Abstract
Color polymorphisms have been traditionally attributed to apostatic selection. The perception of color depends on the visual system of the observer. Theoretical models predict that differently perceived degrees of conspicuousness by two predator and prey species may cause the evolution of polymorphisms in the presence of anti-apostatic and apostatic selection. The spider Gasteracantha cancriformis (Araneidae) possesses several conspicuous color morphs. In orb-web spiders, the prey attraction hypothesis states that conspicuous colors are prey lures that increase spider foraging success via flower mimicry. Therefore, polymorphism could be maintained if each morph attracted a different prey species (multiple prey hypothesis) and each spider mimicked a different flower color (flower mimicry hypothesis). Conspicuous colors could be a warning signal to predators because of the spider’s hard abdomen and spines. Multiple predators could perceive morphs differently and exert different degrees of selective pressures (multiple predator hypothesis). We explored these 3 hypotheses using reflectance data and color vision modeling to estimate the chromatic and achromatic contrast of G. cancriformis morphs as perceived by several potential prey and predator taxa. Our results revealed that individual taxa perceive the conspicuousness of morphs differently. Therefore, the multiple prey hypothesis and, in part, the multiple predator hypothesis may explain the evolution of color polymorphism in G. cancriformis, even in the presence of anti-apostatic selection. The flower mimicry hypothesis received support by color metrics, but not by color vision models. Other parameters not evaluated by color vision models could also affect the perception of morphs and influence morph survival and polymorphism stability.
Keywords: aposematism, Araneidae, Gasteracantha cancriformis, luring, prey attraction, visual communication
The evolution and maintenance of color polymorphisms have traditionally been attributed to apostatic selection (Clarke 1979). Assuming that predators form a search image (Tinbergen 1960), the advantage of rarity promotes the coexistence of multiple prey types and stabilizes polymorphisms (Bond 2007). Nonetheless, other adaptive and nonadaptive explanations for the evolution and maintenance of color polymorphisms have been proposed (Grey and McKinnon 2007). For instance, gene flow between populations with distinct phenotypes that are favored by natural selection could maintain polymorphism within populations (Grey and McKinnon 2007; Farkas et al. 2013).
In the context of visual signaling, the distinct visual systems of prey and predators may play a role in the evolution and maintenance of color polymorphisms (Ruxton et al. 2004; White and Kemp 2015). Animal communication involves the generation, transmission, and processing of the signal by a receiver, in which an appropriate response is elicited (Endler 1993). Any factors that affect these steps can influence signal efficiency and, as a result, affect the direction of communication evolution (Endler 1993). Thus, the diversity of signals is likely influenced by variation in the sensory systems of receivers.
Considering that the same “color” may be perceived as cryptic or conspicuous by different species (Endler and Mappes 2004), each color morph of a polymorphic population may represent an adaptation to particular visual systems of prey or predator species (Endler 1992; Ruxton et al. 2004; White and Kemp 2015). The role of multiple predators on the evolution of prey coloration has been approached by theoretical models (Endler 1988; Endler and Mappes 2004). Endler’s model (1988), for instance, indicated that a stable polymorphism might evolve in the presence of anti-apostatic selection (positive frequency-dependent) and apostatic selection (negative frequency-dependent) from different predators. Additionally, polymorphism might be stable in the presence of anti-apostatic selection from different predators, given that predators perceive prey conspicuousness differently and there is a covariance between the relative degree of crypsis and the degree of frequency-dependent selection by each predator (Endler 1988). Although Endler’s model has been developed to predators’ perception of prey, it could be applied to selection forces arising from other observers (e.g., variation in perception by prey).
Many orb-web spiders exhibit conspicuous coloration. Although sexual selection is a common explanation for bright coloration in other taxa such as birds (Ryan 1990), this scenario is less likely to happen in orb-web spiders, because they may lack color vision (Yamashita and Tateda 1978; Yamashita 1985). Argiope argentata (Araneidae), for instance, seems to possess only 1 photoreceptor (Tiedemann et al. 1986). The prey attraction hypothesis states that the bright coloration of some spiders is used to lure insects, possibly by mimicking flower coloration (e.g., Craig and Ebert 1994; Hauber 2002). The hypothesis has been empirically tested several times, and most studies have found support for it. The polymorphic spider Nephila pilipes (Araneidae) presents a melanic and a bright colored morph (Tso et al. 2004). The bright color patterns of this species are thought to resemble symmetric flower patterns that may attract bees, owing to the innate preference of bees for symmetry (Chiao et al. 2009). Moreover, yellow patches on the spider’s body may be perceived as food resources by flower visitors (Tso et al. 2004). Besides being attractive to pollinators, the yellow patches also seems to attract hymenopteran predators. Therefore, it is possible that there is a trade-off between foraging success and predation risk in polymorphic populations in which some morphs are more cryptic than others (Fan et al. 2009).
The predators of orb-web spiders possess very distinct visual systems. Birds, for example, are tetrachromats, whose photoreceptors are most sensitive to ultraviolet/violet, blue, green, and red (Hart 2001), whereas spider-hunting wasps, such as members of the family Sphecidae, are trichromats, whose photoreceptors are most sensitive to ultraviolet, blue, and green (Peitsch et al. 1992; Briscoe and Chittka 2001). Similarly, the insect prey of orb-web spiders also vary in their types of color vision. For example, bees are trichromats with spectral sensitivities similar to those of sphecid wasps (Briscoe and Chittka 2001), whereas some lepidopterans are tetrachromats (Briscoe and Chittka 2001; Koshitaka et al. 2008; Arikawa 2017), and some dipterans possess photoreceptors with 5 different sensitivity peaks (Schnaitmann and Garbers 2013). Therefore, the maintenance of spider color polymorphism may result not only from a trade-off between prey attraction and predation risk, but also from selective pressure from multiple receivers (Endler 1992; Ruxton et al. 2004; White and Kemp 2015).
The orb-web spider Gasteracantha cancriformis (Araneidae) constructs large webs and rests in the web hub during the day (Levi 1978). Females of the species possess a hard abdomen with 3 pairs of spines, and vary in color, with some morphs quite conspicuous to human observers (Levi 1978; Gawryszewski and Motta 2012). The ventral side of females is mostly black, sometimes with small bright spots. In one studied population, the dorsal side of females possessed black or reddish spines and 4 distinct color patterns: yellow, white (without UV reflectance), red, and a combination of black and white (white patches reflects UV; Gawryszewski 2007; Gawryszewski and Motta 2012). The prey attraction hypothesis does not seem to explain the coloration of G. cancriformis because both naturally bright morphs and yellow-painted individuals failed to capture more prey than either naturally cryptic morphs or black-painted individuals (Gawryszewski and Motta 2012). Nonetheless, it remains possible that each color morph attracts preferentially specific types of prey. Although evidence is still needed, Edmunds and Edmunds (1983) suggested that the conspicuous body coloration of Gasteracantha spiders might serve as a warning signal to predators. Therefore, G. cancriformis color morphs may be an adaptation to warn predators that perceive color differently.
In this study, we aimed to explore old and new hypotheses that could potentially explain the maintenance of color polymorphisms. We investigated 3 hypotheses for the evolution and maintenance of color polymorphism, using G. cancriformis as a model. Two derivations from the prey attraction hypothesis include (1) the multiple prey hypothesis, which posits that each color morph is adapted to lure a specific type of prey; and (2) the flower mimic hypothesis, which posits that the spiders attract prey via aggressive mimicry of flower colors, and that each color morph mimics a different flower color. In the first hypothesis, different prey would exert different degrees of anti-apostatic and apostatic selection depending on the degree of attractiveness for each type of prey. The second hypothesis is a proposed mechanism to explain why spider colors would be attractive to prey. In addition, (3) the multiple predator hypothesis posits that the conspicuous colors found in spiders could serve as warning signals to predators and that color polymorphism could evolve and be maintained if each color morph is adapted to the vision of a specific predator. Comparable to the multiple prey hypothesis, predators could exert apostatic and anti-apostatic selection on color morphs depending on the degree of conspicuousness and palatability for each predator type (see Endler 1988 model).
Materials and Methods
Color vision model
Color perception depends on both the signal reflectance and the visual system of the observer, as well as the background reflectance spectrum and ambient light intensity (Endler 1990). Physiological models of color vision include all these factors and have been effective for objectively studying animal coloration (i.e., independent of human subjective assessment; Renoult et al. 2015).
To estimate the perception of G. cancriformis color morphs by distinct predators and prey groups, we used the color vision model proposed by Chittka (1992). Although this model has been only validated with behavioral experiments on bees, its general form allows us to apply it to other taxa (e.g., Théry and Casas 2002; Kemp et al. 2015). There are other models of color vision (Vorobyev and Osorio 1998; Endler and Mielke 2005), but when applied correctly, their results tend to be highly correlated (Gawryszewski 2018). In addition, receptor noise limited models (Vorobyev and Osorio 1998) are sensitive to accurate measurements of noise at photoreceptors (Lind and Kelber 2009; Olsson et al. 2017), which, for insects, is available only to the honeybee (see Olsson et al. 2017 for a recent review).
The Chittka (1992) model requires 4 inputs: (1) the irradiance reaching the observed object, (2) the observer photoreceptor excitation curves, (3) the background reflectance to which photoreceptors are adapted to, and (4) the reflectance curve of the observed object. First, the sensitivity factor R was determined for each photoreceptor as follows:
(1) |
where IB(λ) is the spectral reflectance function of the background, S(λ) is the spectral sensitivity function of each photoreceptor, and D(λ) is the illuminant irradiance spectrum. Secondly, the quantum flux P (relative amount of photon catch) is calculated as follows:
(2) |
where IS(λ) is the spectral reflectance function of the stimulus. Assuming that the maximum excitation of a photoreceptor is 1, the phototransduction process is determined as follows:
(3) |
Stimulus spectra are projected into specific color spaces. The coordinates of each spectrum are calculated using photoreceptor excitations, as follows (Chittka et al. 1994):
(4) |
(5) |
and for tetrachromat organisms (Théry and Casas 2002), as follows:
(6) |
(7) |
(8) |
Chromatic contrast between a color stimulus and background, or between 2 color stimuli, is calculated as the Euclidean distance (ΔS) between 2 points in color space, as follows:
(9) |
where Xi (i = 1, 2, 3,…, n) represents the coordinate in the color space.
The color spaces are chromaticity diagrams and, thus, do not estimate the achromatic contrast between objects. Nonetheless, achromatic contrasts can be critical visual cues used by both prey and predators. In bees, achromatic contrast is more important than chromatic cues for objects that subtend a visual angle smaller than ∼15 degrees, which means that bees must be close to flowers to use their color vision for discrimination tasks (Giurfa et al. 1997). Similarly, birds use achromatic contrast in the detection of small objects (Osorio et al. 1999). We estimated the achromatic contrast as the excitation (Equation (3)) of the photoreceptor responsible for achromatic discrimination in each organism (Chittka and Kevan 2005).
For our modeling, we used the reflectance data of G. cancriformis color morphs that were collected during a previous study (for reflectance curves, see Figure 1.8 in Gawryszewski 2007, and Figure 5 in Gawryszewski and Motta 2012). These data have already been used to estimate the visual contrast of the yellow, white, and black and white morphs from the perspective of Apis mellifera (Gawryszewski and Motta 2012). The spiders were collected from a Brazilian savanna physiognomy, namely Cerrado sensu stricto, which is characterized by shrubs and trees of 3–8 m tall that are contorted and possess thick, fire-resistant bark, a crown cover of >30%, and additional herbaceous vegetation (Oliveira-Filho and Ratter 2002). The background reflectance was estimated from the average reflectance of leaves, leaf litter, bark, and grasses that were collected from the same area as the spiders (see Figure 5 in Gawryszewski and Motta 2012). To avoid issues with negative values, we adjusted the reflectance data by subtracting the minimum value of each measurement from the reflectance values. For the illuminant spectrum, we used the International Commission on Illumination (CIE) standard illuminant of D65, which is comparable to open areas, such as the Brazilian savanna (Chittka 1996). Visual modeling calculations were conducted using the R package “colourvision” (Gawryszewski 2018).
Figure 1.
Chromatic (left) and achromatic (right) contrasts of 4 Gasteracantha cancriformis morphs (black and white, N = 6; white, N = 8; yellow, N = 12; and red, N = 3) when viewed against a Brazilian savanna background by prey with distinct visual systems: Apis mellifera (Hymenoptera), Drosophila melanogaster (Diptera), and Fabriciana adippe (Lepidoptera). Dotted vertical lines represent the discrimination thresholds for chromatic contrast (0.11) and photoreceptor excitation for the background in achromatic contrast (0.5). Upper bars are the excitation values of photoreceptors to each spider morph. Sets of 3 bars in A. mellifera indicate ultraviolet, blue, and green photoreceptors, respectively. Sets of 4 bars in D. melanogaster and F. adippe represents, respectively, ultraviolet, blue, green and red photoreceptors. The photoreceptors excitation for the background (0.5) is shown by the dotted lines.
Multiple prey hypothesis
Using the model described above, we estimated the chromatic and achromatic conspicuousness of the G. cancriformis morphs (yellow, white, red, and the white patches of the black and white morph) to a suite of potential prey: A. mellifera (Hymenoptera, Apidae), Drosophila melanogaster (Diptera, Drosophilidae), and Fabriciana adippe (Lepidoptera, Nymphalidae). Those species are not necessarily sympatric with G. cancriformis, but orb-webs commonly intercept those orders in field experiments (Craig and Ebert 1994; Tso et al. 2002), and represent the diversity of visual systems among insects (Briscoe and Chittka 2001). The variation in maximum sensitivity is small in Hymenoptera; most species present 3 spectral curves (Briscoe and Chittka 2001). In Diptera, the number of photoreceptors is not so conserved among species (Briscoe and Chittka 2001; Lunau 2014). In Lepidoptera, the red receptor appears to have evolved several times independently (Briscoe and Chittka 2001). Furthermore, there are species with more than 4 photoreceptor spectral curves and with task-specific spectral curves (Briscoe and Chittka 2001). We decided to model Fabriciana adippe as a tetrachromat because circumstantial evidence and behavioral experiments support the tetrachromat color vision for at least some butterfly species (e.g., Koshitaka et al. 2008; Arikawa 2017). Blackiston et al. (2011), for instance, also consider a nymphalid butterfly as tetrachromat on color vision modeling.
For A. mellifera and D. melanogaster, we used photoreceptor sensitivity curves from the literature (Peitsch et al. 1992; Schnaitmann and Garbers 2013; Supplementary Figure S1). The graphical curves were extracted directly from the figures of relevant publications using DataThief III version 1.7 (Tummers 2006). For F. adippe, electrophysiological measurements of photoreceptor sensitivity peaks (λmax; Eguchi et al. 1982) were used to estimate the 4 photoreceptor curves (for details see Govardovskii et al. 2000), considering a beta-band (Supplementary Figure S1).
For achromatic contrast, bees only use the green photoreceptor (Giurfa et al. 1996), whereas flies only use the outer photoreceptors (R1-R6; Kelber and Henze 2013). For Lepidoptera, we assumed that they employ the green photoreceptor, considering that green receptors seem to be involved in motion perception in Papilionidae (Horridge et al. 1984; Takemura et al. 2005; Takemura and Arikawa 2006). The color hexagon model assumes that photoreceptors respond to half their maximum for the background they are adapted to, so that the photoreceptor excitation for the background is equivalent to 0.5 units (Chittka 1992).
The multiple prey hypothesis predicts that different prey taxa perceive color morphs differently. To assess whether each spider morph was perceived differently by prey species, we conducted 2 linear mixed models, 1 for chromatic contrast and 1 for achromatic contrast. Either the chromatic (ΔS) or achromatic contrast was used as the dependent variable, and spider morph and prey taxon were used as the independent variables (contrast = spider morph observer). The spider morph was defined as yellow, white, red, or black and white, and the observers were defined as Hymenoptera, Diptera, or Lepidoptera. Individual spiders were used as random effects.
As a reference point, we used a color discrimination threshold of ΔS = 0.11, which is the threshold value below which trained bees are unable to distinguish different flower colors (Chittka 1996). However, discrimination thresholds are variable, and can change depending on the study species, learning conditions, previous experience, background coloration, whether the task involves discrimination between colors or detection against the background, and whether objects are compared sequentially or simultaneously (Avarguès-Weber and Giurfa 2014). It should also be noted that threshold values were not behaviorally validated for other taxa.
Mimic model hypothesis
To test the multiple mimic models hypothesis, we compared how prey perceive flowers and spider morphs. We gathered all flower reflectance data available in the Floral Reflectance Database (FReD; Arnold et al. 2010), excluding reflectance data from lower flower parts, leaves, bracts, stamens, the inner parts of bell-shaped flowers, and unknown items, as well as spectra that did not cover 300–700 nm. Most species in the database only have 1 reflectance spectrum. For species with multiple reflectance spectra, we randomly selected a single spectrum. We did not average the reflectance of these species because there was no information available on whether these measurements referred to different individuals or different parts of single flowers. In total, we gathered reflectance data from 858 plant species. We grouped flowers visually according to the nine categories proposed by Chittka et al. (1994; see spectral curves in Supplementary Figure S2), which considers whether they reflect or absorb in 4 spectral ranges, UV (300–400 nm), blue (400–500 nm), green (500–600 nm), and red (600–700 nm). We deleted 3 spectral curves that did not seem to fit in any of these categories. Flowers classified as category 10 (N = 16) were not included in the analyses. A caveat of this analysis is that these flowers are not necessarily sympatric to G. cancriformis. However, the spectral curve variations in flowers are subtle, because there is a constraint on the blending of flower pigments (Chittka and Menzel 1992; Chittka et al. 1994). In addition, we computed reflectance curves from different countries available in the FReD database. A qualitative analysis strongly suggests that they all have similar shapes independent of the country of origin (Supplementary Figure S2).
The multiple mimic model hypothesis predicts that different color morphs mimic different flower colors. First, to evaluate the color regardless of the observer, we compared the hue (Equation (10)), saturation (Equation (11)), and brightness (Equation (12)) of flowers and spiders (Andersson and Prager 2006 and references therein):
(10) |
where is the wavelength at the middle point between the minimum and maximum reflectances;
(11) |
where Rmax and Rmin are the maximum and minimum reflectance points; and , where Ri is the reflectance corresponding to each wavelength point, and nw is the total wavelength intervals;
(12) |
where Rmax is the maximum reflectance. In addition, the average color parameters of morphs in relation to individual flowers were illustrated in a histogram, where flowers collected in the Brazilian savanna (N = 7; same biome where the spiders were collected; see below) were treated individually.
Secondly, we computed the chromatic contrast (Euclidean distance) for spider morphs and flowers following the same steps as for the previous section, considering the visual systems of A. mellifera, D. melanogaster, and F. adippe. Then, we calculated a matrix of chromaticity distances between the average color loci of each spider color morph and each flower species and estimated the percentage of values below or equal to the theoretical detection threshold of 0.11. To evaluate if prey species perceived spider morph and flower similarly, we conducted a Wilcoxon test comparing each set of combinations between morph and flower category with the discrimination threshold of 0.11 (Student’s t-test is conventionally used to compare the color contrast to a particular threshold, but our data were not normally distributed).
We computed the achromatic contrast by subtracting each value of photoreceptor excitation of flowers from values of excitation of morphs and finding the absolute value. Here, we also conducted a Wilcoxon test to assess which achromatic contrasts were lower than 0.11. We used the same threshold of chromatic discrimination because threshold values for achromatic discrimination are not available for those prey groups. For chromatic and achromatic analyses, we used a significance level of P < 0.01 in addition to the P < 0.05 convention to account for the increased risk of a type I error due to the multiple comparisons.
Multiple predator hypothesis
The methodology used to investigate the multiple predator hypothesis was similar to that used for the multiple prey hypothesis, except that we used predator species in our models. As predators, we considered the bird Cyanistes caeruleus (Paridae) and the wasp Philanthus triangulum (Sphecidae) because birds and wasps are the primary predators of orb-web spiders (Rayor 1996; Toft and Rees 1998; Blackledge et al. 2003; Gonzaga and Vasconcellos-Neto 2005; Gunnarsson 2007), they are visually guided hunters, and have distinct color vision systems. For C. caeruleus, we used photoreceptor sensitivity curves available in the literature (Hart 2001; Supplementary Figure S1), and for P. triangulum, we used photoreceptor sensitivity peaks to estimate photoreceptor sensitivity curves with a beta-band in the ultraviolet (data available in Briscoe and Chittka 2001; see Govardovskii et al. 2000 for an estimation of sensitivity curves from sensitivity peaks; Supplementary Figure S1). Again, those species are not sympatric with G. cancriformis, but we do not expect a significant variation in photoreceptors types within hymenopterans (Peitsch et al. 1992) nor Passeriformes (Hart 2001).
The multiple predator hypothesis predicts that different predator taxa perceive color morphs differently. To assess this prediction, we established 2 linear mixed models, 1 for chromatic contrast and 1 for achromatic contrast for each predator species. Either chromatic (ΔS) or achromatic contrast was used as the dependent variable, and spider morph and predator taxon were used as the independent variables (contrast = spider morph observer). The spider morph was defined as yellow, white, red, or black and white. Individual spiders were used as random effects.
As in the multiple prey hypothesis, we used discrimination thresholds as reference points. For the chromatic contrast, we considered color discrimination thresholds of ΔS = 0.11 and ΔS = 0.06 for the wasp (Dyer and Chittka 2004) and bird (Théry et al. 2005), respectively. For the achromatic contrast, we considered double cones in birds (Hart 2001), and assumed green photoreceptors for wasps, as in bees, and compared values obtained to an excitation of 0.5.
For all analyses, linear mixed models were performed using the “nlme” (Pinheiro et al. 2016) or “lme4” packages (Bates et al. 2015). Normality and homogeneity were verified by visual inspection of quantile–quantile and residuals vs. fitted values plots. The variance was heteroscedastic; therefore, we included the variable “morph” into the variance structure. We computed all nested models and used the Akaike information criterion (AIC) to select the best model. Marginal and conditional R2 were estimated using the package “piecewiseSEM” (Nakagawa and Schielzeth 2013). Reflectance graphs were plotted using the “pavo” package (Maia et al. 2013; R Core Team 2015).
Results
Multiple prey hypothesis
For chromatic contrast, the model that included interaction between both variables, spider morph and prey taxon, presented the lowest AIC value (Table 1). The yellow morph presented the highest ΔS value for A. mellifera and F. adippe vision, whereas the white spider presented the highest ΔS value for D. melanogaster, followed by the yellow morph (Figure 1). The white patch of the black and white spiders presented a ΔS value that was close to the theoretical discrimination threshold for all prey species (Figure 1). The red spiders presented ΔS values near the theoretical discrimination threshold for A. mellifera and D. melanogaster, but not for F. adippe (Figure 1).
Table 1.
ΔAIC and determination coefficients of linear mixed models of the chromatic and achromatic contrasts of prey and predators
df | ΔAIC | Marginal R² | Conditional R² | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Multiple prey hypothesis | ||||
Chromatic dimension | ||||
ΔS ∼ morph * observer | 17 | 0 | 0.735 | 0.799 |
ΔS ∼ morph + observer | 11 | 55.811 | 0.772 | 0.823 |
ΔS ∼ morph | 9 | 90.834 | 0.511 | 0.532 |
ΔS ∼ observer | 5 | 116.544 | 0.319 | 0.844 |
ΔS ∼ 1 | 6 | 135.038 | 0 | 0.599 |
Achromatic dimension | ||||
Excitation ∼ morph * observer | 17 | 0 | 0.813 | 0.990 |
Excitation ∼ morph | 9 | 120.658 | 0.609 | 0.700 |
Excitation ∼ morph + observer | 11 | 145.370 | 0.801 | 0.956 |
Excitation ∼ 1 | 6 | 168.803 | 0 | 0.771 |
Excitation ∼ observer | 8 | 190.843 | 0.050 | 0.961 |
Multiple predators hypothesis | ||||
Chromatic dimension | ||||
ΔS ∼ morph * observer | 13 | 0 | 0.824 | 0.952 |
ΔS ∼ morph + observer | 10 | 23.489 | 0.814 | 0.922 |
ΔS ∼ observer | 7 | 57.926 | 0.306 | 0.932 |
ΔS ∼ morph | 9 | 76.386 | 0.402 | 0.402 |
ΔS ∼ 1 | 6 | 111.434 | 0 | 0.494 |
Achromatic dimension | ||||
Excitation ∼ morph * observer | 14 | 0 | 0.655 | 0.847 |
Excitation ∼ observer | 7 | 52.660 | 0 | 0.034 |
Excitation ∼ morph | 9 | 60.637 | 0.331 | 0.413 |
Excitation ∼ morph + observer | 10 | 65.573 | 0.359 | 0.437 |
Excitation ∼ 1 | 6 | 76.770 | 0 | 0.034 |
For prey achromatic contrast, the model that considered the interaction between both variables presented the lowest AIC value (Table 1). For A. mellifera and F. adippe, the white morph had the highest excitation value, followed by the black and white, yellow, and red morphs, respectively (Figure 1). In D. melanogaster vision, the yellow morph had the highest achromatic discrimination, followed by white, black and white, and red (Figure 1). The model coefficients are provided in the supplementary material (Supplementary Tables S1 and S2).
Mimic model hypothesis
We found 3 peaks of hue for the flowers, around 400, 500, and 600 nm, which are similar to the average hue of spider morphs (Figure 2A). The saturation metric had only 1 peak for flowers, to which black and white, white, and yellow spider morphs were close (Figure 2B). The brightness of flowers also only presented a single peak. White, red and yellow spider morphs had an average brightness around this peak (Figure 2C).
Figure 2.
Frequency of color properties of flowers (N = 858): (A) hue, (B) saturation, (C) brightness. Average values of each Gasteracantha cancriformis morph are represented by solid colored lines: black and white morph (black line; N = 6), white morph (gray line, N = 8), yellow morph (yellow line, N = 12), and red morph (red line, N = 3). Flowers from the Brazilian savanna (N = 7) are represented by dotted lines.
For all 3 prey species, only the white patch of the black and white morph had a high percentage of values near the chromatic theoretical discrimination threshold of 0.11 when compared with all flower reflectance spectra: 43.86% for A. mellifera, 29.12% for D. melanogaster, and 35.65% for F. adippe. For the other spider morphs, only a small proportion of the Euclidean distances between flowers and morphs presented values <0.11. For A. mellifera, only 1.47% of yellow morphs, 8.13% of white morphs, and 4.88% of red morphs presented values lower than 0.11. For D. melanogaster only 1.29% of yellow morphs, 4.52% of white spiders, and 7.62% of red morphs had values lower than 0.11. For F. adippe, these values were 0.40%, 6.98%, and 0.31%, respectively.
For A. mellifera, only the black and white morph compared with the flower categories “3” (human white), “4” (human reddish blue or purple), and “8” (human white) had values significantly lower than 0.11 (Figure 3, Supplementary Table S3). For D. melanogaster, values lower than 0.11 only included the comparison between black and white morphs and category “8” of flowers (Figure 3, Supplementary Table S3). For F. adippe, only categories “4” and “8” compared with black and white morphs were lower than 0.11 (Figure 3, Supplementary Table S3). All other comparisons between spider and flowers colorations were higher than 0.11 (Figure 3, Supplementary Table S3). For achromatic contrast, almost one-third of the comparisons between flower categories and spider morphs had excitation values between 0 and 0.11 (Figure 3, Supplementary Table S3).
Figure 3.
Correlation plot of chromatic (left) and achromatic (right) contrasts of 4 Gasteracantha cancriformis morphs (black and white, N = 6; white, N = 8; yellow, N = 12; and red, N = 3) when compared with 9 flower categories (Chittka et al. 1994) indicated by numbers. Low values are represented by smaller and lighter circles. These categories are based on reflectance curve shapes and can be translated into human colors as follows: type 1 (N = 93), yellow; type 2 (N = 96), yellow; type 3 (N = 240), white; type 4 (N = 186), reddish blue, purple or pink; type 5 (N = 107), violet; type 6 (N = 19), red; type 7 (N = 34), red; type 8 (N = 12), white; and type 9 (N = 52), pale yellow. These values were computed considering the Brazilian savanna as the background and were based on 3 potential prey: Apis mellifera (Hymenoptera), Drosophila melanogaster (Diptera), and Fabriciana adippe (Lepidoptera). One asterisks represent values significantly lower than the discrimination thresholds for chromatic contrast (0.11) at the P < 0.05 level and 2 asterisks at the P < 0.01 level, according to Wilcoxon tests.
Multiple predator hypothesis
For the chromatic contrast, the model with interaction between variables presented the lowest AIC value (Table 1). The black and white morph presented the lowest ΔS value for both predators (Figure 4; Supplementary Table S4). The white morph was the one with the highest ΔS value for C. caeruleus, though yellow and red morphs presented similar values (Figure 4). For P. triangulum, the white spider morph presented the highest ΔS value, followed by the yellow and red morphs. The latter was near the theoretical discrimination threshold of 0.11 (Figure 4).
Figure 4.
Chromatic (left) and achromatic (right) contrasts of 4 Gasteracantha cancriformis morphs (black and white, N = 6; white, N = 8; yellow, N = 12; and red, N = 3) when viewed against a Brazilian savanna background by predators with distinct visual systems: Cyanistes caeruleus (Passeriformes), and Philanthus triangulum (Hymenoptera). Dotted vertical lines represent the discrimination thresholds for chromatic contrast (0.06) and photoreceptor excitation for background in achromatic contrast (0.5). Upper bars are the excitation values of photoreceptors to each spider morph. For C. caeruleus, sets of 4 bars represent ultraviolet, blue, green, and red photoreceptors, respectively. For P. triangulum, sets of 3 bars indicate ultraviolet, blue, and green photoreceptors, respectively. The photoreceptor excitation for background (0.5) is represented by the dotted lines.
For the achromatic contrast, the model that included the interaction between variables presented the lowest AIC value (Table 1), even though the values of the 2 predator species were similar. For C. caeruleus, the white morph had the highest excitation value, followed by the yellow, black and white, and red morphs (Figure 4). The yellow morph had the highest excitation value for P. triangulum, followed by white, black and white, and red morphs (Figure 4). The model coefficients are provided in the supplementary material (Supplementary Tables S4 and S5).
Discussion
Most studies focus on a single signal receiver; however, we could better understand signal evolution considering multiple receivers (Endler and Mappes 2004; Schaefer et al. 2004). The multiple receiver hypothesis has been evaluated in intersexual and intrasexual relationships (Guindre-Parker et al. 2012), signaler interaction with prey and predators (Endler 1983), and interaction with pollinators and herbivores (Irwin et al. 2003). Here, we provide an exploratory perspective on how multiple receivers may influence color polymorphism evolution in orb-web spiders.
Multiple prey hypothesis
In G. cancriformis, spider morph conspicuousness is likely to be perceived differently by prey species, which provides support for the multiple prey hypothesis. The yellow and white morphs are the most contrasting for all 3 species, but chromatic data suggested that the former is more contrasting from the background for honeybee color vision, and the latter for flies. This scenario may lead to the evolution of polymorphism in the presence of anti-apostatic selection only (Endler 1988). Besides, the red morph showed a marked difference between prey observers. The inspection of photoreceptor values shows that for A. mellifera and D. melanogaster, the red morph does not present much chromatic information (i.e., low photoreceptor outputs and small differences between photoreceptor outputs; Figure 4), but does present chromatic information for F. adippe because of the long-wavelength photoreceptor sensitivity curve (Figure 4). This also means that the ΔS value for A. mellifera and D. melanogaster red morphs (Figure 4) should be considered carefully because the Chittka (1992) model may generate ΔS values higher than expected when all photoreceptor excitation values are low (i.e., a reflectance spectrum perceived as a black or dark gray; Vorobyev et al. 1999; Gawryszewski 2018). Translating into human color terms, our results suggest that A. mellifera and D. melanogaster would probably perceive the red morph as gray (achromatic), but F. adippe would perceive it as colorful.
Other studies evaluated the prey perceptual differences as a possible route to the evolution of color polymorphisms in orb-web spiders. Their results suggested that different morphs may exploit different visual channels in prey species (White and Kemp 2016; Ajuria-Ibarra et al. 2017). In Verrucosa arenata (Araneidae) and G. fornicata (Araneidae), the yellow morph had a higher chromatic contrast than the white morph, whereas in the achromatic dimension the white morph presented a higher contrast for prey taxa (White and Kemp 2016; Ajuria-Ibarra et al. 2017). Therefore, the yellow morphs would benefit from stimulating the chromatic channel, whereas white morphs would benefit from stimulating the achromatic channel. Similarly, our data suggest the maximum chromatic contrast for the yellow morph, but maximum achromatic contrast for the white morph from the perspectives of D. melanogaster and F. adippe (Figure 1).
Two caveats arise from analyses using color vision models, including our study. First, the relationship between supra-threshold values and behavioral discrimination tasks is not yet completely elucidated, but is likely to be nonlinear and varied between species (Garcia et al. 2017). Second, showing that one morph is more conspicuous than another does not necessarily translate into prey preference, because this preference may arise from other factors, such as previous experience (Dyer 2012). Furthermore, the color vision of butterflies is variable, with some species with task specific spectral channels (Briscoe and Chittka 2001). Therefore, experimental studies are needed to corroborate predictions arising from the color vision model data.
Mimic model hypothesis
Our results provided mixed support for the multiple model hypothesis. Considering only color metrics, most G. cancriformis morphs indeed have similar colorations to flowers (Figure 2). However, when we modeled color perception to potential prey, only the black and white morphs were similar to the flower categories (white flowers, white flowers with UV reflectance, and purple flowers). For the achromatic dimension, black and white morphs and white morphs had similar contrasts from 2 to 5 flower categories, whereas yellow morphs had similar contrasts to 3 flower categories for the honeybee, and 4 flower categories for the butterfly. In the Brazilian savanna, G. cancriformis is abundant during the beginning of the dry season, which overlaps partially with the flowering period of woody plants (Oliveira 2008; Gouveia and Felfili 1998), and pollinator occurrence (Oliveira 2008; Gouveia and Felfili 1998). Therefore, flower coloration mimicry may indeed be an advantageous foraging strategy for spiders that are abundant during this period of the year. Despite that, a field experiment conducted with G. cancriformis showed that a bright coloration did not affect the foraging success of spiders (Gawryszewski and Motta 2012). It remains to be tested whether morphs attract specific types of prey, especially flower visitors.
Similar to our results, in G. fornicata the white morphs seem to be indistinguishable from sympatric flowers according to the results of bee color vision modeling, but yellow morphs and flowers were perceptually different (Maia and White 2017). Conversely, a study on various orb-web spider species that also used color vision models found that, as perceived by dipterans and hymenopterans, the colors of spiders are similar to those of flowers (White et al. 2016). Nonetheless, correlation does not mean causation. The similarity found between spider and flower color in White et al. (2016), and partially in our data, may only reflect the diversity of flower and spider colors.
Multiple predator hypothesis
Our results indicated partial support to the multiple predator hypothesis. The statistical results suggested an interaction between predator taxa and chromatic contrast. The red morph presented a much higher chromatic contrast to a bird than to a wasp. Similar to the prey data, an inspection of photoreceptor excitation values suggest that the red morph is perceived as colorless by a wasp, but colorful to a bird (Figure 4). Nonetheless, the white and yellow morphs are highly detectable by birds and wasps in the chromatic and achromatic dimensions for both birds and wasps (Figure 4). Therefore, in the case of anti-apostatic selection from both wasps and birds, applying Endler’s (1988) model to our results would likely suggest the evolution of monomorphism favoring either the white or the yellow morph, despite differences found in the red morph.
Alternatively, the interaction between apostatic and anti-apostatic selection forces from the predators of spiders could lead to the evolution of polymorphism in G. cancriformis. For instance, a scenario where a Gasteracantha sp. is avoided by birds but consumed by wasps could lead to the evolution of polymorphism, because selection from birds would favor the most conspicuous and abundant morph (yellow or white), but selection from wasps would favor the least conspicuous and least abundant morph (black and white or red). In Nephila pilipes (Araneidae), for instance, the yellow patches seem to attract wasp predators (Fan et al. 2009). Nonetheless, it remains to be experimentally tested whether conspicuous colors in Gasteracantha spp. represent aposematic signals for either birds or wasps. Some circumstantial evidence supports at least that Gasteracantha spp. might be more defended than most other orb-web spiders; spiders of the genus Gasteracantha possess spines and a hard abdomen (Edmunds and Edmunds 1983), which may serve as a defense against wasps and/or birds, and the spider-hunting wasp Sceliphron laetum avoids provisioning initial instars with Gasteracantha spiders (Elgar and Jebb 1999).
Alternative hypotheses and conclusion
Variation in the perception of color by receivers may not be sufficient to explain color polymorphism in G. cancriformis. Gene flow, for instance, may act together on the maintenance of color variation (Grey and McKinnon 2007). Additionally, thermoregulation and the effect of illumination on the signaler detectability may also influence perception (Rojas et al. 2014; Rao and Mendoza-Cuenca 2016). Furthermore, color vision models do not include other mechanisms, such as cognition, color categorization, past experiences, or memory imprecision (Renoult et al. 2015), even though these factors may affect detectability and, consequently, influence the survival rate of morphs differently (Théry and Gomez, 2010). Beyond color, other visual parameters, such as the geometry, shape, contour, size, angle, texture, and distance of visual detection (Troscianko et al. 2009) may influence the behavior of both prey and predators toward spiders (Théry and Gomez 2010). Additionally, nonadaptive explanations, such as overdominance and allele equilibrium, in the absence of selection, are often ignored when studying polymorphisms from an ecological perspective.
In conclusion, we presented a small step forward for the multiple receivers hypothesis on the evolution of color polymorphism in orb-web spiders. In short, in G. cancriformis, polymorphism may be possible even if prey exerts anti-apostatic selection only or when there is an interaction between apostatic and anti-apostatic selection from different predators. Flower mimicry is a possible explanation for the evolution of some of these spider morphs. Considering these scenarios, many combinations of complex trade-offs arising from multiple prey and predators are possible. Predation experiments, field experiments that evaluate prey taxa caught by the different spider morphs, and ecological data on the abundance and composition of prey and predator populations that occur sympatrically with G. cancriformis are paramount to validate and complement the findings of the present study.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
We thank CAPES for financial support (CAPES/PROEX), and for a scholarship awarded to N.G.X. We also thank Prof Rodrigo Willemart, Prof Fausto Nomura, and the anonymous reviewers for their comments on the article.
Funding
N.G.X. scholarship was provided by the Coordination for the Improvement of Higher Level -or Education- Personnel (CAPES). Additional financial support was provided by the Academic Excellence Program (PROEX).
Author Contributions
N.G.X. and F.M.G. contributed to the design of the study. N.G.X. wrote the article and ran the statistical analyses. F.M.G. supervised the analyses and commented on the article.
Conflict of Interest
No competing interests declared.
References
- Ajuria-Ibarra H, Tapia-McClung H, Rao D, 2017. Mapping the variation in spider body colouration from an insect perspective. Evol Ecol 31:663–681. [Google Scholar]
- Andersson S, Prager M, 2006. Quantifying colors In: Mcgraw KJ, Hill GE, editors. Bird Coloration, Vol. I Cambridge: Harvard University Press; 41–89. [Google Scholar]
- Arikawa K, 2017. The eyes and vision of butterflies. J Physiol 595:5457–5464. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arnold SEJ, Faruq S, Savolainen V, McOwan PW, Chittka L, 2010. FReD: the floral reflectance database—a web portal for analyses of flower colour. PLoS ONE 5:e14287.. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Avarguès-Weber A, Giurfa M, 2014. Cognitive components of color vision in honeybees: how conditioning variables modulate color learning and discrimination. J Comp Physiol A 200:449–461. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B, Walker S, 2015. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J Stat Softw 67:1–48. [Google Scholar]
- Blackledge TA, Coddington JA, Gillespie RG, 2003. Are three-dimensional spider webs defensive adaptations?. Ecol Lett 6:13–18. [Google Scholar]
- Blackiston D, Briscoe AD, Weiss MR, 2011. Color vision and learning in the monarch butterfly Danaus plexippus (Nymphalidae). J Exp Biol 214:509–520. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bond AB, 2007. The evolution of color polymorphism: crypticity, searching images, and apostatic selection. Annu Rev Ecol Evol Syst 38:489–514. [Google Scholar]
- Briscoe AD, Chittka L, 2001. The evolution of color vision in insects. Annu Rev Entomol 46:471–510. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chiao C-C, Wu W-Y, Chen S-H, Yang E-C, 2009. Visualization of the spatial and spectral signals of orb-weaving spiders Nephila pilipes through the eyes of a honeybee. J Exp Biol 212:2269–2278. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chittka L, 1992. The colour hexagon: a chromaticity diagram based on photoreceptor excitations as a generalized representation of colour opponency. J Comp Physiol A 170:533–543. [Google Scholar]
- Chittka L, 1996. Optimal sets of color receptors and color opponent systems for coding of natural objects in insect vision. J Theor Biol 181:179–196. [Google Scholar]
- Chittka L, Menzel R, 1992. The evolutionary adaptation of flower colours and the insect pollinators' colour vision. J Comp Physiol A 171:171–181. [Google Scholar]
- Chittka L, Kevan PG, 2005. Flower colour as advertisement. In: Dafni A, Kevan PG, Husband BC, editors. Practical pollination biology. Cambridge, Canada: Enviroquest Ltd.157–196. [Google Scholar]
- Chittka L, Shmida A, Troje N, Menzel R, 1994. Ultraviolet as a component of flower reflections, and the colour perception of hymenoptera. Vision Res 34:1489–1508. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clarke BC, 1979. The evolution of genetic diversity. Proc R Soc Lond 205:453–474. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Craig CL, Ebert K, 1994. Colour and pattern in predator-prey interactions: the bright body colours and patterns of a tropical orb-spinning spider attract flower-seeking prey. Funct Ecol 8:616–620. [Google Scholar]
- Dyer AG, 2012. The mysterious cognitive abilities of bees: why models of visual processing need to consider experience and individual differences in animal performance. J Exp Biol 215:387–395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dyer AG, Chittka L, 2004. Fine colour discrimination requires differential conditioning in bumblebees. Naturwissenschaften 91:224–227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Edmunds J, Edmunds M, 1983. The defensive mechanisms of orb weavers (Araneae: Araneidae) in Ghana, West Africa. Proceedings of 9th International of Congress Arachnol, Panama. 73–89.
- Eguchi E, Watanabe K, Hariyama T, Yamamoto K, 1982. A comparison of electrophysiologically determined spectral responses in 35 species of Lepidoptera. J Insect Physiol 28:675–682. [Google Scholar]
- Elgar MA, Jebb M, 1999. Nest provisioning in the mud-dauber wasp Sceliphron laetum (F. Smith): body mass and taxa specific prey selection. Behaviour 136:147–159. [Google Scholar]
- Endler JA, 1983. Natural and sexual selection on color patterns in poeciliid fishes. Environ Biol Fish 9:173–190. [Google Scholar]
- Endler JA, 1988. Frequency-dependent predation, crypsis and aposematic coloration [and Discussion]. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B 319:505–523. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Endler JA, 1990. On the measurement and classification of colour in studies of animal colour patterns. Biol J Linn Soc 41:315–352. [Google Scholar]
- Endler JA, 1992. Signals, signal conditions, and the direction of evolution. Am Nat 139:S125–S153. [Google Scholar]
- Endler JA, 1993. Some general comments on the evolution and design of animal communication systems. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 340:215–225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Endler JA, Mappes J, 2004. Predator mixes and the conspicuousness of aposematic signals. Am Nat 163:532–547. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Endler JA, Mielke JRPW, 2005. Comparing entire colour patterns as birds see them. Biol J Linnean Soc 86:405–431. [Google Scholar]
- Fan CM, Yang EC, Tso IM, 2009. Hunting efficiency and predation risk shapes the color-associated foraging traits of a predator. Behav Ecol 20:808–816. [Google Scholar]
- Farkas TE, Mononen T, Comeault AA, Hanski I, Nosil P, 2013. Evolution of camouflage drives rapid ecological change in an insect community. Curr Biol 23:1835–1843. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garcia JE, Spaethe J, Dyer AG, 2017. The path to colour discrimination is S-shaped: behaviour determines the interpretation of colour models. J Comp Physiol A 203:983–997. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gawryszewski FM, 2007. Policromatismo e stabilimentum em Gasteracantha cancriformis (Araneae, Araneidae): caracterização e as hipóteses da atração de presas e da proteção da teia. [Masters Thesis] Universidade de Brasília.
- Gawryszewski FM, 2018. Color vision models: some simulations, a general n‐dimensional model, and the colourvision R package. Ecol Evol doi: 10.1002/ece3.4288. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gawryszewski FM, Motta PC, 2012. Colouration of the orb–web spider Gasteracantha cancriformis does not increase its foraging success. Ethol Ecol Evol 24:23–38. [Google Scholar]
- Giurfa M, Vorobyev M, Kevan P, Menzel R, 1996. Detection of coloured stimuli by honeybees: minimum visual angles and receptor specific contrasts. J Comp Physiol A 178:699–709. [Google Scholar]
- Giurfa M, Vorobyev M, Brandt R, Posner B, Menzel R, 1997. Discrimination of coloured stimuli by honeybees: alternative use of achromatic and chromatic signals. J Comp Physiol A 180:235–243. [Google Scholar]
- Gonzaga MO, Vasconcellos-Neto J, 2005. Testing the functions of detritus stabilimenta in webs of Cyclosa fililineata and Cyclosa morretes (Araneae: Araneidae): do they attract prey or reduce the risk of predation? Ethol 111:479–491. [Google Scholar]
- Gouveia GP, Felfili JM, 1998. Fenologia de comunidades de cerrado e de mata de galeria no Brasil Central. Rev Árvore 22:443–450. [Google Scholar]
- Govardovskii VI, Fyhrquist N, Reuter T, Kuzmin DG, Donner K, 2000. In search of the visual pigment template. Vis Neurosci 17:509–528. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grey SM, McKinnon JS, 2007. Linking color polymorphism maintenance and speciation. Trends Ecol Evol 22:71–79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guindre-Parker S, Gilchrist HG, Baldo S, Doucet SM, Love OP, 2012. Multiple achromatic plumage ornaments signal to multiple receivers. Behav Ecol 24:672–682. [Google Scholar]
- Gunnarsson B, 2007. Bird predation on spiders: ecological mechanisms and evolutionary consequences. J Arachnol 35:509–529. [Google Scholar]
- Hart NS, 2001. The visual ecology of avian photoreceptors. Prog Retin Eye Res 20:675–703. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hauber ME, 2002. Conspicuous colouration attracts prey to a stationary predator. Ecol Entomol 27:686–691. [Google Scholar]
- Horridge GA, Marcelja L, Jahnke R, 1984. Colour vision in butterflies. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 155:529–542. [Google Scholar]
- Irwin RE, Strauss SY, Storz S, Emerson A, Guibert G, 2003. The role of herbivores in the maintenance of a flower color polymorphism in wild radish. Ecology 84:1733–1743. [Google Scholar]
- Kelber A, Henze MJ, 2013. Colour vision: parallel pathways intersect in Drosophila. Curr Biol 23:R1043–R1045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kemp DJ, Herberstein ME, Fleishman LJ, Endler JA, Bennett AT. et al. 2015. An integrative framework for the appraisal of coloration in nature. Am Nat 185:705–724. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koshitaka H, Kinoshita M, Vorobyev M, Arikawa K, 2008. Tetrachromacy in a butterfly that has eight varieties of spectral receptors. Proc Royal Soc Lond B: Biol Sci 275:947–954. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levi HW, 1978. The American orb-weaver genera Colphepeira, Micrathena and Gasteracantha North of Mexico (Araneae, Araneidae). Bull Mus Comp Zool 148:417–442. [Google Scholar]
- Lind O, Kelber A, 2009. Avian colour vision: effects of variation in receptor sensitivity and noise data on model predictions as compared to behavioural results. Vis Res 49:1939–1947. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lunau K, 2014. Visual ecology of flies with particular reference to colour vision and colour preferences. J Comp Physiol A 200:497–512. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maia R, White T, 2017. Comparing colours using visual models. bioRxiv 175992. [Google Scholar]
- Maia R, Eliason CM, Bitton PP, Doucet SM, Shawkey. 2013. Pavo: an R package for the analysis, visualization and organization of spectral data. Methods Ecol Evol 4:609–613. [Google Scholar]
- Nakagawa S, Schielzeth H, 2013. A general and simple method for obtaining R2 from generalized linear mixed-effects models. Methods Ecol Evol 4:133–142. [Google Scholar]
- Oliveira PEAM, 2008. Fenologia e biologia reprodutiva de espécies do Cerrado. Cerrado: ecologia e Flora 273–290. [Google Scholar]
- Oliveira-Filho AT, Ratter JA, 2002. Vegetation physiognomies and woody flora of the Cerrado biome. In: Oliveira PS, Marquis RJ, editors. The Cerrados of Brazil: Ecology and Natural History of a Neotropical Savanna New York: Columbia University Press, 91–120. [Google Scholar]
- Olsson P, Lind O, Kelber A, 2017. Chromatic and achromatic vision: parameter choice and limitations for reliable model predictions. Behav Ecol 29:287. [Google Scholar]
- Osorio D, Miklósi A, Gonda Z, 1999. Visual ecology and perception of coloration patterns by domestic chicks. Evol Ecol 13:673–689. [Google Scholar]
- Peitsch D, Fietz A, Hertel H, de Souza J, Ventura DF. et al. 1992. The spectral input systems of hymenopteran insects and their receptor-based colour vision. J Comp Physiol A 170:23–40., [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pinheiro J, Bates D, DebRoy S, Sarkar D, R Core Team 2016. R package ‘nlme’: Linear and Nonlinear Mixed Effects Models (version 3.1–128). http://CRAN.R–project.org/package=nlme>.
- R Core Team, 2015. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. https://www.R–project.org/.
- Rao D, Mendoza-Cuenca L, 2016. The effect of colour polymorphism on thermoregulation in an orb web spider. Sci Nat 103:1–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rayor L, 1996. Attack strategies of predatory wasps (Hymenoptera: Pompilidae; Sphecidae) on colonial orb web-building spiders (Araneidae: Metepeira Incrassata). J Kans Entomol Soc 69:67–75. [Google Scholar]
- Renoult JP, Kelber A, Schaefer HM, 2015. Colour spaces in ecology and evolutionary biology. Biol Rev 92:292–315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rojas B, Rautiala P, Mappes J, 2014. Differential detectability of polymorphic warning signals under varying light environments. Behav Process 109:164–172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruxton GD, Sherratt TN, Speed MP, 2004. Avoiding Attack. 1st edn. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Ryan MJ, 1990. Sexual selection, sensory systems and sensory exploitation. Oxf Sur Evol Biol 7:157–195. [Google Scholar]
- Schaefer HM, Schaefer V, Levey DJ, 2004. How plant-–animal interactions signal new insights in communication. Trends Ecol Evol 19:577–584. [Google Scholar]
- Schnaitmann C, Garbers C, 2013. Article color discrimination with broadband photoreceptors. Curr Biol 23:2375–2382. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takemura SY, Kinoshita M, Arikawa K, 2005. Photoreceptor projection reveals heterogeneity of lamina cartridges in the visual system of the Japanese yellow swallowtail butterfly Papilio xuthus. J Comp Neurol 483:341–350. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takemura SY, Arikawa K, 2006. Ommatidial type-specific interphotoreceptor connections in the lamina of the swallowtail butterfly, Papilio xuthus. J Comp Neurol 494:663–672. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tiedemann KB, Ventura DF, Ades C, 1986. Spectral sensitivities of the eyes of the orb web spider Argiope argentata (Fabricius). J Arachnol 14:71–78. [Google Scholar]
- Tinbergen L, 1960. The natural control of insects in pine woods I. Factors influencing the intensity of predation by songbirds. Arch Néerl Zool 13:265–336. [Google Scholar]
- Théry M, Casas J, 2002. Predator and prey views of spider camouflage. Nature 415:133–133.. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Théry M, Gomez D, 2010. Insect colours and visual appearance in the eyes of their predators. Adv Insect Physiol 38:267–353. [Google Scholar]
- Théry M, Debut M, Gomez D, Casas J, 2005. Specific color sensitivities of prey and predator explain camouflage in different visual systems. Behav Ecol 16:25–29. [Google Scholar]
- Toft RJ, Rees JS, 1998. Reducing predation of orb-web spiders by controlling common wasps Vespula vulgaris in a New Zealand beech forest. Ecol Entomol 23:90–95. [Google Scholar]
- Troscianko T, Benton CP, Lovell PG, Tolhurst DJ, Pizlo Z, 2009. Camouflage and visual perception. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 364:449–461. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tso I, Tai P, Ku T, Kuo C, Yang E, 2002. Colour-associated foraging success and population genetic structure in a sit-and-wait predator Nephila maculata (Araneae: Tetragnathidae). Anim Behav 63:175–182. [Google Scholar]
- Tso I, Lin C, Yang E, 2004. Colourful orb-weaving spiders Nephila pilipes through a bee’s eyes. J Exp Biol 207:2631–2637. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tummers B, 2006. DataThief III. http://datathief.org/
- Vorobyev M, Osorio D, 1998. Receptor noise as a determinant of colour thresholds. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 265:351–358. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vorobyev M, Hempel de Ibarra N, Brandt R, Giurfa M, 1999. Do “white” and “green” look the same to a bee? Naturwissenschaften 86:592–594. [Google Scholar]
- White TE, Dalrymple RL, Herberstein ME, Kemp DJ, 2016. The perceptual similarity of orb-spider prey lures and flower colours. Evol Ecol 31:1–20. [Google Scholar]
- White TE, Kemp DJ, 2015. Technicolour deceit: a sensory basis for the study of colour-based lures. Anim Behav 105:231–243. [Google Scholar]
- White TE, Kemp DJ, 2016. Colour polymorphic lures target different visual channels in prey. Evolution 70:1398–1408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yamashita S, 1985. Photoreceptor cells in the spider eye: spectral sensitivity and efferent control In: Barth FG, editor. Neurobiology of Arachnids. Springer, Berlin: Heidelberg: 103–117. [Google Scholar]
- Yamashita S, Tateda H, 1978. Spectral sensitivities of the anterior median eyes of the orb web spiders Argiope bruennichii and A. amoena. J Exp Biol 74:47–57. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.