Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2019 Oct 8;8:e48114. doi: 10.7554/eLife.48114

Pretectal neurons control hunting behaviour

Paride Antinucci 1, Mónica Folgueira 2,3, Isaac H Bianco 1,
Editors: Vatsala Thirumalai4, Ronald L Calabrese5
PMCID: PMC6783268  PMID: 31591961

Abstract

For many species, hunting is an innate behaviour that is crucial for survival, yet the circuits that control predatory action sequences are poorly understood. We used larval zebrafish to identify a population of pretectal neurons that control hunting. By combining calcium imaging with a virtual hunting assay, we identified a discrete pretectal region that is selectively active when animals initiate hunting. Targeted genetic labelling allowed us to examine the function and morphology of individual cells and identify two classes of pretectal neuron that project to ipsilateral optic tectum or the contralateral tegmentum. Optogenetic stimulation of single neurons of either class was able to induce sustained hunting sequences, in the absence of prey. Furthermore, laser ablation of these neurons impaired prey-catching and prevented induction of hunting by optogenetic stimulation of the anterior-ventral tectum. We propose that this specific population of pretectal neurons functions as a command system to induce predatory behaviour.

Research organism: Zebrafish

eLife digest

Hunting is an innate behaviour that relies on predators executing a precise set of actions to identify, approach, target, and strike prey. In vertebrates (animals with a backbone), the identity of the brain circuits that trigger hunting is still unclear. These networks are thought to link sensory perception (seeing prey) with a specialised action (starting an attack).

Larval zebrafish are a good model in which to study these circuits, because they have a tiny, transparent brain where neurons can be observed in real time. In addition, it is expected that the networks that control hunting in this species will be preserved across other vertebrates.

To discover these networks, Antinucci et al. genetically engineered zebrafish larvae so that their brain cells would ‘glow’ when they became active. This revealed which individual brain cells would turn on when zebrafish started to hunt. These neurons were in a part of the brain called the pretectum, which receives visual information about prey from the eye.

Next, Antinucci et al. harnessed a technique called optogenetics to artificially turn on these brain cells in the pretectum, which caused the fish to start hunting even when prey was absent. In fact, stimulating just one pretectal cell was enough to trigger the behaviour. Conversely, killing pretectal brain cells using precise laser surgery hindered hunting in zebrafish exposed to prey.

These experiments suggest that pretectal brain cells act like a command centre that controls hunting. They likely play a decision-making role, determining when animals do and do not respond to events in their surroundings. Similar neurons likely control other types of behaviour. Understanding how these circuits work at the cellular level in zebrafish may help scientist study them in other organisms, such as humans.

Introduction

In response to sensory information and internal states, animals select specific actions from a repertoire of options and produce adaptive behavioural programmes. Neuroethological studies in a variety of species have pinpointed brain regions, and identified neurons, that specifically promote particular behaviours ranging from the courtship songs of fruit flies (von Philipsborn et al., 2011; Inagaki et al., 2014) to parental behaviours of mice (Kohl et al., 2018). Identifying the neural circuits that control specific behaviours, as well as modulatory systems that influence if and how behaviours are performed, will shed light on the neural mechanisms of decision making, action selection and motor sequence generation.

Prey catching is an innate, complex behaviour that is crucial for survival (Sillar et al., 2016). In various species, hunting responses can be evoked by prey-like stimuli, defined by specific conjunctions of sensory features (Ewert, 1997; Anjum et al., 2006; Bianco and Engert, 2015), and predatory behaviour is modulated by internal state variables including associative learning and feeding drive (Ewert et al., 2001; Jordi et al., 2015). Several brain regions show activity during hunting and are expected to subserve neural functions including prey detection and localisation, control of pursuit, capture and consummatory actions, and motivation (e.g. Comoli et al., 2005). Although electrical stimulation of brain regions, including the optic tectum, can evoke hunting actions (Ewert, 1970; Bels et al., 2012) and recent studies in rodents have identified circuits that motivate predatory behaviour (Han et al., 2017; Li et al., 2018; Park et al., 2018), premotor circuits that directly control vertebrate hunting have yet to be identified.

In this study, we sought to identify neurons that control predatory behaviour, using larval zebrafish as a vertebrate model system. In larval zebrafish, hunting is an innate, visually guided behaviour, which involves a sequence of specialised oculomotor and locomotor actions. A defining characteristic is that larvae initiate hunting by rapidly converging their eyes, which substantially increases their binocular visual field (Bianco et al., 2011). A high vergence angle is maintained during prey pursuit, which entails a sequence of discrete orienting turns and approach swims, which culminate in binocular fixation of prey followed by a kinematically distinct capture swim (Borla et al., 2002; McElligott and O'malley, 2005; Bianco et al., 2011; Patterson et al., 2013; Trivedi and Bollmann, 2013; Marques et al., 2018). Neural activity associated with prey-like visual cues has been detected in the axon terminals of a specific class of retinal ganglion cell (RGC), which terminate in retinal arborisation field 7 (AF7) in the pretectum (Semmelhack et al., 2014). Prey-responsive pretectal cells have also been described (Muto et al., 2017) as well as highly prey-selective feature-analysing neurons in the optic tectum (OT) that display non-linear mixed selectivity for conjunctions of visual features (Bianco and Engert, 2015). Premotor activity in localised tectal assemblies immediately precedes execution of hunting responses (Bianco and Engert, 2015) and optogenetic stimulation of the anterior-ventral tectal region can induce hunting-like behaviour (Fajardo et al., 2013). Finally, ablation of RGC input to either AF7 or OT substantially impairs hunting (Gahtan et al., 2005; Semmelhack et al., 2014).

Based on this evidence, we hypothesised that neural circuits controlling the induction of hunting might be located in the vicinity of AF7 or OT. Our experimental requirements for identifying such neurons were (1) that they display neural activity specifically related to execution of hunting behaviour, rather than visual detection of prey, and (2) that direct stimulation of such neurons would induce naturalistic predatory behaviour in the absence of prey. First, we used 2-photon calcium imaging paired with a virtual hunting assay and identified a high density of neurons in the AF7-pretectal region that were specifically recruited when larvae initiated hunting behaviour. We identified a transgenic line that labelled these neurons and found two morphological classes: One projects ipsilaterally to the optic tectum and the second extends long-range projections to midbrain oculomotor nuclei, the nucleus of the medial longitudinal fasciculus and the contralateral hindbrain tegmentum. Remarkably, optogenetic stimulation of single pretectal neurons evoked hunting-like behaviour in the absence of prey. Pretectal projection neurons of either class could evoke hunting routines with naturalistic oculomotor and locomotor kinematics but opposite directional biases. Finally, laser-ablation of the pretectal population impaired hunting of live prey and prevented induction of hunting by optogenetic stimulation of the rostral tectum. In sum, we propose that this specific population of pretectal neurons functions downstream of prey perception to directly control execution of predatory behaviour.

Results

Neural activity associated with hunting initiation

To identify and distinguish neurons with activity related to prey perception and initiation of hunting behaviour, we performed 2-photon calcium imaging while larval zebrafish engaged in a virtual hunting assay (Figure 1A) (Bianco and Engert, 2015). Transgenic elavl3:H2B-GCaMP6s;atoh7:gapRFP larvae (6–7 dpf, N = 8) were partially restrained in agarose gel, but with their eyes and tail free to move, and were presented with a range of visual cues including small moving prey-like spots, which evoke naturalistic hunting responses (Figure 1B) (Bianco et al., 2011; Bianco and Engert, 2015). We imaged a volume that encompassed the majority of the primary retinorecipient sites [arborisation fields (AFs) 2–10] as well as surrounding brain regions including pretectum and OT (310 × 310 × 100 µm volume; Figure 1C and Video 1). Eye and tail kinematics were tracked online, allowing automated detection of hunting responses. These are defined by saccadic convergence of the eyes – an oculomotor behaviour specific to hunting initiation and frequently coincident with lateralised tail movements (91.6 ± 4.9% of convergent saccades paired with tail movement; eye-tail latency 11.6 ± 5.2 ms, mean ± SD; Figure 1D and Figure 1—figure supplement 1A,B) (Bianco et al., 2011; Patterson et al., 2013; Trivedi and Bollmann, 2013; Bianco and Engert, 2015). Larvae preferentially responded to small, dark, moving spots and hunting was initiated most frequently once the stimulus had crossed the midline axis and was moving in a nose-tail direction (Figure 1E,F and Figure 1—figure supplement 1C) (Bianco and Engert, 2015).

Figure 1. Neural activity associated with hunting.

(A) 2-photon GCaMP imaging combined with behavioural tracking during virtual hunting behaviour (see Materials and methods). (B) Schematic of visual stimuli. (C) elavl3:H2B-GCaMP6s;atoh7:gapRFP reference brain showing imaging volume (green box), which encompassed most retinal arborisation fields (AF2–10). In the right hemisphere, RFP has been pseudo-coloured to demarcate specific AFs. (C’) Example of neuronal activity (ΔF/F0) within one focal plane in response to a dark, leftwards moving prey-like spot (mean activity over eight presentations) overlaid onto anatomical image (grey). (D) Example of behavioural tracking data indicating hunting initiation (eye convergence and leftwards J-turn) in response to a dark, leftwards moving prey-like spot. Asterisk indicates time of convergent saccade. cw, clockwise; ccw, counter-clockwise. (E) Distribution of spot locations at time of convergent saccade. Ticks indicates median location for leftwards (blue, –18.13°, N = 162 events in eight fish) and rightwards (red, 22.10°, N = 122 events) moving spots. (F) Hunting response probability (mean + SEM, N = 8 fish) across visual stimuli. (G) Schematic of the visuomotor vector (VMV) generated for each neuron. (H) VMVs of all clustered neurons (N = 93,054 neurons from eight fish). Within each cluster, neurons are ordered according to decreasing correlation with the cluster seed centroid (mean VMV). Coloured lines on the left highlight hunting-related clusters (prey-responsive clusters in blue, hunting-initiation clusters in red). (I) Enlargement showing VMVs of selected hunting-related clusters (1, 4, 25–28). Number of cells in each cluster is shown on right. (J) Stimulus-aligned activity during non-response (top) and response (bottom) trials for neurons in selected clusters (indicated top). (K) Hunting Index (HIx). Top schematic indicates how HIx is computed from calcium signals and bottom shows distribution of HIx scores for selected clusters. Scale bars, 100 µm. A, anterior; D, dorsal; L, leftwards; P, posterior; R, rightwards; V, ventral; Sym, symmetric. See also Figure 1—figure supplements 1 and 2 and Video 1.

Figure 1—source data 1. Source data for Figure 1.
Data provided as a MATLAB structure.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.48114.006

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Behavioural and clustering analyses.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

(A) Fraction of convergent saccades associated with leftwards, rightwards, symmetrical or no tail movement. (B) Mean time from convergent saccade to tail movement, per fish. (C) Cumulative distributions of spot locations at convergent saccade (N = 8 fish). The distributions of median spot locations for individual fish are reported at the top. (D) Scatter plot indicating maximum visual response versus goodness of fit of regression modelling, for all recorded cells. Coloured points indicate cells in the top 5th percentile for at least one metric, which were selected for unsupervised clustering (N = 17,181 cells out of 181,123). (E) Examples of three regressors used for multilinear regression modelling. In each case, three representative behavioural events are shown with the value of the regressor and recorded calcium activity from a well modelled cell. Coefficients from multilinear regression contribute to the VMV of the neuron. cw, clockwise; ccw, counter-clockwise; L, leftwards; R, rightwards; SD, standard deviation. (F) Number of neurons in cluster seeds. (G) Number of neurons in final clusters. (H) Cluster centroids (mean VMVs) for all 36 clusters. (I) Distributions of VMV components for all clusters. Distributions across all clustered neurons are overlaid in grey. Y-axis ranges from 0 to 0.4 (fraction), x-axis ranges from –2 to 6 (SD).
Figure 1—figure supplement 2. Stimulus and motor-triggered calcium responses.

Figure 1—figure supplement 2.

(A–B) Visual stimulus-aligned (A) and eye convergence-aligned (B) ΔF/F0 responses for all 36 clusters. Responses are shown for small moving spots (dark/bright moving leftwards/rightwards, as indicated at top of columns). Traces show mean ±95% confidence intervals across all neurons in each cluster. Dashed vertical lines indicate start/end of stimulus presentation in non-response trials (A), or time of saccadic convergence (B). L, leftwards; R, rightwards. X-axis reports time in seconds.

Video 1. Z-stack of transgenic line used for calcium imaging with annotated RGC arborisation fields.

Download video file (4.9MB, mp4)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.48114.007

Imaging volume (z-stack) of 6 dpf elavl3:H2B-GCaMP6s;atoh7:gapRFP brain (mean of N = 3 fish) with labelled RGC arborisation fields (AFs). The green channel shows the elavl3:H2B-GCaMP6s reference brain used for anatomical registration. Related to Figure 1.

To define groups of neurons with consistent functional properties related to the first stages of hunting behaviour, we first computed, for every cell, a visuomotor vector (VMV) that quantified its sensory and motor-related activity (Figure 1G and Materials and methods). Each VMV described (a) mean GCaMP fluorescence responses to each of the ten visual cues during non-response trials, in which larvae did not release hunting behaviour, and (b) coefficients from multilinear regression of fluorescent timeseries data on a set of motor predictors (derived from eye and tail kinematics; Figure 1—figure supplement 1E and Supplementary file 1). Three motor predictors indicated convergent saccades associated with leftwards, rightwards or symmetrical/no tail movements (labelled ‘Conv tail L’, ‘Conv tail R’ and ‘Conv tail sym’, respectively). Next, we used an unsupervised clustering procedure to identify consistent sensorimotor tuning profiles. A correlation-based agglomerative hierarchical clustering algorithm performed initial clustering of VMVs from cells with either high visually evoked activity or that were well modelled in terms of motor variables (comprising ~10% of neurons; Figure 1—figure supplement 1D and Materials and methods). The centroids of the resultant cluster ‘seeds’ defined a set of functional archetypes (Figure 1—figure supplement 1F) and subsequently, all remaining neurons were assigned to the cluster with the closest centroid within a threshold distance limit (Pearson’s r ≥ 0.7, VMVs below threshold remained unassigned). This approach allowed us to classify more than 50% of imaged neurons (93,054 out of 181,123 cells) into 36 clusters with homogenous functional properties (Figure 1H and Figure 1—figure supplement 1G–I).

This analysis identified neurons that were recruited during hunting initiation. Specifically, four clusters showed activity highly correlated with eye convergence events (clusters 25–28; Figure 1I) yet exhibited little activity in response to visual cues (including prey-like moving spots; Figure 1J and Figure 1—figure supplement 2A,B). In two of these clusters, activity was selective for the direction of motor responses. Thus, cluster 26 was selective for convergent saccades associated with leftwards turns and cluster 28 was tuned to rightwards hunting responses. By contrast, the other two clusters did not show selectivity for the direction of tail movements during hunting initiation (cluster 25, associated with symmetrical/no tail movement and 27, responsive to motion in either direction). Other functional clusters comprised visually responsive neurons that were selectively activated by small prey-like moving spots (‘prey-responsive’ clusters 1–6; Figure 1H–J and Figure 1—figure supplement 2A), but displayed little motor-related activity.

We computed a ‘hunting index’ (HIx) for individual neurons as a direct means to distinguish neural activity associated with hunting initiation from ‘sensory’ activity evoked by prey-like visual stimuli. Briefly, for each hunting response, GCaMP fluorescence in a time window (±1 s) surrounding the convergent saccade was compared to activity at the same time in non-response trials during which the same visual stimulus was presented (Figure 1K, top). The mean of these difference measures across all response events represents the HIx score for the cell and quantifies neural activity attributable to hunting initiation while accounting for any visually evoked response. To account for directional tuning, we separately computed HIx for hunting responses paired with leftwards, rightwards or symmetrical/no tail movements. This analysis revealed that neurons in clusters 25–28 showed considerably higher HIx scores than other cells, including those in prey-responsive clusters (1 and 4, Figure 1K). Tail directional preferences were consistent with those determined by regression modelling.

Overall, our functional analyses identified four clusters of neurons with activity specifically associated with the specialised motor outputs that characterise initiation of hunting behaviour and showed little activity in response to prey-like visual cues. Thus, we will refer to these as ‘hunting-initiation’ clusters.

Pretectal neurons are recruited during hunting initiation

A high density of neurons in hunting-initiation clusters were located in the pretectum. We showed this by registering volumetric imaging data to a reference brain atlas (‘ZBB’ and a high-resolution elavl3:H2B-GCaMP6s volume, see Materials and methods and Video 1) (Marquart et al., 2015; Marquart et al., 2017). Neurons within functionally defined clusters showed distinct anatomical distributions (Figure 2—figure supplement 1). Notably, a high density of neurons belonging to hunting-initiation clusters were found in pretectal regions in the immediate vicinity of AF7 (AF7-pretectum), just anterior to the rostral pole of the optic tectum (Figure 2A,B and Figure 2—figure supplement 2). Neurons selective for the direction of hunting-related tail motion (clusters 26 and 28) showed lateralised, mirror-symmetric distributions, with a larger fraction of cells located on the side of the brain contralateral to the direction of preferred tail movement (Figure 2A, right panel). Specifically, cluster 26, which is tuned to eye convergence associated with leftwards tail movement, had a higher density of cells in the right AF7-pretectum, and vice versa for cluster 28. Hunting-initiation clusters that were agnostic to tail direction (clusters 25 and 27) showed largely symmetric anatomical distributions (Figure 2A, middle panel). Neurons belonging to prey-responsive clusters were also found in AF7-pretectum as well as in the medial thalamus, where direction-selective neurons showed highly lateralised distributions (Figure 2A,B, left panel).

Figure 2. AF7-pretectum contains a high density of hunting initiation neurons.

(A) Anatomical maps of prey-responsive clusters (left) and hunting-initiation clusters (middle and right). Images show dorsal views of intensity sum projections of all neuronal masks in each cluster after registration to the elavl3:H2B-GCaMP6s reference brain (grey). Insets show fraction of neurons in left and right AF7-pretectum or medial thalamus belonging to specified clusters. (B) Ventro-dorsal cross-section views of anatomical maps. (C) Visual stimulus-aligned activity during non-response trials for prey-responsive clusters (left) and hunting-initiation clusters (middle and right; mean ± SEM). Activity is displayed separately for left (blue) and right (red) hemisphere neurons in each cluster. Insets show single-trial responses for a single example cell from each cluster (mean as thick line). (D) Activity aligned to convergent saccades associated with leftwards (top), rightwards (bottom), or symmetrical/no tail movements (middle). Activity during both spontaneous and visually evoked events was used. Scale bars, 100 µm. A, anterior; D, dorsal; L, left; P, posterior; R, right; V, ventral; Stim, stimulus.

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Anatomical maps of clusters.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

Images show dorsal views of intensity sum projections of all neuronal masks in each cluster (magenta) after registration to the elavl3:H2B-GCaMP6s reference brain. Projections (obtained through all focal planes,~100 µm total depth) are overlaid on a maximum-intensity projection image (grey) from the elavl3:H2B-GCaMP6s reference brain (five planes, 5 µm depth, from focal planes with the largest number of neurons in each cluster). Scale bar, 100 µm. A, anterior; L, left; P, posterior; R, right.
Figure 2—figure supplement 2. Anatomical locations of clustered neurons.

Figure 2—figure supplement 2.

(A) Anatomical location of clusters (N = 8 fish). The fraction of cells in each cluster falling within each ZBB anatomical region is shown. Red box highlights AF7-pretectum. Y-axis ranges from 0 to 0.4 (fraction). (B) Fraction of imaged cells within each anatomical region that were assigned to each cluster type. Red box highlights AF7-pretectum. Y-axis ranges from 0 to 0.15 (fraction of imaged neurons in brain region). (C) Fraction of neurons in each cluster located in the left or right brain hemisphere. Y-axis ranges from 0 to 1 (fraction).

To further examine visuomotor tuning, we computed visual stimulus-aligned and convergence-aligned activity profiles separately for left and right hemisphere neurons. This confirmed that prey-responsive neurons in clusters 1 and 4 showed direction-selective activity in response to small dark moving spots, but minimal activity associated with convergent saccades (Figure 2C,D, left columns). In contrast, hunting-initiation neurons (clusters 25–28) showed weak visual responses – as shown by moving spot-triggered activity during non-response trials – but substantial activity triggered on hunting initiation. For clusters 26 and 28, neurons showed stronger activation when convergent saccades were paired with contraversive left and right-sided turns, respectively (Figure 2C,D, right columns).

In summary, we identified populations of neurons in AF7-pretectum that are active in association with two distinct components of hunting – visual responses to prey and initiation of predatory behaviour. We subsequently examined whether pretectal cells with hunting-initiation activity are directly involved in inducing hunting behaviour.

AF7-pretectal neurons are labelled by the KalTA4u508 transgene

To characterise the connectivity and function of AF7-pretectal neurons with hunting-initiation activity, we inspected the expression patterns of a range of transgenic driver lines and identified a transgene, KalTA4u508, which labels neurons in the AF7-pretectal region (Figure 3A–C). Additionally, KalTA4u508 labels populations of cells within the midbrain, hindbrain, sensory ganglia and spinal cord as well as non-neural tissues (Figure 3—figure supplement 1A). However, anatomical registration of KalTA4u508;UAS:mCherry volumetric data to the reference atlas revealed a high density of labelled somata in AF7-pretectum, overlapping with the locations of hunting-initiation clusters (Figure 3A,B,I). We generated KalTA4u508;UAS:RFP;atoh7:GFP larvae to visualise GFP-labelled RGC axon terminals in AF7, and observed that a subset of KalTA4u508-expressing neurons extend dendritic arbours that directly juxtapose RGC terminals (N = 4 fish; Figure 3C,D). In brain sections from adult (3 month old) KalTA4u508;UAS:GCaMP6f;atoh7:gapRFP fish, labelled neurons in the pretectum were very sparse and could be identified only in the accessory pretectal nucleus (APN; N = 8 somata from four fish; Figure 3E). This suggests that at least a subset of KalTA4u508-expressing neurons reside in a region of the larval AF7-pretectum corresponding to the adult APN.

Figure 3. AF7-pretectal neurons with distinct projection patterns labelled by KalTA4u508.

(A) Dorsal view of KalTA4u508;UAS:mCherry expression at 6 dpf (green) registered to the elavl3:H2B-GCaMP6s reference brain (grey). Neurons of all four hunting-initiation clusters combined are shown in purple, colour-coded according to local cell density (clusters 25–28, N = 6,630 cells from eight fish). AF7-pretectum is indicated in yellow and the region is enlarged in (A’). (B) Ventro-dorsal cross-section of data in (A). (C) Left AF7-pretectum in a 6 dpf KalTA4u508;UAS:RFP;atoh7:GFP larva (dorsal view, maximum-intensity projections, 10 planes, 10 µm depth). (D) Dendritic stratification of KalTA4u508 neurons (green) relative to RGC axons (magenta) in AF7. Y-axis indicates distance from the skin in µm (dashed white arrow in C). Mean and individual stratification patterns are reported (N = 4 fish). (E) KalTA4u508 neurons in pretectum of a 3 month-old KalTA4u508;UAS:GCaMP6f;atoh7:gapRFP fish. Pretectal and tectal regions in the left hemisphere are shown. Schematic indicates location of micrograph and pretectal nuclei (transverse plane). Number of KalTA4u508 cells in each pretectal nucleus are reported in (E’) (N = 4 fish). APN, accessory pretectal nucleus; CC, cerebellar corpus; cpop, postoptic commissure; Hb, habenula; HL, hypothalamic lobe; OT, optic tectum; ot, optic tract; PCe, central pretectal nucleus; PO, posterior pretectal nucleus; PSm, magnocellular superficial pretectal nucleus, PSp, parvocellular superficial pretectal nucleus; Tel, telencephalon. (F) Tracings of individual KalTA4u508 projection neurons that innervate the ipsilateral tectum (‘ipsi-projecting’ cells, N = 4 cells from four fish). Data is from 6 to 7 dpf larvae and is registered to the elavl3:H2B-GCaMP6s reference brain (grey). Selected anatomical regions from the ZBB brain atlas are overlaid. To enable morphological comparisons, all traced neurons are shown in the left hemisphere. (G) Fluorescence profiles of neurites of ipsi-projecting KalTA4u508 cells along the rostro-caudal (R-C, left) and ventral-dorsal (V-D, right) axes of the optic tectum (dashed red arrows in F). Mean and individual profiles are reported (N = 4 cells). (H) Tracings of KalTA4u508 projection neurons innervating the contralateral hindbrain (‘contra-projecting’ cells, N = 5 cells from five fish). Dendritic arbours adjacent to AF7 are enlarged in inset (H’). nMLF, nucleus of the medial longitudinal fasciculus; RS, reticulospinal system. (I) Soma location of ipsi- and contra-projecting KalTA4u508 cells in AF7-pretectum. Scale bars, 100 µm, except (A’), (H’), (I), 50 µm, and (C), 20 µm. A, anterior; C, caudal; D, dorsal; L, left; P, posterior; R, right (rostral in G); V, ventral. See also Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

Figure 3—source data 1. Source data for Figure 3.
Data provided as a MATLAB structure.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.48114.013

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. KalTA4u508 neurons innervating cerebellum, and PA-GFP projection mapping from AF7-pretectum.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

(A) Expression pattern of the KalTA4u508 transgene, illustrated by a KalTA4u508;UAS:GCaMP6f transgenic larva (6 dpf). (B) Tracings of KalTA4u508 neurons projecting to ipsilateral medial corpus cerebellum (N = 2 cells from two fish). The bottom image shows tracings overlaid with selected anatomical regions from the ZBB brain atlas. (C) Tracings of PA-GFP-labelled AF7-pretectal cells projecting to oculomotor nuclei and contralateral hindbrain in 7 dpf α-tubulin:C3PA-GFP larvae (N = 4 fish) registered to the elavl3:H2B-GCaMP6s reference brain (grey). The photo-activation site is indicated in magenta. (D) PA-GFP-labelled AF7-pretectal cells in a 7 dpf α-tubulin:C3PA-GFP larva. The photo-activation site is indicated in magenta. Anterogradely labelled axonal terminals are visible in the ipsilateral medial cerebellum (bottom image, z-plane location is relative to top z-plane). (E) A second example of photoactivation that retrogradely labelled cell bodies in the ipsilateral anterior-ventral optic tectum. Scale bars, 100 µm except (A), 200 µm. A, anterior; L, left; P, posterior; R, right.

Next, we sought to examine the morphology of pretectal KalTA4u508 neurons. Because transgene expression is not restricted to the pretectum, we used a transient expression strategy to label individual cells by microinjection of a UAS:CoChR-tdTomato DNA construct into one-cell stage KalTA4u508;elavl3:H2B-GCaMP6s embryos. This allowed us to specifically examine the morphology and function of individual KalTA4u508-expressing pretectal neurons. High-contrast membrane labelling by CoChR-tdTomato facilitated morphological reconstruction (at 6–7 dpf) and single-cell tracings were registered to the brain atlas using the H2B-GCaMP6s channel (Figure 3F, H).

We identified three morphological classes of pretectal neuron labelled by KalTA4u508. One class projects to the ipsilateral optic tectum (Figure 3F), elaborating axon terminal arbours preferentially in the most anterior-ventral aspect of OT (N = 4 cells from four fish; Figure 3G). The second class makes descending projections to the midbrain and hindbrain tegmentum. Axons decussate near the nucleus of the medial longitudinal fasciculus (nMLF) and the oculomotor nucleus (nIII) before extending caudally into the contralateral ventral hindbrain. Axon collaterals could be observed bilaterally in the vicinity of nIII/nMLF and proximal to ventral reticulospinal neurons in the contralateral hindbrain (N = 5 cells from five fish; Figure 3H). This class of projection neuron extended dendrites within a neuropil region that includes AF7 (Figure 3H’), a feature not observed in the other two classes. The third class was characterised by ipsilateral axonal projections to the medial region of the corpus cerebellum (N = 2 cells from two fish; Figure 3—figure supplement 1B). Neurite tracing using photoactivatable GFP confirmed a pretectal projection to the cerebellum (Figure 3—figure supplement 1D) as well as to nIII/nMLF and contralateral ventral hindbrain (Figure 3—figure supplement 1C). This latter projection pattern is compatible with data on APN efferent projections in adult zebrafish (Yáñez et al., 2018). In combination with our adult expression data (Figure 3E), we conclude that the subset of KalTA4u508 pretectal neurons projecting to contralateral hindbrain belong to the larval APN.

Pretectal KalTA4u508-expressing neurons have hunting-initiation activity

Next, we asked whether KalTA4u508 pretectal neurons are responsive to prey-like stimuli and/or are recruited during hunting initiation. We performed 2-photon calcium imaging in KalTA4u508;UAS:GCaMP6f or KalTA4u508;UAS:GCaMP7f transgenic larvae during the virtual hunting assay (6–7 dpf, N = 30 fish). Notably, KalTA4u508 pretectal neurons exhibited negligible activity in response to visual stimuli (Figure 4D and Figure 4—figure supplement 1). Visuomotor vectors were generated for individual cells allowing ~51% to be assigned cluster identities based on the functional archetypes established previously using pan-neuronal imaging (correlation threshold = 0.7, N = 188 out of 369 cells).

Figure 4. KalTA4u508 pretectal neurons are active during hunting initiation.

(A) VMVs of KalTA4u508 neurons with assigned cluster identities (N = 188 neurons from 30 fish). Cell location (blue for left hemisphere, red for right) is reported by the ‘Brain side’ column. L, left; R, right; Sym, symmetric. (B) Fraction of assigned KalTA4u508 neurons in each cluster. (C) Hunting Index (HIx) scores for KalTA4u508 neurons in different clusters (mean + SEM). (D) Visual stimulus-aligned responses of KalTA4u508 neurons during non-response trials (mean ± SEM). Traces are colour-coded according to anatomical laterality (blue for left hemisphere, red for right). Stim, stimulus. (E) Eye convergence-aligned neuronal responses. Activity during both spontaneous and visually evoked convergent saccades was used. See also Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

Figure 4—source data 1. Source data for Figure 4.
Data provided as a MATLAB structure.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.48114.016

Figure 4.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1. Visual responses and activity during spontaneous convergences of KalTA4u508 pretectal neurons.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

Distributions of maximum responses across visual stimuli for all recorded neurons in 6–7 dpf elavl3:H2B-GCaMP6s larvae (grey, N = 181,123 cells from eight fish) and KalTA4u508;UAS:GCaMP6f, or KalTA4u508;UAS:jGCaMP7f larvae (magenta, N = 369 cells from 30 fish). Before determining the maximum responses for each neuron, mean integrated ΔF/F0 for each visual stimulus was normalised by dividing values by the corresponding standard deviation (SD) across all neurons from elavl3:H2B-GcaMP6s larvae.

Of the KalTA4u508 pretectal neurons assigned functional identities, 28% were associated with hunting-initiation clusters (clusters 25–28; 52/188 cells; Figure 4A,B). The remaining neurons were assigned to either conjugate eye movement clusters (57%) or tail movement clusters (15%) and, in line with the absence of visual sensory responses, no cells were assigned to prey-responsive clusters. Of the hunting-initiation neurons, most KalTA4u508 cells were associated with functional clusters 26 and 28, which show direction-selective activity (Figure 4B,E) and neurons in these two clusters were predominantly located contralateral to the direction of preferred tail movement (73% and 80% contralateral for cluster identities 26 and 28, respectively). The absence of visual activity in KalTA4u508 neurons is in contrast to the small responses seen previously in hunting-initiation clusters (Figure 2C), suggesting some functional heterogeneity in clusters derived from pan-neuronal imaging. KalTA4u508 cells assigned to hunting-initiation clusters had higher HIx scores than those assigned to other clusters (Figure 4C) supporting the hunting-response specificity of their activity.

In summary, KalTA4u508 provides genetic access to a subset of AF7-pretectal neurons that are selectively active during initiation of hunting behaviour.

Optogenetic activation of single KalTA4u508-expressing pretectal neurons induces hunting

To test whether KalTA4u508 pretectal cells are capable of inducing predatory behaviour, we optogenetically stimulated single neurons and used high-speed video tracking to monitor free-swimming behaviour (Figure 5A). To do this, we used the same larvae described above in which single KalTA4u508 pretectal cells expressed the optogenetic actuator CoChR-tdTomato (Figure 5B) (Klapoetke et al., 2014). Experiments consisted of repeated trials, each of 8 s duration, in which larvae (6–7 dpf, N = 70) were exposed to 7 s blue light stimulation (470 nm, 0.44 mW/mm2), interleaved with trials with no stimulation.

Figure 5. Optogenetic stimulation of single KalTA4u508 pretectal neurons induces hunting.

(A) Optogenetic stimulation of single neurons paired with behavioural tracking. (B) A single KalTA4u508 neuron in a 7 dpf KalTA4u508;elavl3:H2B-GcaMP6s larva that was injected at the one-cell stage with UAS:CoChR-tdTomato DNA. This ‘contra-projecting’ neuron is ‘Cell 3’ in Figure 3H. A, anterior; L, left; P, posterior; R, right. Scale bar, 100 µm. (C) Example frames from an optogenetically induced hunting event. Labels indicate time relative to saccadic eye convergence/hunting initiation. (D) Tail angle (grey) and ocular vergence angle (red) during an optogenetically induced hunting event (‘ipsi-projecting’ cell located in the left AF7-pretectum; this neuron is ‘Cell 4’ in Figure 3F; see also Video 2). Asterisk indicates time of convergent saccade. (E) Larval location colour-coded by vergence angle during the example hunting event in (D). (F) Hunting response probability for LED-On versus non-stimulation trials for larvae that performed at least one eye convergence during optogenetic stimulation (N = 23 fish). (G) Morphological identity of KalTA4u508 neurons that elicited hunting upon optogenetic stimulation. Numbers of responsive larvae are reported at the bottom (N = 23 fish). (H) Hunting response probability upon optogenetic stimulation of ipsi-projecting (orange, N = 9 cells) and contra-projecting neurons (magenta, N = 14). (I–P) Comparison of behavioural kinematics between optogenetically induced hunting events (blue, N = 23 fish) and Paramecia hunting (dark grey, N = 31). Tail kinematics for non-hunting swim bouts were recorded from the same larvae that were monitored during Paramecia hunting (light grey). In (L–P), data from all bouts are plotted, whereas in (I–K) the median or mean for each larva is reported. (Q–S) Behavioural kinematics of hunting events induced by stimulation of ipsi-projecting KalTA4u508 neurons (orange, N = 9 cells) or contra-projecting neurons (magenta, N = 14). (T) VMVs and cluster identities of KalTA4u508 neurons that induced hunting upon optogenetic stimulation and subsequently underwent calcium imaging (N = 6 cells from six fish). Symbols on the left indicate projection cell class and left/right location. HIx scores are shown on right. See also Figure 5—figure supplements 1,2 and Video 2.

Figure 5—source data 1. Source data for Figure 5.
Data provided as a MATLAB structure.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.48114.020

Figure 5.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1. Behavioural kinematics of optogenetically induced hunting and responses at increased irradiance.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1.

(A–H) Behavioural kinematics for hunting events evoked by optogenetic stimulation of single ipsi-projecting (orange, N = 9 cells) and contra-projecting KalTA4u508 neurons (magenta, N = 14). In (G and H), data from all hunting events are plotted. (I) Hunting response probability upon optogenetic stimulation of single KalTA4u508 pretectal neurons at varying irradiance in sighted (blue, N = 8 fish) and blind atoh7-/- larvae (pink, N = 5). (J) Response latency at varying irradiance for all hunting events.
Figure 5—figure supplement 2. Optogenetic stimulation of the KalTA4u508 pretectal population induces hunting with short latency.

Figure 5—figure supplement 2.

(A) Digital micromirror device (DMD) setup used for patterned illumination of pretectum and behavioural tracking in partially tethered larvae. (B) Opsin expression in a 6 dpf KalTA4u508;UAS:CoChR-tdTomato larva (image acquired with 2-photon microscope). Coloured boxes indicate AF7-pretectal regions targeted for stimulation. A, anterior; L, left; P, posterior; R, right. Scale bar, 50 µm. (C) Frames from an optogenetically induced hunting event in a tethered 7 dpf KalTA4u508;UAS:CoChR-tdTomato larva. Times relative to light onset. Eye position is marked by blue lines. (D) Hunting response probability of opsin-expressing (blue) and control, opsin-negative (grey), larvae (mean ± SEM, N = 7 fish per group). Between 6 and 9 trials per stimulated region were performed in each animal. (E) Mean latency of hunting responses for left (blue) and right (red) pretectal stimulations. (F) Percentage of hunting responses in which the first tail movement was contralaterally directed.

Video 2. Hunting behaviour evoked by optogenetic stimulation of a single KalTA4u508 pretectal neuron.

Download video file (2.6MB, mp4)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.48114.021

Data from a 6 dpf larva in which a single ipsi-projecting KalTA4u508 neuron, located in the left AF7-pretectum (Cell 4 in Figure 3F), expressed CoChR. Tracking data is reported in Figure 5D,E. The video was acquired at 250 frames per second and plays at 0.4 times the original speed. The raw movie is showed on the left, and a background-subtracted inset centred on the larva is showed on the right. Related to Figure 5.

Strikingly, we found that optogenetic stimulation of individual KalTA4u508 pretectal neurons could induce sustained, hunting-like behavioural routines. As in naturalistic hunting, optogenetically induced hunting routines comprised a sequence of swim bouts that coincided with a sustained period of elevated ocular vergence (Figure 5C–E). These sequences were initiated by a convergent saccade accompanied by a lateralised swim bout (Figure 5C) and in several cases terminated with a bout resembling a capture swim and paired with jaw opening (Figure 5—figure supplement 1C,D). The fraction of KalTA4u508 pretectal neurons that induced hunting (32%, 23 out of 70 cells) was similar to the proportion that were assigned to hunting-initiation clusters (28%; Figure 4B). Hunting-like responses were entirely dependent on blue light stimulation. For responsive fish, we observed 18.1% median response probability in LED-On trials vs. 0% in LED-Off trials (p<0.0001, N = 23 fish; Figure 5F). Furthermore, control experiments demonstrated that opsin-negative animals do not produce hunting behaviours in response to blue light stimulation alone (0/16 animals; Figure 5—figure supplement 1I). By examining single-cell morphology, we found that the KalTA4u508 cells that could evoke hunting behaviour belonged to the projection classes that innervated the ipsilateral optic tectum (9/23 cells, hereafter abbreviated ‘ipsi-projecting’) or that belong to the presumptive APN and connect to the contralateral tegmentum (14/23 cells, ‘contra-projecting’; Figure 5G,H).

How closely do the hunting-like routines induced by optogenetic stimulation of KalTA4u508 pretectal neurons compare to hunting behaviour? To address this question, we compared a variety of oculomotor and locomotor kinematics between optogenetically induced hunting routines versus hunting of live Paramecia. The duration of hunting sequences, defined by the period of elevated ocular vergence, as well as vergence angles, were equivalent between natural and optogenetically evoked behaviour (Figure 5I,J). In 100% of optogenetically-evoked hunting routines, a lateralised swim bout immediately followed saccadic convergence with short latency, comparable to natural hunting (Figure 5K). We analysed several kinematic features of these initiating bouts, which resemble ‘J-turns’, focussing on features that distinguish hunting swims from spontaneous exploratory behaviour. This revealed a high degree of kinematic similarity between natural hunting bouts and optogenetically induced hunting bouts. In both cases, bouts comprised a highly lateralised sequence of half-beats (‘bout asymmetry’; Figure 5L, see Materials and methods for details), a large fraction of curvature was localised to the distal segments of the tail (Figure 5M) and bouts displayed low tail beat frequency (Figure 5N). Notably, all such parameters were significantly different as compared to spontaneous swims. Optogenetically induced hunting bouts showed a reduction in average vigour and theta-1 angles (maximum tail angle during the first half beat) compared to bouts during Paramecia hunting (Figure 5O,P). However, the latter displayed a bimodal distribution of theta-1 and optogenetically induced hunting bouts overlapped with the lower amplitude component of this distribution (Figure 5P, bottom). In sum, our data indicate that optogenetic stimulation of single KalTA4u508 pretectal neurons can induce naturalistic hunting-like behaviour.

Next, we compared hunting routines induced by optogenetic activation of ipsi- versus contra-projecting KalTA4u508 neurons. We did not observe differences in response latency (Figure 5Q), duration of evoked hunting sequences or number of swim bouts they contained (Figure 5—figure supplement 1A,B) or oculomotor kinematics (Figure 5—figure supplement 1E–H). A similar fraction (~40%) of both types of pretectal neuron could drive hunting sequences that terminated with capture swims (Figure 5—figure supplement 1C,D). However, the laterality of evoked hunting responses differed between the two projection classes (Figure 5R,S): Stimulation of ipsi-projecting KalTA4u508 pretectal neurons most frequently induced hunting in which the first swim bout was oriented in the ipsilateral direction (i.e. a left pretectal neuron evoked leftward turning). By contrast, contra-projecting neurons most frequently induced contralaterally directed hunting responses.

To directly establish whether the KalTA4u508 pretectal neurons that can induce predatory behaviour are the same cells that are recruited during visually evoked hunting, we combined optogenetic stimulation and functional calcium imaging of single neurons. To achieve this, we first established that optogenetic stimulation of a given KalTA4u508 pretectal neuron could induce hunting and then tethered the larva in agarose and performed calcium imaging of H2B-GCaMP6s, expressed in the nucleus of the same neuron, while the animal engaged in the virtual hunting assay. Visuomotor fingerprinting and cluster assignment of these neurons showed that they all belonged to hunting-initiation clusters (clusters 25–27) and had high HIx scores (N = 6 cells from six fish; Figure 5T).

Optogenetic activation of multiple KalTA4u508-expressing pretectal neurons induces short-latency hunting responses

Optogenetic stimulation of single KalTA4u508 pretectal neurons induced hunting routines with relatively low response probability (~20%) and long latency (~4 s). We investigated if response rates might be increased by stimulating a larger number of pretectal neurons or increasing light intensity.

First, we optogenetically stimulated single KalTA4u508 pretectal neurons at a range of irradiance levels (0.44–2.55 mW/mm2, 459 nm). To test whether the bright blue stimulation light might visually interfere with induction of hunting, we also tested retinally blind ‘lakritz’ larvae, homozygous for the atoh7th241 mutation, in which no RGCs are generated (Kay et al., 2001). In one of the two blind larvae in which we could optogenetically evoke hunting, response rate was positively modulated by irradiance but mean response latency did not fall below 2 s (Figure 5—figure supplement 1I,J). Although preliminary, this suggests that single pretectal cells may not be able to recruit downstream motor pattern generator circuits to execute hunting programmes with short latency.

Optogenetic activation of multiple KalTA4u508 pretectal neurons reliably evoked hunting behaviour at short latency. We showed this by using patterned illumination to focally activate the AF7-pretectal region in tethered KalTA4u508;UAS:CoChR-tdTomato transgenic larvae (7 dpf, 4 s LED-On periods, 470 nm, 21.1 mW/mm2; Figure 5—figure supplement 2A,B). We unilaterally stimulated the population of transgenically labelled neurons in the AF7-pretectal region and observed convergent saccades paired with lateralised tail movements with high response probability (55.43 ± 35.26%, mean ± SD, N = 7 larvae) and short latency (82.87 ± 5.46 ms, mean ± SD; Figure 5—figure supplement 2C–E). Every convergent saccade was associated with tail movement, which tended to be directed contralateral to the stimulated pretectum (Figure 5—figure supplement 2F) similar to activation of single contra-projecting KalTA4u508 cells. No hunting-like behaviour was induced by stimulation of an off-target region or in opsin-negative animals (N = 7 larvae; Figure 5—figure supplement 2D).

In summary, KalTA4u508 labels a specific group of pretectal neurons that are recruited during hunting initiation and which are capable of inducing a naturalistic predatory motor programme in the complete absence of prey.

Ablation of KalTA4u508-expressing pretectal neurons impairs hunting

To what extent are KalTA4u508 pretectal neurons required for hunting? To address this question, we assessed hunting performance in freely swimming larvae provided with Paramecia, both before and after laser-ablation of KalTA4u508 pretectal neurons (Figure 6A–C). To enable evaluation of the specificity of behavioural phenotypes, we also presented looming stimuli and drifting gratings to test visually evoked escape and optomotor response (OMR), respectively. Ablations were performed at 6 dpf in KalTA4u508;UAS:mCherry;elavl3:itTA;Ptet:ChR2-YFP larvae and their efficacy was confirmed by reimaging the pretectum the following day. We estimated that ~90% of the fluorescently labelled KalTA4u508 pretectal population was typically ablated in both brain hemispheres (Figure 6D,E). Importantly, RGC axons in AF7 were not damaged (Figure 6—figure supplement 1A). We also evaluated two sets of control larvae: ‘Non-ablated’, which underwent the same manipulations other than laser ablation, and ‘sham ablated’ in which an equivalent number of cells were laser-ablated in the thalamus, medially adjacent to AF7-pretectum (Figure 6—figure supplement 1B–D).

Figure 6. Ablation of KalTA4u508 pretectal neurons impairs hunting.

(A) Laser ablation of KalTA4u508 pretectal neurons and assessment of visuomotor behaviours. (B) Time-projection of larval behaviour (duration 8 s) showing trajectories of Paramecia and larval zebrafish swimming in the arena. (C) Time course of behavioural tests, ablation and brain imaging. (D) Pretectal neurons before (top, 6 dpf) and 24 hr after (bottom, 7 dpf) bilateral laser-ablations in a KalTA4u508;UAS:mCherry;elavl3:itTA;Ptet:ChR2-YFP larva. Images show maximum-intensity projections (red channel, 10 planes, 10 µm depth). A, anterior; P, posterior. Scale bar, 20 µm. (E) Quantification of cell ablation in left (blue) and right (red) AF7-pretectum (N = 14 fish). (F–H) Assessment of hunting performance before and after bilateral ablation of KalTA4u508 neurons (N = 14 fish). Mean ± SEM is reported for each condition. (I–L) Average swim speed, loom-evoked escape and OMR behaviour, before and after ablations. See also Figure 6—figure supplement 1.

Figure 6—source data 1. Source data for Figure 6.
Data provided as a MATLAB structure.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.48114.024

Figure 6.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1. Assessment of visuomotor behaviours in control larvae.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1.

(A) RGC axonal arborisations in AF7 (magenta) before (top, 6 dpf) and 24 hr after (bottom, 7 dpf) bilateral laser-ablation of KalTA4u508 pretectal neurons (green) in a KalTA4u508;UAS:mCherry;atoh7:GFP larva. (B) Sham ablations. Thalamic neurons before (top, 6 dpf) and 24 hr after (bottom, 7 dpf) bilateral laser-ablations in a atoh7:GFP;gata2a:GFP larva. Images show maximum-intensity projections (20 µm depth). Targeted cells are outlined in yellow. (C) Mean number of cells labelled in the thalamus per hemisphere before and after ablations (N = 10 fish). (D) Mean number of ablated thalamic cells per hemisphere. (E) Paramecia consumption in atoh7:GFP;gata2a:GFP larvae, before and after thalamic ablations (N = 10 fish). Black symbols show mean ± SEM across animals. (F–H) Non-ablated control. Assessment of Paramecia hunting in KalTA4u508;UAS:mCherry;elavl3:itTA;Ptet:ChR2-YFP control larvae at 6 and 7 dpf (N = 16 fish). (I–L) Average swim speed, loom-evoked escape and OMR, in control larvae. (M) Hunting response probability upon optogenetic stimulation of avOT in non-ablated control larvae. Scale bars, 50 µm, except in (A), 20 µm. A, anterior; L, left; P, posterior; R, right.

Analysis of prey consumption revealed that ablation of KalTA4u508 pretectal neurons resulted in decreased hunting performance (Figure 6F). Further analysis revealed that this reduction in prey-capture was associated with a reduced rate of hunting initiation (Figure 6G) as well as a reduction in the duration of hunting routines (Figure 6H). By contrast, we did not observe changes in average swim speeds, loom-evoked escapes or OMR performance (Figure 6I–L) and control larvae did not show changes in any of the tested behaviours (Figure 6—figure supplement 1E–L).

Together, these results indicate that KalTA4u508 pretectal neurons are specifically required for normal initiation and maintenance of predatory behaviour.

Optogenetic stimulation of optic tectum evokes hunting-like behaviour

Optogenetic stimulation of the anterior-ventral optic tectum (avOT) in elavl3:itTA;Ptet:ChR2-YFP transgenic larvae has been previously reported to induce convergent saccades and J-turns (Fajardo et al., 2013). To confirm this and explore a potential interaction between avOT and KalTA4u508 pretectal neurons, we first examined ChR2-YFP expression in 6 dpf elavl3:itTA;Ptet:ChR2-YFP larvae. We found that the opsin is highly expressed in avOT, as previously reported, as well as AF7 (Figure 7B and Figure 7—figure supplement 1A), but is absent from KalTA4u508 pretectal neurons (Figure 7D). Expression in AF7 raised the possibility that stimulation of the axon terminals of the prey-responsive RGCs that innervate this AF (Semmelhack et al., 2014) might contribute to the hunting behaviour observed in this transgenic line. We therefore crossed elavl3:itTA;Ptet:ChR2-YFP to the atoh7th241 mutant to eliminate RGCs (Figure 7C). In blind (atoh7-/-) transgenic animals, ChR2 expression was reduced in retinal arborisation fields, as expected, but retained in avOT (Figure 7—figure supplement 1B). We subsequently examined whether optogenetic stimulation of atoh7-/- transgenic animals was able to induce hunting-like behaviour (Figure 7A).

Figure 7. Optogenetic stimulation of avOT induces hunting in the absence of RGCs.

(A) Optogenetic stimulation of anterior-ventral optic tectum (avOT). (B–C) Dorsal view of ChR2-YFP expression in sighted (B) and blind atoh7-/- (C) 6 dpf elavl3:itTA;Ptet:ChR2-YFP;atoh7:gapRFP larvae registered to the ZBB brain atlas. Axonal projections of RGCs labelled by the atoh7:gapRFP transgene are displayed in magenta and are absent in atoh7-/- larvae. Images derive from median datasets across multiple immunostained brains (N = 6 sibling and N = 7 blind fish) and show maximum-intensity projections through focal planes encompassing AF7-pretectal regions. (D) ChR2 expression relative to KalTA4u508 neurons in a 6 dpf KalTA4u508;UAS:mCherry;elavl3:itTA;Ptet:ChR2-YFP larva. Images are single focal planes obtained from the left AF7-pretectum (plane one is dorsal relative to the other z-planes). KalTA4u508 neurons are not labelled with ChR2-YFP. (E) Time sequence composite image showing selected frames from an example optogenetically induced hunting sequence in a blind atoh7-/- 6 dpf elavl3:itTA;Ptet:ChR2-YFP;atoh7:gapRFP larva (see also Video 3). (F) Vergence angle overlaid onto larval location during the optogenetically induced hunting sequence from (E). Asterisks indicate time of convergent saccades. (G) Tail angle (grey) and ocular vergence angle (red) during the optogenetically induced hunting sequence shown in (E) and (F). Asterisks indicate time of convergent saccades. (H) Fraction of larvae that performed eye convergences during optogenetic stimulations. Larvae were either sighted (atoh7+/+ or atoh7+/-) or blind (atoh7-/-) and either opsin-positive (elavl3:itTA;Ptet:ChR2-YFP;atoh7:gapRFP) or opsin-negative (atoh7:gapRFP). Numbers of responsive larvae are reported above bars. (I) Response probability of larvae that performed eye convergence in at least one optogenetic stimulation trial. (J–L) Behavioural kinematics for optogenetically induced hunting events elicited in sighted (blue, N = 13 fish) and blind atoh7-/- (pink, N = 29) elavl3:itTA;Ptet:ChR2-YFP;atoh7:gapRFP larvae. Scale bars, 100 µm, except in (D), 30 µm. A, anterior; L, left; P, posterior; R, right. See also Figure 7—figure supplement 1 and Video 3.

Figure 7—source data 1. Source data for Figure 7.
Data provided as a MATLAB structure.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.48114.027

Figure 7.

Figure 7—figure supplement 1. ChR2 expression in elavl3:itTA;Ptet:ChR2-YFP;atoh7:gapRFP larvae and additional behavioural kinematics of optogenetically induced hunting.

Figure 7—figure supplement 1.

(A) Overlap between ChR2-YFP and tERK immunostain in elavl3:itTA;Ptet:ChR2-YFP larvae, computed as ratio between ChR2-YFP-positive voxels and tERK-positive voxels in each brain region. Mean + SEM values are reported only for anatomical regions showing overlap greater than zero (N = 6 fish). AF7-pretectum encompasses the ZBB masks ‘Optic tract - AF7’ and ‘Griseum tectale (AF7)’. (B) Overlap between ChR2-YFP and tERK immunostain in sighted (blue, N = 6 fish) and blind atoh7-/- (pink, N = 7) larvae. (C–F) Behavioural kinematics for optogenetically induced hunting events elicited in sighted (blue, N = 13 fish) and blind atoh7-/- (pink, N = 29) elavl3:itTA;Ptet:ChR2-YFP;atoh7:gapRFP larvae. In (E) and (F), data from all hunting events are plotted, whereas in the other plots the mean for each larva is reported.

Strikingly, we found that blind elavl3:itTA;Ptet:ChR2-YFP transgenics were more likely to display optogenetically induced hunting than their sighted (atoh7+/+ or atoh7+/-) siblings (29/30 responsive blind larvae vs. 13/30 sighted larvae; Figure 7E–H and Video 3). In addition, responsive atoh7-/- transgenics showed an increased probability of optogenetically induced hunting events, longer hunting routine durations and a substantial reduction in response latency (Figure 7I–K). When hunting was evoked, blind and sighted larvae showed comparable eye and tail kinematics (Figure 7L and Figure 7—figure supplement 1C–F).

Video 3. Hunting behaviour evoked by optogenetic stimulation of the anterior-ventral optic tectum in a blind larva.

Download video file (1.7MB, mp4)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.48114.028

Optogenetically induced hunting behaviour in a blind atoh7-/- 6 dpf elavl3:itTA;Ptet:ChR2-YFP;atoh7:gapRFP larva. Tracking data is reported in Figure 7E–G. The video was acquired at 250 frames per second and plays at 0.4 times the original speed. The raw movie is showed on the left, and a background-subtracted inset centred on the larva is showed on the right. Related to Figure 7.

These data are compatible with the conclusion of Fajardo et al. (2013), namely that optogenetic stimulation of avOT elicits hunting in elavl3:itTA;Ptet:ChR2-YFP larvae and indicate that RGC stimulation (either visually with bright blue light, or optogenetically) interferes with induction of hunting responses.

KalTA4u508-expressing pretectal neurons are required for tectally evoked hunting behaviour

Does hunting behaviour evoked by tectal activation require AF7-pretectal circuits? To investigate this, we tested whether hunting could be evoked by optogenetic stimulation of avOT in larvae in which KalTA4u508 pretectal cells were ablated (Figure 8A). Following laser-ablation of KalTA4u508 neurons in the same elavl3:itTA;Ptet:ChR2-YFP;KalTA4u508;UAS:mCherry larvae tested above, we observed that the probability of optogenetically induced hunting was substantially reduced (Figure 8B). By contrast, non-ablated control larvae showed no change in response probability between 6 and 7 dpf (Figure 6—figure supplement 1M).

Figure 8. KalTA4u508 pretectal neurons are required for tectally induced hunting.

Figure 8.

(A) Optogenetic stimulation of avOT before and after ablation of KalTA4u508 pretectal neurons. (B) Hunting response probability upon optogenetic stimulation of KalTA4u508;UAS:mCherry;elavl3:itTA;Ptet:ChR2-YFP larvae, before and after bilateral ablation of KalTA4u508 pretectal neurons (N = 14 fish). (C) Model of the neural circuit that induces hunting behaviour. Two classes of AF7-pretectal projection neuron are capable of inducing hunting. Contralaterally projecting APN neurons are likely to induce hunting by recruiting activity in oculomotor and locomotor pattern generating circuits in the mid/hindbrain tegmentum. Ipsilaterally projecting AF7-pretectal neurons may recruit ipsilateral tectofugal pathways. Activation of anterior-ventral tectum requires AF7-pretectal neurons to induce hunting and likely operates via the contralaterally projecting APN population because unilateral avOT stimulation produces contralaterally directed responses (Fajardo et al., 2013). APN, accessory pretectal nucleus.

Figure 8—source data 1. Source data for Figure 8.
Data provided as a MATLAB structure.
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.48114.030

These data indicate that KalTA4u508 pretectal neurons are required for release of predatory behaviour by circuits in the anterior optic tectum.

Discussion

In this study, we combined multi-photon calcium imaging, optogenetic stimulation and laser-ablations to identify a population of pretectal neurons that controls hunting behaviour. Calcium imaging during naturalistic behaviour revealed that KalTA4u508-expressing pretectal neurons are recruited when larval zebrafish initiate hunting. Optogenetic activation of single KalTA4u508 pretectal cells could release predatory behaviour in the absence of prey, and ablation of these cells impaired both natural hunting as well as hunting-like behaviour evoked by avOT stimulation. Based on functional and anatomical data, we propose that KalTA4u508 pretectal cells function as a command system linking visual perception of prey-like stimuli to activation of tegmental pattern generating circuits that coordinate specialised hunting motor programmes.

Do AF7-pretectal neurons comprise a command system controlling predation?

We suggest that KalTA4u508 pretectal neurons satisfy the criteria for a command system for induction of predatory behaviour. A command neuron has been defined as ‘a neuron that is both necessary and sufficient for the initiation of a given behaviour’ (Kupfermann and Weiss, 1978). Although a small number of striking examples of such cells have been identified in invertebrate models (Frost and Katz, 1996; Flood et al., 2013), it is recognised that the ‘necessity’ criterion is unlikely to be fulfilled in larger nervous systems where individual neurons display functional redundancy (Yoshihara and Yoshihara, 2018). Thus, command systems (sometimes referred to as decision neurons, command-like neurons or higher-order neurons) have been defined as interneurons that are active in association with a well-defined behaviour and whose activity can induce that behaviour, but without the strict necessity requirement (Jing, 2009; Yoshihara and Yoshihara, 2018).

To what extent do KalTA4u508 pretectal neurons satisfy these criteria? First, we showed that these cells are recruited during naturalistic hunting. By comparing neural activity in response versus non-response trials we were able to disambiguate ‘sensory’ activity, evoked by prey-like visual cues, from activity specifically associated with execution of hunting behaviour. We discovered that KalTA4u508 pretectal neurons, located close to AF7, display no detectable activity in response to prey-like stimuli but are reliably activated when larvae release convergent saccades at the commencement of hunting. This activity signature is compatible with a premotor role in directing predatory actions, as opposed to visual encoding of prey.

Strikingly, optogenetic stimulation of single KalTA4u508 neurons could evoke sustained hunting routines. As such, this is one of the first examples of a behavioural action sequence induced by activation of a single neuron in a vertebrate. Crucially, this induction occurred in the absence of any prey-like stimulus, supporting a role for these neurons in release of predatory behaviour downstream of perceptual recognition of prey. Optogenetic stimulation of single cells allowed us to test behaviour in freely swimming larvae and interrogate the roles of two distinct classes of AF7-pretectal cell. However, hunting was induced with surprisingly long average latency (~4 s) and in only ~20% of trials. In contrast, optogenetic stimulation directed at the entire transgenically labelled AF7-pretectal population reliably induced behaviour with very short latency,~80 ms. These findings are compatible with the KalTA4u508 population comprising a command system controlling hunting and suggest that during natural behaviour, presumed downstream pattern generating circuits are unlikely to be brought to threshold by the action of single pretectal neurons.

Ablations targeting KalTA4u508 pretectal neurons impaired, but did not eliminate, hunting. One contributing factor is likely to be that ablations were incomplete. We observed that 10% of labelled KalTA4u508 neurons survived ablation and due to variegation of transgene expression (Akitake et al., 2011), this is probably a lower bound on the size of the surviving pretectal population. Notably, the observation that hunting can be evoked by stimulation of only a single neuron suggests few surviving cells could in principle suffice to release predatory behaviour. Although hunting evoked by stimulation of anterior-ventral OT was strongly diminished in KalTA4u508-ablated animals, we do not rule out the possibility that there might be redundant, distributed circuitry involved in hunting initiation.

Overall, our data support the conclusion that a discrete population of pretectal neurons comprises a command system controlling predatory hunting.

Neural circuitry controlling visually guided hunting

How might these pretectal neurons fit within a sensorimotor pathway controlling visually guided hunting (Figure 8C)? Current evidence suggests visual recognition of prey is mediated by tectal and/or pretectal circuits. The axon terminals of zebrafish ‘projection class 2’ retinal ganglion cells in AF7 appear tuned to prey-like stimuli (Semmelhack et al., 2014) and a subpopulation of tectal neurons show non-linear mixed selectivity for conjunctions of visual features that best evoke predatory responses (Bianco and Engert, 2015). In accordance with these findings, we observed prey-responsive neurons in tectum and AF7-pretectum which were activated by small moving spots regardless of whether or not the animal produced a hunting response. Ablations of either AF7 or tectal neuropil have been shown to substantially impair hunting (Gahtan et al., 2005; Semmelhack et al., 2014), compatible with an important function for these retinorecipient regions in visual perception of prey.

Command systems are thought to sit at the sensorimotor ‘watershed’, linking sensory processing to activation of motor hierarchies. Our data support a circuit organisation where downstream of prey detection, the recruitment of KalTA4u508 pretectal neurons might be the neural event that corresponds to the decision of the animal to initiate hunting. The dendritic arbours of these pretectal neurons lie in immediate apposition to RGC terminals in AF7, suggesting a biological interface for transforming visual sensory activity into premotor commands to release behaviour. Photoactivation data also suggests the AF7-pretectal region is closely interconnected with ipsilateral OT (Figure 3—figure supplement 1E), providing a further route for visual prey detectors in OT to recruit a hunting response. Consistent with this, our ablation data indicate that induction of hunting by stimulation of anterior-ventral OT (Fajardo et al., 2013) requires KalTA4u508 pretectal neurons.

Two morphological classes of KalTA4u508 neuron were able to induce predatory behaviour with similar efficacy and motor kinematics, but opposite directional biases. Both morphologies appear similar to neurons previously identified in the AF7 region by Semmelhack et al. (2014). Contralaterally projecting neurons extended axon collaterals around the nMLF and oculomotor nuclei as well as in close apposition to reticulospinal neurons in the hindbrain. This projection pattern, as well as the identification of KalTA4u508 neurons in adult brain sections, suggests these cells reside in the larval zebrafish APN. Their axonal projections provide a pathway for recruitment of tegmental motor pattern generators to produce the specialised eye and tail movements observed during hunting (McElligott and O'malley, 2005; Bianco et al., 2011; Marques et al., 2018). Optogenetic stimulation of ipsilaterally projecting KalTA4u508 neurons evoked hunting routines in which the first orienting turn displayed an ipsilateral bias. It is possible that this arises from recruitment of tectal efferent pathways to the ipsilateral tegmentum, which have recently been suggested to mediate prey-directed orienting turns (Helmbrecht et al., 2018).

Whilst it is remarkable that stimulation of single pretectal neurons could induce hunting-like behaviour in the absence of prey, natural hunting is a precise orienting behaviour directed towards a (visual) target. Optogenetically induced hunting routines involved swim bouts where turn angle fell within the lower portion of the distribution measured during prey hunting. A probable explanation is that in the presence of prey, appropriate steering signals derive from OT to guide lateralised orienting turns. Compact tectal assemblies show premotor activity immediately preceding hunting initiation and are anatomically localised in relation to the directionality of hunting responses (Bianco and Engert, 2015). Furthermore, focal stimulation of the retinotopic tectal map has long been known to evoke goal-directed orienting responses including predatory manoeuvres (Herrero et al., 1998; Bels et al., 2012) and suppression of specific populations of neurons in superior colliculus (SC) impairs visual prey detection and accurate pursuit (Hoy et al., 2019). We hypothesise that pretectal activity releases predatory behaviour and operates in parallel with prey-directed steering signals, most likely from OT.

Command systems have been identified that evoke both discrete actions (Korn and Faber, 2005) as well as entire behavioural programmes (Flood et al., 2013). Constant stimulation of pretectal KalTA4u508 neurons produces extended hunting sequences that commenced with saccadic eye convergence followed by serial execution of multiple swim bouts during which elevated ocular vergence – a hallmark of predatory behaviour – was maintained. In some cases, optogenetically induced hunting sequences even terminated in capture swims associated with jaw movements. This suggests that the function of pretectal activity is to induce the overall hunting programme and that individual component actions (i.e. discrete tracking swim bouts) are coordinated by downstream motor pattern generating circuits.

Induction and modulation of predatory behaviour

In recent years, several studies in rodents have demonstrated that pathways converging on the periaqueductal grey (PAG) can potently modulate predatory behaviour. The central amygdala (CeA) displays activity changes during hunting (Comoli et al., 2005) and optogenetic activation of the CeA→ventral PAG pathway motivates prey pursuit (Han et al., 2017). Stimulation of the medial preoptic area to vPAG pathway promotes acquisition/handling (grabbing, biting) of objects, including prey (Park et al., 2018), and activation of a GABAergic projection from lateral hypothalamus to lateral/ventrolateral PAG motivates attack on prey (Li et al., 2018). In these studies, the presence of prey, or a prey-like stimulus, was required for neuronal stimulation to induce predatory behaviour. This suggests these pathways promote hunting by potentiating visuomotor activity, rather than directly inducing release of motor programmes downstream of sensory perception. Recently, a glutamatergic pathway from SC to the subthalamic zona incerta (ZI) (Shang et al., 2019) and a GABAergic pathway from medial ZI to PAG (Zhao et al., 2019) have been shown to respond to prey-like sensory cues and promote hunting when optogenetically stimulated. However, optogenetic stimulation evokes limited expression of a subset of hunting-related behaviour in the absence of prey. The ZI→PAG pathway appears to integrate multiple visual, vibrissal and auditory prey-related sensory signals, discriminate prey from non-prey objects, and generate a positive valence signal to motivate predation (Zhao et al., 2019). Comparing and contrasting the properties of hunting-related circuits identified in different vertebrates may prove a useful means to characterise the neural substrates involved in detecting and attending to prey, motivating hunting behaviour, and controlling and sustaining (Henriques et al., 2019) predatory action sequences.

Notably, neurons that control the release of behavioural programmes, such as the KalTA4u508 pretectal cells described here, represent key circuit loci at which sensory information might be integrated with internal state signals (Flood et al., 2013). In the context of vertebrate hunting, it will be important to elucidate the circuit mechanisms that modulate recruitment probability of such decision-making circuits, enabling animals to vary the expression of predatory behaviour in accordance with internal physiological states, experience, and competing behavioural demands.

Materials and methods

Key resources table.

Reagent type
(species) or
resource
Designation Source or reference Identifiers Additional
information
Genetic reagent(Danio rerio) Tg(KalTA4u508) u508Tg This study Transgene
Genetic reagent(Danio rerio) Tg(UAS:jGCaMP7f)u341Tg This study Transgene
Genetic reagent(Danio rerio) Tg(UAS:CoChR-tdTomato)u332Tg This study ZFIN ID:
ZDB-ALT-190226–4
Transgene
Genetic reagent
(Danio rerio)
Tg(elavl3:H2B-GCaMP6s)jf5Tg PMID: 25068735 ZFIN ID:
ZDB-ALT-141023–2
Vladimirov et al., 2014
Genetic reagent
(Danio rerio)
Tg(atoh7:gapRFP) cu2Tg PMID: 17147778 ZFIN ID:
ZDB-ALT-070129–2
Zolessi et al., 2006
Genetic reagent
(Danio rerio)
Tg(UAS:GCaMP6f, cryaa:mCherry)icm06Tg PMID: 28623664 ZFIN ID:
ZDB-ALT-160119–5
Knafo et al. (2017)
Genetic reagent(Danio rerio) Tg(UAS-E1b:NfsB-mCherry)jh17Tg PMID: 17335798 ZFIN ID:
ZDB-ALT-110222–4
Davison et al. (2007)
Genetic reagent(Danio rerio) TgBAC(slc17a6b:loxP-DsRed-loxP-GFP)nns14Tg PMID: 21199937 ZFIN ID:
ZDB-ALT-110413–5
Koyama et al. (2011)
Genetic reagent (Danio rerio) Tg(UAS:RFP)tpl2Tg PMID: 24179142 ZFIN ID:
ZDB-ALT-131119–25
Auer et al. (2014)
Genetic reagent(Danio rerio) Tg(atoh7:GFP)rw021Tg PMID: 12702661 ZFIN ID:
ZDB-ALT-050627–2
Masai et al. (2003)
Genetic reagent(Danio rerio) Tg(Cau.Tuba1:c3paGFP)a7437Tg PMID: 22704987 ZFIN ID:
ZDB-ALT-120919–1
Bianco et al. (2012)
Genetic reagent (Danio rerio) Tg(elavl3:ITETA-PTET:Cr.Cop4-YFP)fmi2Tg PMID: 23641200 ZFIN ID:
ZDB-ALT-120209–3
Fajardo et al. (2013)
Genetic reagent
(Danio rerio)
Tg(gata2a:GFP)pku2Et PMID: 18164283 ZFIN ID:
ZDB-ALT-080514–1
Wen et al., 2008
Genetic reagent
(Danio rerio)
atoh7th241 PMID: 11430806 ZFIN ID:
ZDB-ALT-980203–363
Kay et al. (2001)
Recombinant DNA reagent UAS:CoChR-tdTomato This study Plasmid
Sequence-based reagent CoChR_fw
PCR primer
This study CTCAGCGTAAAGCCACCATGCTGGGAAACG
Sequence-based reagent CoChR_rev
PCR primer
This study TACTACCGGTGCCGCCACTGT
Sequence-based reagent CoChR_tdT_fw
PCR primer
This study ACAGTGGCGGCACCGGTAGTA
Sequence-based reagent tdT_rev
PCR primer
This study CTAGTCTCGAGATCTCCATGTTTACTTATACAGCTCATCCATGCC
Sequence-based reagent UAS_jGCaMP7_fw
PCR primer
This study CGTAAAGCCACCATGGGTTCTCATC
Sequence-based reagent UAS_jGCaMP7_rev
PCR primer
This study CTCGAGATCTCCATGTTTACTTCGCTGTCATCATTTGTACAAAC
Sequence-based reagent pvalb_fw
PCR primer
This study GGGGACAAGTTTGTACAAAAAAGCAGGCTGGATGGTGGGCCAAATCAAAGGCTAC
Sequence-based reagent pvalb_rev
PCR primer
This study GGGGACCACTTTGTACAAGAAAGCTGGGTGGAACGAGACCGGCAACACACAG
Antibody Rabbit polyclonal anti-GFP; TP401 AMS Biotechnology RRID: AB_10890443 1:1000
Antibody Mouse monoclonalanti-44/42 MAPK (Erk1/2); 4696 Cell Signaling Technology RRID: AB_390780 1:500
Antibody Rat monoclonal anti-GFP; 04404–26 Nacalai Tesque RRID: AB_10013361 1:1000
Antibody Rabbit polyclonal anti-RFP/DsRed; PM005 MBL International RRID: AB_591279 1:1000
Software, algorithm MATLAB MathWorks RRID: SCR_001622 https://uk.mathworks.com/products/matlab.html
Software, algorithm LabView National Instruments RRID: SCR_014325 http://www.ni.com/en-gb/shop/labview.html
Software, algorithm Prism GraphPad RRID: SCR_002798 https://www.graphpad.com/scientific-software/prism/
Software, algorithm FIJI PMID: 22743772 RRID: SCR_002285 https://imagej.net/Fiji/Downloads
Software, algorithm Simple NeuriteTracer (ImageJ plugin) PMID: 21727141 RRID: SCR_016566 https://imagej.net/Simple_Neurite_Tracer
Software, algorithm Advanced Normalization Tools (ANTs) PMID: 20851191 RRID: SCR_004757 http://stnava.github.io/ANTs/
Software, algorithm Psychophysics Toolbox PMID: 9176952 RRID: SCR_002881 http://psychtoolbox.org/
Software, algorithm Zebrafish Brain Browser (ZBB) PMID: 26635538 https://science.nichd.nih.gov/confluence/display/burgess/Brain+Browser
Software, algorithm MATLAB script for cell detection PMID: 27881303 https://github.com/ahrens-lab/Kawashima_et_al_Cell_2016/
Software, algorithm Kernel Density Estimation Toolbox for MATLAB https://www.ics.uci.edu/~ihler/code/kde.html

Experimental model and transgenic lines

Animals were reared on a 14/10 hr light/dark cycle at 28.5°C. For all experiments, we used zebrafish larvae homozygous for the mitfaw2 skin-pigmentation mutation (Lister et al., 1999). Larvae used for pan-neuronal Ca2+ imaging experiments were double-transgenic Tg(elavl3:H2B-GCaMP6s)jf5Tg (Vladimirov et al., 2014) and Tg(atoh7:gapRFP)cu2Tg (Zolessi et al., 2006). For AF7-pretectal Ca2+ imaging, larvae were double-transgenic for Tg(–2.5pvalb6:KalTA4)u508Tg [i.e. Tg(KalTA4u508); generated in this study, see below] and either Tg(UAS:GCaMP6f,cryaa:mCherry)icm06Tg (Knafo et al., 2017) or Tg(UAS:jGCaMP7f)u341Tg (generated in this study, see below). Larvae used to determine whether KalTA4u508-expressing neurons reside in AF7-pretectum were triple-transgenic Tg(KalTA4u508), Tg(UAS-E1b:NfsB-mCherry)jh17Tg (Davison et al., 2007) and TgBAC(slc17a6b:loxP-DsRed-loxP-GFP)nns14Tg (Koyama et al., 2011). Larvae used for AF7 dendritic stratification analyses were triple-transgenic Tg(KalTA4u508), Tg(UAS:RFP)tpl2Tg (Auer et al., 2014), and Tg(atoh7:GFP)rw021Tg (Masai et al., 2003). Fish used for mapping of cell location in the adult pretectum were triple-transgenic Tg(KalTA4u508), Tg(UAS:GCaMP6f,cryaa:mCherry)icm06Tg and Tg(atoh7:gapRFP)cu2Tg. Larvae used for photo-activatable GFP labelling were Tg(Cau.Tuba1:c3paGFP)a7437Tg (Bianco et al., 2012). Larvae used for single cell labelling and optogenetic stimulation of AF7-pretectal cells were double-transgenic Tg(KalTA4u508) and Tg(elavl3:H2B-GCaMP6s)jf5Tg. Larvae used for single-cell optogenetic stimulation of AF7-pretectal cells in blind fish were double-transgenic Tg(KalTA4u508) and Tg(atoh7:gapRFP)cu2Tg with homozygous mutation of the atoh7th241 gene (Kay et al., 2001). Blind atoh7th241 homozygous fish were selected based on Tg(atoh7:gapRFP)cu2Tg expression being visible only in the eye but with no RGC projections in the brain. Larvae used for patterned optogenetic stimulation of the AF7-pretectal population were double-transgenic Tg(KalTA4u508) and Tg(UAS:CoChR-tdTomato)u332Tg (generated in this study, see below). Larvae used for pretectal cell ablations and free-swimming behaviour analyses were triple-transgenic Tg(KalTA4u508), Tg(UAS-E1b:NfsB-mCherry)jh17Tg (Davison et al., 2007) and Tg(elavl3:ITETA-PTET:Cr.Cop4-YFP)fmi2Tg (Fajardo et al., 2013). Larvae used for assessment of AF7 axonal arborisations following ablation of KalTA4u508 pretectal neurons were triple-transgenic Tg(KalTA4u508), Tg(UAS-E1b:NfsB-mCherry)jh17Tg and Tg(atoh7:GFP)rw021Tg. Larvae used for sham ablations of thalamic cells were double-transgenic Tg(gata2a:GFP)pku2Et (Wen et al., 2008) and Tg(atoh7:GFP)rw021Tg. Larvae used for optogenetic stimulation of the avOT were double-transgenic Tg(atoh7:gapRFP)cu2Tg and Tg(elavl3:ITETA-PTET:Cr.Cop4-YFP)fmi2Tg with either homozygous, heterozygous or no mutation of the atoh7th241 gene. All larvae were fed Paramecia from 4 dpf onward. Animal handling and experimental procedures were approved by the UCL Animal Welfare Ethical Review Body and the UK Home Office under the Animal (Scientific Procedures) Act 1986.

2-photon calcium imaging and behavioural tracking

The procedure was very similar to that described in Bianco and Engert (2015). Larval zebrafish were mounted in 3% low-melting point agarose (Sigma-Aldrich) at 5 dpf or 6 dpf and allowed to recover overnight before functional imaging at 6 dpf or 7 dpf. Imaging was performed using a custom-built 2-photon microscope [XLUMPLFLN 20 × 1.0 NA objective (Olympus), 580 nm PMT dichroic, bandpass filters: 510/84 (green), 641/75 (red) (Semrock), R10699 PMT (Hammamatsu Photonics), Chameleon II ultrafast laser (Coherent Inc)]. Imaging was performed at 920 nm with average laser power at sample of 5–10 mW. For imaging of Tg(elavl3:H2B-GCaMP6s) larvae, images (500 × 500 pixels, 0.61 µm/px) were acquired by frame scanning at 3.6 Hz and for each larva 10–14 focal planes were acquired with a z-spacing of 8 µm. For imaging of Tg(KalTA4u508;UAS:GCaMP6f) or Tg(KalTA4u508;UAS:jGCaMP7f) larvae, the same image size and scanning rate were used but 5–6 focal planes with a z-spacing of 5 µm were acquired for each larva. Visual stimuli were back-projected (Optoma ML750ST) onto a curved screen placed in front of the animal at a viewing distance of ~7 mm while a second projector provided constant background illumination below the fish. A coloured Wratten filter (Kodak, no. 29) was placed in front of both projectors to block green light from the PMT. Visual stimuli were designed in MATLAB using Psychophysics toolbox (Brainard, 1997). For all experiments, stimuli were presented in a pseudo-random sequence with 30 s inter-stimulus interval. Stimuli comprised 5° or 16°, dark or bright spots moving at 30°/s either left→right or right→left across ~200° of frontal visual space. Bright/dark spots had Weber contrast of 1 /– 1, respectively. In addition, 3 s whole-field bright/dark flashes were presented.

Eye movements were tracked at 60 Hz under 720 nm illumination using a FL3-U3-13Y3M-C camera (Point Grey) that imaged through the microscope objective. Tail movements were imaged at 430 Hz under 850 nm illumination using a sub-stage GS3-U3-41C6NIR-C camera (Point Grey). Microscope control, stimulus presentation and behaviour tracking were implemented using LabVIEW (National Instruments) and MATLAB (MathWorks).

Calcium imaging analysis

All calcium imaging data analysis was performed using MATLAB scripts. Motion correction of fluorescence imaging data was performed as per Bianco and Engert (2015). Regions of interest (ROIs) corresponding to cell nuclei were extracted using the cell detection code from Kawashima et al. (2016). The time-varying fluorescence signal F(t) for each cell was extracted by computing the mean value of all pixels within the corresponding ROI binary mask at each time-point (imaging frame). The proportional change in fluorescence (∆F/F0) at time t was calculated as

ΔF/F0 = F(t)  F0 F0

where F0 is a reference fluorescence value, taken as the median of F(t) during the 30 frames prior to all visual stimulus presentations.

We used multilinear regression to model the fluorescent timeseries of each imaged neuron (ROI) in terms of simultaneously recorded kinematic predictors (‘regressors’). Regressors were generated for oculomotor and locomotor variables (7 eye and 3 tail kinematics, see Supplementary file 1) by convolving time-series vectors for the relevant kinematic with a calcium impulse response function [CIRF, approximated as the sum of a fast-rising exponential, tau 20 ms, and a slow-decaying exponential, tau 420 ms for GCaMP6f and jGCaMP7f or 3 s for H2B-GCaMP6s; (Miri et al., 2011). To account for delays between neural activity and behaviour and/or indicator dynamics, we time-shifted the regressor matrix relative to the fluorescent response variable so as to minimise the residual squared error of an ordinary least squares regression model. We used elastic-net regularised regression to improve interpretability and prediction accuracy in the presence of multicollinearity between the regressors (Zou and Hastie, 2005). Elastic net models were fit using the ‘glmnet’ package for MATLAB (Qian et al., 2013) and hyperparameters controlling the ratio of L1 vs. L2 penalty (alpha) and the degree of regularisation (lambda) were selected to minimise ten-fold cross-validated squared error. Model coefficients were then used to construct visuomotor vectors.

Visuomotor vectors (VMVs) were generated for each neuron by concatenating (a) the integral of ∆F/F0 in response to each visual stimulus (12 s time window from stimulus onset, mean integral across stimulus presentations) for presentations in which no eye convergence was performed by the larva (components 1–10); (b) multilinear regression coefficients (β values) for eye, tail and motion correction regressors (components 11–21). VMVs from all imaged neurons were assembled into a matrix and each component was normalised across cells by dividing each column by its standard deviation.

VMV clustering was performed using a two-step procedure. First, we performed hierarchical agglomerative clustering of VMVs using a correlation distance metric (Bianco and Engert, 2015). For this first step, we selected only neurons that either exhibited strong visual responses (specifically, the maximum value of components 1–10 had to be within the top 5th percentile of such values across all neurons) or was well modelled in terms of behavioural kinematics (R2 had to be within the top 5th percentile of cross-validated R2 across all neurons). The centroids of the ‘seed’ clusters generated in this step (correlation threshold, 0.7) constituted a set of archetypal response profiles. Next, the VMVs of the remaining neurons were assigned to the cluster with the closest centroid (within a correlation distance threshold of 0.7) to produce the final clusters.

To assign cluster identities to KalTA4u508 pretectal neurons (e.g. Figures 4A, 5T), VMVs were generated as described above and the same assignment strategy and correlation distance threshold (0.7) were used. Note that normalisation of components was performed using the standard deviations computed for the initial matrix of VMVs.

Hunting Index (HIx) scores were calculated for each cell as follows. For each hunting response, convergence-triggered activity was measured by computing the mean of z-scored GCaMP fluorescence in a time window (±1 s) centred on the convergent saccade, xRi. Next, activity was measured at the same time during non-response trials in which the same visual stimulus was presented. The difference between xRi and the mean of non-response activity, μNR, was computed:

di=xRiμNR

HIx scores were computed as the mean of these di distance values across all response trials during which the cell was imaged. HIx values were computed separately for convergence events paired with leftwards tail movements, rightwards tail movements, and symmetrical/no tail movements.

3D image registration

Registration of image volumes was performed using the ANTs toolbox version 2.1.0 (Avants et al., 2011). Images were converted to the NRRD file format required by ANTs using ImageJ. As an example, to register the 3D image volume in ‘fish1_01.nrrd’ to the reference brain ‘ref.nrrd’, the following parameters were used:

antsRegistration -d 3 -float 1 -o [fish1_, fish1_Warped.nii.gz] -n BSpline -r [ref.nrrd, fish1_01.nrrd, 1] -t Rigid[0.1] -m GC[ref.nrrd, fish1_01.nrrd, 1, 32, Regular, 0.25] -c [200 × 200×200 × 0,1e-8, 10] -f 12 × 8×4 × 2 s 4 × 3×2 × 1 t Affine[0.1] -m GC[ref.nrrd, fish1_01.nrrd, 1, 32, Regular, 0.25] -c [200 × 200×200 × 0,1e-8, 10] -f 12 × 8×4 × 2 s 4 × 3×2 × 1 t SyN[0.1, 6, 0] -m CC[ref.nrrd, fish1_01.nrrd, 1, 2] -c [200 × 200×200x200 × 10,1e-7, 10] -f 12 × 8×4x2 × 1 s 4 × 3×2x1 × 0

The deformation matrices computed above were then applied to any other image channel N of fish1 using:

antsApplyTransforms -d 3 v 0 -float -n BSpline -i fish1_01.nrrd -r ref.nrrd -o fish1_0N_Warped.nii.gz -t fish1_1Warp.nii.gz -t fish1_0GenericAffine.mat

All brains were registered onto the ZBB brain atlas (1 × 1 × 1 xyz µm/px) (Marquart et al., 2015; Marquart et al., 2017) and onto a high-resolution Tg(elavl3:H2B-GCaMP6s) reference brain (0.76 × 0.76 × 1 xyz µm/px, mean of 3 larvae), with some differences between experiments:

  • For functional calcium imaging volumes, a three-step registration was used: the imaging volume, composed of 10–14 image planes (500 × 500 px, 0.61 µm/px, 8 µm z-spacing), was first registered to a larger volume of the same brain acquired at the end of the experiment (1 µm z-spacing), using affine and warp transformations. Then, the larger volume was registered to the Hi-Res Tg(elavl3:H2B-GCaMP6s) reference brain. Because the high-resolution volume had already been registered onto the ZBB atlas, the transformations were concatenated to bring the functional imaging volume to the ZBB atlas (calcium imaging stack → post-imaging stack → Hi-Res → ZBB).

  • The brain regions displayed in Figure 3F,H, and Figure 3—figure supplement 1B,C correspond to volumetric binary image masks in the ZBB atlas that have been registered to the Hi-Res Tg(elavl3:H2B-GCaMP6s) reference brain using the ZBB Tg(elavl3:H2B-RFP) volume and performing affine and warp transformations (ZBB elavl3:H2B-RFP → Hi-Res).

  • For the registration displayed in Figure 3A,B of KalTA4u508 neurons to the Hi-Res Tg(elavl3:H2B-GCaMP6s) reference brain, the imaging volume was registered to the ZBB Tg(vglut:DsRed) volume [previously registered to the Hi-Res reference brain (ZBB elavl3:H2B-RFP → Hi-Res] using the vglut2a channel [TgBAC(slc17a6b:loxP-DsRed-loxP-GFP)] acquired in parallel with Tg(KalTA4u508;UAS-E1b:NfsB-mCherry) imaging and performing affine and warp transformations.

  • For single KalTA4u508 neuron tracing experiments, the imaging volume was registered to the Hi-Res Tg(elavl3:H2B-GCaMP6s) reference brain using the H2B-GCaMP6s channel acquired in parallel with Tg(KalTA4u508);UAS-CoChR-tdTomato-injected imaging. Affine and warp transformations were performed to bring the imaging volume and associated neuron tracing (see below) to the Hi-Res reference brain.

  • Imaging volumes related to photo-activation of PA-GFP were registered to a whole-brain reference from a 6 dpf Tg(α-tubulin:C3PA-GFP) larva in which no photo-activation was performed, using affine and warp transformations. The Tg(α-tubulin:C3PA-GFP) reference volume was then registered to the ZBB atlas. The photo-activation volume was transported to the Hi-Res Tg(elavl3:H2B-GCaMP6s) reference by concatenating the transformations (photo-activation stack → Tg(α-tubulin:C3PA-GFP) reference → ZBB → Hi-Res).

  • For the images and analyses displayed in Figure 7B,C and Figure 7—figure supplement 1A,B of sighted and blind Tg(elavl3:itTA;Ptet:ChR2-YFP;atoh7:gapRFP) larvae, the imaging volume was registered to the ZBB anti-tERK volume using the anti-tERK immunostain channel and performing affine and warp transformations.

All registration steps were manually assessed for global and local alignment accuracy. All brain regions referred to in this paper correspond to the volumetric binary image masks in the ZBB atlas, with the exception of regions in the anterior-ventral optic tectum, AF7-pretectum, and cholinergic nucleus isthmi (Henriques et al., 2019). These image masks, in ZBB reference space, can be downloaded as Supplementary file 24.

DNA cloning and transgenesis

To generate the UAS:CoChR-tdTomato DNA construct used for single cell labelling and optogenetic stimulations and for creating the Tg(UAS:CoChR-tdTomato)u332Tg line, the coding sequence of the blue light-sensitive opsin CoChR (from pAAV-Syn-CoChR-GFP) and the red fluorescent protein tdTomato (from pAAV-Syn-Chronos-tdTomato) were cloned in frame into a UAS Tol1 backbone (pT1UciMP). The pAAV-Syn-CoChR-GFP and pAAV-Syn-Chronos-tdTomato plasmids were gifts from Edward Boyden (Addgene plasmid # 59070 and # 62726, respectively) (Klapoetke et al., 2014). The pT1UciMP plasmid was a gift from Harold Burgess (Addgene plasmid # 62215) (Horstick et al., 2015). The cloning was achieved using the In-Fusion HD Cloning Plus CE kit (Clontech) with the following primers:

  • CoChR_fw, CTCAGCGTAAAGCCACCATGCTGGGAAACG

  • CoChR_rev, TACTACCGGTGCCGCCACTGT

  • CoChR_tdT_fw, ACAGTGGCGGCACCGGTAGTA

  • tdT_rev, CTAGTCTCGAGATCTCCATGTTTACTTATACAGCTCATCCATGCC

To generate the UAS:jGCaMP7f DNA construct used for creating the Tg(UAS:jGCaMP7f)u341Tg line, the coding sequence of the genetically encoded calcium indicator jGCaMP7f (from pGP-CMV-jGCaMP7f) was cloned into the pT1UciMP UAS Tol1 backbone. The pGP-CMV-jGCaMP7f plasmid was a gift from Douglas Kim (Addgene plasmid # 104483) (Dana et al., 2019). As above, the cloning was achieved using the In-Fusion HD Cloning Plus CE kit (Clontech) with the following primers:

  • UAS_jGCaMP7_fw, CGTAAAGCCACCATGGGTTCTCATC

  • UAS_jGCaMP7_rev, CTCGAGATCTCCATGTTTACTTCGCTGTCATCATTTGTACAAAC

To generate the Tg(UAS:jGCaMP7f) and the Tg(UAS:CoChR-tdTomato) lines, purified UAS:jGCaMP7f or UAS:CoChR-tdTomato DNA constructs (35 ng/µl) were co-injected with Tol1 transposase mRNA (80 ng/µl) into Tg(KalTA4u508) zebrafish embryos at the early one-cell stage. Transient expression, visible as jGCaMP7f or tdTomato fluorescence, was used to select injected embryos that were then raised to adulthood. Tol1 transposase mRNA was prepared by in vitro transcription from NotI-linearised pCS2-Tol1.zf1 plasmid using the SP6 transcription mMessage mMachine kit (Life Technologies). The pCS2-Tol1.zf1 was a gift from Harold Burgess (Addgene plasmid # 61388) (Horstick et al., 2015). RNA was purified using the RNeasy MinElute Cleanup kit (Qiagen). Germ line transmission was identified by mating sexually mature adult fish to mitfaw2/w2 fish and, subsequently, examining their progeny for jGCaMP7f or tdTomato fluorescence. Positive embryos from a single fish were then raised to adulthood. Once this second generation of fish reached adulthood, positive embryos from a single ‘founder’ fish were again selected and raised to adulthood to establish stable Tg(KalTA4u508;UAS:jGCaMP7f) and Tg(KalTA4u508;UAS:CoChR-tdTomato) double-transgenic lines.

The Tg(–2.5pvalb6:KalTA4)u508Tg [i.e. Tg(KalTA4u508)] line was isolated as follows. First, we used Gateway cloning (Invitrogen) to construct an expression vector in which ~ 2.5 kb of zebrafish genomic sequence upstream of the pvalb6 gene start codon was placed upstream of the KalTA4 (Distel et al., 2009) open reading frame. The genomic sequence was cloned using the following primers and Phusion PCR polymerase (Thermo Fisher Scientific):

  • pvalb_fw: GGGGACAAGTTTGTACAAAAAAGCAGGCTggatggtgggccaaatcaaaggctac

  • pvalb_rev: GGGGACCACTTTGTACAAGAAAGCTGGGTggaacgagaccggcaacacacag

(where capital letters indicate the attB1/B2 extension sequences).

The expression vector was then micro-injected into one-cell stage Tg(UAS-E1b:Kaede)s1999t (Davison et al., 2007) embryos at 30 ng/µl along with tol2 mRNA (30 ng/µl) and adult fish were screened for germline transmission by outcrossing as described above. This expression vector generated a wide range of expression patterns, one of which was designated the allele u508Tg and labelled AF7-pretectal neurons, as well as numerous other neuronal populations in the brain, spinal cord and sensory ganglia as reported here.

Immunohistochemistry

Larvae

Samples were fixed overnight in 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA) in 0.1 M phosphate buffered saline (PBS, Sigma-Aldrich) and 4% sucrose (Sigma-Aldrich) at 4°C. Brains were manually dissected with forceps prior to immunostaning. First, dissected brains were permeabilised by incubation in proteinase-K (40 µg/ml) in PBS with 1% Triton-X100 (PBT, Sigma-Aldrich) for 15 min. This was followed by 3 × 5 min washes in PBT, 20 min fixation in 4% PFA at room temperature and 3 × 5 min washes in PBT. Second, brains were incubated in block solution (2% goat serum, 1% DMSO, 1% BSA in PBT, Sigma-Aldrich) for 2 hr. Subsequently, brains were incubated in block solution containing primary antibodies overnight, followed by 6 × 1 hr washes in PBT on a slowly rotating shaker. Third, brains were incubated in block solution containing secondary antibodies overnight, followed by 6 × 1 hr washes in PBT. Finally, PBT was rinsed out by doing washes in PBS and brains were stored at 4°C. Imaging was performed using the two-photon microscope described above at 790 nm. Primary antibodies were: rabbit anti-GFP (AMS Biotechnology, TP401, dilution 1:1000) and mouse anti-ERK (Cell Signaling Technology, 9102, p44/42 MAPK (Erk1/2), dilution 1:500). Secondary antibodies were: goat anti-rabbit Alexa Fluor 488-conjugated (Thermo Fisher Scientific, A-11034, dilution 1:200), and goat anti-mouse Alexa Fluor 594-conjugated (Thermo Fisher Scientific, A-11005, dilution 1:200).

Adults

Tg(KalTA4u508;UAS:GCaMP6f;atoh7:gapRFP) fish (3 months old) were deeply anesthetised in 0.2% tricaine (MS222, Sigma) and fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA) for 24 hr at 4°C. Brains were then carefully dissected under a stereomicroscope and transferred to saline phosphate buffer (PBS), where they were maintained for at least half an hour. Two different procedures were used for sectioning the brains. For cryostat sectioning, brains were cryopreserved, embedded in Tissue Tek OCT compound (Sakura Finetek) and frozen using liquid nitrogen cooled methylbutane. Transverse sections of the brain (12 µm thick) were obtained using a cryostat and collected in gelatine-coated slides. For vibratome sectioning, brains were first embedded into 3% agarose in PBS. Transverse sections of the brains (100 µm thick) were obtained using a vibratome and transferred to PBS in microtubes. Immunostaining was performed by either adding solutions onto the cryostat sections or changing the solutions inside the microtubes. Sections were incubated first in normal goat serum (Sigma, dilution 1:10) in PBS with 0.5% Triton for 1 hr at room temperature, and then with a cocktail of two primary antibodies (rat anti-GFP, Nacalai Tesque, 04404–26, dilution 1:1000, and rabbit anti-RFP, MBL International, PM005, dilution 1:1000) for 24 hr at room temperature. Next, after three washes in PBS, brain sections were incubated with a cocktail of two secondary antibodies (goat anti-rat Alexa 488, Thermo Fisher Scientific, A-11006, dilution 1:500, and goat anti-rabbit Alexa 568, Thermo Fisher Scientific, A-11011, dilution 1:500) for 1 hr at room temperature. Sections were washed in PBS, mounted with 50% glycerol in PBS and imaged using a Nikon A1R confocal microscope equipped with a Nikon Plan Fluor 20 × 0.50 NA objective. Excitation light was provided by an argon ion multichannel laser tuned to 488 nm (green channel), and a 561 nm diode laser (red channel).

Single cell labelling

To label individual KalTA4u508 neurons, UAS:CoChR-tdTomato DNA constructs were injected into 1–4 cell stage Tg(KalTA4u508) or Tg(KalTA4u508;atoh7:gapRFP) embryos with homozygous or no mutation of the atoh7th241 gene. Plasmid DNA was purified using midi-prep kits (Qiagen) and injected at a concentration of 30 ng/µl in distilled water. Larvae (4 dpf) were then screened for CoChR-tdTomato expression. Only larvae showing expression in a single KalTA4u508 pretectal neuron were subsequently used for optogenetic stimulations and neuronal tracing experiments. Single cell morphologies were traced using the Simple Neurite Tracer plugin for ImageJ (Longair et al., 2011).

Anatomical analyses

Cell density of neurons belonging to hunting-initiation clusters (25–28) was computed in the following way. First, we obtained the soma 3D coordinates of all neurons in clusters 25–28 following anatomical registration to the high-resolution Tg(elavl3:H2B-GCaMP6s) reference brain. Then, we computed the local cell density at each soma location using an adaptive Gaussian-based kernel density estimate (Breiman et al., 1977), with the bandwidth at each point constrained to be proportional to the kth nearest neighbour distance where:

k=n

and n is the number of neurons (N = 6,630 cells from 8 fish). To compute the kernel density estimate, we used a MATLAB-based toolbox developed by Alexander Ihler (www.ics.uci.edu/~ihler/code/kde.html). Images in Figure 3A,B represent volume projections in which hunting-initiation neurons are colour-coded according to local cell density.

Neurite stratification and axon projection profiles in Figure 3D,G were obtained by measuring fluorescence intensity values along the axes indicated on figure panels using ImageJ Line and Plot Profile tools. For each image channel, a maximum intensity projection image was generated before measuring fluorescence intensity. Each intensity profile i was then rescaled to generate a profile, I, ranging from 0 to 1 as follows:

I=i-iminimax-imin

where imin and imax are the minimum and maximum values of profile i, respectively.

Photo-activation of PA-GFP

Larvae (5 dpf) homozygous for the Tg(α-tubulin:C3PA-GFP) transgene were anaesthetised and mounted in 2% low-melting temperature agarose. The same custom-built 2-photon microscope used for functional imaging was used to photo-activate PA-GFP in a small region (9 × 9 µm) containing cell bodies located in AF7-pretectum. The photo-activation site was selected by imaging the brain at 920 nm. Photo-activation was performed by continuously scanning at 790 nm (5 mW at sample) for 4 min. Larvae were then unmounted and allowed to recover. At 7 dpf, an image stack (1200 × 800 px, 0.38 µm/px, ∼200 µm z-extent) was acquired at 920 nm covering a large portion of the midbrain, tegmentum and hindbrain. Axonal projections were traced using the Simple Neurite Tracer plugin for ImageJ (Longair et al., 2011).

Monitoring of free-swimming behaviour

The same behavioural tracking system was used for both optogenetic stimulations and assessment of visuomotor behaviours with some differences. Images were acquired under 850 nm illumination using a high-speed camera [Mikrotron MC1362, 250 Hz (optogenetic stimulations) or 700 Hz (visuomotor behaviours assessment), 500 µs shutter-time] equipped with a machine vision lens (Fujinon HF35SA-1) and an 850 nm bandpass filter to block visible light. In all experiments, larvae were placed in the arena and allowed to acclimate for around 2 min before starting experiments.

Optogenetic stimulations

Larvae were placed in a petri dish with a custom-made agarose well (28 mm diameter, 3 mm depth) filled with fish facility water. Blue light was delivered across the whole arena from above using a 470 nm LED (OSRAM Golden Dragon Plus, LB W5AM) or a 459 nm LED (OSRAM OSTAR Projection Power, LE B P2W). Irradiance (0.44–2.55 mW/mm2) was varied using constant current drive electronics with pulse-width modulation at 5 kHz. ‘LED-On’ trials included 7 s periods of continuous blue light illumination, interleaved with ‘no-stimulation’ trials in which no blue light was provided. Both LED-On and no-stimulation trials lasted 8 s. A minimum of 10 LED-On trials were acquired for each fish. LED and camera control were implemented using LabVIEW (National Instruments).

Assessment of visuomotor behaviours

Larvae were placed in a 35 mm petri dish filled with 3.5 ml of fish facility water. Visual stimuli were projected onto the arena from below using an AAXA P2 Jr Pico Projector via a cold mirror. Visual stimuli were designed using Psychophysics Toolbox (Brainard, 1997). Looming stimuli expanded from 10 to 100° with L/V ratio of 255 ms (Dunn et al., 2016). Optomotor gratings had a period of ∼10 mm and moved at one cycle/s. Optomotor gratings and looming spots were presented in egocentric coordinates such that directional gratings always moved 90° to left or right sides with respect to fish orientation and looming spots were centred 5 mm away from the body centroid and at 90° to left or right. Stimuli were presented in pseudo-random order with an inter-stimulus interval of minimum 60 s. Stimuli were only presented if the body centroid was within a predefined central region (11 mm from the edge of the arena). If this was not the case, a concentric grating was presented that moved towards the centre of the arena to attract the fish to the central region. At the beginning of each experiment, 60 Paramecia were added to arena. Each experiment typically lasted ~1 hr. Final Paramecia numbers were counted manually from full-frame video data from the final 10 s of each experiment and adjusted for consumption in 60 min [multiplying by 60/experiment duration (min)]. During experiments, eye and tail kinematics were tracked online as described below. Camera control, online tracking and stimulus presentation were implemented using LabVIEW (National Instruments) and MATLAB (MathWorks).

Analyses of free-swimming behaviour

Data analysis was performed using LabVIEW (National Instruments) and MATLAB (MathWorks). Eye and tail kinematics were tracked offline for optogenetic experiments, and online for assessment of visuomotor behaviours with some differences. First, images were background-subtracted using a background model generated over 8 s in which the larva was moving (offline tracking), or a continuously updated background model (online tracking). Next, images were thresholded and the body centroid was found by running a particle detection routine for binary objects within suitable area limits. For online tracking, eye centroids were detected using a second threshold and particle detection procedure with the requirement that these centroids were in close proximity to the body centroid. For offline tracking, eye centroids were detected using a particle detection procedure that uses both binary and greyscale images to identify the two centroids within suitable area limits that had the lowest mean intensity values. For online tracking, body and eye orientations were computed using second- and third-order image moments. For offline tracking, body orientation was computed as the angle of the vector formed by the centre of mass of the body centroid (origin) and the midpoint between the eye centroids. Eye orientation was computed as the angle between the major axis of the eye and the body orientation vector. Vergence angle was computed as the difference between the left and right eye angles. The tail was tracked by performing consecutive annular line-scans, starting from the body centroid and progressing towards the tip of the tail so as to define nine equidistant x-y coordinates along the tail. Inter-segment angles were computed between the eight resulting segments. Reported tail curvature was computed as the sum of these inter-segment angles. Rightward bending of the tail is represented by positive angles and leftward bending by negative angles. To identify periods of high ocular vergence, which represent hunting routines, a vergence angle threshold was computed for each fish by fitting a two-term Gaussian model to its vergence angle distribution. A fish was considered to be hunting if vergence angle exceeded this vergence threshold. For experiments assessing visuomotor behaviours, the vergence angle distribution was invariably bimodal and the vergence threshold was computed as one standard deviation below the centre of the higher angle Gaussian. For optogenetic experiments, in cases where the vergence angle distribution was not bimodal, a fixed vergence threshold of 55° was used.

Optogenetic experiments

Response probability was computed as the fraction of LED-On trials in which at least one hunting routine (i.e. period with ocular vergence above threshold) was detected during the 7 s stimulation period. Similarly, for no-stimulation trials, response probability is the fraction of trials in which at least one hunting-like routine was detected. Response latency for LED-On trials was calculated from light stimulus onset. Swim bouts were identified using velocity thresholds (800°/s for bout onset, 200°/s for bout offset) applied to smoothed absolute tail angular velocity traces. Tail beat frequency was computed as the reciprocal of the mean full-cycle period during a swim bout. Tail vigour is computed by integrating absolute tail angular velocity (smoothed with a 40 ms box-car filter) over the first 120 ms of a swim bout. Bout asymmetry measures the degree to which tail curvature during a bout shows the same laterality as that determined during the first half-beat. It is computed as the fraction of time points in which the sign of tail angle matches the direction of the first half beat. This metric is high for hunting related J-turns but close to zero for forward swims. For each bout, the fraction of total curvature localised to the distal third of the tail was computed for the first half beat. To identify capture swim-like movements during optogenetically evoked hunting routines, movies were individually inspected and the following criteria were used to classify a swim bout as capture swim: (1) small change in body orientation associated with (2) divergence of the eyes and (3) jaw movement/suction and/or (4) dorsiflexion of the body.

Assessment of visuomotor behaviours

Escape responses to loom stimuli were identified if the instantaneous speed of the body centroid exceeded 75 mm/s. An optomotor response gradient [OMR turn rate (°/s)] was calculated for each presentation as the total change in orientation during the stimulus presentation divided by the duration of the presentation [for leftwards OMR stimuli, the OMR turn rate (typically negative) was multiplied by –1 to group the data with rightwards OMR stimuli]. Mean swim speed was calculated as the total distance covered by the larva in the central region of the arena divided by the total time spent in this region.

Optogenetic stimulation of AF7-pretectum in tethered larvae

Patterned illumination was delivered to the pretectum of tethered larvae using a custom-built digital micromirror device (DMD) rig. The DMD (Texas Instruments DLP LightCrafter 6500) was illuminated using a liquid light-guide coupled 470 nm LED (Mightex BLS-LCS-0470-50-22). A relayed image of the DMD was expanded and projected onto the sample plane of the objective (Zeiss N-Achroplan 20 × 0.5 NA) such that individual micromirrors were 0.46 × 0.46 µm at sample. Larvae, mounted in the same way as for calcium imaging, were imaged at 100 Hz under 850 nm illumination using a sub-stage FL3-U3-13Y3M-C camera (Point Grey). Trials were 10 s long and included a 4 s period of continuous blue light illumination of either the left or right AF7-pretectum (~75 × 75 µm target, 22.1 mW/mm2). A minimum of 6 stimulation trials were performed for each target region. Movies were individually inspected to identify hunting-like events, which were defined by execution of saccadic eye convergence. The apparatus was controlled using LabVIEW (National Instruments).

Laser ablations

KalTA4u508 pretectal neurons were targeted for ablation in 6 dpf Tg(KalTA4u508;UAS:mCherry;elavl3:itTA;Ptet:ChR2-YFP) or Tg(KalTA4u508;UAS:mCherry;atoh7:GFP) larvae, which were anaesthetised using MS222 and mounted in 1% low-melting temperature agarose (Sigma-Aldrich). Ablations were performed using a MicroPoint system (Andor) attached to a Zeiss Axioplan-2 microscope equipped with a Zeiss Achroplan water-immersion 63 × 0.95 NA objective. A pulsed nitrogen-pumped tunable dye laser (Coumarin-440 dye cell) was focused onto individual KalTA4u508 neurons and pulses were delivered at a frequency of 10 Hz for 60–120 s. All visible KalTA4u508 neurons in both hemispheres were targeted for ablation and cell damage was confirmed under DIC optics. Larvae were then unmounted and allowed to recover overnight. Sham ablations of thalamic neurons were performed in 6 dpf Tg(gata2a:GFP;atoh7:GFP) larvae in a similar manner. The number of thalamic cells targeted for ablation was equivalent to the number of targeted KalTA4u508 pretectal neurons (10–16 per hemisphere). Non-ablated larvae were mounted in agarose and underwent the same manipulations except for laser-ablation. Pre- and post-ablation image stacks were acquired with a 2-photon microscope at 790 nm (800 × 800 px, 0.38 µm/px, ∼40 µm z-extent). Cell counting was performed manually in ImageJ using the multi-point tool.

Quantification and statistical analysis

Statistical analyses were performed in Prism 8 (GraphPad) and MATLAB R2017b (MathWorks). Statistical tests, p-values, N-values, and additional information are reported in Supplementary file 1. All tests were two-tailed and were chosen after data were tested for normality and homoscedasticity.

Data/resource sharing

Data generated or analysed during this study are included in the manuscript and supporting files. Source data files have been provided for Figures 1 and 28.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank members of the Bianco lab, David Attwell, Tiago Branco, Tara Keck and Steve Wilson for helpful discussions and critical feedback on the manuscript and UCL Fish Facility staff for fish care and husbandry. We thank Richard Poole and Arantza Barrios for help with the laser-ablation system and Claire Wyart for sharing the Tg(UAS:GCaMP6f,cryaa:mCherry)icm06Tg line prior to publication. PA was supported by a Sir Henry Wellcome Postdoctoral Fellowship (204708/Z/16/Z). IHB was supported by a Sir Henry Dale Fellowship from the Royal Society and Wellcome Trust (101195/Z/13/Z) and a UCL Excellence Fellowship.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Isaac H Bianco, Email: i.bianco@ucl.ac.uk.

Vatsala Thirumalai, National Centre for Biological Sciences, India.

Ronald L Calabrese, Emory University, United States.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • Wellcome Sir Henry Dale Fellowship, 101195/Z/13/Z to Isaac H Bianco.

  • Wellcome Sir Henry Wellcome Postdoctoral Fellowship, 204708/Z/16/Z to Paride Antinucci.

  • University College London Excellence Fellowship to Isaac H Bianco.

  • Royal Society Sir Henry Dale Fellowship, 101195/Z/13/Z to Isaac H Bianco.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Software, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Writing—review and editing.

Investigation, Methodology.

Conceptualization, Resources, Software, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Writing—original draft, Project administration, Writing—review and editing.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: Animal handling and experimental procedures were approved by the UCL Animal Welfare Ethical Review Body and the UK Home Office under the Animal (Scientific Procedures) Act 1986.

Additional files

Supplementary file 1. Regression and statistical details.

Spreadsheet containing details of statistical analyses (test used, test statistic, values of N, centre and spread), and description of kinematic regressors.

elife-48114-supp1.xlsx (22.3KB, xlsx)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.48114.031
Supplementary file 2. Anatomical mask – avOT.

TIFF stack containing binary mask defining the ‘avOT’ anatomical region, in ZBB space.

elife-48114-supp2.tif (685.7KB, tif)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.48114.032
Supplementary file 3. Anatomical mask – AF7-pretectum.

TIFF stack containing binary mask defining the ‘AF7-pretectum’ anatomical region, in ZBB space.

elife-48114-supp3.tif (682.4KB, tif)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.48114.033
Supplementary file 4. Anatomical mask – NI chata.

TIFF stack containing binary mask defining the ‘NI chata’ anatomical region, in ZBB space.

elife-48114-supp4.tif (680.3KB, tif)
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.48114.034
Transparent reporting form
DOI: 10.7554/eLife.48114.035

Data availability

Data generated or analysed during this study are included in the manuscript and supporting files. Source data files have been provided for Figures 1 and 2—8.

References

  1. Akitake CM, Macurak M, Halpern ME, Goll MG. Transgenerational analysis of transcriptional silencing in zebrafish. Developmental Biology. 2011;352:191–201. doi: 10.1016/j.ydbio.2011.01.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Anjum F, Turni H, Mulder PG, van der Burg J, Brecht M. Tactile guidance of prey capture in Etruscan shrews. PNAS. 2006;103:16544–16549. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0605573103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Auer TO, Duroure K, De Cian A, Concordet JP, Del Bene F. Highly efficient CRISPR/Cas9-mediated knock-in in zebrafish by homology-independent DNA repair. Genome Research. 2014;24:142–153. doi: 10.1101/gr.161638.113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Avants BB, Tustison NJ, Song G, Cook PA, Klein A, Gee JC. A reproducible evaluation of ANTs similarity metric performance in brain image registration. NeuroImage. 2011;54:2033–2044. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bels VL, Aerts P, Chardon M, Vandewalle P, Berkhoudt H, Crompton A, de Vree F, Dullemeijer P, Ewert J, Frazzetta T. Biomechanics of Feeding in Vertebrates. Vol. 18. Springer Science & Business Media; 2012. [Google Scholar]
  6. Bianco IH, Kampff AR, Engert F. Prey capture behavior evoked by simple visual stimuli in larval zebrafish. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience. 2011;5:101. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2011.00101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bianco IH, Ma LH, Schoppik D, Robson DN, Orger MB, Beck JC, Li JM, Schier AF, Engert F, Baker R. The tangential nucleus controls a gravito-inertial vestibulo-ocular reflex. Current Biology. 2012;22:1285–1295. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.05.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bianco IH, Engert F. Visuomotor transformations underlying hunting behavior in zebrafish. Current Biology. 2015;25:831–846. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.01.042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Borla MA, Palecek B, Budick S, O'Malley DM. Prey capture by larval zebrafish: evidence for fine axial motor control. Brain, Behavior and Evolution. 2002;60:207–229. doi: 10.1159/000066699. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Brainard DH. The psychophysics toolbox. Spatial Vision. 1997;10:433–436. doi: 10.1163/156856897X00357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Breiman L, Meisel W, Purcell E. Variable kernel estimates of multivariate densities. Technometrics. 1977;19:135–144. doi: 10.1080/00401706.1977.10489521. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  12. Comoli E, Ribeiro-Barbosa ER, Negrão N, Goto M, Canteras NS. Functional mapping of the prosencephalic systems involved in organizing predatory behavior in rats. Neuroscience. 2005;130:1055–1067. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.10.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Dana H, Sun Y, Mohar B, Hulse BK, Kerlin AM, Hasseman JP, Tsegaye G, Tsang A, Wong A, Patel R, Macklin JJ, Chen Y, Konnerth A, Jayaraman V, Looger LL, Schreiter ER, Svoboda K, Kim DS. High-performance calcium sensors for imaging activity in neuronal populations and microcompartments. Nature Methods. 2019;16:649–657. doi: 10.1038/s41592-019-0435-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Davison JM, Akitake CM, Goll MG, Rhee JM, Gosse N, Baier H, Halpern ME, Leach SD, Parsons MJ. Transactivation from Gal4-VP16 transgenic insertions for tissue-specific cell labeling and ablation in zebrafish. Developmental Biology. 2007;304:811–824. doi: 10.1016/j.ydbio.2007.01.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Distel M, Wullimann MF, Köster RW. Optimized Gal4 genetics for permanent gene expression mapping in zebrafish. PNAS. 2009;106:13365–13370. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0903060106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Dunn TW, Gebhardt C, Naumann EA, Riegler C, Ahrens MB, Engert F, Del Bene F. Neural circuits underlying visually evoked escapes in larval zebrafish. Neuron. 2016;89:613–628. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.12.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Ewert JP. Neural mechanisms of prey-catching and avoidance behavior in the toad (Bufo bufo L.) Brain, Behavior and Evolution. 1970;3:36–56. doi: 10.1159/000125462. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Ewert JP. Neural correlates of key stimulus and releasing mechanism: a case study and two concepts. Trends in Neurosciences. 1997;20:332–339. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(96)01042-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Ewert JP, Buxbaum-Conradi H, Dreisvogt F, Glagow M, Merkel-Harff C, Röttgen A, Schürg-Pfeiffer E, Schwippert WW. Neural modulation of visuomotor functions underlying prey-catching behaviour in anurans: perception, attention, motor performance, learning. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology Part A: Molecular & Integrative Physiology. 2001;128:417–460. doi: 10.1016/S1095-6433(00)00333-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Fajardo O, Zhu P, Friedrich RW. Control of a specific motor program by a small brain area in zebrafish. Frontiers in Neural Circuits. 2013;7:67. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2013.00067. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Flood TF, Iguchi S, Gorczyca M, White B, Ito K, Yoshihara M. A single pair of interneurons commands the Drosophila feeding motor program. Nature. 2013;499:83–87. doi: 10.1038/nature12208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Frost WN, Katz PS. Single neuron control over a complex motor program. PNAS. 1996;93:422–426. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.1.422. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Gahtan E, Tanger P, Baier H. Visual prey capture in larval zebrafish is controlled by identified reticulospinal neurons downstream of the tectum. Journal of Neuroscience. 2005;25:9294–9303. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2678-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Han W, Tellez LA, Rangel MJ, Motta SC, Zhang X, Perez IO, Canteras NS, Shammah-Lagnado SJ, van den Pol AN, de Araujo IE. Integrated control of predatory hunting by the central nucleus of the amygdala. Cell. 2017;168:311–324. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2016.12.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Helmbrecht TO, Dal Maschio M, Donovan JC, Koutsouli S, Baier H. Topography of a visuomotor transformation. Neuron. 2018;100:1429–1445. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.10.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Henriques PM, Rahman N, Jackson SE, Bianco IH. Nucleus isthmi is required to sustain target pursuit during visually guided Prey-Catching. Current Biology. 2019;29:1771–1786. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2019.04.064. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Herrero L, Rodríguez F, Salas C, Torres B. Tail and eye movements evoked by electrical microstimulation of the optic tectum in goldfish. Experimental Brain Research. 1998;120:291–305. doi: 10.1007/s002210050403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Horstick EJ, Jordan DC, Bergeron SA, Tabor KM, Serpe M, Feldman B, Burgess HA. Increased functional protein expression using nucleotide sequence features enriched in highly expressed genes in zebrafish. Nucleic Acids Research. 2015;43:e48. doi: 10.1093/nar/gkv035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Hoy JL, Bishop HI, Niell CM. Defined cell types in superior colliculus make distinct contributions to prey capture behavior in the mouse. bioRxiv. 2019 doi: 10.1101/626622. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  30. Inagaki HK, Jung Y, Hoopfer ED, Wong AM, Mishra N, Lin JY, Tsien RY, Anderson DJ. Optogenetic control of Drosophila using a red-shifted channelrhodopsin reveals experience-dependent influences on courtship. Nature Methods. 2014;11:325–332. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2765. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Jing J. Encyclopedia of Neuroscience. Elsevier Ltd; 2009. Command systems; pp. 1149–1158. [Google Scholar]
  32. Jordi J, Guggiana-Nilo D, Soucy E, Song EY, Lei Wee C, Engert F. A high-throughput assay for quantifying appetite and digestive dynamics. American Journal of Physiology-Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology. 2015;309:R345–R357. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00225.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Kawashima T, Zwart MF, Yang CT, Mensh BD, Ahrens MB. The serotonergic system tracks the outcomes of actions to mediate Short-Term motor learning. Cell. 2016;167:933–946. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2016.09.055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Kay JN, Finger-Baier KC, Roeser T, Staub W, Baier H. Retinal ganglion cell genesis requires Lakritz, a zebrafish atonal homolog. Neuron. 2001;30:725–736. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00312-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Klapoetke NC, Murata Y, Kim SS, Pulver SR, Birdsey-Benson A, Cho YK, Morimoto TK, Chuong AS, Carpenter EJ, Tian Z, Wang J, Xie Y, Yan Z, Zhang Y, Chow BY, Surek B, Melkonian M, Jayaraman V, Constantine-Paton M, Wong GK, Boyden ES. Independent optical excitation of distinct neural populations. Nature Methods. 2014;11:338–346. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2836. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Knafo S, Fidelin K, Prendergast A, Tseng PB, Parrin A, Dickey C, Böhm UL, Figueiredo SN, Thouvenin O, Pascal-Moussellard H, Wyart C. Mechanosensory neurons control the timing of spinal microcircuit selection during locomotion. eLife. 2017;6:e25260. doi: 10.7554/eLife.25260. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Kohl J, Babayan BM, Rubinstein ND, Autry AE, Marin-Rodriguez B, Kapoor V, Miyamishi K, Zweifel LS, Luo L, Uchida N, Dulac C. Functional circuit architecture underlying parental behaviour. Nature. 2018;556:326–331. doi: 10.1038/s41586-018-0027-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Korn H, Faber DS. The mauthner cell half a century later: a neurobiological model for decision-making? Neuron. 2005;47:13–28. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.05.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Koyama M, Kinkhabwala A, Satou C, Higashijima S, Fetcho J. Mapping a sensory-motor network onto a structural and functional ground plan in the hindbrain. PNAS. 2011;108:1170–1175. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1012189108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Kupfermann I, Weiss KR. The command neuron concept. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 1978;1:3–10. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00059057. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  41. Li Y, Zeng J, Zhang J, Yue C, Zhong W, Liu Z, Feng Q, Luo M. Hypothalamic circuits for predation and evasion. Neuron. 2018;97:911–924. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Lister JA, Robertson CP, Lepage T, Johnson SL, Raible DW. nacre encodes a zebrafish microphthalmia-related protein that regulates neural-crest-derived pigment cell fate. Development. 1999;126:3757–3767. doi: 10.1242/dev.126.17.3757. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Longair MH, Baker DA, Armstrong JD. Simple Neurite Tracer: open source software for reconstruction, visualization and analysis of neuronal processes. Bioinformatics. 2011;27:2453–2454. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btr390. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Marquart GD, Tabor KM, Brown M, Strykowski JL, Varshney GK, LaFave MC, Mueller T, Burgess SM, Higashijima S-ichi, Burgess HA. A 3D Searchable Database of Transgenic Zebrafish Gal4 and Cre Lines for Functional Neuroanatomy Studies. Frontiers in Neural Circuits. 2015;9:78. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2015.00078. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Marquart GD, Tabor KM, Horstick EJ, Brown M, Geoca AK, Polys NF, Nogare DD, Burgess HA. High-precision registration between zebrafish brain atlases using symmetric diffeomorphic normalization. GigaScience. 2017;6:1–15. doi: 10.1093/gigascience/gix056. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Marques JC, Lackner S, Félix R, Orger MB. Structure of the zebrafish locomotor repertoire revealed with unsupervised behavioral clustering. Current Biology. 2018;28:181–195. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Masai I, Lele Z, Yamaguchi M, Komori A, Nakata A, Nishiwaki Y, Wada H, Tanaka H, Nojima Y, Hammerschmidt M, Wilson SW, Okamoto H. N-cadherin mediates retinal lamination, maintenance of forebrain compartments and patterning of retinal neurites. Development. 2003;130:2479–2494. doi: 10.1242/dev.00465. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. McElligott MB, O'malley DM. Prey tracking by larval zebrafish: axial kinematics and visual control. Brain, Behavior and Evolution. 2005;66:177–196. doi: 10.1159/000087158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Miri A, Daie K, Burdine RD, Aksay E, Tank DW. Regression-based identification of behavior-encoding neurons during large-scale optical imaging of neural activity at cellular resolution. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2011;105:964–980. doi: 10.1152/jn.00702.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Muto A, Lal P, Ailani D, Abe G, Itoh M, Kawakami K. Activation of the hypothalamic feeding centre upon visual prey detection. Nature Communications. 2017;8:15029. doi: 10.1038/ncomms15029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Park SG, Jeong YC, Kim DG, Lee MH, Shin A, Park G, Ryoo J, Hong J, Bae S, Kim CH, Lee PS, Kim D. Medial preoptic circuit induces hunting-like actions to target objects and prey. Nature Neuroscience. 2018;21:364–372. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0072-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Patterson BW, Abraham AO, MacIver MA, McLean DL. Visually guided gradation of prey capture movements in larval zebrafish. Journal of Experimental Biology. 2013;216:3071–3083. doi: 10.1242/jeb.087742. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Qian J, Hastie T, Friedman J, Tibshirani R, Simon N. Glmnet for matlab. 2013 http://www.stanford.edu/~hastie/glmnet_matlab/
  54. Semmelhack JL, Donovan JC, Thiele TR, Kuehn E, Laurell E, Baier H. A dedicated visual pathway for prey detection in larval zebrafish. eLife. 2014;3:e04878. doi: 10.7554/eLife.04878. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Shang C, Liu A, Li D, Xie Z, Chen Z, Huang M, Li Y, Wang Y, Shen WL, Cao P. A subcortical excitatory circuit for sensory-triggered predatory hunting in mice. Nature Neuroscience. 2019;22:909–920. doi: 10.1038/s41593-019-0405-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Sillar KT, Picton L, Heitler WJ. The Neuroethology of Predation and Escape. Wiley Blackwell; 2016. [Google Scholar]
  57. Trivedi CA, Bollmann JH. Visually driven chaining of elementary swim patterns into a goal-directed motor sequence: a virtual reality study of zebrafish prey capture. Frontiers in Neural Circuits. 2013;7:86. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2013.00086. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Vladimirov N, Mu Y, Kawashima T, Bennett DV, Yang CT, Looger LL, Keller PJ, Freeman J, Ahrens MB. Light-sheet functional imaging in fictively behaving zebrafish. Nature Methods. 2014;11:883–884. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.3040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. von Philipsborn AC, Liu T, Yu JY, Masser C, Bidaye SS, Dickson BJ. Neuronal control of Drosophila courtship song. Neuron. 2011;69:509–522. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.01.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Wen L, Wei W, Gu W, Huang P, Ren X, Zhang Z, Zhu Z, Lin S, Zhang B. Visualization of monoaminergic neurons and neurotoxicity of MPTP in live transgenic zebrafish. Developmental Biology. 2008;314:84–92. doi: 10.1016/j.ydbio.2007.11.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Yáñez J, Suárez T, Quelle A, Folgueira M, Anadón R. Neural connections of the pretectum in zebrafish (Danio rerio) The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2018;526:1017–1040. doi: 10.1002/cne.24388. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Yoshihara M, Yoshihara M. 'Necessary and sufficient' in biology is not necessarily necessary - confusions and erroneous conclusions resulting from misapplied logic in the field of biology, especially neuroscience. Journal of Neurogenetics. 2018;32:53–64. doi: 10.1080/01677063.2018.1468443. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Zhao ZD, Chen Z, Xiang X, Hu M, Xie H, Jia X, Cai F, Cui Y, Chen Z, Qian L, Liu J, Shang C, Yang Y, Ni X, Sun W, Hu J, Cao P, Li H, Shen WL. Zona incerta GABAergic neurons integrate prey-related sensory signals and induce an appetitive drive to promote hunting. Nature Neuroscience. 2019;22:921–932. doi: 10.1038/s41593-019-0404-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Zolessi FR, Poggi L, Wilkinson CJ, Chien CB, Harris WA. Polarization and orientation of retinal ganglion cells in vivo. Neural Development. 2006;1:2. doi: 10.1186/1749-8104-1-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Zou H, Hastie T. Regularization and variable selection via the elastic net. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B. 2005;67:301–320. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9868.2005.00503.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: Vatsala Thirumalai1
Reviewed by: Vatsala Thirumalai2

In the interests of transparency, eLife includes the editorial decision letter and accompanying author responses. A lightly edited version of the letter sent to the authors after peer review is shown, indicating the most substantive concerns; minor comments are not usually included.

Thank you for submitting your article "A pretectal command system controls hunting behaviour" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by three peer reviewers, including Vatsala Thirumalai as the Reviewing Editor and Reviewer #1, and the evaluation has been overseen by Ronald Calabrese as the Senior Editor.

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

Summary:

In this manuscript, the authors uncover the role of a group of pretectal neurons in generating hunting behaviour in larval zebrafish. By using an impressive combination of techniques and detailed functional, anatomical and behavioural analyses the authors have identified a brain region that contains a fraction of cells that can be stimulated to induce hunting-like behaviour and ablated to diminish the same behaviour. This work is technically impressive and the conceptual presentation is excellent. The activation of single neurons to induce hunting-like behaviour is already impressive, and the authors go beyond that by identifying fish that do show behavioural responses on activating a given cell, and image the same cell in that fish in a virtual behavioural assay to functionally characterize it. The reviewers suggest the following essential revisions to improve what is an already strong manuscript.

Essential revisions:

The authors make a strong statement that these neurons constitute a 'command system' for initiating hunting based on their finding that activation of single neurons could elicit hunting routines even in the absence of prey-like stimuli. However, the reviewers felt that such a strong claim was not sufficiently substantiated by the data presented for the following reasons:

1) Each neuron that was able to trigger prey capture did so only in a minority of trials (~18%) and did so with very long latencies (~4s). If they think that the optogenetic stimulation is triggering hunts by direct activation of AF7-pretec neurons, why is it successful only in a small percentage of trials and why does it take several seconds especially in the case of the neurons that project to the nMLF? Previous reports (Thiele et al., 2014; Del Maschio et al., 2017) suggest that direct activation of nMLF generates tail bends in less than a second after photostimulation. Even if AF7-pretec were 1-3 synapses away, delay seems longer than would be predicted for a command system. If this is likely due to low levels of stimulation of ChR2, did the authors test a range of light intensities? A likely alternative interpretation would be that AF7-pretec neurons encode "prey" value – activation of these neurons signal the existence of prey in the visual field, which then increases the probability of downstream generation of the hunting command.

2) The criterion for deciding that the stimulation induced a "hunting routine" is only observing a "period with ocular vergence above threshold". An alternative explanation would be that these neurons are premotor neurons driving eye vergence, and when the animal finds its eyes converged, it responds (with some delay) with a tail movement. Can the authors rule this out?

Given these concerns, the reviewers suggest that the authors reconsider their nomenclature of these neurons as a command system. The results are entirely consistent with the AF7-pretectal neurons playing a 'hunt-promoting' role. Indeed, this study parallels a recently published study (Zhao et al., 2019) identifying GABAergic neurons in zona incerta of mice as 'hunt-promoting' based on similar lines of experimentation. We believe that toning down on the "command system" hypothesis and instead positing this group of neurons as a "hunt-promoting" nucleus will be closer to the results and yet not take away from the importance of these findings.

In addition, reviewers have the following suggestions grouped broadly into experiments, analysis and presentation:

Experiments:

1) The authors stimulate single neurons using the KalTA4u508 reagent. They do not report on the results of stimulating the entire population of AF7-pretectal neurons labeled by the KalTA4u508 stable transgenic line. Is this because the line labels many neurons elsewhere in the brain? If not, it would be important to include this information. It is also essential to provide more information on the expression pattern in the KalTA4u508 line across all regions of the brain and in the spinal cord.

2) Authors ablated these neurons to show effects on hunting. However, the control included no photo-ablation of any cells, and therefore does not control for damage to other nearby cells, or axons of RGCs. The authors should either perform a control with ablation of similar numbers of cells nearby, or justify the current control experiment in the text while acknowledging any shortcomings.

Analysis:

1) There is no 'Eye vergence only' regressor included in the analysis of activity. If the authors claim that eye convergence without tail movement never occurs then this might be important to show explicitly (again making clear what the delays are between the eye and tail movements).

2) Clustering: To obtain their functional clusters, the authors cluster the top 5% responsive neurons then assign the rest of the neurons to these clusters based on distance to cluster centroids. This analysis would need some additional information to make it more transparent. (a) What is the distribution between the cells selected with the two criteria, namely, high visually evoked activity or well modeled in terms of motor variables? (b) What numbers of cells formed the seeds for the 36 clusters (min-max range and median)? It is also recommended to add this criteria (that clustering was done on 5% of cells) in the Results text as well.

3) It was hard to be sure that the description of AF7 neurons scoring high on the "hunting index" (HIx) was a significant result as opposed to falling out of the nature of the regression. HIx measures predominance of activity during hunting as compared to non-response trials. But, in a sense, these are correlated: clustering picks out those cells having motor responses, hence likely to have a high HIx? What is the additional value of the HIx?

4) Figure 1E and F: In Bianco and Engert (2015), using the same moving spot paradigm the authors find that the "distribution of spot locations at time of convergent saccade did not differ for left-right versus right-left stimuli". In contrast this manuscript shows in Figure 1E and F a difference for left to right vs. right to left stimuli. Is this significant? What might account for this difference?

5) Cluster 26 and 28 show lateralized responses and in the fifth paragraph of the subsection “Pretectal neurons are recruited during hunting initiation”, authors state that their distributions are also lateralized but Figure 1—figure supplement 2D shows about equal numbers for cluster 26 on both sides. An explanation seems necessary.

Presentation:

1) The authors need to be clearer about the nature of the visuomotor vector that they use to regress Ca fluorescence. Is it possible to visualize an example VMV or two in the supplementary figures? The figures could be better explicated: for example, in Figure 1G, meaning of terms such as "Conv tail sym" are not immediately obvious until one gets fairly deep into the Materials and methods section.

2) To help the reader, the four tightly packed figures should be broken into perhaps 6 or 7. Currently, each figure is so dense that the font sizes are difficult to read. Further, it is harder to follow the logic when each figure is making multiple, often complex, points. Specifically, Figure 1J-O could become their own figure. The relevant subset of data shown in Figure 1—figure supplement 2A (i.e., the calcium responses during hunting and non-response for the key clusters of 1, 4, and 25-28) could be brought forward into that figure or another new figure. Figure 2 could likewise be split into anatomy of the AF7 neurons and their behavioral responses. The authors make a very nice analysis of the effect of the anterior ventral optic tectum (avOT) and retinal inputs on prey capture behavior. Unfortunately this has been placed in a supplementary figure (Figure 4—figure supplement 1). We would suggest that this be made into a figure on its own.

3) Subsection “Pretectal neurons labelled by KalTA4u508 with hunting-initiation activity”, fourth paragraph: It's notable that the KalTA4u508 neurons don't respond to visual stimuli alone, in marked contrast to the clusters of which they are members (cf. Figure 1N). Presumably this is because these neurons are a small subset of those clusters, but the authors should acknowledge this distinction in the Results and perhaps speculate on the implications – other cluster members must have more significant visual responses.

4) The Introduction and Discussion should be rewritten to position the AF7-pretectal neurons as a hunt-promoting nucleus instead of a command system. The title should also be suitably modified. It is best to avoid poorly defined terms such as 'behavioral epistasis' (in describing the interaction between avOT and AF7-pretec).

eLife. 2019 Oct 8;8:e48114. doi: 10.7554/eLife.48114.038

Author response


Essential revisions:

The authors make a strong statement that these neurons constitute a 'command system' for initiating hunting based on their finding that activation of single neurons could elicit hunting routines even in the absence of prey-like stimuli. However, the reviewers felt that such a strong claim was not sufficiently substantiated by the data presented for the following reasons:

1) Each neuron that was able to trigger prey capture did so only in a minority of trials (~18%) and did so with very long latencies (~4s). If they think that the optogenetic stimulation is triggering hunts by direct activation of AF7-pretec neurons, why is it successful only in a small percentage of trials and why does it take several seconds especially in the case of the neurons that project to the nMLF? Previous reports (Thiele et al., 2014; Del Maschio et al., 2017) suggest that direct activation of nMLF generates tail bends in less than a second after photostimulation. Even if AF7-pretec were 1-3 synapses away, delay seems longer than would be predicted for a command system. If this is likely due to low levels of stimulation of ChR2, did the authors test a range of light intensities? A likely alternative interpretation would be that AF7-pretec neurons encode "prey" value – activation of these neurons signal the existence of prey in the visual field, which then increases the probability of downstream generation of the hunting command.

We agree with the concerns about long latency and modest response probability observed with single neuron stimulation. Therefore, we have performed two additional experiments (which are now presented within Figure 5—figure supplement 1 and 2).

1) First, because the light intensities we could achieve for free swimming experiments were low (<1 mW/mm2) we tested whether response rates might increase (and latency decrease) at higher irradiance (using a new higher power LED enabling up to 2.5 mW/mm2). In addition, the observation that blind fish showed a substantial reduction in latency and increase in response rate in the context of avOT stimulation, suggested that perhaps the bright blue light interfered with induction of hunting. Therefore, we have also now generated and tested blind (lakritz-/-) fish expressing CoChR-tdTomato in single AF7-pretectal neurons. In one blind larva we found that response rate increased with irradiance, reaching 100% at 2.55 mW/mm2 whereas latency showed only a modest decrease at higher intensity. Notably, we could only generate a very small number of larvae that were both homozygous for the lakritz mutation, carriers of the necessary KalTA4u508 and atoh7:gapRFP transgenes and had a single pretectal neuron labelled. Thus, these are preliminary observations, but we believe it is nonetheless informative to include this data within a figure supplement (Figure 5—figure supplement 1).

2) Second, we investigated the effect of stimulating multiple AF7-pretectal neurons. The KalTA4u508 transgene indeed labels various other cell types (see below), preventing specific optogenetic targeting of pretectal neurons in free-swimming animals. Therefore, we used a patterned illumination system (a DMD rig) to focally stimulate the pretectal region in tethered KalTA4u508;UAS:CoChR-tdTomato larvae (Figure 5—figure supplement 2). In this experiment, where the full complement of KalTA4u508-labelled pretectal cells are stimulated and higher irradiance levels were possible (22 mW/mm2), we observed much higher hunting response probabilities and an order of magnitude reduction in response latency (to ~80 ms).

This new data indicates that the transgenically labelled population of AF7-pretectal cells can induce hunting with very short latency, compatible with a command system function. The long latencies observed with single cell stimulation might represent a limited capability of single pretectal neurons to bring downstream pattern generating circuits to threshold. We do not favour the interpretation that ‘AF7-pretectal neurons encode "prey" value – activation of these neurons signal the existence of prey in the visual field’ as KalTA4u508 cells explicitly do not respond to the existence of prey but rather are recruited at initiation of hunting.

2) The criterion for deciding that the stimulation induced a "hunting routine" is only observing a "period with ocular vergence above threshold". An alternative explanation would be that these neurons are premotor neurons driving eye vergence, and when the animal finds its eyes converged, it responds (with some delay) with a tail movement. Can the authors rule this out?

It is correct that we use the period of elevated vergence to detect the occurrence and duration of (natural and optogenetically evoked) hunting routines. However, although this is our detection criterion, ocular vergence is not the only feature of those routines. In all fish where we could induce hunting, 100% of convergent saccades were paired with tail movement. This was true both in the free-swimming and tethered (DMD) optogenetic stimulation paradigms. The delay between saccadic convergence and tail movement averaged <10 ms, which is very similar to natural hunting behaviour. This data was already included in our first submission but for emphasis we have now included the eye-tail lag as an additional panel in the main figure. The tail movements that are coincident with saccadic convergence showed kinematic similarities with those observed during natural hunting (i.e. asymmetry, distal curvature, tail-beat frequency etc). Multiple swim bouts were typically observed during optogenetically evoked hunting (Figure 5—figure supplement 1B) and remarkably, the final swim bout sometimes resembled a capture swim, characterised by jaw-opening, dorsiflexion and eye divergence (observed for ~40% of cells and now quantified in Figure 5—figure supplement 1C, D). It is on this basis that we consider optogenetic stimulation of KalTA4u508 cells induces hunting routines, rather than simply convergent saccades.

Given these concerns, the reviewers suggest that the authors reconsider their nomenclature of these neurons as a command system. The results are entirely consistent with the AF7-pretectal neurons playing a 'hunt-promoting' role. Indeed, this study parallels a recently published study (Zhao et al., 2019) identifying GABAergic neurons in zona incerta of mice as 'hunt-promoting' based on similar lines of experimentation. We believe that toning down on the "command system" hypothesis and instead positing this group of neurons as a "hunt-promoting" nucleus will be closer to the results and yet not take away from the importance of these findings.

The reviewers raised two valid concerns, but we believe that the new experimental data and analyses in this revised manuscript largely tackle both. Specifically, stimulating the KalTA4u508 pretectal population triggers hunting with very short latency, equivalent to that observed for command neurons in other species (e.g. Inagaki et al., 2014). Second, evoked responses are not limited to saccadic convergence but comprise a sequence of oculomotor and locomotor actions with kinematic signatures of natural hunting behaviour. In the revised Discussion we now discuss the recent findings related to the SC–ZI–PAG pathway, published while this manuscript was under review. In distinction to AF7-pretectal neurons, ZI cells show sensory responses to prey-like features, appear to generate a positive valence signal to motivate predation and evoke limited hunting-like behaviour in absence of prey. This is quite distinct to the properties of KalTA4u508 cells in AF7-pretectum. Moreover, KalTA4u508 cells satisfy the criteria for command system neurons as defined by Yoshihara and Yoshihara (2018), as summarised in the Discussion.

However, in consideration of the reviewers’ comments we have made the following changes:

i) We have changed the title to ‘Pretectal cells control hunting behaviour’ and edited the Abstract to make clear that a command system function is our interpretation of the role of the cells ‘We propose that…’.

ii) We have removed all comments concerning command neurons from the Introduction and Results.

iii) We have edited the Discussion to further clarify that a command system role is a proposal based upon the experimental results. This is mostly confined to one section entitled ‘Do AF7-pretectal neurons comprise a command system controlling predation?’ which also includes discussion of the new experimental data and further clarification.

We hope that the reviewers consider these revisions accurately represent the experimental data and make explicit the basis for our interpretation.

In addition, reviewers have the following suggestions grouped broadly into experiments, analysis and presentation:

Experiments:

1) The authors stimulate single neurons using the KalTA4u508 reagent. They do not report on the results of stimulating the entire population of AF7-pretectal neurons labeled by the KalTA4u508 stable transgenic line. Is this because the line labels many neurons elsewhere in the brain? If not, it would be important to include this information. It is also essential to provide more information on the expression pattern in the KalTA4u508 line across all regions of the brain and in the spinal cord.

We should have made this clearer in the original manuscript. Indeed, the KalTA4u508 transgene labels various additional cell populations within brain, spinal cord and sensory ganglia (e.g. lateral line), as well as some expression in muscle and skin. We now state this in the Results and explain that it is for this reason that we adopted a transient expression strategy to allow optogenetic stimulation specifically of the AF7-pretectal neurons. We also include an image of an entire transgenic larva (Figure 3—figure supplement 1A). The results of stimulating the entire (transgenically labelled) population of pretectal neurons, using the DMD set-up, is now presented (see above).

2) Authors ablated these neurons to show effects on hunting. However, the control included no photo-ablation of any cells, and therefore does not control for damage to other nearby cells, or axons of RGCs. The authors should either perform a control with ablation of similar numbers of cells nearby, or justify the current control experiment in the text while acknowledging any shortcomings.

We have now performed two ablation controls (see Figure 6—figure supplement 1). First, we ablated AF7-pretectal cells in larvae carrying the atoh7:GFP transgene to visualise RGC axons in AF7. These appeared undamaged by the ablation. Second, we ablated an equivalent number of neuronal somata slightly medial to the AF7-pretectal region (labelled by gata2a:GFP) and observed no impairment of hunting performance. These controls indicate our ablations were well targeted and cause impairment of hunting due to loss of KalTA4u508 cells.

Analysis:

1) There is no 'Eye vergence only' regressor included in the analysis of activity. If the authors claim that eye convergence without tail movement never occurs then this might be important to show explicitly (again making clear what the delays are between the eye and tail movements).

Eye convergence without tail movement is very rare, occurring for <10% of saccades. We now show this data in Figure 1—figure supplement 1 along with the eye-tail time lag during the virtual hunting assay (~10 ms). Due to the very limited number of events we did not generate an ‘eye vergence only’ regressor.

2) Clustering: To obtain their functional clusters, the authors cluster the top 5% responsive neurons then assign the rest of the neurons to these clusters based on distance to cluster centroids. This analysis would need some additional information to make it more transparent. (a) What is the distribution between the cells selected with the two criteria, namely, high visually evoked activity or well modeled in terms of motor variables? (b) What numbers of cells formed the seeds for the 36 clusters (min-max range and median?. It is also recommended to add this criteria (that clustering was done on 5% of cells) in the Results text as well.

The distributions of max visual response and regression goodness-of-fit (i.e. the metrics used for cell selection) are now shown in Figure 1—figure supplement 1D. The total fraction of cells used for clustering was 9.5% (top 5th percentile for either visual or motor components). The numbers of cells in all of the seeds (as well as the final clusters) has been added as Figure 1—figure supplement 1F and G and the Results text has been edited as suggested.

3) It was hard to be sure that the description of AF7 neurons scoring high on the "hunting index" (HIx) was a significant result as opposed to falling out of the nature of the regression. HIx measures predominance of activity during hunting as compared to non-response trials. But, in a sense, these are correlated: clustering picks out those cells having motor responses, hence likely to have a high HIx? What is the additional value of the HIx?

The HIx result does not necessarily follow from the nature of the regression but is expected based on the results of the clustering. Because convergent saccades are correlated with presentation of prey-like stimuli, it is possible that a cell may have a significant ‘motor’ coefficient for convergence but in fact be encoding the presence of prey. The results of the clustering indicate this is, however, unlikely as the convergent saccade (hunt initiation) clusters have very minimal visual activity (extracted from non-response trials). The HIx analysis confirms this and isolates the modulation attributable to hunt initiation (which is not explicitly represented by any single VMV element). We believe this analysis (as well as the stimulus and motor-triggered calcium profiles) show in a very direct and convincing way that hunt initiation neurons are specifically activated in response trials, when the fish release convergent saccades.

4) Figure 1E and F: In Bianco and Engert (2015), using the same moving spot paradigm the authors find that the "distribution of spot locations at time of convergent saccade did not differ for left-right versus right-left stimuli". In contrast this manuscript shows in Figure 1E and F a difference for left to right vs. right to left stimuli. Is this significant? What might account for this difference?

The difference versus Bianco and Engert (2015) is likely due to pooling of locations for fast (30°/s) and slow (15°/s) stimuli in that prior study. Using exclusively 30°/s prey-like cues in this study, we consistently see a preference for the larvae to respond once the spot has crossed the midline and is moving nose–tail. Cumulative distribution functions with p-value have now been added in Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

5) Cluster 26 and 28 show lateralized responses and in the fifth paragraph of the subsection “Pretectal neurons are recruited during hunting initiation”, authors state that their distributions are also lateralized but Figure 1—figure supplement 2D shows about equal numbers for cluster 26 on both sides. An explanation seems necessary.

Figure 1—figure supplement 2D (now Figure 2—figure supplement 2C) shows data for all clustered neurons, whereas the statement in the Results text refers to cluster 26/28 cells in AF7-pretectum. The strongly lateralised anatomical distributions of these cells in pretectum is shown in the insets in Figure 2A.

Presentation:

1) The authors need to be clearer about the nature of the visuomotor vector that they use to regress Ca fluorescence. Is it possible to visualize an example VMV or two in the supplementary figures? The figures could be better explicated: for example, in Figure 1G, meaning of terms such as "Conv tail sym" are not immediately obvious until one gets fairly deep into the Materials and methods section.

VMVs for all clustered neurons are displayed in Figure 1; these are not used to regress Ca fluorescence but instead describe the visual responses and regression coefficients. We have edited the schematic in Figure 1G to make this clearer. We suspect the reviewers are asking for examples of kinematic regressors alongside Ca data. This has now been added in Figure 1—figure supplement 1E. We have added a description of the Conv tail L/R/sym regressors/coefficients to the Results text to aid the reader.

2) To help the reader, the four tightly packed figures should be broken into perhaps 6 or 7. Currently, each figure is so dense that the font sizes are difficult to read. Further, it is harder to follow the logic when each figure is making multiple, often complex, points. Specifically, Figure 1J-O could become their own figure. The relevant subset of data shown in Figure 1—figure supplement 2A (i.e., the calcium responses during hunting and non-response for the key clusters of 1, 4, and 25-28) could be brought forward into that figure or another new figure. Figure 2 could likewise be split into anatomy of the AF7 neurons and their behavioral responses. The authors make a very nice analysis of the effect of the anterior ventral optic tectum (avOT) and retinal inputs on prey capture behavior. Unfortunately this has been placed in a supplementary figure (Figure 4—figure supplement 1). We would suggest that this be made into a figure on its own.

In this revised manuscript we now present the results across 8 main figures. We opted to split Figure 1 after panel K so that this figure now covers functional identification of hunting-related neurons, while localisation of hunting-initiation cells to the AF7-pretectal region is now presented separately in Figure 2. As suggested, the anatomy and response properties of KalTA4u508 cells is now divided (into Figures 3 and 4). We have added an additional supplementary figure to present the DMD stimulation data and in line with the reviewers’ suggestion, now present the avOT data in a main figure, Figure 7. Figure 8 presents the effect of KalTA4u508 ablation on avOT-induced hunting and the final circuit model. We have increased font sizes and hope that the new layout has improved presentation of the data.

3) Subsection “Pretectal neurons labelled by KalTA4u508 with hunting-initiation activity”, fourth paragraph: It's notable that the KalTA4u508 neurons don't respond to visual stimuli alone, in marked contrast to the clusters of which they are members (cf. Figure 1N). Presumably this is because these neurons are a small subset of those clusters, but the authors should acknowledge this distinction in the Results and perhaps speculate on the implications – other cluster members must have more significant visual responses.

We have added the following statement to the Results: ‘The absence of visual activity in KalTA4u508 neurons is in contrast to the small responses seen previously in hunting-initiation clusters (Figure 2), suggesting some functional heterogeneity in clusters derived from pan-neuronal imaging.’

4) The Introduction and Discussion should be rewritten to position the AF7-pretectal neurons as a hunt-promoting nucleus instead of a command system. The title should also be suitably modified. It is best to avoid poorly defined terms such as 'behavioral epistasis' (in describing the interaction between avOT and AF7-pretec).

As discussed above, the Introduction, Discussion and title have been modified. The term ‘behavioural epistasis’ has been removed.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Figure 1—source data 1. Source data for Figure 1.

    Data provided as a MATLAB structure.

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.48114.006
    Figure 3—source data 1. Source data for Figure 3.

    Data provided as a MATLAB structure.

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.48114.013
    Figure 4—source data 1. Source data for Figure 4.

    Data provided as a MATLAB structure.

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.48114.016
    Figure 5—source data 1. Source data for Figure 5.

    Data provided as a MATLAB structure.

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.48114.020
    Figure 6—source data 1. Source data for Figure 6.

    Data provided as a MATLAB structure.

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.48114.024
    Figure 7—source data 1. Source data for Figure 7.

    Data provided as a MATLAB structure.

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.48114.027
    Figure 8—source data 1. Source data for Figure 8.

    Data provided as a MATLAB structure.

    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.48114.030
    Supplementary file 1. Regression and statistical details.

    Spreadsheet containing details of statistical analyses (test used, test statistic, values of N, centre and spread), and description of kinematic regressors.

    elife-48114-supp1.xlsx (22.3KB, xlsx)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.48114.031
    Supplementary file 2. Anatomical mask – avOT.

    TIFF stack containing binary mask defining the ‘avOT’ anatomical region, in ZBB space.

    elife-48114-supp2.tif (685.7KB, tif)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.48114.032
    Supplementary file 3. Anatomical mask – AF7-pretectum.

    TIFF stack containing binary mask defining the ‘AF7-pretectum’ anatomical region, in ZBB space.

    elife-48114-supp3.tif (682.4KB, tif)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.48114.033
    Supplementary file 4. Anatomical mask – NI chata.

    TIFF stack containing binary mask defining the ‘NI chata’ anatomical region, in ZBB space.

    elife-48114-supp4.tif (680.3KB, tif)
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.48114.034
    Transparent reporting form
    DOI: 10.7554/eLife.48114.035

    Data Availability Statement

    Data generated or analysed during this study are included in the manuscript and supporting files. Source data files have been provided for Figures 1 and 2—8.


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES