Abstract
Vestibular migraine (VM) is the most common cause of spontaneous vertigo but remains poorly understood. We investigated the hypothesis that central vestibular pathways are sensitized in VM by measuring self-motion perceptual thresholds in patients and control subjects and by characterizing the vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) and vestibular and headache symptom severity. VM patients were abnormally sensitive to roll tilt, which co-modulates semicircular canal and otolith organ activity, but not to motions that activate the canals or otolith organs in isolation, implying sensitization of canal-otolith integration. When tilt thresholds were considered together with vestibular symptom severity or VOR dynamics, VM patients segregated into two clusters. Thresholds in one cluster correlated positively with symptoms and with the VOR time constant; thresholds in the second cluster were uniformly low and independent of symptoms and the time constant. The VM threshold abnormality showed a frequency-dependence that paralleled the brain stem velocity storage mechanism. These results support a pathogenic model where vestibular symptoms emanate from the vestibular nuclei, which are sensitized by migraine-related brainstem regions and simultaneously suppressed by inhibitory feedback from the cerebellar nodulus and uvula, the site of canal-otolith integration. This conceptual framework elucidates VM pathophysiology and could potentially facilitate its diagnosis and treatment.
Subject terms: Sensory processing, Migraine
Introduction
Migraine is characterized by recurrent headache but more generally is considered a neurologic disorder of sensitization, as the headaches are characteristically associated with heightened sensitivity to light, sound, and other sensory stimuli1. Vestibular symptoms are particularly common in migraine2–in addition to enhanced motion sickness susceptibility3 many migraineurs experience discrete episodes of vertigo or ataxia that differ from other common migraine auras, as vestibular episodes often occur without a headache and have a highly variable range of durations, lasting from seconds to weeks4. These episodic vestibular symptoms caused by migraine, referred to as vestibular migraine (VM), have been estimated to affect 1% of the general population2 and to cause about 50% of episodic dizziness in children5 and 35% in adults. Indeed, it is now considered the most common cause of episodic vestibular symptoms6.
Despite its prevalence and morbidity, VM remains poorly understood. In particular, no physical signs or test abnormalities have been identified that are pathognomonic for VM so its diagnosis is based on criteria defined by an expert panel7,8. Further, the mechanisms that relate migraine to vestibular symptoms remain uncertain, although there are numerous potential anatomic substrates given the extensive projections between brainstem regions associated with migraine and the vestibular nuclei (see9 for a review). Since peripheral and central sensory systems are sensitized in migraine10,11, a reasonable hypothesis is that VM differs from other forms of migraine because vestibular pathways are sensitized in a manner that leads to both enhanced motion sickness susceptibility12 and episodic vertigo. Our prior studies13–15 suggested that VM patients may be particularly sensitive to the combined modulation of semicircular canal (angular velocity) and otolith (gravito-inertial) vestibular cues produced by roll tilt of the head, and recent studies from other laboratories16–18 support the contention that spatial orientation in roll is abnormal in VM.
We therefore measured the frequency-dependence of self-motion perceptual thresholds in VM patients during movements that activated the canals and otolith organs in combination and in isolation, the vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR), and the severity of dizziness, motion sickness, and migraine headaches. Our results provide evidence for a VM perceptual biomarker, since roll tilt thresholds in VM were abnormally reduced in a frequency-dependent pattern. Overall, our findings support a pathogenic mechanism that could explain some of the vestibular symptoms caused by migraine, namely that these symptoms could emanate from the vestibular nuclei, which are simultaneously activated by migraine regions in the brainstem and suppressed by a negative feedback loop through the cerebellar nodulus and uvula (the anatomic locus for canal-otolith integration19), where sensitization is compensatory and evidenced by reduced tilt thresholds.
Results
Data described in sections 1–3 are from the cohort of subjects who underwent all threshold protocols (12 normal, migraine, and VM patients), and the 8 Meniere’s disease patients who were tested on roll tilt at 0.2 Hz. Sections 4–5 describe data from a larger (n = 29) VM group whose testing consisted of roll tilt perceptual thresholds at 0.05, VOR testing, and quantification of symptom severity. Patient groups were identified using the standard, accepted diagnostic criteria for migraine20, VM8 and Meniere’s disease21.
Basic characteristics of the VM and control groups
Symptom severity and demographics of the VM and control groups are summarized in Table 1. The principal observations are that VM patients had significantly greater dizziness as assessed with the Dizziness Handicap Inventory (DHI22) than the migraine or normal groups, and motion sickness susceptibility (MSSQ23) was significantly increased in VM patients compared to migraine or normal controls. Headache severity (assessed with the Headache Impact Test24) was more pronounced in the migraine and VM groups than the normal subjects, as expected, with no significant difference between the migraine and VM groups. Headache severity was uncorrelated with vestibular symptoms (DHI, MSSQ) in normal and migraine subjects, but it did correlate with dizziness (DHI) in VM patients (Pearson R test p = 0.02). Anxiety assessed with the Beck Anxiety inventory25 did not differ significantly between the VM, migraine, and normal control groups. The VM, migraine, and normal groups did not differ in age, and the migraine and VM groups were gender-matched while the normal group was evenly divided between genders.
Table 1.
Clinical measure | Vestibular migraine (VM) | Migraine (M) | Normal (N) | Statistical comparisons |
---|---|---|---|---|
*DHI | 26.7 +/− 6.5 | 0.2 +/− 0.2 | 0.8 +/− 0.9 | M-W: p < 0.001 for VM-M, p = 0.002 for VM-N |
*MSSQ | 32.2 +/− 4.5 | 10.4 +/− 3.4 | 13.9 +/− 4.9 | t-test: p < 0.001 for VM-M, p = 0.02 for VM-N |
HIT | 56.7 +/− 2.3 | 53.9 +/− 4.2 | 41.7 +/− 1.2 | t-test: p = 0.6 for VM-M |
BA | 13.0 +/− 3.2 | 8.0 +/− 1.8 | 5.6 +/− 2.3 | t-test: >0.1 for VM-M and VM-N |
Age | 36.5 +/− 2.7 | 34.0 +/− 1.1 | 38.1 +/− 3.1 | ANOVA p = 0.39 |
Sex (F:M) | 11:1 | 10:2 | 6:6 |
Standard questionnaires were used to quantify the severity of dizziness (DHI22), motion sickness susceptibility (MSSQ23), headache (HIT24), and anxiety (BA25), which are shown as means +/− one SEM for the 12 subjects in each category (as is age). Gender distribution is shown as female:male. Statistical tests are Mann-Whitney (M-W), t-tests, and one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA).
Roll tilt perceptual thresholds are reduced in VM at mid and low frequencies
The geometric means of the roll tilt perceptual thresholds for the four subject groups, recorded using standard motion stimuli26 and psychometric methods27, are shown in Fig. 1. All groups showed a similar frequency-dependence whereby displacement thresholds (the smallest position change that was accurately perceived as rightward or leftward) were smallest at the higher frequencies and larger at the lower frequencies – consistent with earlier normative data28. Roll tilt thresholds depended significantly on both the subject group (VM, migraine, normal, ANOVA p < 0.001) and on the motion frequency (ANOVA p < 0.001). Furthermore, in the mid-to-low frequency range (0.03, 0.05, 0.1, and 0.2 Hz) VM thresholds were significantly lower than normal subjects (t-test: p = 0.001) and migraine patients (t-test: p = 0.001), while migraine and normal subjects did not differ (t-test: p = 0.81). Over the higher frequency range (1.0 Hz and above) thresholds in the three groups converged and did not differ significantly (ANOVA p = 0.1; t-tests between groups all > 0.05). Migraine patients had thresholds that were equivalent to normal controls except for 0.1 and 0.5 Hz, where their thresholds fell between the normal and VM groups but did not differ significantly from normal (t-tests p = 0.1 for 0.5 Hz, p = 0.4 for 0.1 Hz). Patients with Meniere’s disease, who had recurrent vertigo not due to migraine, had normal thresholds at 0.2 Hz, a frequency where VM thresholds were significantly lower than the control groups. The conclusion from these data is that roll tilt thresholds are abnormally small in VM patients over the mid to low-frequency range, but this was not due to migraine (without episodic vertigo) or episodic vertigo (without migraine).
Reduced roll tilt thresholds in VM reflect abnormal canal-otolith integration
Roll tilt modulates activity in the vertical canals, the graviceptive organs (primarily the utricles for the small tilt angles we employed), and the tactile cues applied to the head and trunk. We therefore tested each of these three sensory components in isolation to determine if they were responsible for the threshold reduction in VM during roll tilt. Figure 2 shows perceptual thresholds for roll rotation while supine (vertical canals), inter-aural (IA) translation (utricles), and tactile thresholds over 3 dermatomes on each body side, and in all cases thresholds in VM and control subjects were indistinguishable (roll rotation: 2-way ANOVA, p = 0.79 for group, 0.1 for frequency, and 0.9 for interaction; IA translation: 2-way ANOVA, p = 0.51 for group, p < 0.001 for frequency, and p = 0.97 for interaction; tactile Mann-Whitney p = 0.34). In particular, below 0.5 Hz roll tilt thresholds were abnormally low in VM but roll rotation and IA translation thresholds were normal. Together these results indicate that the roll tilt threshold reduction in VM is not due to sensitization of canal or otolith signals in isolation, nor does it reflect increased tactile sensitivity. Since prior work in normal subjects (e.g.28) demonstrated that roll tilt thresholds at mid and low frequencies require central integration of canal and otolith cues, abnormal roll tilt thresholds in VM appear to result from changes in the synthesis of canal and otolith inputs in the brain.
Roll tilt thresholds in VM are related to vestibular symptoms
The relationship between vestibular symptom severity (DHI score) and roll tilt thresholds at 0.05 Hz is shown in Fig. 3A for the expanded VM population tested at this frequency. The data appear to be distributed in a pattern that approximates a tilted ‘V’ with the two limbs converging at the lower left corner of the plot. To facilitate analysis of the data we considered the two limbs separately, dividing patients into the VM1 (grey icons) and VM2 (black icons) clusters (Fig. 3A) with an intuitive method based on visual inspection of the plot, and a soft clustering method which required no assumptions about the data’s distribution except that it was divisible into two subsets: (i) for the visual approach, we calculated the (black dashed) line that connected the average (DHI, threshold) values for all VM patients with the average values for the three data points located in the lower left corner of the plot where the two limbs of the ‘V’ converge. This line was used to separate the VM patients into VM1 and VM2 clusters and the VM1 and VM2 data were each fit with a linear regression (grey and black lines, respectively) and with 95% confidence ellipses (Fig. 3A); and (ii) to verify this visual classification, we also segregated the VM data using soft clustering, in which the probability of the patient belonging to each cluster is calculated29, with points at the edge of a cluster having lower probabilities of membership to that cluster30,31. As detailed in the methods, the results of the visual and soft clustering approaches were very similar–ROC AUC analysis comparing these two classification methods yielded a value of 0.93.
The principal difference between VM1 and VM2 was the relationship between vestibular symptom severity (DHI) and roll tilt threshold. Using the visual classification as our standard approach, VM1 showed a positive correlation between tilt threshold and symptom severity (Pearson R for DHI and tilt threshold, R = 0.94, p < 0.001) and therefore a negative correlation between tilt sensitivity and dizziness. In contrast, VM2 patients had DHI values that varied independently of their tilt thresholds. Furthermore, tilt thresholds in VM2 were uniformly low (less than normal) but thresholds in VM1 were more widely distributed and included values above, within, and below the normal range. Comparison of other VM1 and VM2 characteristics (Table 2) indicates that they were equivalent except for the roll tilt threshold, which was lower in VM2, and the motion sickness susceptibility score, which was higher in VM1. Otherwise, VM1 and VM2 s shared equivalent dizziness and headache symptom severity and equivalent ages and gender distributions.
Table 2.
Clinical measure | VM1 | VM2 | Statistical comparisons |
---|---|---|---|
DHI | 32.1 +/− 4.6 | 31.7 +/− 4.6 | t-test: p = 0.47 |
*MSSQ | 30.0 +/− 3.1 | 19.7 +/− 3.4 | t-test: p = 0.02 |
HIT | 55.2 +/− 1.9 | 56.0 +/− 2.1 | t-test: p = 0.40 |
Age (years) | 38.9 +/− 3.3 | 34.0 +/− 3.4 | t-test: p = 0.17 |
Sex (F:M) | 9 F:3 M | 11 F:2 M | Rank test: p = 0.5 |
*Roll tilt threshold (deg) | 1.94 +/− 0.17 | 1.01 +/− 0.04 | M-W RST: p < 0.001 |
Abbreviations are defined in the Table 1 legend; roll tilt thresholds were measured at 0.05 Hz. All values are shown as means +/− one SEM. M-W is Mann-Whitney and M-W RST is rank sum test. Asterisks indicate clinical measures that differ between VM1 and VM2.
Roll tilt thresholds in VM are related to VOR dynamics
Velocity storage is the brainstem mechanism that mediates the dynamic and spatial characteristics of vestibular behaviors32,33 and in particular it determines the time constant (Tc) of the VOR. Similar to the DHI-threshold data shown in Fig. 3A, the relationship between the VOR Tc and roll tilt thresholds (Fig. 3B) also approximated a tilted ‘V’ with the two limbs converging at the lower left corner of the plot. We employed the same two approaches (visual and soft clustering), therefore, to separate the two limbs of the Tc-threshold distribution into VM1 (grey icons) and VM2 (black icons) clusters. Again, as detailed in the methods the results of the visual and soft clustering approaches were very similar with ROC value of 0.99, and all VM patients with DHI and VOR Tc data available (filled icons in Fig. 3A,B) independently received the same label (VM1 or VM2) for both the DHI-threshold and Tc-threshold analyses. Paralleling the DHI-threshold data described above, the principal difference between VM1 and VM2 for the Tc-threshold data was the relationship between the VOR time constant and roll tilt threshold, asVM1 patients showed a positive correlation between the VOR time constant and tilt threshold (Fig. 3B, grey data points and line, Pearson R, R = 0.76. p = 0.01) while VM2 patients had Tc values that varied independently of the uniformly low tilt thresholds (Fig. 3B, black data points and line). VM1 and VM2 patients identified with the Tc-threshold analysis (Table 3) were otherwise comparable except for the roll tilt threshold, which was lower in VM2 (note that some patients had only DHI or Tc data available, open icons in Fig. 3, so values in Tables 2 and 3 differ).
Table 3.
Clinical measure | VM1 | VM2 | Statistical comparisons |
---|---|---|---|
VOR time constant (sec) | 15.5 +/− 0.7 | 18.8 +/− 1.7 | M-W RST: p = 0.2 |
MSSQ | 26.4 +/− 4.6 | 22.7 +/− 5.4 | t-test: p = 0.6 |
HIT | 56.9 +/− 2.2 | 51 +/− 5.9 | M-W RST: p = 0.7 |
Age (years) | 39.4 +/− 2.7 | 34.5 +/− 4.5 | t-test: p = 0.35 |
Sex (F:M) | 9 F:2 M | 9 F:1 M | Rank test: p = 0.6 |
*Roll tilt threshold (deg) | 1.83 +/− 0.15 | 0.97 +/− 0.5 | M-W RST: p < 0.001 |
Abbreviations are defined in the legend for Table 2.
Discussion
Our primary findings are that roll tilt perceptual thresholds in VM: i) are abnormally low for mid and low frequency motion stimuli; ii) are not due to migraine (when it is unassociated with vestibular symptoms), or due to vestibular symptoms (when they are not due to migraine); iii) reflect abnormal integration of canal and otolith information in the brain; iv) have a ‘V’ shaped distribution with respect to the severity of vestibular symptoms, with data clustered about one leg displaying a positive correlation between tilt threshold and symptom severity and data clustered about the other leg displaying threshold-independent symptoms but uniformly low tilt thresholds; and v) have a very similar ‘V’ shaped distribution with respect to velocity storage (VOR time constant). These results are discussed below in terms of their potential diagnostic, pathophysiologic, and therapeutic implications.
Diagnostic implications
VM is the most common cause of episodic vestibular symptoms6 but lacks any pathognomonic finding and is diagnosed using criteria determined by an expert panel7,8. Identifying an approach that segregates VM patients from vestibular patients whose symptoms are not due to migraine would therefore be of great clinical value. Our results show that for populations of patients, VM differs from vestibular (without migraine) patients, migraine (without vestibular) patients, and normal subjects with respect to mid and low frequency roll tilt perceptual thresholds. The mechanisms responsible for these findings are considered below, but from a clinical perspective these results could provide a biomarker that allows individual patients to be classified as VM or non-VM based on their roll tilt threshold when considered in tandem with their DHI and/or VOR time constant. To determine the sensitivity and specificity of this approach, substantially larger patient populations must be studied to more accurately define the VM confidence ellipses and to determine the data structure for vestibular patients whose symptoms are not due to migraine. Ultimately a multivariate analysis that includes the low frequency roll tilt thresholds, DHI, and VOR time constant, and other perceptual (e.g., z-axis translation thresholds34) or VOR (e.g., response variability) measures may be a way to most accurately separate VM and non-VM vestibular patients.
Such a multivariate analysis may be particularly helpful because there are a plethora of possible interactions between migraine and the vestibular system9, so a heterogeneous set of mechanisms almost certainly contributes to the vestibular symptoms of VM. For example, we followed the current VM classification criteria7,8 in this study, so patients with evidence of a peripheral vestibular damage were excluded. Migraine is epidemiologically linked to Meniere’s disease35, however, some migraine patients develop evidence of labyrinthine damage36, and animal models of migraine show evidence of biochemical changes in the labyrinth37, implying that vestibular dysfunction in VM may have a peripheral component in some patients38. While the current VM diagnostic criteria allowed us to focus on central mechanisms without confounding peripheral effects that could potentially mask central changes, diagnostic approaches for VM must ultimately consider potential central (infratentorial, supratentorial) and peripheral pathophysiologic changes.
Pathophysiologic implications
To facilitate interpretation of our results, we first summarize the relevant concepts of central vestibular processing (reviewed in33); then consider the implications of our findings when examined within this conceptual framework; and finally provide a hypothesis for mechanisms that could contribute to the vestibular symptoms of VM that is consistent with our results and with known vestibular physiology and migraine pathophysiology.
Overview of relevant central vestibular processing
As shown schematically in Fig. 4, the labyrinth transduces high-pass filtered angular velocity in three dimensions (w, canals) and the vector sum of gravity and linear acceleration (GIA, otoliths). The brain estimates angular velocity (w’) by combining the direct signal from the canal afferents (gD pathway) with a temporal integration of the canal afferent signal (via velocity storage, with a gain gVS and a time constant TVS). Velocity storage (light grey box, Fig. 4) is generated in the commissural fibers between the medial and superior vestibular nuclei and hence can be considered a marker of vestibular nuclear activity39,40. It is responsible for the dynamics of VOR and perceptual responses induced by head rotation (primarily in yaw)41,42. The nodulus and uvula of the cerebellum (NU) are the location of the ‘tilt estimator” (dark grey box, Fig. 4) where canal and otolith inputs are first synthesized43. Specifically, the NU integrates the off-vertical component of the angular velocity signal to update the estimated orientation of gravity G’44,45, and G’ is subtracted from the GIA sensed by the otoliths to estimate A’ (linear acceleration)46,47. The NU tilt estimator has GABAergic inhibitory projection to the vestibular nuclei48 (Fig. 4, grey arrow #1), including the velocity storage network, as NU ablation lengthens49 and stimulation shortens50 the VOR Tc.
Evidence that reduced VM tilt thresholds reflect sensitization of the cerebellar NU
Three types of evidence suggest that the NU is sensitized in VM patients – i) VM thresholds were reduced when canal and otolith cues co-modulated (e.g., during tilt, Fig. 1) but not when either the canals or otoliths modulated in isolation (Fig. 2). Since the NU is the location of canal-otolith integration, the reduction of tilt but not rotational or translational thresholds in VM suggests that central sensitization localizes at least in part to the NU; ii) if the NU is sensitized in VM, a positive correlation is predicted between tilt thresholds and the VOR time constant because of the inhibitory projection (Fig. 4, grey arrow #1) from the NU (where tilt is calculated) to velocity storage in the brainstem (which sets the VOR Tc). Specifically, NU sensitization (reflected by lowered tilt thresholds) should result in increased inhibition of velocity storage (reflected by lowered Tc). Indeed, one cluster of VM patients (Fig. 3B, VM1 = grey icons and line) showed this predicted correlation, as did the non-VM subjects (data not shown). In contrast, the second VM cluster (Fig. 3B, VM2 = black icons and line) had uniformly low tilt thresholds that were independent of the VOR Tc (discussed below); and iii) since the NU projects to and receives reciprocal projections from vestibular nuclei48,51 where velocity storage resides (Fig. 4), if reduced thresholds indicate sensitization of the NU then the frequency-dependence of the threshold abnormality should mirror that of velocity storage52. Figure 5 compares the frequency-dependence of the VM threshold abnormality and velocity storage and demonstrates their close similarity (this figure includes “pseudo-static” DC28 results from13), supporting our contention that tilt threshold abnormalities in VM localize to the NU because they are closely related to velocity storage (which is inhibited by the NU).
Evidence that the vestibular nuclei contribute to the symptoms of VM and are modulated by (inhibitory) cerebellar and (excitatory) brainstem projections
The relationship between tilt thresholds and the VOR Tc in VM patients (Fig. 3B) is structurally the same as the relationship between tilt thresholds and vestibular symptom severity (DHI, Fig. 3A). Both the threshold-Tc and threshold-DHI plots have two clusters of data, one grouped about a positive regression (grey icons and lines) and one grouped about a line that is nearly parallel to the y-axis with uniformly low thresholds (black icons and lines). Since the VOR Tc is a well characterized measure of vestibular nuclear activity (velocity storage in particular), one logical explanation for the similarity in plots 3 A and 3B is that vestibular symptoms in VM are generated, at least partially, within the brainstem vestibular nuclei in a manner that correlates with the processing of the VOR Tc. Like the threshold-Tc data (Fig. 3B), the positive correlation between tilt thresholds and vestibular symptoms in Fig. 3A (grey regression line) likely reflects the inhibitory projections48 from the cerebellar NU to the vestibular nuclei (Fig. 4, grey arrow #1). Further, the vestibular nuclei receive multiple projections from brainstem regions associated with migraine initiation and propagation, including the trigeminal nuclei, locus coeruleus, and dorsal raphe nucleus53–55. We propose, therefore that both the vestibular symptoms of VM and the VOR Tc can also be modified independent of NU activity/tilt thresholds (black regression lines in Fig. 3A,B) because of the effects of excitatory projections from these migraine-related brainstem regions to the vestibular nuclei (Fig. 4, black arrow #2).
The VM patients have a ‘V’ like distribution when tilt thresholds are plotted against dizziness severity or the VOR Tc, with the VM1 and VM2 legs merging in the low threshold–low DHI/Tc corner of the plots (Fig. 3). This pattern suggests that VM1 and VM2 could be distinct but overlapping phenotypes or that they could represent two components of a single VM phenotype. Our pathogenic model, discussed above, supports the single phenotype explanation with the VM1 and VM2 clusters representing a continuum of patient states that are determined by the relative contributions of inhibitory (cerebellar) and excitatory (brainstem) projections to the vestibular nuclei. Since patients with less severe vestibular symptoms and/or short VOR Tc always have low tilt thresholds, feedback inhibition supplied by the cerebellar NU to the vestibular nuclei (Fig. 4, grey arrow #1) may be sensitized as an early response to the aberrant vestibular nuclei activation produced by migraine (as evidenced by the positive correlation in VM1 between DHI/Tc and tilt thresholds, Fig. 3, grey lines). This inhibitory feedback may be adequate in some patients, namely those with low thresholds but also low DHI or VOR Tc values (bottom left corner of Fig. 3A,B). Cerebellar-mediated inhibition presumably saturates when the NU has reached maximum sensitization, associated with the lowest possible roll tilt thresholds, and then additional stimulation of the vestibular nuclei by brainstem migraine regions (Fig. 4, black arrow #2) increases the DHI and VOR Tc without influencing the already low threshold (VM2 in Fig. 3A,B, black lines).
Two other aspects of the data distributions shown in Fig. 3 warrant further discussion. Firstly, it is evident that, unlike most of the VM patients who have low tilt thresholds, some VM1 patients have tilt thresholds that are in the high-normal range. The exact explanation for this result is not known but presumably the mechanisms underlying vestibular symptoms in migraine are heterogeneous and can result in a distribution of tilt thresholds that are independent of the model described above. We suggest, therefore, that high-normal tilt thresholds in some VM patients reflect both the inherent variability associated with this disorder and suboptimal engagement of the inhibitory cerebellar feedback loop through the NU that we propose is responsible for threshold reduction. Secondly, our results are qualitatively consistent with prior work that suggested the severity of vestibular symptoms in VM (e.g., motion sickness) correlate with the length of the VOR time constant12. In particular, comparison of the VM1 clusters in Fig. 3A,B indicates that higher tilt thresholds are associated with both longer time constants and higher DHI scores, implying that longer time constants correlate with greater vestibular symptomatology. While this correlation was not statistically significant in our relatively small VM data set (as it was in the much larger patient population that was previously described12), the general pattern is recapitulated by our results.
This model provides a potential explanation for the relationship between vestibular symptoms and migraine
The vestibular nuclei are a node in a negative feedback loop (Fig. 4), as they project to (and could therefore sensitize) the NU, which then suppresses vestibular nuclear activity through reciprocal inhibitory projections. This feedback loop contributes to the brain’s ability to generate accurate motion and orientation estimates and may play a fundamental role in the generation of motion sickness56. Persistent dysfunction of this neural circuit in VM could therefore contribute to the characteristic increase in inter-ictal motion sickness susceptibility3,12. This neural circuit can oscillate when the NU is damaged and it has been suggested that it is inherently unstable and is stabilized by NU inhibition50. In VM patients, a migraine episode could sensitize the vestibular nuclei to the extent that NU suppression is no longer adequate to maintain stability, particularly when NU activity is modulated by changing head orientation, resulting in ictal episodes of vestibular dysfunction that are temporally linked to migraines and induced by head tilts (e.g., are “positional”), both of which are common in VM4,38.
Correlations with imaging and other behavioral studies
Our model is supported by the one ictal functional imaging study of VM patients57 which showed prominent activation of the cerebellum and brainstem during vestibular episodes. Interestingly, the primary anatomic changes in VM appear to be supratentorial, and these changes in the thalamus58 and cerebral cortex59 may be due to primary migraine-related dysfunction in these brain regions or could in part reflect their role as targets for infratentorial projections. More generally, symptoms of VM are almost certainly heterogeneous in origin and most likely are generated by mechanisms that localize to both infra- and supratentorial60 brain structures and possibly to the vestibular labyrinth as well. Only infratentorial mechanisms were investigated in our study so our findings do not preclude significant contributions from other brain regions or from the inner ear.
Therapeutic implications
There are no placebo-controlled prospective clinical trials that assess the efficacy of different pharmacologic treatments for VM61 and there is no rationale to guide the choice of medication for individual patients. Clearly, placebo-controlled prospective clinical trials are necessary to develop evidence-based guidelines for VM medical therapy, and we suggest that our results (and perceptual threshold testing prior to therapy) could help provide a framework for future clinical trials. Specifically, vestibular symptoms in VM patients with relatively high thresholds (e.g., falling near the VM1 grey cluster on Fig. 3A) may respond best to drugs that activate GABA receptors, since these would increase inhibition of the brainstem vestibular nuclei and potentially suppress ictal and inter-ictal vestibular symptoms. Potential drugs would include those that increase synaptic GABA such as tiagabine62 or the aminopyridines that augment GABA release by cerebellar Purkinje cells63. This approach has not been tried but positive results from a trial of acetazolamide64, which augments GABAergic neuronal activity as one of its properties, offer support for the contention that increasing suppression of brainstem vestibular nuclei may benefit VM patients. Conversely, patients with low tilt thresholds (falling near the VM2 black cluster on Fig. 3A) may have saturated GABAergic inhibition of the vestibular nuclei and may therefore respond best to medications that affect other neurotransmitters such as beta blockers61.
While their effects on vestibular symptoms due to migraine have not yet been studied, drugs that inhibit the action of calcitonin gene-related peptide (CGRP) in the trigeminal ganglion such as erenumab (reviewed in65) could be important in preventing or aborting vertigo episodes in patients with VM. In particular, since migraine-related sensitization of the trigeminal nuclei appears to affect the sensitivity of structures that receive trigeminal nuclear projections (e.g.66, for the visual system), it is possible that the well-characterized trigeminal nuclei → vestibular nuclei projections in the brainstem55 could contribute to vestibular sensitization in migraine patients who develop VM. Attenuating the effects of this pathway with drugs the inhibit CGPR activity in the trigeminal ganglion could potentially minimize trigeminal-mediated sensitization of the vestibular nuclei and thereby stabilize the central vestibular feedback loop (Fig. 4).
Materials and Methods
All subjects provided informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and all studies were approved by the Massachusetts Eye and Ear Infirmary (MEEI) IRB.
Experimental design
The objective of this study was to measure self-motion perceptual thresholds and other information about vestibular function and symptoms in patients with VM and in the appropriate control groups, with the goal of elucidating the pathophysiology of VM. Blinding was not employed for the vestibular threshold tests since the threshold values were calculated using an automated “expert advisor” that chose the motion parameters and converged on the threshold in a manner that was independent of the experimenter67. For tactile threshold tests, the experimenter was blinded to the subject category. Test order was randomized as described below. A priori power calculations were performed using data from our prior preliminary study13, with an α = 0.05 and a power of 0.8. The thresholds during roll tilt at 0.01 Hz were 0.55 for VM and 1.4 for migraine, with standard deviations of 0.8 and 0.7, so 8 subjects in each group were calculated as the minimum to achieve the desired power.
Subject selection/characterization
(a) VM patients were culled from the otoneurology clinics at MEEI. They met the currently accepted diagnostic criteria for definite VM8 which includes a history of episodic vestibular symptoms that are temporally associated with headaches that meet the International Headache Society (IHS) criteria for migraine20; and an absence of any other neurologic or otologic dysfunction responsible for the vestibular symptoms, as assessed with history, physical examination, audiogram, MRI of the brain, and standard clinical rotational testing68. 12 VM patients were tested on the full range of perceptual tests, while an additional 17 VM patients were tested only on roll tilt at 0.05 Hz, since that motion profile was identified in the initial cohort of 12 subjects as one that showed a large difference between VM and control subjects and was feasible to test in this larger population (details below); (b) Migraine (n = 12) patients met the IHS criteria for migraine with or without aura, lacked a history of any vestibular symptoms (other than motion sickness), lacked evidence of any other neurologic or otologic dysfunction as described above, and had normal brain MRI and rotational testing; (c) Normal (n = 12) subjects similarly had no evidence of neurologic or otologic disease, no history of migraine or vestibular symptoms, and had normal rotational testing; and d) Meniere’s Disease (n = 8) patients who met diagnostic criteria for definite Meniere’s Disease21, had episodic vertigo but no migraine history, and had no evidence of permanent peripheral vestibular damage as assessed with rotational testing and cervical vestibular evoked myogenic potentials.
Other patient features
No migraine or VM patient was on prophylactic migraine medication and all tests were performed during inter-ictal periods and followed the most recent migraine or vestibular episode by at least two weeks. The VM, migraine, and normal groups were age-matched and the VM and migraine groups were sex-matched. The normal control group did not share the female preponderance of the two migraine groups but a large recent study of 105 healthy asymptomatic subjects found no sex-related differences in self-motion perceptual thresholds for any of the five motion stimuli tested69, including roll tilt thresholds at both 1 Hz and 0.2 Hz.
Non-threshold data
All subjects filled out standard questionnaires to quantify the severity of their dizziness (Dizziness Handicap Inventory, DHI22), motion sickness susceptibility (MSSQ using the Golding revised questionnaire23), headache severity (Headache Impact Test, HIT24) and anxiety (Beck Anxiety Inventory25). Yaw-axis VOR responses were characterized with standard low-frequency (0.01 to 1.0 Hz) sinusoidal testing which yielded phase, gain, and bias values for each frequency, and these were used to calculate the overall VOR time, gain, and bias constants68,70 for each subject.
Threshold testing
Three separate test sessions were used to obtain the complete perceptual threshold data set for each subject. Each session lasted a maximum of three hours with breaks provided at a regular schedule to minimize fatigue. The testing order was randomized to eliminate possible order effects and each of the three test sessions were separated by a minimum of one week.
Perceptual task
Self-motion perceptual thresholds were determined using a standard forced-choice, one-interval, direction-discrimination method as used previously in our and other labs for numerous vestibular threshold studies (e.g.26). Briefly, after each motion the subjected responded by pressing one of two buttons to indicate if they perceived the motion towards their right or left, and if they were unsure they were required to guess. An adaptive three-down, one-up protocol was used (magnitude of motion reduced after three correct responses and increased after one incorrect response), the direction of motion was randomized, and the test terminated when the coefficient of variation of the fitted threshold parameter reached a value of less than 0.2. A psychometric function was fit to these binary response data using published methods71 and the width (sigma) of fitted psychometric function was defined as the threshold27 which is the stimulus magnitude at which 84% of responses are predicted to be correct.
Motion profiles
With the exception of “pseudo-static” roll tilt (see below), all motions were based on a single-cycle sinusoidal acceleration yielding a unidirectional bell-shaped velocity profile26. The frequency of the motion was defined as the inverse of the period and ranged from 0.03 Hz (period of 33.3 sec) to 5.0 Hz (period of 0.2 sec). It was not feasible to test subjects at frequencies below 0.03 Hz because the long duration of each trial impaired attention and precluded acquisition of an adequate data set. Each type of motion (roll tilt while upright about a naso-occipital, earth-horizontal axis centered between the ears; roll rotation while supine about an earth-vertical, naso-occipital axis centered between the ears), and inter-aural (IA) translation while upright along an earth-horizontal axis) was tested in separate blocks. Static (DC) thresholds (in Fig. 5) were provided for the VM, migraine, and normal groups from our prior study13–they were estimated using “pseudo-static” roll tilt28, a method where subjects are tilted at very low angular velocities below the canal threshold.
Tactile thresholds
These were determined in 3 regions on each side of the body including the zygomatic arch, scapula, and ischial tuberosity using standard Semmes-Weinstein monofilaments72.
Categorizing VM patients into two subsets
As outlined above, the VM patients appeared to segregate into two clusters when their low-frequency roll tilt thresholds where plotted against DHI or the VOR time constant. We used two methods to separate the VM patients into clusters – i) a visual, intuitive method was used, since the line connecting the mean of all data points on these plots with the values where the two sets converge in the lower left corner of the plots (dashed black lines in Fig. 3A,B) divided the patients into two visually-distinct groups; and ii) to avoid any subjective influences on patient categorization, we also used a commonly employed fuzzy c-means clustering algorithm29,30 where membership probabilities for the two clusters are assigned in a way that minimizes distances to each centroid (e.g., minimizing ∑i∑j wi uijm dij where wi is the weight assigned to observation i, uij is the membership of observation i in cluster j, m is the degree of fuzzification30, and dij is the distance between observation i and the centroid of cluster j). The degree of fuzzification was chosen as m = 1.5 since larger or smaller values did not improve the efficiency of the classification measured by AUC of ROC. The two methods yielded very similar classifications and classification probabilities (shown graphically in Fig. 6), given the high ROC AUC values comparing the two approaches.
Statistical analysis
Since vestibular perceptual thresholds, including roll tilt thresholds, have been described to follow a lognormal distribution73, they were plotted as geometric means (c.f., Fig. 1), standard parametric statistical tests (analysis of variance, t-tests) were performed with the thresholds in log-units, and lognormal distributions were confirmed using Kolmogorov-Smirnov tests. Standard non-parametric tests (Mann-Whitney) were used when distributions were not normal or log-normal. P-values of 0.05 or less were accepted as significant and all tests were corrected for repeated measures.
De-identified data that were used in this study are available on the laboratory’s webpage: www.masseyeandear.org/research/otolaryngology/investigators/laboratories/jenks-vestibular-physiology-laboratory.
Acknowledgements
We thank David Balkwill for assistance with simulations and David Zee, Rami Burstein, Anne Luebke, and Egilius Spierings for comments on the manuscript. This work was supported by NIH/NIDCD grants R01 DC012528 (to RFL) and R01 DC014924 (to RFL and DMM).
Author Contributions
S.K. data collection, data analysis, writing the paper. A.J.P. experimental design, writing the paper. S.E.D. data collection, writing the paper. D.M.M. experimental design, data analysis, writing the paper. F.E. data analysis, writing the paper. R.F.L. experimental design, data analysis, writing the paper.
Competing Interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Tommaso M, et al. Altered processing of sensory stimuli in patients with migraine. Nat Rev Neurol. 2014;10:144–55. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2014.14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Lempert T, Neuhauser H. Epidemiology of vertigo, migraine and vestibular migraine. J Neurol. 2009;256:333–38. doi: 10.1007/s00415-009-0149-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Murdin L, et al. Motion sickness in migraine and vestibular disorders. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2015;86:585–7. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2014-308331. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Dieterich M, Brandt T. Episodic vertigo related to migraine (90 cases): vestibular migraine? J Neurol. 1999;246:883–892. doi: 10.1007/s004150050478. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Riina N, Ilmari P, Kentala E. Vertigo and imbalance in children: a retrospective study in a Helsinki University otorhinolaryngology clinic. Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 2005;131:996–1000. doi: 10.1001/archotol.131.11.996. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Dieterich M, Obermann M, Celebisoy N. Vestibular migraine: the most frequent entity of episodic vertigo. J Neurol. 2016;263:S82–9. doi: 10.1007/s00415-015-7905-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Lempert T, et al. Vestibular migraine: diagnostic criteria. J Vestib Res. 2012;22:167–72. doi: 10.3233/VES-2012-0453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Bisdorff AR, Staab JP, Newman-Toker DE. Overview of the International Classification of Vestibular Disorders. Neurol Clin. 2015;33:541–50. doi: 10.1016/j.ncl.2015.04.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Furman JM, Marcus DA, Balaban CD. Vestibular migraine: clinical aspects and pathophysiology. Lancet Neurol. 2013;12:706–15. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(13)70107-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Olesen J, Burstein R, Ashina M, Tfelt-Hansen P. Origin of pain in migraine: evidence for peripheral sensitization. Lancet Neurol. 2009;8:679–90. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(09)70090-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Nye M, Haney R, You S, Albu S, Meagher M. The role of central sensitization in pain hypersensitivity in individuals with symptoms of migraine headache. J Pain. 2016;17:S55. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2016.01.226. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Jeong SH, Oh SY, Kim HJ, Koo JW, Kim JS. Vestibular dysfunction in migraine: effects of associated vertigo and motion sickness. J Neurol. 2010;257:905–12. doi: 10.1007/s00415-009-5435-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Lewis RF, Priesol AJ, Nicoucar K, Lim K, Merfeld DM. Abnormal motion perception in vestibular migraine. Laryngoscope. 2011;121:1124–25. doi: 10.1002/lary.21723. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Wang J, Lewis RF. Abnormal tilt perception during centrifugation in patients with vestibular migraine. J Assoc Res Otolaryngol. 2016;17:253–8. doi: 10.1007/s10162-016-0559-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Wang J, Lewis RF. Contribution of intravestibular sensory conflict to motion sickness and dizziness in migraine disorders. J Neurophysiol. 2016;116:1586–91. doi: 10.1152/jn.00345.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Ashish G, Augustine AM, Tyagi AK, Lepcha A, Balraj A. Subjective visual vertical and horizontal in vestibular migraine. J Int Adv Otol. 2017;13:254–8. doi: 10.5152/iao.2017.4056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Winnick A, Sadegour S, Otero-Millan J, Chang T-P, Kheradmand A. Errors of upright perception in patients with vestibular migraine. Front Neurol. 2018;9:982. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2018.00892. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Miller MA, Crane BT. Static and dynamic visual vertical perception in subjects with migraine and vestibular migraine. World J Otorhinolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 2016;2:175–80. doi: 10.1016/j.wjorl.2016.08.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Angelaki DE, Yakusheva TA, Green AM, Dickman JD, Blazqueuz PM. Computation of egomotion in the macaque cerebellum. Cerebellum. 2010;9:174–98. doi: 10.1007/s12311-009-0147-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Headache Classification Committee of the International Headache Society (IHS) The international classification of headache disorders, 3rd edition. Cephalagia. 2018;38:1–211. doi: 10.1177/0333102417738202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Lopez-Escamez JA, et al. Diagnostic criteria for Menière’s disease. Consensus document of the Bárány Society, the Japan Society for Equilibrium Research, the European Academy of Otology and Neurotology (EAONO), the American Academy of Otolaryngology-Head and Neck Surgery (AAO-HNS) and the Korean Balance Society. Acta Otorrinolaringol Esp. 2016;67:1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.otorri.2015.05.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Mutlu B, Serbetcioglu B. Discussion of the dizziness handicap inventory. J Vestib Res. 2013;23:271–7. doi: 10.3233/VES-130488. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Golding JF. Motion sickness susceptibility questionnaire revised and its relationship to other forms of sickness. Brain Res Bull. 1998;47:507–16. doi: 10.1016/S0361-9230(98)00091-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Pryse-Phillips W. Evaluating migraine disability: the headache impact test instrument in context. Can J Neurol Sci. 2002;29:S11–5. doi: 10.1017/S0317167100001888. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Therrien Z, Hunsley J. Assessment of anxiety in older adults: a systematic review of commonly used measures. Aging Ment Health. 2012;16:1–16. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2011.602960. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Valko Y, Lewis RF, Priesol AJ, Merfeld DM. Vestibular labyrinth contributions to human whole-body motion discrimination. J Neurosci. 2012;32:13537–42. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2157-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Merfeld DM. Signal detection theory and vestibular thresholds: I. Basic theory and practical considerations. Exp Brain Res. 2011;210:389–405. doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2557-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Lim K, Karmali F, Nicoucar K, Merfeld DM. Perceptual precision of passive body tilt is consistent with statistically optimal cue integration. J Neurophysiol. 2017;117:2037–52. doi: 10.1152/jn.00073.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Kaufman, L. & Rousseeuw, P. J. Finding Groups in Data: An Introduction to Cluster Analysis, 1 edition. Hoboken, N. J. Wiley-Interscience (2005).
- 30.Bezdek, J. C. Pattern Recognition with Fuzzy Objective Function Algorithms. Norwell, MA, USA: Kluwer Academic Publishers (1981).
- 31.Hathaway RJ, Davenport JW, Bezdek JC. Relational duals of the c-means clustering algorithms. Pattern Recognition. 1989;22:205–212. doi: 10.1016/0031-3203(89)90066-6. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Cohen B, Wearne S, Dai M, Raphan T. Spatial orientation of the angular vestibulo-ocular reflex. J Vestib Res. 1999;9:163–72. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Laurens J, Angelaki DE. The functional significance of velocity storage and its dependence on gravity. Exp Brain Res. 2011;210:407–22. doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2568-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Bremova T, et al. Comparison of linear motion perception thresholds in vestibular migraine and Meniere’s disease. Eur Arch Otorhinolaryngol. 2016;273:2931–39. doi: 10.1007/s00405-015-3835-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Ray J, Carr SD, Popli G, Gibson WP. An epidemiological study to investigate the relationship between Meniere’s disease and migraine. Clin Otolaryngol. 2016;41:707–10. doi: 10.1111/coa.12608. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Arslan Y, et al. The etiological relationship between migraine and sudden hearing loss. Otol Neurotol. 2017;38:1411–14. doi: 10.1097/MAO.0000000000001617. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Koo JW, Balaban CD. Serotonin-induced plasma extravasation in the murine inner ear: possible mechanism of migraine-associated inner ear dysfunction. Cephalalgia. 2006;26:1310–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2982.2006.01208.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Polensek SH, Tusa RJ. Nystagmus during attacks of vestibular migraine: an aid in diagnosis. Audiol Neurootol. 2010;15:241–6. doi: 10.1159/000255440. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Wearne S, Raphan T, Cohen B. Contribution of vestibular commissural pathways to spatial orientation of the angular vestibuloocular reflex. J Neurophysiol. 1997;78:1193–17. doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.78.2.1193. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Holstein GR, Martinelli GP, Cohen B. The ultrastructure of GABA-immunoreactive vestibular commissural neurons related to velocity storage in the monkey. Neuroscience. 1999;93:171–81. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(99)00141-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Robinson, D. A. Vestibular and optokinetic symbiosis: an example of explaining by modeling. In: Control of Gaze by Brain Stem Neurons. Developments in Neuroscience. Baker R, Berthoz A. Elsevier 1, 49–58 (1977).
- 42.Bertolini G, et al. Velocity storage contribution to vestibular self-motion perception in healthy human subjects. J Neurophysiol. 2011;105:209–23. doi: 10.1152/jn.00154.2010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Wearne S, Raphan T, Cohen B. Control of spatial orientation of the angular vestibuloocular reflex by the nodulus and uvula. J Neurophysiol. 1998;79:2690–715. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.79.5.2690. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Angelaki DE, Yakusheva TA. How vestibular neurons solve the tilt/translation ambiguity. Comparison of brainstem, cerebellum, and thalamus. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2009;1164:19–28. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.03939.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Merfeld DM, Young L, Oman C, Shelhamer M. A multi-dimensional model of the effect of gravity on the spatial orientation of the monkey. J Vest Res. 1993;3:141–61. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Merfeld DM, Zupan L, Peterka RJ. Humans use internal models to estimate gravity and linear acceleration. Nature. 1999;398:615–8. doi: 10.1038/19303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Laurens J, Meng H, Angelaki DE. Computation of linear acceleration through an internal model in the macaque cerebellum. Nat Neurosci. 2013;16:1701–8. doi: 10.1038/nn.3530. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Meng H, Blazquez PM, Dickman JD, Angelaki DE. Diversity of vestibular nuclei neurons targeted by cerebellar nodulus inhibition. J Physiol. 2014;592:171–88. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2013.259614. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Waespe W, Cohen B, Raphan T. Dynamic modification of the vestibulo-ocular reflex by the nodulus and uvula. Science. 1985;228:199–202. doi: 10.1126/science.3871968. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Solomon D, Cohen B. Stimulation of the nodulus and uvula discharges velocity storage in the vestibulo-ocular reflex. Exp Brain Res. 1994;102:57–68. doi: 10.1007/BF00232438. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Walberg F, Dietrichs E. The interconnection between the vestibular nuclei and the nodulus: a study of reciprocity. Brain Res. 1988;449:47–53. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(88)91022-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Raphan T, Sturm D. Modeling the spatiotemporal organization of velocity storage in the vestibuloocular reflex by optokinetic studies. J Neurophysiol. 1991;66:1410–21. doi: 10.1152/jn.1991.66.4.1410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Schuerger RJ, Balaban CD. Organization of the coeruleo-vestibular pathway in rats, rabbits, and monkeys. Brain Res. 1999;30:189–217. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(99)00015-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Halberstadt AL, Balaban CD. Selective anterograde tracing of the individual serotonergic and nonserotonergic components of the dorsal raphe nucleus projection to the vestibular nuclei. Neuroscience. 2007;147:207–23. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2007.03.049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Buisseret-Delmas C, Compoint C, Delfini C, Buisseret P. Organization of reciprocal connections between trigeminal and vestibular nuclei in the rat. J Comp Neurol. 1999;409:153–68. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19990621)409:1<153::AID-CNE11>3.0.CO;2-#. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Dai M, Kunin M, Raphan T, Cohen B. The relation of motion sickness to the spatial-temporal properties of velocity storage. Exp Brain Res. 2003;151:173–89. doi: 10.1007/s00221-003-1479-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Shin JH, Kim YK, Kim H-J, Kim J-S. Altered brain metabolism in vestibular migraine: comparison of interictal and icatal findings. Cephalalgia. 2014;34:58–67. doi: 10.1177/0333102413498940. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Messina R, et al. Structural brain abnormalities in patients with vestibular migraine. J Neurol. 2017;264:295–303. doi: 10.1007/s00415-016-8349-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Obermann M, et al. Central vestibular system modulation in vestibular migraine. Cephalalgia. 2014;34:1053–61. doi: 10.1177/0333102414527650. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Bednarczuk NF, et al. Abnormal visuo-vestibular interactions in vestibular migraine: a cross sectional study. Brain. 2019;142:606–16. doi: 10.1093/brain/awy355. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Fernandez MM, et al. Pharmacologic agents for the prevention of vestibular migraine. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2015;6:CD010600. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD010600.pub2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Czuczwar SJ, Patsalos PN. The new generation of GABA enhancers. Potential in the treatment of epilepsy. CNS Drugs. 2001;15:339–50. doi: 10.2165/00023210-200115050-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Alviña K, Khodakhah K. The therapeutic mode of action of 4-aminopyridine in cerebellar ataxia. J Neurosci. 2010;30:7258–68. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3582-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Çelebisoy N, et al. Acetazolamide in vestibular migraine prophylaxis: a retrospective study. Eur Arch Otorhinolaryngol. 2016;273:2947–51. doi: 10.1007/s00405-015-3874-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Edvinsson L, Haanes KA, Warfvinge K, Krause DN. CGRP as the target of new migraine therapies– successful translation from bench to clinic. Nature Rev Neuro. 2018;14:338–50. doi: 10.1038/s41582-018-0003-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Noseda R, et al. A neural mechanism for exacerbation of headache by light. Nat Neurosci. 2010;13:239–45. doi: 10.1038/nn.2475. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Karmali F, Chaudhuri SE, Yi Y, Merfeld DM. Determining thresholds using adaptive procedures and psychometric fits: evaluating efficiency using theory, simulations, and human experiments. Exp Brain Res. 2016;234:773–89. doi: 10.1007/s00221-015-4501-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Dimitri PS, Wall C, 3rd, Oas JG. Classification of human rotation test results using parametric modeling and multivariate statistics. Acta Otolaryngol. 1996;116:497–506. doi: 10.3109/00016489609137880. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Bermudez Rey MC, et al. Vestibular perceptual thresholds increase above the age of 40. Front Neurol. 2016;7:162. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2016.00162. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Wall CW. The sinusoidal harmonic acceleration rotary chair test. Theoretical and clinical basis. Neurol Clin. 1990;8:269–85. doi: 10.1016/S0733-8619(18)30355-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Chauduri SE, Merfeld DM. Signal detection theory and vestibular perception: III Estimating unbiased fit parameters for psychometric functions. Exp Brain Res. 2013;225:133–46. doi: 10.1007/s00221-012-3354-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Feng Y, Schlösser FJ, Sumpio BE. The Semmes Weinstein monofilament examination as a screening tool for diabetic peripheral neuropathy. J Vasc Surg. 2009;50:675–82. doi: 10.1016/j.jvs.2009.05.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Benson AJ, Hutt EC, Brown SF. Thresholds for the perception of whole-body angular movement about a vertical axis. Aviat Space Environ Med. 1989;60:205–13. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]