Abstract
Advances in genetics have led to an increased understanding of the role of the genotype on behavioural functioning. The purpose of the present study was to examine differences in intellectual functioning in individuals with Prader–Willi syndrome (PWS) with a paternal 15q11–q13 deletion versus maternal uniparental disomy (UPD) of chromosome 15. Measures of intelligence and academic achievement were administered to 38 individuals with PWS (24 with deletion and 14 with UPD). The subjects with UPD had significantly higher verbal IQ scores than those with deletion (P < 0.01). The magnitude of the difference in verbal IQ was 9.1 points (69.9 versus 60.8 for UPD and deletion PWS subjects, respectively). Only 17% of subjects with the 15q11–q13 deletion had a verbal IQ ⩾ 70, while 50% of those with UPD had a verbal IQ ⩾ 70. Performance IQ scores did not differ between the two PWS genetic subtype groups. This is the first report to document the difference between verbal and performance IQ score patterns among subjects with PWS of the deletion versus the UPD subtype.
Keywords: chromosome 15q deletion, genetic subtype differences, intellectual functioning, maternal uniparental disomy, Prader–Willi syndrome
Introduction
Advances in genetic techniques and our understanding of this subject have renewed interest in the genetic causes and classification of intellectual disability. Prader–Willi syndrome (PWS) shares behavioural features with other disorders and disabilities, such as obsessive-compulsive disorder and autism, but only PWS includes the unique combination of characteristics which distinguishes this syndrome. Prader–Willi syndrome is a classical genetic condition with two primary genetic subtypes and unusual behavioural characteristics.
Prader–Willi syndrome is characterized by infantile hypotonia, hypogonadism, feeding difficulties, early childhood obesity, short stature, small hands and feet, intellectual disability and characteristic facial abnormalities (Cassidy 1984; Butler 1990; Thompson et al. 1996; Cassidy et al. 1997). Prader–Willi syndrome most often results from a paternal deletion of 15q11–q13 (in about 70% of cases), or sometimes, from maternal disomy of chromosome 15 (in about 30% of cases) (Ledbetter et al. 1981; Butler et al. 1986; Mascari et al. 1992; Nicholls 1993; Cassidy et al. 1997). The prevalence of this syndrome is estimated to be one in 10–20 000 live births and is the most common syndromal cause of marked human obesity (Butler 1990). Prader– Willi syndrome and Angelman syndrome, which is caused by a maternal deletion of chromosome 15q but is an entirely different clinical condition, were the first examples in humans of genetic imprinting or the differential expression of genetic information depending on the parent of origin.
Intellectual disability and related adaptive behaviour deficits are common in PWS, although some relative strengths in self-help skills have been noted (Holm 1981; Thompson et al. 1996). In addition, people with PWS show problem behaviours, including stubbornness, demanding attention and arguing (Taylor 1988; Dykens et al. 1992). Many people with PWS engage in compulsive skin picking and impulsive temper outbursts, verbal abuse and regressive behaviour which may reach ‘psychotic proportions’ (Sulzbacher et al. 1981; Clarke et al. 1989). Clarke (1998) reported a higher than non-chance association of PWS and psychotic symptoms which was not entirely accounted for by the increased prevalence of psychosis associated with intellectual disability. The presence of visual and auditory hallucinations, delusions, irrational fears and paranoia seem to suggest that people with PWS are vulnerable to psychotic symptoms in adult life (Clarke et al. 1998; Verhoeven et al. 1998). Compulsive behaviour frequently appears in PWS and is more disruptive to daily functioning than in other mixed populations with intellectual disability (Vitiello et al. 1989; Dykens et al. 1996). These behavioural problems may take a greater toll on caregivers than adherence to strict weight-management protocols (Greenswag 1987; Hodapp et al. 1997).
Phenotypic differences relating to the PWS genotypes are of interest to geneticists and behavioural scientists because these may reveal genes causing specific clinical manifestations or enhance understanding of the impact of imprinting on genotype and phenotype studies in PWS. In the past, hypopigmentation (i.e. lighter hair, eye and skin colours compared with similarly aged family members) was noted to occur at a higher frequency in PWS patients with chromosome 15 deletions (Butler et al. 1986; Butler 1989; Spritz et al. 1997). Cassidy et al. (1997) reported relatively less impairment in articulation and skin picking among PWS individuals with UPD in comparison to those with deletion. Symons et al. (1999) found that individuals with deletion injure significantly more body sites (via skin picking) than subjects with UPD. Dykens et al. (1999) noted that a group of UPD subjects had higher full-scale IQ scores than the matched deletion subgroup. The above author also saw differences in maladaptive behaviour on the Child Behavior Checklist in the deleted subgroup on domains of internalizing, externalizing and total domains. A limitation of the above study was that some subjects were tested with the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test (K-BIT; Kaufman & Kaufman 1990), while the IQ of others was derived via parental report of scores from different tests (which were administered at different ages).
Previous studies have also revealed that individuals with PWS with the chromosome 15 deletion have more homogeneity in anthropometric variables, including radiographic measurements of the bones of the hand, as reflected in the metacarpophalangeal pattern profile (MCPP) and in dermatoglyphic patterns (Butler et al. 1982). Individuals with deletion PWS were much more homogeneous than non-deletion cases with respect to plantar patterns with a lack of plantar interdigital II–IV patterns with almost exclusive hallucal distal loops (Reed & Butler 1984). However, earlier studies were conducted before the recognition of uniparental disomy (UPD) as the most common cause of the non-deletion status among people with PWS. With the recent advent of molecular testing for all cases of PWS, other differences between individuals with deletion and UPD have been reported. Gillessen-Kaesbach et al. (1995) noted lower birth weights in individuals with the deletion subtype. Mitchell et al. (1996) reported shorter birth length in males with uniparental maternal disomy than males with the 15q deletion, and a shorter course of gavage feeding and later onset of hyperphagia in females with UPD. Cassidy et al. (1997) observed that people with PWS with UPD were less likely to have a typical facial appearance and were also less likely to show minor behavioural characteristics of PWS, including skin picking, skill with jigsaw puzzles, a high pain threshold and articulation problems. Gunay-Aygun et al. (1997) reported that the diagnosis of PWS among individuals with maternal UPD of chromosome 15 was typically reported later than those with a deletion. Thus, there is evidence of differences in people with PWS having the chromosome 15q deletion compared with those with UPD. The present paper reports the findings of a study on the intellectual characteristics of people with PWS with the 15q11–q13 deletion and those with UPD of chromosome 15.
Materials and methods
Subjects
Thirty-eight individuals with Prader–Willi syndrome (16 males and 22 females) served as subjects. These people represent all individuals with Prader–Willi syndrome (of the deletion and UPD subtypes) who have been studied to date as part of a Program Project which addresses a variety of cognitive, behavioural and metabolic aspects of PWS at the John F. Kennedy Center, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, Tennessee, USA. The age of the subjects ranged from 10 to 44 years (mean = 22.2, SD = 9.1). There were 24 subjects (nine males and 15 females) with a 15q11–q13 deletion and 14 subjects (seven males and seven females) with UPD of chromosome 15 identified by high-resolution chromosome analysis, in situ hybridization, and DNA microsatellite analysis of 15q11–q13 probes from patients and their parents using currently established techniques (Mutirangura et al. 1993; Christian et al. 1995; Butler et al. 1996; Spritz et al. 1997). There were two black people in the deletion subgroup (one male and one female) and none in the maternal UPD subgroup. The imbalance in the subgroup sample sizes is to be expected given the natural frequencies of the two aetiologies (deletion versus UPD) in the PWS population. The subject characteristics with respect to body composition and age are presented in Table 1.
Table 1.
Percentage fat |
||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Subgroup | Age (years) | Height (cm) | Weight (kg) | Body mass index (kg m−2) | 1* | 2† |
Deletion | 21.8 ± 8.3 | 150.2 ± 7.9 | 78.1 ± 0.5 | 34.5 ± 8.2 | 42.2 ± 7.2 | 51.4 ± 7.4 |
Uniparental disomy | 22.8 ± 10.6 | 149.6 ± 8.4 | 82.3 ± 22.9 | 36.7 ± 9.8 | 42.1 ± 5.1 | 51.4 ± 5.5 |
Derived from bioelectrical impedance (Segal et al. 1985).
Derived from dual energy X-ray absorptiometry (DEXA; Mazess et al. 1990).
Procedure
The subjects received a chronological-age-appropriate version of the Wechsler scales (either the WAIS-R or the WISC-III; Wechsler 1981, 1991), and the Mathematics and Reading portions of the Woodcock–Johnson Revised scales (Woodcock & Johnson 1990). Spelling skills were assessed using the WRAT-3 (Wilkinson 1993). The intellectual and achievement tests were administered by a licensed psychological examiner experienced with the PWS population as part of a comprehensive evaluation spanning a 3-day period. Parents and guardians or primary caregivers served as informants for the adaptive and maladaptive behaviour measures.
Results and discussion
Summary statistics (means and standard deviations) for the intelligence and achievement measures are displayed by genetic subgroup (deletion and UPD) in Table 2. Statistical tests for between-group differences (Bonferroni t-tests) revealed a statistically significant effect of genetic subtype on verbal IQ [t (36) = –3.45, P < 0.01]. In addition, the distribution of verbal IQ scores did not deviate from normal, as reflected by the Shapiro–Wilk statistic (deletion, P = 0.31; UPD, P = 0.97), and the 9.1-point difference between the deletion and UPD mean scores may be expressed in terms of an effect size of > 1.0. This large effect size provides evidence that the difference in verbal IQ is not only statistically significant, but that its magnitude is substantial (see Cohen 1988).
Table 2.
Test | Deletion | Uniparental disomy |
---|---|---|
Intelligence | ||
Wechsler Verbal IQ* | 60.8 ± 8.6 | 69.9 ± 6.4 |
Wechsler Performance IQ | 64.7 ± 9.3 | 62.2 ± 9.7 |
Wechsler Full-Scale IQ | 61.0 ± 9.2 | 64.1 ± 7.9 |
Achievement | ||
W-J/R Read | 53.1 ± 25.5 | 69.5 ± 35.0 |
W-J/R Math | 53.8 ± 20.6 | 65.1 ± 26.6 |
WRAT-3 Spelling | 60.2 ± 16.5 | 62.4 ± 12.6 |
P < 0.01.
Interestingly, the UPD subjects’ average verbal IQ of 69.9 falls at the classification point for ‘mild mental retardation’, while the deletion group averages 60.8, which is well within the classification range for ‘mild mental retardation’, as defined by the American Psychological Association (Editorial Board 1996). Only 17% (four out of 24) of the subjects in the deletion group had a verbal IQ ⩾ 70, while 50% (seven out of 14) of those with UPD had verbal IQ scores ⩾ 70, a finding which carries substantial implications for individual classification, placement and service decisions. However, the apparent relative strength in verbal IQ evidenced by the UPD group did not translate into statistically significant, concordant effects on measures of academic achievement as measured by the Woodcock–Johnson Reading and Math clusters or the WRAT-3 Spelling subtest. The UPD subgroup demonstrated significantly higher scores on four verbal subtests: Information and Arithmetic (P < 0.05), and Vocabulary and Comprehension (P < 0.01). The Arithmetic subtest is a measure of numeric calculation skill and attention, while the Vocabulary, Information and Comprehension subtests are measures of word meanings, factual knowledge and social judgement/reasoning, respectively. In contrast, the deletion group demonstrated a significant strength in Object Assembly (P < 0.05), a performance subtest measuring visual–perceptual skills.
The effect of PWS genetic subtype on verbal IQ was explored further by characterizing each subject on the basis of: (1) the difference between verbal and performance IQ; and (2) whether verbal IQ was greater than performance IQ. Subjects with deletion had a mean verbal-performance IQ difference of −4.0 ± 7.2, which indicates that performance IQ was, on average, 4.0 points higher than verbal IQ in this group. In contrast, the verbal IQ of subjects with UPD averaged 7.6 ± 8.0 points higher than their performance IQ. The between-group difference on this measure (verbal-performance IQ difference) was statistically significant [t (36) = −4.60, P < 0.001]. This effect is also reflected in the odds ratio, which addressed the extent to which subjects with UPD are more likely than those with a deletion to have a verbal IQ score which exceeded their performance IQ score. Subjects with UPD were 31.6 times more likely to have a verbal IQ that was higher than their performance IQ than subjects with deletion (P = 0.001). Moreover, the 95% confidence interval of the odds ratio indicated that subjects with UPD were at least 5.1 times as likely to have a positive verbal-performance IQ differential.
On average, the UPD subgroup attained higher verbal IQ scores than the deletion subgroup. Specific subtest differences were noted in numeric calculation skill, attention, word meanings, factual knowledge and social reasoning, with the UPD subgroup scoring higher than the deletion subgroup. Another interesting subtest difference was noted on the object assembly subtest, with the deletion subgroup scoring higher than the UPD subgroup. Specific visual perceptual skills may be a relative strength for the deletion subgroup and may explain anecdotal accounts of subjects with PWS having an uncanny ability to assemble jigsaw puzzles.
The mechanisms whereby certain skills appear to be preserved in the UPD subgroup have yet to be identified. Whether this phenomenon is caused by genetic imprinting versus the hemizygous state of the 15q11–q13 region in the deletion PWS patients is not known. The presence of more intact genes in UPD individuals in contrast to those with a deletion may be a relative strength for the PWS individual with the UPD subtype.
Acknowledgements
We thank Lee Beth Kilgore for her expert assistance in the preparation of the manuscript and two anonymous referees for their helpful comments. This research was supported by a grant from the National Institutes of Health (HD30329).
References
- Butler MG, Kaler SG & Meaney FJ. (1982) Metacarpophalangeal pattern profile analysis in Prader–Willi syndrome. Clinical Genetics 22, 315–20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Butler MG, Meaney FJ & Palmer CG. (1986) Clinical and cytogenetic survey of 39 individuals with Prader–Labhart–Willi syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genetics 23, 793–809. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Butler MG. (1989) Hypopigmentation: a common feature of Prader–Labhart–Willi syndrome. American Journal of Human Genetics 45, 140–6. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Butler MG. (1990) Prader–Willi syndrome: current understanding of cause and diagnosis. American Journal of Medical Genetics 35, 319–32. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Butler MG, Christian SL, Kubota T & Ledbetter DH. (1996) A 5-year-old white girl with Prader–Willi syndrome and a submicroscopic deletion of chromosome 15q11–q13. American Journal of Medical Genetics 65, 137–41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cassidy SB. (1984) Prader–Willi syndrome. Current Problems in Pediatrics 14, 1–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cassidy SB, Forsythe M, Heeger S, Nicholls RD, Schork N, Benn P & Schwartz S. (1997) Comparison of phenotype between patients with Prader–Willi syndrome due to deletion 15q and uniparental disomy 15. American Journal of Medical Genetics 68, 433–40. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Christian SL, Robinson WP, Huang B, Mutirangura A, Lino MR, Nakao M, Surti U, Chakravarti A & Ledbetter DH. (1995) Molecular characterization of two proximal deletion breakpoint regions in both Prader-Willi and Angelman syndrome patients. American Journal of Human Genetics 57, 40–8. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clarke D. (1998) Prader–Willi syndrome and psychotic symptoms: 2. A preliminary study of prevalence using the Psychopathology Assessment Schedule for Adults with Developmental Disability checklist. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research 42, 451–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clarke D, Boer H, Webb T, Scott P, Frazer S, Vogels A, Borghgraef M & Curfs LMG. (1998) Prader–Willi syndrome and psychotic symptoms: I. Case descriptions and genetic studies. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research 42, 440–50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clarke DJ, Waters J & Corbett JA. (1989) Adults with Prader–Willi syndrome: abnormalities of sleep and behavior. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine 82, 21–4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen J. (1988) Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd edn. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ. [Google Scholar]
- Dykens EM, Hodapp RM, Walsh K & Nash LJ. (1992) Adaptive and maladaptive behavior in Prader– Willi syndrome. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 31, 113–16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dykens EM, Leckman JF & Cassidy SB. (1996) Obsessions and compulsions in Prader–Willi syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 37, 995–1002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dykens EM, Cassidy SB & King BH. (1999) Maladaptive behavior differences in Prader–Willi syndrome due to paternal deletion versus maternal uniparental disomy. American Journal of Mental Retardation 104, 67–77. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Editorial Board (1996) Definition of mental retardation In: Manual of Diagnosis and Professional Practice in Mental Retardation (eds Jacobsen JW & Mulick JA), pp. 13–53. American Psychological Association, Washington, DC. [Google Scholar]
- Gillessen-Kaesbach G, Robinson W, Lohmann D, Kaya-Westerloh S, Passarge E & Horsthemke B. (1995) Genotype-phenotype correlation in a series of 167 deletion and non-deletion patients with Prader–Willi syndrome. Human Genetics 96, 638–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greenswag LR. (1987) Adults with Prader–Willi syndrome: a survey of 232 cases. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 41, 294–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gunay-Aygun M, Heeger S, Schwartz S & Cassidy SB. (1997) Delayed diagnosis in patients with Prader–Willi syndrome due to maternal uniparental disomy 15. American Journal of Medical Genetics 71, 106–10. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hodapp RM, Dykens EM & Masino LL. (1997) Families of children with Prader–Willi syndrome: stress-support and relations to child characteristics. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 27, 11–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holm VA. (1981) The diagnosis of Prader–Willi syndrome In: Prader–Willi Syndrome (eds Holm VA, Sulzbacher SJ & Pipes PL), pp. 27–44. University Park Press, Baltimore, MD. [Google Scholar]
- Kaufman AS & Kaufman NL. (1990) Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test. American Guidance Service, Circle Pines, MN. [Google Scholar]
- Ledbetter DH, Riccardi VM, Airhart SD, Strobel RJ, Keenan BS & Crawford JD. (1981) Deletions of chromosome 15 as a cause of the Prader–Willi syndrome. New England Journal of Medicine 304, 325–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mascari MJ, Gottlieb W, Rogan PK, Butler MG, Waller DA, Armour JAL, Jeffreys AJ, Ladda RL & Nicholls RD. (1992) The frequency of uniparental disomy in Prader–Willi syndrome. New England Journal of Medicine 326, 1599–607. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mazess RB, Barden HS, Bisek JP & Hanson J. (1990) Dual-energy X-ray absorptiometry for total-body and regional bone-mineral and soft-tissue composition. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 51, 1106–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitchell J, Schinzel A, Langlois S, Gillessen-Kaesbach G, Michaelis RC, Abeliovich D, Lerer I, Schuffenhauer S, Christian S, Guitart M, McFadden DE & Robinson WP. (1996) Comparison of phenotype in uniparental disomy and deletion Prader– Willi syndrome: sex-specific differences. American Journal of Medical Genetics 65, 133–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mutirangura A, Greenberg F, Butler MG, Malcolm S, Nicholls RD, Chakravarti A & Ledbetter DH. (1993) Multiplex PCR of three dinucleotide repeats in the Prader–Willi/Angelman critical region (15q11–q13): molecular diagnosis and mechanism of uniparental disomy. Human Molecular Genetics 2, 143–51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nicholls RD. (1993) Genomic imprinting and candidate genes in the Prader–Willi and Angelman syndromes. Current Opinion in Genetics and Development 3, 445–56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reed T & Butler MG. (1984) Dermatologic features in Prader–Willi syndrome with respect to chromosomal findings. Clinical Genetics 25, 341–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Segal KR, Gutin B, Presta E, Wang J & Van Itallie TB. (1985) Estimation of human body composition by electrical impedance methods: a comparative study. Journal of Applied Physiology 58, 1565–71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spritz RA, Bailin T, Nicholls RD, Lee S, Park S, Mascari MJ & Butler MG. (1997) Hypopigmentation in the Prader–Willi syndrome correlates with, p gene deletion but not with haplotype of the hemizygous, p allele. American Journal of Medical Genetics 71, 57–62. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sulzbacher S, Crnic KA & Snow J. (1981) Behavioral and cognitive disabilities in Prader–Willi syndrome In: Prader–Willi Syndrome (eds Holm VA, Sulzbacher SJ & Pipes PL), pp. 147–160. University Park Press, Baltimore, MD. [Google Scholar]
- Symons FJ, Butler MG, Sanders MD, Feurer ID & Thompson T. (1999) Self-injurious behavior and Prader–Willi syndrome: behavioral forms and body locations. American Journal on Mental Retardation 104, 260–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor RL. (1988) Cognitive and behavioral characteristics In: Prader–Willi Syndrome: Selected Research and Management Issues (eds Caldwell ML & Taylor RL), pp. 29–42. Springer-Verlag, Berlin. [Google Scholar]
- Thompson T, Butler MG, MacLean WE & Joseph B. (1996) Prader–Willi syndrome: genetics and behavior. Peabody Journal of Education 71, 187–212. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Verhoeven WMA, Curfs LMG & Tuinier S. (1998) Prader–Willi syndrome and cycloid psychoses. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research 42, 455–62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vitiello B, Spreat S & Behar D. (1989) Obsessive-compulsive disorder in mentally retarded patients. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 177, 232–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler D. (1981) Manual for the Wechsler Intelligence Scale-Revised (WEIS-R). The Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, TX. [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler D. (1991) Manual for the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, 3rd edn (WISC-III). The Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, TX. [Google Scholar]
- Wilkinson GS. (1993) The Wide Range Achievement Test, 3rd edn (WRAT-3). Jastak Associates Inc., Wilmington, DE. [Google Scholar]
- Woodcock RW & Johnson MB. (1990) Woodcock–Johnson Psycho-Educational Battery-Revised (W-J Achievement Test). DLM Teaching Resources, Allen, TX. [Google Scholar]