Skip to main content
The Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education logoLink to The Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education
. 2019 May 7;24(4):435–447. doi: 10.1093/deafed/enz019

Morphological Accuracy in the Speech of Bimodal Bilingual Children with CIs

Corina Goodwin 1,, Diane Lillo-Martin 2
PMCID: PMC6786513  PMID: 31063195

Abstract

Sign language use in the (re)habilitation of children with cochlear implants (CIs) remains a controversial issue. Concerns that signing impedes spoken language development are based on research comparing children exposed to spoken and signed language (bilinguals) to children exposed only to speech (monolinguals), although abundant research demonstrates that bilinguals and monolinguals differ in language development. We control for bilingualism effects by comparing bimodal bilingual (signing-speaking) children with CIs (BB-CI) to those with typical hearing (BB-TH). Each child had at least one Deaf parent and was exposed to ASL from birth. The BB-THs were exposed to English from birth by hearing family members, while the BB-CIs began English exposure after cochlear implantation around 22-months-of-age. Elicited speech samples were analyzed for accuracy of English grammatical morpheme production. Although there was a trend toward lower overall accuracy in the BB-CIs, this seemed driven by increased omission of the plural -s, suggesting an exaggerated role of perceptual salience in this group. Errors of commission were rare in both groups. Because both groups were bimodal bilinguals, trends toward group differences were likely caused by delayed exposure to spoken language or hearing through a CI, rather than sign language exposure.


Although cochlear implants (CIs) greatly improve the potential to develop spoken language skills in children who are deaf, a wide variability in outcomes has been observed (e.g., Niparko et al., 2010; Peterson, Pisoni, & Miyamoto, 2010). Individual characteristics, such as female sex, high IQ, and absence of additional disabilities, have all been associated with better spoken language outcomes in children with CIs (e.g., Boons et al., 2012; Carter, Dillon, & Pisoni, 2002; Dillon, Cleary, Pisoni, & Carter, 2004; Edwards & Anderson, 2014; Geers, 2002; Geers, Moog, Biedenstein, Brenner, & Hayes, 2009). Family features, such as higher parental education level, higher socio-economic status (SES), and fewer siblings are also associated with better outcomes (e.g., Geers & Sedey, 2011; Geers et al., 2009; Geers, Brenner, & Davidson, 2003; Szagun & Stumper, 2012). Audiological characteristics are integral as well, with better pre-implant hearing, later onset of deafness, earlier implantation, implantation in the right rather than left ear, and bilateral cochlear implantation or contralateral hearing aid use correlating with better spoken language outcomes (e.g., Anderson et al., 2004; Barnard et al., 2015; Boons et al., 2012; Bouchard, Ouellet, & Cohen, 2009; Caselli, Rinaldi, Varuzza, Giuliani, & Burdo, 2012; Colletti, Mandalà, & Colletti, 2012; Dettman, Pinder, Briggs, Dowell, & Leigh, 2007; Dillon, Cleary et al., 2004; Geers, Nicholas, Tobey, & Davidson, 2016; Henkin et al., 2008; Spencer, 2004). While all this research demonstrates an intense focus on the factors that affect spoken language development in children with CIs, the role of sign language in their (re)habilitation remains controversial. A recent review concluded that there is insufficient high-quality evidence to determine whether children with CIs should be exposed to a sign language (Fitzpatrick et al., 2016). One complicating factor in this debate is the diversity of manual systems that are used with children, ranging from various systems for representing English manually to American Sign Language (ASL), which is a distinct language with its own grammar (first demonstrated by Stokoe, 1960).

In this study, we assert that children exposed to two languages should be considered bilinguals, even when one language is produced in the visual/manual modality. We examined the English morphological development of children with CIs who were acquiring ASL and English, compared to children with typical hearing who were acquiring the same language pair. Both groups of children were born to Deaf1 parents who used ASL with them from birth. This comparison allowed us to investigate the effects of delayed exposure to spoken English and hearing through a CI on spoken language outcomes while controlling for the effects of simultaneous exposure to ASL. We focused on morphological development because it has been found to be delayed in children with CIs compared to monolingual hearing children (e.g., Guo, Spencer, & Tomblin, 2013; Werfel, 2018).

Effects of Sign Exposure on Language Outcomes in Children with CIs

Some researchers have found that placement in total communication educational settings (i.e., those that include some form of a signed communication system) rather than oral-only communication settings is associated with worse spoken language outcomes, especially speech perception and intelligibility (e.g., Cullington, Hodges, Butts, Dolan-Ash, & Balkany, 2000; Dillon, Pisoni, Cleary, & Carter, 2004; Geers et al., 2000, 2017; Geers, Spehar, & Sedey, 2002; Jiménez, Pino, & Herruzo, 2009; Tobey, Rekart, Buckley, & Geers, 2004). Other studies (e.g., Yoshinaga-Itano, Baca, & Sedey, 2010) have found that children with CIs exposed to both sign and spoken language can achieve age-appropriate vocabulary and grammar skills based on standardized test norms. Additionally, several studies have found benefits of exposure to sign language in total communication educational settings. The types of advantages that have been observed include earlier first words/signs, more mature early communicative functions (informative and heuristic rather than directive) and greater verbal fluency (e.g., Jiménez et al., 2009; Nicholas & Geers, 2003). Most of these advantages only become apparent when both signed and spoken communication are taken into consideration, and they tend to diminish with age.

Because forcing parents to choose one communication mode for research purposes would be unethical, there are several confounds in this line of research that are difficult to avoid. Many of these studies show trends for children in total communication settings to have mothers with lower levels of education, families with lower income levels, later ages of implantation and worse auditory perception skills (e.g., Cullington et al., 2000; Geers et al., 2000; Geers et al., 2017). All of these factors have been linked to poorer speech outcomes in the broader population of children with CIs. Because the children in total communication and oral communication educational settings often differ on these background characteristics, it is difficult to distinguish the influence of sign language exposure from these other factors. Furthermore, it is likely that early developments in spoken language ability greatly influence later educational placement, with children who show greater early proficiency in speech entering classrooms that use speech only and those struggling with spoken language going into total communication classrooms (e.g., Hall, Hall, & Caselli, 2019).

Another weakness of the existing literature is that children with CIs learning both a signed and a spoken language have often not been considered as bilinguals, despite the fact that they were acquiring two languages. Many differences in language development have been documented between monolinguals and bilinguals (see Serratrice, 2012 for a review). For instance, bilinguals with typical hearing often have smaller vocabularies than monolinguals when each of their languages are considered separately, although they tend to know a comparable (or even larger) number of words for different concepts across both languages (e.g., Pearson, Fernandez, & Oller, 1993). While predicting language outcomes in bilingual children is a complex task that depends on various input factors as well as the language domain under question, failure to consider signing children with CIs as bilinguals may set unreasonable standards for them to meet.

It is important to note that more than 90% of deaf children are born to typically hearing parents (Mitchell & Karchmer, 2004), most of whom communicate via spoken and not sign language. While the adoption of universal newborn hearing screening has greatly reduced the age of identification of hearing loss in the United States (e.g., Halpin, Smith, Widen, & Chertoff, 2010), many of these children will continue to experience some period of language deprivation before cochlear implantation if sign language use is avoided. Although the United States Food and Drug Administration requires hearing aid trials before cochlear implantation (except when there is a concern for cochlear ossification following meningitis infection), only children who receive little to no benefit from such amplification are considered appropriate CI candidates per FDA label indications (e.g., Carlson et al., 2015; Gifford, 2011). This means that many children who ultimately receive CIs cannot perceive speech sounds at a level that is sufficient for spoken language acquisition before cochlear implantation. Thus, adequate exposure to spoken language is frequently delayed until after cochlear implantation despite earlier identification of hearing loss and pre-implantation hearing aid use.

Despite the lack of consensus regarding whether sign language exposure is helpful, detrimental, or inconsequential for the acquisition of spoken language, some clinicians discourage the use of sign language with children who are deaf, even before cochlear implantation when access to spoken language is minimal (e.g., Mauldin, 2016). In addition to the studies cited above that compared bilingual children with CIs to monolingual benchmarks, concern that a visual language will cause neural reorganization of the auditory cortex for visual input may be used as justification for this recommendation (ibid). However, in the absence of auditory input, this reorganization happens whether or not sign language input is provided, and there is ample evidence of a sensitive period for language exposure regardless of modality (Lyness, Woll, Campbell, & Cardin, 2013). Additionally, results from behavioral studies have demonstrated that late first language acquisition leads to reduced proficiency levels in phonology, morphology, syntax, and semantics, even after decades of language experience (see Mayberry & Kluender, 2017 for review). Thus, when sign language exposure is avoided until a child with a CI does not achieve the desired spoken language proficiency, the consequences of this prolonged period of language deprivation can be considerable and long-lasting (see Humphries et al., 2014 for a review of the linguistic, cognitive, social and occupational hazards).

Furthermore, numerous studies have also provided evidence for facilitative effects of early first language acquisition on second-language proficiency, irrespective of language modality (Mayberry & Kluender, 2017). For example, two groups of adults with early first language exposure (i.e., deaf individuals with early ASL exposure and hearing individuals with exposure to a spoken language from birth) who learned English as a second language were found to outperform deaf individuals with later simultaneous exposure to English and ASL on English grammaticality judgment tasks (Mayberry & Lock, 2003). Evidence from the study of native bimodal (sign-speech) bilingual children with CIs also suggests that early sign language exposure can positively influence later spoken language development in this population. Children who are born deaf to at least one Deaf, signing parent represent less than 5% of the total population of children who are deaf (Mitchell & Karchmer, 2004). Of this subset, some (possibly small) proportion receive CIs. Because these children are exposed to sign language from birth, they experience no period of language deprivation, although their exposure to spoken language is delayed until after cochlear implantation. Research with this population indicates that bimodal bilingual children with CIs achieve spoken language outcomes on par with monolingual and bilingual children with typical hearing (Davidson, Lillo-Martin, & Chen Pichler, 2014) or even better than spoken language monolinguals with CIs (Hassanzadeh, 2012).

It is unclear the extent to which the sign language input provided by Deaf parents resembles the input offered to children with CIs born to hearing parents. There is some evidence that bimodal language exposure does not negatively affect spoken language development when hearing parents choose to pursue both sign and spoken language proficiency after their child’s hearing loss is identified (e.g., Rinaldi & Caselli, 2014). For Deaf parents, even if they were not exposed to signing from birth, they likely will have used this language for many years before their child is born. In contrast, when a hearing, non-signing parent decides to use sign language with their child, they must learn the language at the same time that they are providing input for their child. It is, therefore, reasonable to expect that outcomes for bimodal bilingual children born to Deaf parents would differ from those reported for children in total communication programs.

Additionally, schools that use total communication vary in the nature of the sign language used, with some using artificial, manually encoded English systems, while others use American Sign Language, a full, natural language with a grammar that differs from spoken English (e.g., Klima & Bellugi, 1979). Previous research in this area has not specified the exact type of system used in the total communication educational programs child participants attended and/or combined children with different types of sign input, and it is possible that different varieties of sign input would lead to different language outcomes.

Morphological Development in Children with CIs

Few studies have focused on the English morphological development of children with CIs, but of those that have, all have found morphological deficits. In samples of elicited speech targeting English tense morphemes, children with CIs produced these morphemes less accurately than comparison groups with typical hearing, even when groups were matched on hearing age (i.e., time since cochlear implantation) rather than chronological age (Guo & Spencer, 2017; Guo et al., 2013). Error types were similar in both groups, with morphemes more likely to be omitted than incorrectly produced (ibid). Similar deficits have been found in other languages with more complex morphology than English (e.g., Hallé & Duchesne, 2015).

Some researchers have also investigated the influence of the perceptual properties of each grammatical morpheme on its acquisition. This line of research is motivated by the Surface Account of Specific Language Impairment/Developmental Language Disorder (e.g., Leonard, 1989), which hypothesizes that morphemes which are more difficult to perceive are more likely to be omitted. For example, the English progressive morpheme -ing is syllabic (i.e., longer in duration), more perceptually salient and therefore less likely to be omitted than a morpheme like the third person present -s, which is a nonsyllabic consonant (i.e., shorter in duration) of higher pitch but lower amplitude. Even though children with CIs may show an overall different pattern of performance than children with SLI/DLD (de Hoog, Langereis, van Weerdenburg, Knoors, & Verhoeven, 2016), any difficulties with morphemes of low perceptual salience could be exacerbated in children with CIs, who might struggle to perceive sounds of short duration and low amplitude.

Thus, far results from children with CIs have been somewhat mixed, with some authors finding that perceptual salience plays a role for children with CIs (Spencer, Tye-Murray, & Tomblin, 1998; Svirsky, Stallings, Lento, Ying, & Leonard, 2002; Werfel, 2018) and others not (Ruder, 2004). However, in this population, performance on tests of speech perception are often positively correlated with later accuracy in morphological production (Guo & Spencer, 2017; Guo et al., 2013; Spencer et al., 1998), which lends additional support to the theory that perceptual salience is integral for acquisition of spoken language morphology by children with hearing loss.

Morphological Development in Bilinguals Learning Two Spoken Languages

Children with typical hearing exposed to two or more spoken languages are often less accurate in their morphological production when compared to monolinguals (e.g., Nicholls, Eadie, & Reilly, 2011; Nicoladis, Song, & Marentette, 2012). However, it is important to consider more specific properties of the language-learning environment for bilinguals because some researchers have found that bilingual children are as accurate as monolinguals if they receive at least 50% of their input in the language being assessed (Paradis, Nicoladis, Crago, & Genesee, 2011; Thordardottir, 2014). Those children who experience delayed exposure to their second language until school-age are likely to exhibit more pronounced deficits (e.g., Paradis, 2006). Additional factors, such as the typological characteristics of a bilingual’s two languages, have also been found to be important. For example, the English morphological development of children who are also exposed to a morphologically rich language, such as Spanish, has been found to outpace that of children exposed to a language with a less elaborate morphological system, such as Mandarin Chinese (ibid).

English Morphological Development in ASL-English Bilinguals

American Sign Language is similar to Mandarin in that it has few, if any obligatory grammatical morphemes parallel in function to English grammatical morphemes (Sandler & Lillo-Martin, 2006). For example, verb tense is often expressed via adverbial modification (e.g., yesterday, today, tomorrow) and only a subset of verbs exhibits person agreement. Verbal aspect can be expressed through modifications of sign movement rather than the addition of concatenative morphology. There is a class of predicative signs that is generally analyzed as having a highly complex morphological composition, the classifier signs, but these have no direct analog to English morphology. Plurality of nouns can be indicated by repetitions of sign movement, but such marking is found on only a few signs. All of these typological properties suggest that the concurrent acquisition of ASL likely does not facilitate the acquisition of spoken English morphology specifically in the way that acquiring a morphologically rich spoken language does.

Studies analyzing morphological accuracy in the spoken English of ASL-English bilinguals with typical hearing have generally found higher error rates in these bilinguals compared to similarly aged monolinguals (Johnson, Watkins, & Rice, 1992; Schiff & Ventry, 1976, but see Goodwin, Davidson, & Lillo-Martin, 2017 for a counter-example). Additionally, bimodal bilinguals differ from speech–speech bilinguals (i.e., those acquiring two spoken languages) in that both of their languages can be produced simultaneously, a phenomenon often referred to as code-blending (Emmorey, Borinstein, Thompson, & Gollan, 2008). When assessing morphological accuracy in bimodal bilinguals, it is important to consider the mode of language production because English morphology tends to be omitted more frequently in code-blended utterances (Petroj, 2017; Petroj, Guerrera, & Davidson, 2014).

Present Study

The aim of this study was to compare the accuracy of English morpheme use in ASL-English bimodal bilingual children with CIs (BB-CI) to that of similar bimodal bilinguals with typical hearing (BB-TH). We addressed three main issues:

  • Do BB-CIs produce English morphology as accurately as BB-THs? The BB-CIs may be less accurate than the BB-THs because this group experienced delayed exposure to spoken English and this input was filtered through a cochlear implant, which may not have conveyed the speech signal with fidelity.

  • Does relative perceptual salience explain the pattern of performance across different English morphemes for the BB-CIs and/or the BB-THs? The duration and loudness of morphemes might be more important for children listening through a cochlear implant than those with typical hearing; therefore, it is especially important to consider this factor for the BB-CIs.

  • Do BB-CIs tend to produce the same types of morphological errors as BB-THs? Previous research has found that errors of omission are more common than errors of commission in both children with typical hearing and children with CIs.

Method

Participants

Seven ASL-English bimodal bilinguals with typical hearing (BB-TH; M age2, = 5;09 SD = 0;05, range = 5;01–6;03) and five Deaf bimodal bilinguals with a CI (BB-CI; M age = 5;05, SD = 0;10, range = 4;01–6;05) participated in this study. The groups were balanced for gender, with four males in the BB-TH group and three in the BB-CI group (57% and 60% males, respectively). All participants had at least one Deaf, signing parent and acquired ASL from birth. The BB-THs were also exposed to English from birth by hearing family and friends, whereas the BB-CIs began acquiring English from similar sources once their implants were activated (M age = 1;10, SD = 0;08, range = 1;04–2;11). The BB-CIs also received speech and language therapy as recommended by their speech-language pathologists. Both groups of children were of high SES as measured by mother’s education, with all but two of the hearing children’s mothers possessing a bachelor’s degree or higher. Both groups of children were relatively balanced bilinguals, with home language environments predominately ASL-based and school language environments predominately English-based. Individual background characteristics are summarized in Table 1.

Table 1.

Participant background characteristics

Pseudonym Group Chronological age at testing Hearing age at testing Maternal education Language use at home (%ASL/Eng/mix) Language use at school (%ASL/Eng/mix)
Ben BB-TH 6;00 6;00 16+ 50/30/20 0/100/0
Kim BB-TH 5;02 5;02 16+ 90/0/10 0/50/0*
Lex BB-TH 5;10 5;10 16+ 50/20/30 60/0/40
Lyn BB-TH 6;03 6;03 12; 75/10/15 0/100/0
Tom BB-TH 6;00 6;00 16+ 80/10/10 0/100/0
Val BB-TH 5;02 5;02 16+
Zig BB-TH 6;01 6;01 13 50/0/50 0/100/0
Mean (SD) 5;09 (.05) 5;09 (.05)
Fin BB-CI 5;08 4;01 16 50/0/50 0/50/50
Gia BB-CI 5;07 4;00 16+ 75/5/20 0/100/0
Max BB-CI 6;05 4;09 16 65/20/15 0/100/0
Nik BB-CI 5;06 4;02 16+
Pam BB-CI 4;01 1;02 16 60/30/10 0/100/0
Mean (SD) 5;05 (.10) 3;08 (1.05)

Note: BB-TH = bimodal bilingual with typical hearing; BB-CI = bimodal bilingual with cochlear implant; Maternal Education is provided in years, with 12 = high school diploma or GED, 16 = Bachelor’s Degree, and 16+ = post baccalaureate or graduate school; Language use is expressed as percent reported use of ASL, English, or mix of sign and speech; *50% Spanish input; Language use data are unavailable for Val and Nik.

Table 2 provides additional information about the audiological characteristics of the BB-CI group. All of these children were prelingually deafened and received their cochlear implants before the age of two years, except for Pam, whose pre-aided hearing thresholds were lower, and her age of implantation was higher.

Table 2.

Audiological characteristics of bimodal bilingual participants with cochlear implants

Pseudonym Age of diagnosis Age at 1st implant Age at 2nd implant Pre-aided HL Pre-device HA use
Fin Prenatal 1;07 1;07 Profound Unknown
Gia 7 days 1;06 Unknown Profound Unknown
Max 1 month 1;08 N/A Severe-Prof 11 weeks
Nik Unknown 1;04 3;06 Unknown Unknown
Pam 9 days 2;11 N/A Mod-Prof 7 weeks

Note: HL = hearing level; HA = hearing aid; Age at 1st/2nd implant = age of activation of cochlear implant.

Although the two groups were matched for chronological age (Mann–Whitney U(10) = 13, Z = −.73, p = .53, r = −.21), the hearing age (i.e., time since cochlear implantation/length of exposure to spoken language) of the BB-CIs was significantly lower than that of the BB-THs (U(10) = 0, Z = −2.87, p = .003, r = −.83). All of the BB-CIs had been exposed to English for at least one year (M = 3;07, SD = 1;04.28, range = 1;02–4;09).

The data reported here were collected as part of a larger project investigating the language development of bimodal bilingual children. As part of this project, the children also completed a non-standardized assessment of auditory perception (Minimal Pairs: Pronovost & Dumbleton, 1953) and standardized assessments of speech (Goldman–Fristoe Test of Articulation-2: Goldman & Fristoe, 2000), expressive vocabulary (Expressive Vocabulary Test: Williams, 1997) and overall language development (Preschool Language Scales 4: Zimmerman, Steiner, & Pond, 2002). In the Minimal Pairs task, children heard two words (e.g., goat and coat) and were asked to point to the matching picture out of an array of three pairs of pictures (e.g., a coat and a goat, a coat and a coat, a goat and a goat). Each word pair in this task can differ by at most a single phoneme. The standardized assessments were administered as per test instructions. All of these tests were administered by typically hearing researchers who presented all instructions and feedback in English. The results of these and other tests are discussed in depth in Davidson et al. (2014) and Cruz et al. (2014).

Results from these tests are included in Table 3 to provide a broader picture of our participants’ overall speech and language development. Unfortunately, the youngest child with a CI (pseudonym Pam) did not receive the GFTA-2, but all other children scored within the average range on this test, suggesting that any observed difficulties with spoken English morphological production were not caused by articulatory difficulties. Pam also did not complete the Minimal Pairs task. Although the sample sizes are small, which limits our power to detect differences, a Mann–Whitney U test found no significant differences between the two groups on the Minimal Pairs Task, suggesting that the BB-CIs showed no major deficits in speech perception in the relatively quiet testing atmosphere (U(6) = 13, Z = −1.53, p = .16, r = −.46). All but one child (pseudonym Fin) also scored within one standard deviation or higher on all the grammar and vocabulary assessments. Group comparisons for the standardized speech and language tasks also show no significant differences (Preschool Language Scales 4: U(10) = 13, Z = −.73, p = .53, r = −.21; Expressive Vocabulary Test: U(10) = 15, Z = −.33, p = .76, r = −.09; Goldman–Fristoe Test of Articulation-2: U(9) = 9.5, Z = −.85, p = .41, r = −.26)

Table 3.

Speech and language test scores

Pseudonym Group PLS-4 EVT GFTA-2 Minimal pairs
Ben BB-TH 117 119 110 37
Kim BB-TH 98 114 115 28
Lex BB-TH 108 109 112 39
Lyn BB-TH 91 87 106 37
Tom BB-TH 92 104 107 34
Val BB-TH 105 96 115 37
Zig BB-TH 92 110 90 38
Mean (SD) BB-TH 100.4 (9.9) 105.6 (11) 107.9 (8.6) 35.71 (3.7)
Fin BB-CI 79 100 100 35
Gia BB-CI 104 108 112 30
Max BB-CI 95 90 102 36
Nik BB-CI 96 112 109 32
Pam BB-CI 96 110
Mean (SD) BB-CI 94 (9.1) 104 (9.1) 105.8 (5.7) 33.3 (2.8)

Note: PLS-4 = Preschool Language Scale, Fourth Edition; EVT = Expressive Vocabulary Test; GFTA-2 = Goldman–Fristoe Test of Articulation-2; Standard Scores provided for the PLS-4, EVT and GFTA-2, with a mean of 100 and standard deviation of 15; Raw Scores Provided for the Minimal Pairs Task with a maximum possible of 40.

Procedure

The data for this study came from short videos (about 5–10 min) collected at “language fairs” in which bimodal bilingual children completed a series of standardized language tests and played games designed to elicit particular types of language constructions (Quadros et al., 2015). All tests were administered by researchers with typical hearing using spoken English. Some experimenters were hearing native signers and others were second-language learners of ASL, but instructions and feedback during testing was provided in English to model for children that English was the appropriate language to use in this context.

We analyzed fifty utterances of each participants’ data to estimate how frequently code-blending (simultaneous production of ASL and English) occurred. In the BB-CI group, code-blended utterances were produced an average of 2.6 times out of 50 utterances (range = 0–7, SD = 3.21). In the BB-TH group, code-blended utterances were produced an average of 4.29 times out of 50 utterances (range = 0–17, SD = 5.94). This is consistent with previous research that has found code-blending to be infrequent in bimodal bilinguals when they are using the dominant spoken language of the broader community (Lillo-Martin, Quadros, Chen Pichler, & Fieldsteel, 2014). These code-blended utterances tended to consist of a complete, although possibly ungrammatical, English sentence with a single and often redundant ASL sign, such as a number or a verb. All clear English utterances, including code-blended utterances, were analyzed for morphological accuracy.

Children were administered the standardized tests and language games individually and in a randomized order. The present study analyzed language samples from two tasks: verbal morphology and narrative.3 In the verbal morphology task, children were shown cards with four similar pictures involving characters engaged in different activities, one of which had a yellow box drawn around it. They were asked to describe the picture outlined in yellow so that an experimenter could match the picture to the same one on their card (which did not have a yellow outline). The children had to describe the pictures by telling the experimenter what the characters were doing in each one, eliciting person/number and tense/aspect agreement with singular and plural subjects. For example, a child needed to produce a sentence such as “the cows are drinking water” to distinguish a picture with three cows drinking water from an adjacent picture with a single cow drinking water. There was one training item for which the experimenter modeled the correct sentence form for the child. These cards were developed specifically for this task. All children received all eleven items in the same order.

In the narrative task, children watched animated videos or were shown a series of pictures that depicted a story. They then had to tell the story to an experimenter who had not seen the video or the pictures. The videos were short (under two minutes), unnarrated excerpts from the French television series Minuscule—The Private Life of Insects (Giraud & Szabo, 2006). There were two different sets of pictures used for the picture narrative task. The first set was from the picture book Tuesday (Wiesner, 1991). The second set of pictures was developed specifically for this task (Quadros et al. 2015). Each child received only one version of the narrative task, with distribution of narrative tasks based on the year of data collection. Five of seven BB-TH’s received the video-based narrative task and four of five BB-CI’s received a picture-based narrative. A Spearman’s rank-order correlation was run to determine the relationship between morphological accuracy in the verbal morphology and narrative tasks and, by extension, whether there was evidence of an effect of task type. There was a statistically significant, strong positive correlation (rs(10) = .694, p = .012), demonstrating that participant performance was consist across tasks, regardless of the type of narrative task used.

Transcription and Coding

All videos were transcribed by native English-speaking undergraduate research assistants using ELAN Linguistic Annotator software (Sloetjes & Wittenburg, 2008). The first author (a native English speaker) coded all transcripts for mean length of utterance (MLU) and morphological accuracy. As part of a larger study on morphological development in bimodal bilingual children, two additional coders unaware of the study’s objectives but not to participant group (cochlear implants were visible or visibly absent in the videos used for analysis) independently coded 9% of the total number of transcripts. Cohen’s kappa for the first author and second coder was .83, indicating near perfect agreement. Cohen’s kappa for the first author and third coder was .74, indicating substantial agreement.

MLU was calculated using standard rules for English as established by Brown (1973). All utterances not excluded as interjections, imitations, repetitions, or unintelligible were analyzed for their MLU in words (MLUw) and morphemes (MLUm). MLU calculations were based on a total of 50 utterances, half drawn from each task. Two children, Kim & Lyn, produced fewer than 25 analyzable utterances (18 and 22, respectively) during the verbal morphology task; therefore, additional utterances were drawn from the narrative task in order to reach a total of 50 utterances for these children. These two children seemed to understand the verbal morphology task well and gave adequate, although not necessarily grammatically correct, descriptions of all eleven verbal morphology cards with a minimal number of utterances.

Accuracy of use in obligatory contexts of verbal morphology (e.g., 3rd person present –s, progressive -ing, past -ed), nominal morphology (i.e., regular plural –s and irregular plural) and the definite and indefinite articles the and a(n) was analyzed for all clear utterances in both tasks. Errors were coded as belonging to one of four categories: omission, commission, over-regularization, and other. Omission errors were missing the target morpheme (e.g., she run or two dog). Commission errors included morphemes that were ungrammatical in the context (e.g., I runs or one dogs). Over-regularization errors occurred when general rules were applied to exceptions (e.g., she runned or sheeps). Finally, errors categorized as “other” included any error that did not fit into a previously mentioned category, such as double past tense marking (e.g., walkeded, ranned), incorrect forms of to be verbs (e.g., I is, she am, we was, he were), contraction of uncontractible verbs (e.g., she’s running for she was running), and errors of verb choice (e.g., have monkeys for there are monkeys). This category was only relevant for verbal morphology. Both groups of children did sometimes use articles inappropriately (e.g., using definite the for indefinite a(n) or using an article in a context in which an adult would not), but coding of this depends on subtle contextual judgment and was less reliable, so these error types were excluded for all analyses. Additionally, although no children consistently used the indefinite article allomorph an before a noun that began with a vowel phoneme, we did not code these non-adult-like forms as errors. Therefore, errors of omission were the only type possible for articles.

Results

MLU

The BB-TH group had a mean MLU in words of 4.86 (SD = .54, range = 3.70–5.36) and the BB-CIs had a mean of 4.75 (SD = .67, range = 4.16–5.86). For MLU in morphemes, the BB-TH group had a mean of 5.74 (SD = .52, range = 4.62–6.22) and the BB-CIs had a mean of 5.54 (SD = .65, range = 5.04–6.66). Mann–Whitney U tests showed no significant differences between the two groups whether MLU was measured in words (U(10) = 12, Z = −.89, p = .43, r = −.26) or morphemes (U(10) = 11, Z = −1.06, p = .34, r = −.31).

Overall Accuracy

Figure 1 depicts the overall accuracy on all morphemes for both groups. Although the overall accuracy was higher for the BB-TH group (M = 91.43%, SD = 3.78%, range = 86–97%) than the BB-CI group (M = 84.2%, SD = 8.01%, range = 76–92%), this difference was not statistically significant (U(10) = 8, Z = −1.55, p = .15, r = −.45).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Overall morphological accuracy by group.

Figure 2 depicts accuracy on individual English morphemes by group. Based on Guo et al. (2013), we required a minimum of four obligatory contexts for each morpheme to be considered individually in order to decrease the effect of outliers. Only six morphemes met this minimum requirement in all children and are included in the boxplot in Figure 2 (definite article the, indefinite article a, progressive -ing, contractible copula, contractible auxiliary, and regular plural -s). The morphemes in this figure are generally arranged from most to least salient, with those that are pronounced as independent words listed first, then those that are syllabic, followed by those that are (usually) nonsyllabic on the right of the figure. While the regular plural has a syllabic allomorph (-es), obligatory contexts for this were exceedingly rare in the data.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Group accuracy by morpheme in order of perceptual salience.

Performance for each morpheme was then compared between the groups using Bonferroni adjusted alpha levels of .008 per test (.05/6). Only the regular plural morpheme approached significance (U(10) = 2.5, Z = −2.45, p = .01, r = −.71), while differences in accuracy for the definite article (U(10) = 15, Z = −.41, p = .76, r = −.12), indefinite article (U(10) = 12, Z = −.91, p = .43, r = −.26), progressive (U(10) = 17, Z = −.09, p = .93, r = −.03), contractible copula (U(10) = 14, Z = −.57, p = .64, r = −.17), and contractible auxiliary (U(10) = 14, Z = −.61 p = .64, r = −.18) did not approach statistical significance.

Table 4 provides accuracy scores for each child across all six morphemes discussed above, as well as means and standard deviations for each group. Figure 2 and Table 4 illustrate that there is a wider variation in performance in the BB-CI group.

Table 4.

Accuracy in percentages by morpheme type for individual participants

Pseudonym Status Definite article Indefinite article Progressive-ing Contractible copula Contractible auxiliary Regular plural
Ben BB-TH 94 96 100 97 100 98
Kim BB-TH 91 100 67 75 71 75
Lex BB-TH 98 94 100 97 100 83
Lyn BB-TH 100 88 89 92 100 93
Tom BB-TH 97 96 100 93 72 100
Val BB-TH 85 96 95 72 71 100
Zig BB-TH 95 90 100 91 100 91
Mean (SD) 94.3 (5) 94.3 (4.1) 93 (12.2) 88.1 (10.3) 87.7 (15.3) 91.4 (9.4)
Fin BB-CI 87 92 86 67 71 40
Gia BB-CI 100 93 100 100 100 75
Max BB-CI 93 100 100 100 100 19
Nik BB-CI 100 100 100 100 80 87
Pam BB-CI 96 97 78 59 60 31
Mean (SD) 95.2 (5.5) 96.4 (3.8) 92.8 (10.3) 59 (37.2) 82.2 (17.7) 50.4 (29.2)

Note: BB-TH = bimodal bilingual with typical hearing; BB-CI = bimodal bilingual with cochlear implant.

Perceptual Salience Hypothesis

Results from the six individual morphemes appearing in a minimum of four obligatory contexts were also used to address the prediction that perceptual salience would influence development, by comparing performance across morphemes within each group. A Friedman test showed that, for the BB-TH group, there was no significant difference in accuracy rate across morphemes (χ2(5) = 3.40, p = .64). For the BB-CIs, there was a significant difference in accuracy rates across morphemes (χ2(5) = 13.31, p = .02). Post hoc analysis with Wilcoxon signed-rank tests was conducted. In order to limit the number of comparisons, only three comparisons were made with one morpheme from each of the perceptual categories of independent word (the), syllabic (progressive -ing), and nonsyllabic (regular plural -s). The contractible copula was considered a category between syllabic and nonsyllabic, because it can also appear as an independent word when uncontracted. A Bonferroni adjusted alpha level of .017 was used per test (.05/3). Given this stringent alpha level and the small sample size, none of the comparisons reached statistical significance. The results were as follows for the comparisons between (1) definite article & progressive, Z = .54, p = .59, r = .17, (2) progressive & contractible copula, Z = 1.41, p = .16, r = .45, and (3) progressive & regular plural, Z = 2.02, p = .04, r = .64.

Age and Morphological Accuracy

A single sample was formed by combining the two groups of bimodal bilinguals and a Spearman’s rank-order correlation was conducted to determine the relationship between morphological accuracy and chronological age. There was not a statistically significant correlation between chronological age and overall accuracy (rs(10) = .217, p = .25). A second Spearman’s correlation was conducted to determine the relationship between hearing age and morphological accuracy. This correlation approached significance (rs(10) = .49, p = .052).

Although the mean chronological ages of our two groups of participants was not significantly different, there was one participant in the BB-CI group who is quite young (i.e., Pam at age 4;01). Our small sample sizes provide limited power to detect differences between the two groups and thus insufficient power to consider age as a covariate in our analysis. Nevertheless, if Pam is removed from the analysis, the two groups still differ significantly on hearing age (U(9) = 0, Z = −2.68, p = .006, r = −.82), but not chronological age (U(9) = 13, Z = −.19, p = .93, r = −.06). Furthermore, all comparisons of our language measures remain statistically not significant with similar effect sizes (e.g., for overall accuracy, U(9) = 8, Z = −1.14, p = .315, r = −.27 and for regular plural -s with an adjusted p-value of .008, U(9) = 2.5, Z = −2.18, p = .02, r = −.66).

Error Type Frequencies

Tables 5 and 6 show the raw numbers of errors and the percentages out of total errors broken down into the morpheme types of verbal and plural and separated by group (BB-TH and BB-CI). Data from articles the/a(n) are not displayed because only errors of omission were possible given our coding scheme. In both groups and for both morpheme types, errors of commission were quite rare. The BB-CIs omitted plural morphemes more frequently than BB-THs, who over-regularized plural about as often as they omitted plural morphemes. The raw number of errors also demonstrated that there were many more errors of plural omission within the BB-CI group.

Table 5.

Number (percent) of verbal errors produced

Omission (%) Commission (%) Over-regularization (%) Other (%)
BB-TH 61 (58) 5 (5) 12 (11) 28 (26)
BB-CI 63 (59) 3 (3) 3 (3) 38 (36)

Note: BB-TH = bimodal bilingual with typical hearing; BB-CI = bimodal bilingual with cochlear implant.

Table 6.

Number (percent) of plural errors produced

Omission (%) Comission (%) Over-regularization (%)
BB-TH 11 (52) 1 (5) 9 (43)
BB-CI 47 (85) 6 (11) 2 (4)

Note: BB-TH = bimodal bilingual with typical hearing; BB-CI = bimodal bilingual with cochlear implant.

Discussion

This study investigated the development of English morphology in bimodal bilingual children with CIs (BB-CI) and did not find that they were less accurate than bimodal bilingual children with typical hearing (BB-TH) at a statistically significant level. We focused on morphology here because previous research has found it to be particularly difficult for children with CIs (e.g., Guo et al., 2013; Werfel, 2018).

The BB-CIs did not differ significantly on MLU in words or in morphemes, despite the fact that they had had access to spoken English input for a much shorter period of time than the BB-THs. This could indicate that the two groups were at a similar stage of syntactic development at the time of testing. The difference in overall morphemic accuracy between the two groups only approached significance, and this trend seemed to be driven by a lower accuracy rate with regular plurals in the BB-CI group. These results could mean that verbal morphology is a relative strength for these BB-CIs, while plurals are a weakness. This conclusion is not yet warranted though, because our elicitation method failed to obtain large numbers of regular past tense and 3rd person singular present morphemes, which may be particularly difficult for children with CIs based on their low perceptual salience and/or syntactic and semantic complexity (cf, Werfel, 2018). Future research with these populations should include tasks that have been shown to reliably elicit all morphemes of interest, including regular past tense and 3rd person present tense morphemes. As Figures 1 and 2 illustrate, there was also wider variation in accuracy levels in the BB-CI group, an observation that has also been made about children with cochlear implants more generally (e.g., Niparko et al., 2010). Additionally, Spearman’s correlations demonstrated that our participants’ overall morphological accuracy was more closely related to hearing age (i.e., time since cochlear implantation/length of exposure to spoken English) than chronological age.

The relatively lower performance by the BB-CI participants on plurals could be attributable to their low perceptual salience. For children with typical hearing, numerous studies of monolingual development have found that it is one of the earliest acquired grammatical morphemes, with many children mastering it by 36 months of age or younger (Brown, 1973; de Villiers & de Villiers, 1985; Lahey, Liebergott, Chesnick, Menyuk, & Adams, 1992). Even children with Specific Language Impairment/Developmental Language Disorder also acquire plurals and other nominal morphology earlier than tense/aspect morphology (e.g., Paradis, 2010). If the relatively lower perceptual salience of plurals affects very young children with typical hearing, other factors, such as higher input frequency or lower semantic complexity seem to outweigh this given the early age of acquisition. On the other hand, monolingual children with hearing loss do show evidence of difficulty with plurals (e.g., Werfel, 2018). Overall, then, it is likely that the lower performance of the BB-CI participants on plurals, as for monolingual children with hearing loss, is due to the morphemes’ lower perceptual salience rather than the children’s bilingual status.

As has been found in research with monolinguals and speech–speech bilinguals, omission errors occur much more frequently than errors of commission. There was also a tendency for the BB-TH group to over-regularize irregular verbs and nouns more than the BB-CIs, but future research using a task specifically eliciting inflections of irregular verbs and nouns would be necessary to determine if true group differences exist. Counter-intuitively, more over-regularization errors could be indicative of more sophisticated morphological understanding. For example, Nicholls et al. (2011) found that bilinguals were more accurate with irregular noun plural than monolinguals while also being simultaneously less accurate inflecting regular noun plurals. This was presumably because the monolinguals had mastered the regular English plural morpheme and begun overapplying it to exceptions, while the bilinguals had memorized individual irregular forms, but had not yet mastered the regular plural morpheme. In this study, such a discrepancy in knowledge is plausible given that the BB-TH group had received more spoken input than the BB-CIs, who had a significantly lower hearing age. Yet both the regular past tense and plural morphemes are also of low perceptual salience, so it is difficult to disentangle the effects of delayed exposure from the possibility that these morphemes were not well-represented through the CI.

One limitation in this study is the small sample sizes, which reflects the fact that it remains relatively uncommon for culturally Deaf parents to choose cochlear implants for their children with hearing impairments (e.g., Mitchiner & Sass-Lehrer, 2011). Unfortunately, this fact also means that our study is underpowered and only a quite large effect size (i.e., r ≥ .73 or Cohen’s d ≥ 2.13) would lead to statistically significant differences. To provide some context for this, Werfel (2018) found that children with hearing loss (using hearing aids and/or CIs) were less accurate than age-matched and language-matched peers in the production of various English morphemes, with effect sizes ranging from r = .33–.58 (originally reported as Cohen’s d = .69–1.43). One of our results, overall accuracy across all morphemes, fell within this range (r = .45, d = 1.00) and another result, accuracy for regular plural, had an even larger effect size (r = .71, d = 1.99). This suggests that we may have found statistically significant differences in these comparisons with larger groups or if our stimuli had elicited more types of morphemes that other researchers have found to be especially difficult for children with CIs (e.g., 3rd person singular present or regular past tense, as in Werfel, 2018). On the other hand, all other comparisons had much smaller effect sizes (r = .03–.31, d = .05–.53), except for group differences in mean length of utterance in morphemes, which had an effect size of r = .31 (d = .64). This is consistent with the fact that the BB-CIs tended to omit plural morphemes more frequently than the BB-THs, leading to a smaller MLUm.

Another limiting factor is that both groups of children in this study were of relatively high SES. While this might be representative of the children with CIs who are born to Deaf parents, such a sample does not accurately reflect the larger population of children with CIs (Chang, Ko, Murray, Arnold, & Megerian, 2010). This difference is noteworthy because higher SES has been consistently linked to better language outcomes in both children with typical hearing (e.g., Rowe, 2008) and children with CIs (e.g., Geers et al., 2003). Regardless, our groups were well-matched for SES, meaning that any differences between the two groups were unlikely to be related to this factor.

While it is unlikely that transfer from ASL in the domain of morphology facilitates acquisition of these morphemes in English, it is possible that other aspects of the linguistic environment of bimodal bilingual children with CIs could enhance their ability to detect English morphological elements in their input. Future comparisons with well-matched monolingual English-speaking children with CIs could help determine whether early ASL exposure is beneficial for later spoken English morphological development in this population. Furthermore, as noted earlier, it is unclear to what extent the sign input these children received from their Deaf parents would resemble that produced by hearing parents of children with CIs. The nature of the sign input provided by Deaf and hearing parents remains to be studied.

Our results suggest that large amounts of fluent ASL input from birth do not impede subsequent spoken language development. While there was some evidence that our group of bimodal bilingual children with CIs had difficulties with the regular plural morpheme, this result is consistent with studies of monolingual children with CIs (e.g., Werfel, 2018). Our participants with CIs also demonstrated relative strengths on the same morphemes that Werfel’s (2018) monolingual children with hearing loss produced with high accuracy (e.g., present progressive -ing and articles the/a). These similarities between monolingual and bimodal bilingual children with hearing loss suggests that hearing acuity and the perceptual salience of English morphemes are critical factors for both groups. In separate research, we are investigating the possibility that some aspects of morphological accuracy might be affected by bilingual transfer from ASL. If so, this is expected to affect both groups of bimodal bilingual participants relatively equally. Future studies should incorporate comparison groups of monolinguals with typical hearing and with CIs to directly test whether ASL-English bimodal bilinguals exhibit any bilingualism effects in their spoken language, as has been found in speech–speech bilinguals.

In summary, we found little evidence that the spoken English of 5-year-old Deaf children with exposure to a natural sign language from birth differed significantly from that of their bimodal bilingual peers with typical hearing despite significantly delayed exposure to spoken English. As in monolingual children with CIs, the perceptual salience of specific morphemes seemed to drive trends toward group differences in our two groups of bimodal bilinguals. Finally, we stress the importance of comparing bilingual children to other bilinguals rather than to monolinguals when assessing overall language development in order to control for bilingualism effects. We recognize that the population we studied is quite special, since the participants experienced no delay or disturbance in the quality of linguistic input from birth. Nevertheless, this population is important for determining the potential outcomes for bimodal bilingual children with CIs and optimal input conditions. Further study with this population may help to reveal any limitations under these conditions (such as the lower accuracy we observed with plurals), as well as specific aspects of the linguistic environment that could facilitate English morphological development and which might be applicable to a wider range of children.

Acknowledgments

We are very grateful to the participants in our research and their families, without whose longstanding patience this research could not take place. We also thank the research assistants who contributed to this project through data collection, transcription of speech and sign, and coding.

Footnotes

1

“Deaf” is capitalized here to refer to individuals who are culturally and linguistically deaf and utilize sign language as their main form of communication.

2

Ages are provided in the form y;mm, with y = year and m = month.

3

Materials from both tasks can be retrieved from https://slla.lab.uconn.edu/instruments/.

Funding

Research reported in this publication was supported in part by the National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders of the National Institutes of Health under award number R01DC009263 to the second author. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. Support was also provided by awards from the Gallaudet Research Institute to Deborah Chen Pichler.

Conflict of interest

No conflicts of interest were reported.

References

  1. Anderson I., Weichbold V., D’Haese P. S., Szuchnik J., Quevedo M. S., Martin J., …Phillips L. (2004). Cochlear implantation in children under the age of two – What do the outcomes show us? International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolarygology, 68, 425–431. 10.1016/j.ijporl.2003.11.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Barnard J. M., Fisher L. M., Johnson K. C., Eisenberg L. S., Wang N., Quittner A. L., …Niparko J. K., & The CDaCI Investigative Team (2015). A prospective longitudinal study of U.S. children unable to achieve open-set speech recognition 5 years after cochlear implantation. Otology & Neurotology, 36, 985–992. 10.1097/MAO.0000000000000723. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Boons T., Brokx J. P. L., Dhooge I., Frijns J. H. M., Peeraer L., Vermeulen A., …van Wieringen A. (2012). Predictors of spoken language development following pediatric cochlear implantation. Ear & Hearing, 33, 627–639. 10.1097/AUD.0b013e3182503e47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bouchard M., Ouellet C., & Cohen H. (2009). Speech development in prelingually deaf children with cochlear implants. Language and Linguistics Compass, 3(1), 1–18. 10.1111/j.1749-818X.2008.00079.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  5. Brown R. (1973). A first language: The early stages. London: George Allen & Unwin. [Google Scholar]
  6. Carlson M. L., Sladen D. P., Haynes D. S., Driscoll C. L., DeJong M. D., Erickson H. C., …Gifford R. H. (2015). Evidence for the expansion of pediatric cochlear implant candidacy. Otology & Neurotology, 36, 43–50. 10.1097/MAO.0000000000000607. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Carter A. K., Dillon C. M., & Pisoni D. P. (2002). Imitation of nonwords by hearing impaired children with cochlear implants: Suprasegmental analyses. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 16, 619–638. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Caselli M. C., Rinaldi P., Varuzza C., Giuliani A., & Burdo S. (2012). Cochlear implant in the second year of life: Lexical and grammatical outcomes. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 55, 382–394. 10.1044/1092-4388(2011/10-0248). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Chang D. T., Ko A. B., Murray G. S., Arnold J. E., & Megerian C. A. (2010). Lack of financial barriers to pediatric cochlear implantation: Impact of socioeconomic status on access and outcomes. Archives of Otolaryngology – Head and Neck Surgery, 136, 648–657. 10.1001/archoto.2010.90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Colletti L., Mandalà M., & Colletti V. (2012). Cochlear implants in children younger than 6 months. Otolaryngology – Head and Neck Surgery, 147, 139–146. 10.1177/0194599812441572. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Cruz, C. R., Kozak, L. V., Pizzio, A. L., de Quadros, R. M. & Chen Pichler, D. (2014). Phonological Memory and Phonological Acquisition in BImodal BIlingual Children. Proceedings of the Boston University Conference on Language Development (BUCLD) 38. Boston, MA. [Google Scholar]
  12. Cullington H., Hodges A. V., Butts S. L., Dolan-Ash S., & Balkany T. J. (2000). Comparison of language ability in children with cochlear implants placed in oral and total communication educational settings. The Annals of Otology, Rhinology, & Laryngology, 185, 121–123. 10.1177/0003489400109S1253. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Davidson K., Lillo-Martin D., & Chen Pichler D. (2014). Spoken English language development in native signing children with cochlear implants. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 19, 238–250. 10.1093/deafed/ent045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. de Hoog B. E., Langereis M. C., van Weerdenburg M., Knoors H. E. T., & Verhoeven L. (2016). Linguistic profiles of children with CI as compared with children with hearing or specific language impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 51, 518–530. 10.1111/1460-6984.12228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. de Villiers J. G., & de Villiers P. A. (1985). The acquisition of English In Slobin D. I. (Ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition. Volume 1: The data (pp. 27–139). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [Google Scholar]
  16. Dettman S. J., Pinder D., Briggs R. J. S., Dowell R. C., & Leigh J. R. (2007). Communication development in children who receive the cochlear implant younger than 12 months: risks versus benefits. Ear & Hearing, 28, 11S–18S. 10.1097/AUD.0b013e31803153f8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Dillon C. M., Cleary M., Pisoni D. B., & Carter A. K. (2004). Imitation of nonwords by hearing-impaired children with cochlear implants: segmental analyses. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 18, 39–55. 10.1080/0269920031000151669. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Dillon C., Pisoni D. B., Cleary M., & Carter A. K. (2004). Nonword imitation by children with cochlear implants: Consonant analyses. Archives of Otolaryngology - Head Neck Surgery, 130, 587–591. 10.1001/archotol.130.5.587. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Edwards L., & Anderson S. (2014). The association between visual, nonverbal cognitive abilities and speech, phonological processing, vocabulary and reading outcomes in children with cochlear implants. Ear & Hearing, 35, 366–374. 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Emmorey K., Borinstein H. B., Thompson R., & Gollan T. H. (2008). Bimodal bilingualism. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11, 43–61. 10.1017/S1366728907003203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Fitzpatrick E. M., Hamel C., Stevens A., Pratt M., Moher D., Doucet S. P., …Na E. (2016). Sign language and spoken language for children with hearing loss: A systematic review. Pediatrics, 137(1), 1–17. 10.1542/peds.2015-1974. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Geers A. E. (2002). Factors affecting the development of speech, language, and literacy in children with early cochlear implantation. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 33, 172–183. 10.1044/0161-1461(2002/015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Geers A. E., Nicholas J., Tye-Murray N., Uchanski R., Brenner C., Davidson L. S., …Tobey E. A. (2000). Effects of communication mode on skills of long-term cochlear implant users. Annals of Otology, Rhinology & Laryngology, 109, 89–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Geers A. E., Spehar B., & Sedey A. (2002). Use of speech by children from total communication programs who wear cochlear implants. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 11, 50–58. 10.1044/1058-0360(2002/006). [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  25. Geers A., Brenner C., & Davidson L. (2003). Factors associated with development of speech perception skills in children implanted by age five. Ear & Hearing, 24, 24S–35S. 10.1097/01.AUD.0000051687.99218.0F. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Geers A. E., Moog J. S., Biedenstein J., Brenner C., & Hayes H. (2009). Spoken language scores of children using cochlear implants compared to hearing age-mates at school entry. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 14, 371–385. 10.1093/deafed/enn046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Geers A. E., & Sedey A. L. (2011). Language and verbal reasoning skills in adolescents with 10 or more years of cochlear implant experience. Ear & Hearing, 32, 39S–48S. 10.1097/AUD.0b013e3181fa41dc. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Geers A. E., Nicholas J., Tobey E., & Davidson L. (2016). Persistent language delay versus late language emergence in children with early cochlear implantation. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 59, 155–170. 10.1044/2015_JSLHR-H-14-0173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Geers A. E., Mitchell C. M., Warner-Czyz A., Wang N., & Eisenberg L. S., & the CDaCI Investigative Team (2017). Early sign language exposure and cochlear implantation benefits. Pediatrics, 140(1), 1–9. 10.1542/peds.2016-3489. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Gifford R. H. (2011). Who is a cochlear implant candidate? The Hearing Journal, 64, 16–22. [Google Scholar]
  31. Giraud H., & Szabo T. (2006). Minuscule – la vie privée des insectes [Motion Picture]. France.
  32. Goldman R., & Fristoe M. (2000). Goldman–Fristoe Test of Articulation 2 (GFTA-2). San Antonio, TX: Pearson. [Google Scholar]
  33. Goodwin C., Davidson K., & Lillo-Martin D. (2017). English article use in bimodal bilingual children with cochlear implants: Effects of language transfer and early language exposure In LaMendola M., & Scott J. (Eds.), Proceedings of the Boston University Conference on Language Development 41 (pp. 283–295). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press. [Google Scholar]
  34. Guo L., Spencer L. J., & Tomblin B. (2013). Acquisition of tense marking in English-speaking children with cochlear implants: A longitudinal study. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 18, 187–205. 10.1093/deafed/ens069. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Guo L., & Spencer L. J. (2017). Development of grammatical accuracy in English-speaking children with cochlear implants: A longitudinal study. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 60, 1062–1075. 10.1044/2016_JSLHR-H-16-0182. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Hall M. L., Hall W. C., & Caselli N. K. (published online, March 13, 2019). Deaf children need language, not (just) speech. First Language. 10.1177/0142723719834102. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  37. Hallé F., & Duchesne L. (2015). Morphosyntactic skills in deaf children with cochlear implants: A systematic review. Canadian Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology, 39, 260–297. [Google Scholar]
  38. Halpin K. S., Smith K. Y., Widen J. E., & Chertoff M. E. (2010). Effects of universal newborn hearing screening on an early intervention program for children with hearing loss, birth to 3 yr of age. Journal of the American Academy of Audiology, 21, 169–175. 10.3766/jaaa.21.3.5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Hassanzadeh S. (2012). Outcomes of cochlear implantation in deaf children of deaf parents: Comparative study. The Journal of Laryngology & Otology, 126, 989–994. 10.1017/S0022215112001909. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Henkin Y., Taitelbaum-Swead R., Hildesheimer M., Mirigov L., Kronenberg J., & Kishon- Rabin L. (2008). Is there a right cochlear implant advantage? Otology & Neurotology, 29, 489–494. 10.1097/MAO.0b013e31816fd6e5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Humphries T., Kushalnagar P., Mathur G., Napoli D. J., Padden C., Rathman C., & Smith S. (2014). Bilingualism: A pearl to overcome certain perils of cochlear implants. Journal of Medical Speech-Language Pathology, 21, 107–125. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Jiménez M. S., Pino M. J., & Herruzo J. (2009). A comparative study of speech development between deaf children with cochlear implants who have been educated with spoken or spoken+sign language. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 73, 109–111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Johnson J. M., Watkins R. V., & Rice M. L. (1992). Bimodal bilingual language development in a hearing child of deaf parents. Applied Psycholinguistics, 13, 31–52. 10.1017/S0142716400005415. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  44. Klima E., & Bellugi U. (1979). The signs of language. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
  45. Lahey M., Liebergott L., Chesnick M., Menyuk P., & Adams J. (1992). Variability in children’s use of grammatical morphemes. Applied Psycholinguistics, 13, 373–398. 10.1017/S0142716400005683. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  46. Leonard L. B. (1989). Language learnability and specific language impairment in children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 10, 179–202. 10.1017/S0142716400008511. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  47. Lillo-Martin D., de Quadros R. M., Chen Pichler D., & Fieldsteel Z. (2014). Language choice in bimodal bilingual development. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1–15. 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Lyness C. R., Woll B., Campbell R., & Cardin V. (2013). How does visual language affect crossmodal plasticity and cochlear implant success? Neuroscience and Behavioral Reviews, 37, 2621–2630. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.08.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Mauldin L. (2016). Made to hear: Cochlear implants and raising deaf children. Minneapolis, MI: University of Minnesota Press. [Google Scholar]
  50. Mayberry R. I., & Lock E. (2003). Age constraints on first versus second language acquisition: Evidence for linguistic plasticity and epigenesis. Brain and Language, 87, 369–384. 10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00137-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Mayberry R. I., & Kluender R. (2017). Rethinking the critical period for language: New insights into an old question from American Sign Language. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21, 886–905. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Mitchell R., & Karchmer M. A. (2004). Chasing the mythical ten percent: Parental hearing status of deaf and hard of hearing students in the United States. Sign Language Studies, 4, 138–163. 10.1353/sls.2004.0005. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  53. Mitchiner J., & Sass-Lehrer M. (2011). My child can have more choices: Reflections of Deaf mothers on cochlear implants for their children In Paludneviciene R., & Leigh I. W. (Eds.), Cochlear Implants: Evolving Perspectives (pp. 71–94). Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press. [Google Scholar]
  54. Nicholas J. G., & Geers A. E. (2003). Hearing status, language modality, and young children’s communicative and linguistic behavior. Journal of Deaf Studies and Education, 8, 422–437. 10.1093/deafed/eng029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Nicholls R. J., Eadie P. A., & Reilly S. (2011). Monolingual versus multilingual acquisition of English morphology: What can we expect at age 3? International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 46, 449–463. 10.1111/j.1460-6984.2011.00006.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Nicoladis E., Song J., & Marentette P. (2012). Do young bilinguals acquire past tense morphology like monolinguals only later? Evidence from French–English and Chinese–English Bilinguals. Applied Psycholinguistics, 33, 457–479. 10.1017/S0142716411000439. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  57. Niparko J. K., Tobey E. A., Thal D. J., Eisenberg L. S., Wang N., Quittner A. L., & Fink N. E. (2010). Spoken language development in children following cochlear implantation. The Journal of the American Medical Association, 303, 1498–1506. 10.1001/jama.2010.451. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Paradis J. (2006). Grammatical morphology in children learning English as a second language: Implications of similarities with specific language impairment. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 36, 172–187. 10.1044/0161-1461(2005/019). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Paradis J. (2010). The interface between bilingual development and specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 31, 227–252. 10.1017/S0142716409990373. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  60. Paradis J., Nicoladis E., Crago M., & Genesee F. (2011). Bilingual children’s acquisition of the past tense: A usage-based approach. Journal of Child Language, 38, 554–578. 10.1017/S0305000910000218. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Pearson B. Z., Fernandez S. C., & Oller D. K. (1993). Lexical development in bilingual infants and toddlers: Comparison to monolingual norms. Language Learning, 43, 93–120. 10.1111/j.1467-1770.1993.tb00174.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  62. Peterson N. R., Pisoni D. B., & Miyamoto R. T. (2010). Cochlear implants and spoken language processing abilities: Review and assessment of the literature. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 28, 237–250. 10.3233/RNN-2010-0535. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Petroj V. (2017). Article distribution in English/American Sign Language (ASL) whispered code-blended speech In LaMendola M., & Scott J. (Eds.), Proceedings of the Boston University Conference on Language Development 41 (pp. 532–545). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press. [Google Scholar]
  64. Petroj V., Guerrera K., & Davidson K. (2014). ASL dominant code-blending in the whispering of bimodal bilingual children In Orman W., & Valleau M. J. (Eds.), Proceedings of the Boston University Conference on Language Development 38 (pp. 319–330). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press. [Google Scholar]
  65. Pronovost W., & Dumbleton C. (1953). A picture-type speech sound discrimination test. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 18, 258–266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Quadros R. M., Chen Pichler D., Lillo-Martin D., Cruz C. R., Kozak L. V., Palmer J. L., …, & Reynolds W. (2015). Methods in bimodal bilingualism research: experimental studies In Orifanidou E., Woll B., & Morgan G. (Eds.), Research methods in sign language studies: A practical guide (pp. 251–275). John Wiley & Sons: Malden, MA. [Google Scholar]
  67. Rinaldi P., & Caselli M. C. (2014). Language development in a bimodal bilingual child with cochlear implant: A longitudinal study. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17, 798–809. 10.1017/S1366728913000849. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  68. Rowe M. (2008). Child-directed speech: Relation to socioeconomic status, knowledge of children development and child vocabulary skill. Journal of Child Language, 35, 185–205. 10.1017/S0305000907008343. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Ruder C. C. (2004). Grammatical morpheme development in young cochlear implant users. International Congress Series, 1273, 320–323. 10.1016/j.ics.2004.08.033. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  70. Sandler W., & Lillo-Martin D. (2006). Sign language and linguistic universals. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  71. Schiff N. B., & Ventry I. M. (1976). Communication problems in hearing children of deaf parents. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, XLI, 348–358. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Serratrice L. (2012). The bilingual child In Bhatia, T. K. & Ritchie, W. C. (Eds.), The Handbook of Bilingualism and Multilingualism (pp. 87–108). Blackwell Publishing: Chicester, United Kingdom. [Google Scholar]
  73. Sloetjes H., & Wittenburg P. (2008). Annotation by category – ELAN and ISO DCR. Proceedings of the 6th International Conference on Language Resources and Evaluation (LREC 2008).
  74. Spencer P. E. (2004). Individual differences in language performance after cochlear implantation at one to three years of age: Child, family, and linguistic factors. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 9, 395–412. 10.1093/deafed/enh033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Spencer L. J., Tye-Murray N., & Tomblin J. B. (1998). The production of English inflectional morphology, speech production and listening performance in children with cochlear implants. Ear & Hearing, 19, 310–318. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Stokoe W. (1960). Sign language structure: An outline of the visual communication systems of the American Deaf. Studies in linguistics. Buffalo, New York: University of Buffalo. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Svirsky M., Stallings L., Lento C., Ying E., & Leonard L. (2002). Grammatical morphological development in pediatric cochlear implant users may be affected by the perceptual prominence of the relevant markers. Annals of Otology, Rhinology & Laryngology, 111, 109–112. 10.1177/00034894021110S522. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Szagun G., & Stumper B. (2012). Age or experience? The influence of age at implantation and social and linguistics environment on language development in children with cochlear implants. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 55, 1640–1654. 10.1044/1092-4388(2012/11-0119). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Thordardottir E. (2014). The relationship between bilingual exposure and morphosyntactic development. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 17, 97–114. 10.3109/17549507.2014.923509. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Tobey E. A., Rekart D., Buckley K., & Geers A. E. (2004). Mode of communication and classroom placement impact on speech intelligibility. Archives of Otolaryngology - Head Neck Surgery, 130, 639–643. 10.1001/archotol.130.5.639. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Werfel K. L. (2018). Morphosyntax production of preschool children with hearing loss: An evaluation of the extended optional infinitive and surface accounts. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 61, 2313–2324. 10.1044/2018_JSLHR-L-17-0406. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Wiesner D. (1991). Tuesday. New York, NY: Clarion Books. [Google Scholar]
  83. Williams K. T. (2007). Expressive Vocabulary Test (EVT)-2. San Antonio, TX: Pearson. [Google Scholar]
  84. Yoshinaga-Itano C., Baca R. L., & Sedey A. L. (2010). Describing the trajectory of language development in the presence of severe-to-profound hearing loss: A look at children with cochlear implants versus hearing aids. Otology & Neurotology, 31, 1268–1274. 10.1097/MAO.0b013e3181f1ce07. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Zimmerman I. L., Steiner V., & Pond R. E. (2002). Pre-school language scale, fourth edition (PLS-4) English edition. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]

Articles from The Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES