
History of arthritis | 0.946 (0.804, 1.113) | 1.859 (1.279, 2.702)** | 0.997 (0.691, 1.439) |
History of osteoporosis | 1.122 (0.861, 1.463) | 1.264 (0.849, 1.882) | 0.892 (0.551, 1.444) |
History of diabetes | 0.908 (0.718, 1.147) | 1.463 (1.006, 2.127)** | 0.924 (0.614, 1.390) |
History of lung disease | 0.911 (0.701, 1.183) | 1.426 (0.963, 2.111) | 1.246 (0.780, 1.990) |
History of stroke | 1.035 (0.731, 1.464) | 1.274 (0.785, 2.068) | 1.481 (0.861, 2.550) |
Subjective well-being | 1.004 (0.968, 1.041) | 0.943 (0.890, 1.000)** | 0.992 (0.934, 1.053) |
Notes: ADL = activities of daily living.
aSubdistribution hazard ratios (95% confidence intervals) are presented from a Fine & Gray competing risks model, which treated each of the patterns of incident frailty/CI occurrence as mutually exclusive outcomes, accounting for mortality and other respective patterns of occurrence, where ** p < .05. Model is adjusted for age, sex, race, education, income, and residence.
bIncident dementia comprises of both “Probable” and “Possible” dementia developed anytime during the follow-up period after baseline (2012–2016).
Sensitivity Analyses
Across sensitivity analyses, inferences remained consistent for associations related to incident dementia and number of comorbidities. Specifically, number of comorbidities remained strongly and positively associated with developing frailty first when we excluded 619 participants with possible and probable dementia (1 comorbidity: SHR=2.88, 95% ci 1.18 to 7.00; 4+ comorbidities: SHR=8.35, 95% ci 2.11 to 22.44) or when we excluded 350 participants with severe depressive symptoms (1 comorbidity: SHR = 2.35, 95% ci 1.06 to 5.20; 4+ comorbidities: SHR = 6.48, 95% ci 2.66 to 15.80). Among participants free of severe depressive symptoms, those with incident dementia had a higher risk of developing CI first (SHR = 2.79, 95% ci 2.21 to 3.51) and an even greater risk of developing CI-frail co-occurrence (SHR = 9.14, 95% ci 5.77 to 15.49), but had a reduced risk of developing frailty first (SHR = 0.46, 95% ci 0.25 to 0.85). Similarly, direction and magnitude of associations for number of comorbidities and incident dementia were robust after stratification by white and black race. Despite direction and magnitudes remaining similar among blacks, due to smaller sample sizes, the association between dementia and frailty before CI was not statistically significant, nor was the association between number of comorbidities and developing frailty first among blacks.
Discussion
In this study, different patterns of frailty and CI occurrence were documented and found to be associated with different correlates. Of note, results related to incident dementia and comorbidities with regard to patterns were consistent. Specifically, incident dementia anytime during the follow-up period, as a surrogate of underlying neurodegenerative pathologies, was strongly associated in all models with a high risk of developing CI before frailty (SHR = 2.60 95% ci 2.09 to 3.24), and an even higher risk of CI-frailty co-occurrence (SHR = 8.77 95% ci 5.79 to 13.28), as well as a reduced risk of developing frailty before CI (SHR = 0.38, 95% ci 0.21 to 0.68). Comorbidities were consistently associated with developing frailty first, but not CI first or CI-frailty co-occurrence.
Our findings suggest that order of occurrence may indicate varying underlying etiologic processes with distinct pathological trajectories. Dementia is a syndrome that develops over decades and manifests with severe cognitive deficits across multiple cognitive domains, ultimately affecting daily function (40). AD is recognized as the most common type of dementia among older adults in the United States (41–43), followed by vascular dementia (44), though growing evidence suggests that individuals are likely to have mixed pathologies (44,45). In this study, CI-frailty co-occurrence was the pattern most strongly associated with incident dementia. It is possible that these participants were already on the dementia pathway at the time co-occurring CI and frailty were observed. Similarly, CI before frailty occurrence demonstrated a positive association with incident dementia, although to a lesser degree. Unlike with other patterns of frailty-CI occurrence, participants with incident dementia anytime during the study period had a reduced risk of frailty onset before CI. We hypothesize that dementia and other age-associated pathologies are important influences on the emergence of frailty when CI co-occurs or precedes frailty. We further hypothesize that frailty onset before CI represents a pathway that is distinct from other onset patterns with etiologies that may be vascular or inflammatory in nature given its relation to history of arthritis, diabetes, and lung disease. Furthermore, CI that develops subsequent to frailty may differ in terms of etiology from CI in other patterns.
There are several notable limitations to this study. We would be remiss not to mention the potential biases introduced by measurement error. However, the non-trivial sample sizes for all three groups makes it unlikely that the different patterns of associations were primarily driven by measurement error. Additionally, using cognitive performance measures to screen for impairment is particularly challenging. Measures with higher sensitivity to impairment (eg, Mini-Mental State Examination) are not designed to discriminate well at higher levels of cognitive function. The clock drawing test was selected in NHATS for its wide-spread use as screening tools for dementia, and compared to the Mini-Mental State Examination, the clock drawing test is known to have less education bias and language barrier (46,47), greater sensitivity to AD-related cognitive decline, and is better able to identify executive dysfunction among people with normal Mini-Mental State Examination (48). Therefore, it is suitable for a variety of evaluation settings where speed and ease of assessment are essential in this large, heterogeneous sample. Additionally, NHATS lacks any biomarker data or data on dementia sub-types and related pathologies, both of which would help assess underlying mechanisms associated with different patterns of frailty-CI onset. Furthermore, issues regarding overlapping operational definitions of CI and dementia (ie, endogeneity) in analyses examining both dementia and CI, could be a notable limitation. However, our goal was not to model dementia relative to CI as a time-dependent outcome; instead, dementia was treated as time-independent covariate, whose purpose was to distinguish neurodegenerative pathologies from others underlying CI, using the same logic from the definition of cognitive frailty given by IAGG/IANA, which requires the absence of dementia to try to exclude neurodegenerative etiologies (28). We are aware of the difficulty of “pinpointing” etiology without the use of diagnostic neuroimaging tools (volume, default mode network) or genetic markers (eg, ApoE4), which would be cost-prohibitive in large survey studies such as NHATS. To address this issue by taking full advantage of all available information, we used a “temporal criterion” as recently recommended in the definition of “cognitive frailty” (49). The validity of the “temporal criterion” hinges on the assumption that the order by which physical and CIs manifest clinically matches their underlying sequence of pathological development, which may or may not be true.
Notwithstanding these limitations, this study provides useful etiological insights for the next generation of research to inform future, targeted use of more costly imaging methods. Several strengths support the validity of these findings. The use of this heterogeneous, nationally-representative sample of U.S. Medicare beneficiaries aged 65 years and older provides sufficient power and supports generalizability of these findings to older adults nationwide. Secondly, NHATS provided up to 5 years of longitudinal data, providing sufficient time to assess temporal ordering of frailty and CI occurrence.
Recently, with increased attention on the frailty syndrome and its relationship to cognitive aging, consensus papers have suggested expanding the definition of frailty to include cognition (28). This novel construct, “cognitive frailty,” represents the presence of both frailty and CI in the absence of dementia (13,28), and was proposed with the goal of identifying individuals with “reduced cognitive reserve” that is a potentially reversible consequence of frailty rather than the result of neurodegenerative disorders (28). Growing evidence suggests that frailty may be preventable and modifiable as indicated by prior randomized clinical trials and cohort studies (7,12), which provides a promising avenue for developing better-targeted interventions to prevent CI related to reduced cognitive reserve caused by frailty. We hypothesize that unlike those who develop CI before frailty or CI-frailty co-occurrence, persons who develop frailty before CI represent this “cognitive frailty” group who might benefit most from interventions designed to target CI caused by reduced physiologic reserve. Whether this hypothesis holds or not, our findings emphasize the importance of assessing temporal ordering of frailty and CI in the study of causal mechanisms, improving upon the cross-sectional definition of cognitive frailty, and validating it as a potentially useful clinical entity (50), as well as highlighting the potential value in targeting interventions to improve care in older adults.
Funding
This work was supported by the National Institute on Aging at the National Institutes of Health (R03AG053743 to Q.X., T32AG000247 to N.M.C., K01AG050699 to A.L.G., P30AG021334 to K.B.R. and J.D.W., and P50AG005146 to K.B.R.).
Conflict of interest statement None declared.
Acknowledgments
N.M.C. participated in data analysis, interpretation, drafting, critical revision, and approval of the article. K.B.R. participated in concept design, interpretation, critical revision, and approval of the article. J.T. participated in data analysis and critical revision of the article. A.L.G. participated in critical revision and approval of the article. J.K. participated in data collection, critical revision, and approval of the article. M.C. participated in concept design, critical revision, and approval of the article. Q.X. participated in concept design, interpretation, securing funding, critical revision, and approval of the article.
References
- 1. Roberts R, Knopman DS. Classification and epidemiology of MCI. Clin Geriatr Med. 2013;29:753–772. doi: 10.1016/j.cger.2013.07.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Sperling RA, Aisen PS, Beckett LA, et al. Toward defining the preclinical stages of Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7(3):280–292. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.003. Epub 2011 Apr 21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Petersen RC. Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. J Intern Med. 2004;256:183–194. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01388.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Meyer JS, Xu G, Thornby J, Chowdhury MH, Quach M. Is mild cognitive impairment prodromal for vascular dementia like Alzheimer’s disease? Stroke. 2002;33:1981–1985. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Fried LP, Tangen CM, Walston J, et al. ; Cardiovascular Health Study Collaborative Research Group Frailty in older adults: evidence for a phenotype. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2001;56:M146–M156. doi: 10.1093/gerona/56.3.m146 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Bandeen-Roche K, Xue QL, Ferrucci L, et al. Phenotype of frailty: characterization in the women’s health and aging studies. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2006;61:262–266. doi: 10.1093/gerona/61.3.262 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Xue QL. The frailty syndrome: definition and natural history. Clin Geriatr Med. 2011;27:1–15. doi: 10.1016/j.cger.2010.08.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Fried LP, Ferrucci L, Darer J, Williamson JD, Anderson G. Untangling the concepts of disability, frailty, and comorbidity: implications for improved targeting and care. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2004;59:255–263. doi: 10.1093/gerona/59.3.m255 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Bandeen-Roche K, Seplaki CL, Huang J, et al. Frailty in older adults: a nationally representative profile in the united states. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2015;70:1427–1434. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glv133 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Robertson DA, Savva GM, Kenny RA. Frailty and cognitive impairment–a review of the evidence and causal mechanisms. Ageing Res Rev. 2013;12:840–851. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2013.06.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Canevelli M, Cesari M, van Kan GA. Frailty and cognitive decline: how do they relate? Curr Opin Clin Nutr Metab Care. 2015;18:43–50. doi: 10.1097/MCO.0000000000000133 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Puts MTE, Toubasi S, Andrew MK, et al. Interventions to prevent or reduce the level of frailty in community-dwelling older adults: a scoping review of the literature and international policies. Age Ageing. 2017;46:383–392. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afw247 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Panza F, Solfrizzi V, Barulli MR, et al. Cognitive frailty: a systematic review of epidemiological and neurobiological evidence of an age-related clinical condition. Rejuvenation Res. 2015;18:389–412. doi: 10.1089/rej.2014.1637 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Gross AL, Xue QL, Bandeen-Roche K, et al. Declines and impairment in executive function predict onset of physical frailty. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2016;71:1624–1630. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glw067 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Auyeung TW, Lee JS, Kwok T, Woo J. Physical frailty predicts future cognitive decline - a four-year prospective study in 2737 cognitively normal older adults. J Nutr Health Aging. 2011;15:690–694. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Samper-Ternent R, Al Snih S, Raji MA, Markides KS, Ottenbacher KJ. Relationship between frailty and cognitive decline in older Mexican Americans. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2008;56:1845–1852. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2008.01947.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Mitnitski A, Fallah N, Rockwood MR, Rockwood K. Transitions in cognitive status in relation to frailty in older adults: a comparison of three frailty measures. J Nutr Health Aging. 2011;15:863–867. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Boyle PA, Buchman AS, Wilson RS, Leurgans SE, Bennett DA. Physical frailty is associated with incident mild cognitive impairment in community-based older persons. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2010;58(2):248–255. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2009.02671.x. Epub 2010 Jan 8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Gray SL, Anderson ML, Hubbard RA, et al. Frailty and incident dementia. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2013;68:1083–1090. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glt013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Buchman AS, Boyle PA, Wilson RS, Tang Y, Bennett DA. Frailty is associated with incident Alzheimer’s disease and cognitive decline in the elderly. Psychosom Med. 2007;69:483–489. doi: 10.1097/psy.0b013e318068de1d [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Raji MA, Al Snih S, Ostir GV, Markides KS, Ottenbacher KJ. Cognitive status and future risk of frailty in older Mexican Americans. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2010;65:1228–1234. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glq121 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Doba N, Tokuda Y, Goldstein NE, Kushiro T, Hinohara S. A pilot trial to predict frailty syndrome: the Japanese Health Research Volunteer Study. Exp Gerontol. 2012;47:638–643. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2012.05.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Kasper JD, Freedman VA. National Health and Aging Trends Study User Guide: Rounds 1, 2, 3 & 4 Final Release. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Bloomberg School of Public Health 2015. www.NHATS.org.
- 24. Freedman VA, Kasper JD, Cornman JC, et al. Validation of new measures of disability and functioning in the National Health and Aging Trends Study. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2011;66:1013–1021. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glr087 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Kasper JD, Freedman VA, Niefeld MR. Construction of performance-based summary measures of physical capacity in the National Health and Aging Trends Study. NHATS Technical Paper #4. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Bloomberg School of Public Health 2012. www.NHATS.org.
- 26. Guralnik JM, Simonsick EM, Ferrucci L, et al. A short physical performance battery assessing lower extremity function: association with self-reported disability and prediction of mortality and nursing home admission. J Gerontol. 1994;49:M85–M94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Galvin JE, Roe CM, Powlishta KK, et al. The AD8: a brief informant interview to detect dementia. Neurology. 2005;65:559–564. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000172958.95282.2a [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Kelaiditi E, Cesari M, Canevelli M, et al. ; IANA/IAGG Cognitive frailty: rational and definition from an (I.A.N.A./I.A.G.G.) international consensus group. J Nutr Health Aging. 2013;17:726–734. doi: 10.1007/s12603-013-0367-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Kasper JD, Freedman VA, Spillman B.. Classification of Persons by Dementia Status in the National Health and Aging Trends Study. Technical Paper #5. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Bloomberg School of Public Health. 2013. www.NHATS.org. [Google Scholar]
- 30. Montaquila J, Freedman VA, Kasper JD. National Health and Aging Trends Study Round 1 Income Imputation. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Bloomberg School of Public Health 2012. www.NHATS.org.
- 31. Miller DR, Rogers WH, Kazis LE, Spiro A III, Ren XS, Haffer SC. Patients’ self-report of diseases in the Medicare Health Outcomes Survey based on comparisons with linked survey and medical data from the Veterans Health Administration. J Ambul Care Manage. 2008;31:161–177. doi: 10.1097/01.JAC.0000314707.88160.9c [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Freedman VA, Kasper JD, Spillman BC. Successful aging through successful accommodation with assistive devices. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2017;72:300–309. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbw102 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Löwe B, Wahl I, Rose M, et al. A 4-item measure of depression and anxiety: validation and standardization of the Patient Health Questionnaire-4 (PHQ-4) in the general population. J Affect Disord. 2010;122:86–95. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2009.06.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JB, Löwe B. An ultra-brief screening scale for anxiety and depression: the PHQ-4. Psychosomatics. 2009;50:613–621. doi: 10.1176/appi.psy.50.6.613 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JB. The Patient Health Questionnaire-2: validity of a two-item depression screener. Med Care. 2003;41:1284–1292. doi: 10.1097/01.MLR.0000093487.78664.3C [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Dumurgier J, Artaud F, Touraine C, et al. Gait speed and decline in gait speed as predictors of incident dementia. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2017;72:655–661. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glw110 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Graham NL, Emery T, Hodges JR. Distinctive cognitive profiles in Alzheimer’s disease and subcortical vascular dementia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2004;75:61–71. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Fine JP, Gray RJ. A proportional Hazards model for the subdistribution of a competing risk. J Am Stat Assoc. 1999;94(446):496–509. [Google Scholar]
- 39. Montaquila J, Freedman VA, Edwards B, Kasper JD. National Health and Aging Trends Study Round 1 Sample Design and Selection. NHATS Technical Paper #1. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Bloomberg School of Public Health 2012. www.NHATS.org.
- 40. McKhann GM, Knopman DS, Chertkow H, et al. The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7:263–269. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Querfurth HW, LaFerla FM. Alzheimer’s Disease. N Engl J Med. 2010;362:329–344. doi: 10.1056/NEJMra0909142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. The National Institute on Aging. Alzheimer’s Disease Fact Sheet. National Institutes of Health; NIA Alzheimer’s and related Dementias Education and Referral (ADEAR) Center. 2016. (NIH Publication No. 16-AG-6423). [Google Scholar]
- 43. Taylor D, Sloan F, Doraiswamy PM. Marked increase in Alzheimer’s disease identified in medicare claims records between 1991 and 1999. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2004;59(7):M762–M766. doi: 10.1093/gerona/59.7.m762 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Rizzi L, Rosset I, Roriz-Cruz M. Global epidemiology of dementia: Alzheimer’s and vascular types. Biomed Res Int. 2014;2014:908915. doi: 10.1155/2014/908915 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Bennett DA, Schneider JA, Buchman AS, Barnes LL, Boyle PA, Wilson RS. Overview and findings from the rush Memory and Aging Project. Curr Alzheimer Res. 2012;9:646–663. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Borson S, Brush M, Gil E, et al. The Clock Drawing Test: utility for dementia detection in multiethnic elders. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 1999;54:M534–M540. doi: 10.1093/gerona/54.11.m534 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Parker C, Philp I. Screening for cognitive impairment among older people in black and minority ethnic groups. Age Ageing. 2004;33:447–452. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afh135 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Nair AK, Gavett BE, Damman M, et al. Clock drawing test ratings by dementia specialists: interrater reliability and diagnostic accuracy. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2010;22:85–92. doi: 10.1176/jnp.2010.22.1.85 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Panza F, Seripa D, Solfrizzi V, et al. Targeting cognitive frailty: clinical and neurobiological roadmap for a single complex phenotype. J Alzheimers Dis. 2015;47:793–813. doi: 10.3233/JAD-150358 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Dartigues JF, Amieva H. Cognitive frailty: rational and definition from an (I.a.N.a./i.a.g.g.) international consensus group. J Nutr Health Aging. 2014;18:95. doi: 10.1007/s12603-013-0437-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]