Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2020 Dec 1.
Published in final edited form as: Addict Behav. 2019 Jul 30;99:106067. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2019.106067

Retail Outlets Prompt Associative Memories Linked to the Repeated Use of Nicotine and Tobacco Products among Alternative High School Students in California

James Russell Pike 1, Yusuke Shono 2, Nasya Tan 1, Bin Xie 1, Alan W Stacy 1
PMCID: PMC6791762  NIHMSID: NIHMS1537550  PMID: 31421582

Abstract

Numerous studies have examined how youth are influenced by the presence of tobacco retail outlets that use point-of-sale marketing tactics to promote nicotine and tobacco products. The current investigation extends this research by assessing whether tobacco retail outlets function as environmental cues that prompt associative memories linked to the repeated use of cigarettes, electronic cigarettes, and cigars. Students (N = 1,060) from 29 alternative high schools in California were recruited into a three-year cohort study. A repeated measures latent profile analysis was conducted to identify latent subgroups of students. Analyses suggested the presence of one subgroup of students that did not use nicotine and tobacco products and five subgroups of students that used multiple products. A multinomial logistic regression revealed that images of gas stations, convenience stores, and liquor stores presented in the first year of the study prompted spontaneous associations in memory that increased the odds a student would belong to one of the five subgroups that repeatedly used nicotine and tobacco products over a three-year period. These findings suggest that tobacco retail outlets may act as environmental cues that prompt the use of addictive products among at-risk youth. Policymakers should consider implementing strategies that reduce the potency and prevalence of these cues.

Keywords: Marketing, Habits, Tobacco Use, Nicotine Addiction, Latent Profile Analysis

INTRODUCTION1

Making Responsible Decisions at the Point-of-Sale

Picture the scenario. A 16-year old student in California finishes her school day and begins walking home. In the distance, she sees a convenience store and spontaneously thinks ‘soda’ and ‘cigarettes.’ As she gets closer, she notices six exterior ads for cigarettes (Henriksen, Feighery, Schleicher, Haladjian, & Fortmann, 2004) and one for electronic cigarettes (Miller, et al., 2017). Out of habit, she enters the store and is immediately exposed to twenty-five ads for cigarettes (Henriksen, Feighery, Schleicher, Haladjian, & Fortmann, 2004) and fifteen for e-cigarettes (Miller, et al., 2017). As she waits in line to pay for her soda, she sees that it costs $5.35 USD for a pack of Marlboro cigarettes, $10.10 USD for a disposable Blu e-cigarette, and $1.03 USD for two Swisher Sweets cigars (Beleva, et al., 2018). She considers buying the e-cigarette but it is too costly so she chooses the cigars (Delnevo, Giovenco, & Miller Lo, 2017). She purchases the product even though she is underage (Landrine, Klonoff, & Alcaraz, 1996). Under current tobacco control policy in the United States, this is the environment in which youth are expected to make a reasoned decision about the consequences of using nicotine and tobacco products (NTPs).

The dangers of NTPs have been thoroughly documented. In the United States, cigars are estimated to cause 9,000 premature deaths each year (Nonnemaker, Rostron, Hall, MacMonegle, & Apelberg, 2014). Cigarettes are estimated to cause 480,000 premature deaths (US Department of Health and Human Services, 2014). The long-term impact of e-cigarettes is unknown but like cigarettes (US Department of Health and Human Services, 2014) and cigars (Chang, Corey, Rostron, & Apelberg, 2015) the product contains carcinogens (Goniewicz, et al., 2014) and poses serious health risks (Pisinger & Dossing, 2014; US Department of Health and Human Services, 2016). Despite this, retail outlets are permitted to sell NTPs within walking distance of schools (Finan, et al., 2018) and use point-of-sale (POS) marketing tactics (Ribisl, et al., 2017) to promote these addictive products. In 2016, over 6 billion USD was spent promoting cigarettes in stores (Federal Trade Commission, 2018). The amount spent on cigars and e-cigarettes is not systematically monitored but current evidence suggests that more than two-thirds of middle school and high school students in the United States have been exposed to POS advertising for e-cigarettes (Marynak, Gentzke, Wang, Neff, & King, 2018).

Numerous studies have shown that tobacco retail outlet (TRO) density is associated with experimentation with cigarettes (Lipperman-Kreda, Grube, & Friend, 2012; McCarthy, et al., 2009) and cigars (Cantrell, et al., 2016). Systematic reviews suggest a causal relationship between exposure to POS tobacco marketing and youth cigarette use (Lovato, Watts, & Stead, 2011; Robertson, McGee, Marsh, & Hoek, 2015). Longitudinal studies have provided evidence for a dose-response relationship between exposure to POS marketing and the use of cigarettes (Hanewinkel, Isensee, Sargent, & Morgenstern, 2011; Henriksen, Schleicher, Feighery, & Fortmann, 2010), e-cigarettes, and cigars (Beleva, et al., 2018). Based on these findings, it is not surprising that more and more youth are choosing to smoke a variety of NTPs (El-Toukhy, Sabado, & Choi, 2017). Current estimates suggest that in the United States over 3 million high school students have used e-cigarettes, over 1 million have used cigarettes, and over 1 million have used cigars in the past 30 days (Gentzke, et al., 2019). An estimated 1.68 million high school students have used two or more products in the same timeframe.

How Tobacco Retail Outlets Influence Habitual Behavior

Many of the mechanisms through which TROs facilitate the use of NTPs are easily discernible. TROs offer a means of purchasing NTPs and certain types of TROs, such as gas stations, convenience stores, and liquor stores (Feighery, Ribisl, Shleicher, & Halvorson, 2001; Miller, et al., 2017), have a large number of ads promoting specific brands (National Cancer Institute, 2008). What may be less apparent is the role that TROs play in establishing and perpetuating habits. Habits can be conceptualized as links between cues and behaviors (Hull, 1943; James, 1890; Wood & Rünger, 2016). At a neurological level, habits are characterized by lesser activation in the prefrontal areas of the brain associated with the cognitive control of behavior and greater activation in the basal ganglia (Sjoerds, Luigjes, Van Den Brink, Denys, & Yucel, 2014; Yin & Knowlton, 2006a). Habits can be formed through temporal proximity (i.e. Hebbian learning) and are strengthened by reward mechanisms such as the pharmacological effects of nicotine (Wood, 2017). Over time, habits can become so strong that cues that seem innocuous may trigger unwanted behaviors (Yin & Knowlton, 2006b; Wood & Neal, 2007).

Multiple prospective studies have found that associative memories predict unhealthy habitual behaviors including the use of alcohol (Ames, Xie, Shono, & Stacy, 2017; Kelly, Masterman, & Marlatt, 2005; Salemink & Wiers, 2014; Stacy, 1997) and marijuana (Ames, Xie, Shono, & Stacy, 2017; Shono, Edwards, Ames, & Stacy, 2018; Stacy, 1997). Research suggests that cues prompt spontaneous associations in memory which can activate a behavioral sequence that may circumvent reasoned decision-making (Stacy, Ames, & Knowlton, 2004; Wiers, et al., 2007; Wiers & Stacy, 2006). Although most studies examine a range of cues (Ames, Xie, Shono, & Stacy, 2017; Shono, Edwards, Ames, & Stacy, 2018; Stacy, 1997), from a policy perspective there is value to narrowing the investigation to TROs frequented by youth (Henriksen, Feighery, Schleicher, Haladjian, & Fortmann, 2004; Sanders-Jackson, Parikh, Schleicher, Fortmann, & Henriksen, 2015). If these TROs function as cues that encourage the use of NTPs, then regulations can be enacted to limit the prevalence and potency of the cues (Henriksen, 2012).

The effect of TROs on associative memories and the subsequent use of NTPs can be assessed utilizing various types of top-of-mind awareness (TOMA) measures. Assessments using TOMA date back to the pioneering work of Galton (1880). For decades, TOMA has been used in marketing to assess the salience (Miller & Berry, 1998) of brands and products in the minds of consumers (Axelrod, 1968; Axelrod, 1980; Gruber, 1969; Keller, 1993; Keller, 2012; Taylor & Fiske, 1978). In TOMA research, consumers are presented with a cue and asked to report the first brand or product that comes to mind. If a consumer spontaneously associates a cue with a specific brand or product, then that cue can be presented again at the right moment to influence behavior. Locations (e.g. movie theaters) that operate as cues to behavior (e.g. eating popcorn) are particularly powerful because they may trigger a habit (Neal, Wood, Wu, & Kurlander, 2011) while simultaneously providing the means to enact the behavior. The same mechanism may allow certain types of TROs to perpetuate habitual NTP use among subgroups of youth. The TOMA assessment utilized in the current investigation was designed to tap into this process by measuring associations in memory between TROs and NTPs.

Using Latent Subgroup Analysis to Examine the Repeated Use of Multiple Products

Latent subgroup analyses are designed to detect clusters of similar individuals within heterogeneous populations. Instead of examining how an independent variable (e.g. gender) influences a dependent variable (e.g. cigarette use), researchers can assess whether an independent variable is associated with membership in a subgroup (e.g. students who use multiple NTPs). For example, latent class analysis (Lazarsfeld, 1950; Lazarsfeld & Henry, 1968) has been applied to dichotomous indicators of tobacco use to distinguish nonusers from polytobacco users and to reveal that males are more likely to be poly-tobacco users (Gilreath, et al., 2016). The same technique has been extended to models with continuous indicators (Gibson, 1959; Lazarsfeld & Henry, 1968) and termed latent profile analysis.

When examining longitudinal data, researchers have identified subgroups by adapting latent growth curve models (Duncan, Duncan, Stryker, Li, & Alpert, 2006) into models that detect distinct developmental trajectories (Muthén, 2001a; Muthén, 2001b; Nagin, 1999). These techniques have been used to understand why one group of individuals may abstain from smoking cigarettes while another develops a lifelong habit during adolescence (Chassin, Presson, Pitts, & Sherman, 2000; Costello, Dierker, Jones, & Rose, 2008; Xie, Palmer, Yan, Lin, & Johnson, 2013). A limitation of these studies is that they are often designed to chart the rise and fall in the use of a single product over time by fitting continuous polynomial functions. In the modern NTP landscape, the diversity of products may cause youth to exhibit discontinuous patterns as they transition from one product to another (Soneji, et al., 2017) or use multiple NTPs (Gilreath, et al., 2016; Haardörfer, et al., 2016). Given that subgroups of individuals who use multiple NTPs report greater levels of nicotine dependence (Sung, Wang, Yao, Lightwood, & Max, 2018; Timberlake, 2008) and may be at greater risk for developing tobacco-related diseases (Backinger, et al., 2008), a closer examination of the factors that may contribute to the discontinuous but repeated use of NTPs is warranted.

In the present study, a repeated measures latent class analysis (RMLCA; Collins & Lanza, 2010) was adapted into a repeated measures latent profile analysis (RMLPA) to investigate the use of multiple NTPs over a three-year period. This form of analysis was chosen to capture the complex and potentially discontinuous longitudinal behavioral patterns that may exist among at-risk youth (Johnson, McMorris, & Kubik, 2013) attending alternative high schools (AHS). To our knowledge, no prior study has examined the repeated use of multiple NTPs in a population known to exhibit high rates of cigarette (Barnett, et al., 2013; Pokhrel, Sussman, & Stacy, 2014), e-cigarette (Miller, Pike, Stacy, Xie, & Ames, 2017), and cigar use (Beleva, et al., 2018). Furthermore, no study has tested whether TROs frequented by AHS students (Beleva, et al., 2018) act as cues that prompt associative memories that are linked to membership in subgroups of students who use multiple NTPs.

METHODS

Sampling

Data provided by the California Department of Education was used to identify 183 alternative high schools that had at least 100 students and were within 100 miles of the program offices in Claremont, California. On February 6th, 2014 research staff began contacting each school in a randomly selected order. All schools were invited to participate in accordance with the protocol approved by the Claremont Graduate University Institutional Review Board. Schools were accepted on a first-come, first-served basis until 29 sites were enrolled. By June 5th, 2014 each participating school provided a letter confirming their involvement.

Research staff visited the schools between October 14th, 2014 and May 18th, 2015. Interest forms were distributed to 6,870 students who were in attendance at the schools. Completed forms were returned by 2,726 students. The response rate was 39.7% (2726/6870). Each student that returned a form was assigned to a specific staff member. The staff member obtained written consent and provided a link to a web-based survey. Parental consent and youth assent were obtained for students under the age of 18. All students were given until September 1st, 2015 to complete the approximately 90-minute survey. A total of 1,060 students took part in the first assessment for a completion rate of 15.4% (1060/6870). Each of these students was given a $45 gift card to compensate them for their time.

Research staff maintained contact with the students using established procedures (Booker, Harding, & Benzeval, 2011; Hall, et al., 2003; McCuller, Sussman, Holiday, Craig, & Dent, 2002; Scott, 2004). One-year follow-up assessments were completed by 892 students. An additional 31 students without access to a web-enabled device completed a computer-assisted telephone interview. All assessments were administered between September 21st, 2015 and September 1st, 2016. The average length of time between the first assessment and the second assessment was 330 days (SD = 26.6). The retention rate was 87.1% (923/1060). Among the 137 students who did not complete the second assessment, 93.5% failed to respond to repeated contact attempts, 5.8% withdrew from the study, and 0.7% were incarcerated. Each student that completed the second assessment received a $50 gift card.

The retention rate for the two-year follow-up was 81.0% (859/1060). 832 students completed a web-based survey while 27 students completed a computer-assisted telephone interview. All assessments occurred between September 26th, 2016 and September 1st, 2017. The average assessment took place 695 days (SD = 33.7) after the first assessment. Among the 201 students that did not complete the third assessment, 93.0% failed to respond to repeated contact attempts, 4.5% withdrew from the study, 1.5% had died, 0.5% were incarcerated, and 0.5% were deployed overseas after enrolling in the military. A $100 gift card was provided to each student that completed the third assessment.

Measures

Demographics.

Students were asked to report their gender and ethnicity. Students also provided their birthdate which was used to calculate their age at the initial assessment.

Exposure to Advertising for Nicotine and Tobacco Products Outside the Retail Environment (α = .86).

To examine the influence of advertising that occurs outside of TROs, a four-item scale was adapted from the Youth Tobacco Survey (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2014). The scale assessed the extent to which students had been exposed to (a) television commercials, (b) online ads, (c) newspaper and magazine ads, and (d) radio spots for NTPs. Response options included ‘None’, ‘1-3 times in the past 30 days’, ‘1-3 times per week’, ‘Daily or almost daily’, and ‘More than once a day’. The mean of these four items was computed for each student.

Frequency of Visits to Gas Stations, Convenience Stores, and Liquor Stores (α = . 65).

A modified version of a previously validated scale (Henriksen, Feighery, Schleicher, Haladjian, & Fortmann, 2004; Feighery, Henriksen, Wang, Schleicher, & Fortmann, 2006) was used to determine the frequency with which students visited gas stations, convenience stores, and liquor stores. Students indicated whether they visited each type of store ‘Never’, ‘Once a month’, ‘Two or three times a month’, ‘Once a week’, ‘Two or three times a week’, or ‘Almost every day’. The mean of the six-point scale was calculated for each student.

Associative Memories Prompted by Images of Gas Stations, Convenience Stores, and Liquor Stores (α = .77).

In July and August of 2014, Samsung Galaxy S III phones were used to photograph TROs near alternative high schools for a separate investigation of POS marketing (Miller, et al., 2017). Many of the photographs were of stores that were within walking distance of schools that were eligible to enroll in the current study but chose not to participate. From this subset of photographs, one gas station, one convenience store, and one liquor store without exterior POS ads for NTPs were selected. Each image depicted the exterior of the store in the early afternoon without any people present. The store was photographed from the sidewalk to replicate the vantage point of someone walking by. The three images were resized so that no side was greater than 650 pixels. During the survey, each student was informed that they would be presented with an image and asked to quickly enter the first word that came to mind. When these top of mind instructions are used, the response is strongly implicated in implicit memory processes because conscious recollection is not necessary (Levy, Stark, & Squire, 2004; Schacter, 1985; Shimamura & Squire, 1984; Vaidya, Gabrieli, Keane, & Monti, 1995). The task was performed at the beginning of the survey to prevent priming effects (Ames, et al., 2007). After a single practice trial in which students were presented with the image of a fast food store, six images were displayed in a randomly selected order. To prevent response chaining, the photographs of the TROs were interspersed with three photographs of stores that did not sell NTPs. These filler images were generated using the same protocol for photographing TROs. Once the task was completed, students were presented with the word they entered and the image that caused them to think of the word. Utilizing a procedure validated in prior studies (Frigon & Krank, 2009; Krank, Schoenfeld, & Frigon, 2010; Shono, Ames, & Stacy, 2016), the students were asked to indicate whether the word was related to one of fourteen categories (e.g. cigarettes, soda, snacks, etc.). Responses were inspected to ensure that students had selected categories that were plausibly related to the original word. Examples of entries related to NTPs include ‘cigarettes’, ‘swishers’, and ‘tobacco.’ If the student indicated that their response was associated with cigarettes, e-cigarettes, or cigars a value of one was assigned. A sum score was then calculated. A score of zero meant that none of the TROs were spontaneously associated with NTPs while a score of three indicated that all of the TROs were associated with NTPs.

Use of Nicotine and Tobacco Products in the Past Year.

A validated drug use questionnaire (Graham, et al., 1984; Stacy, et al., 1990) was adapted to ask students about their use of (a) cigarettes, (b) e-cigarettes, vaporizers, or vape pens, and (c) cigars, cigarillos, or little cigars in the past year. Response options included ‘0 times’, ‘1-10 times’, ‘11-20 times’, ‘21-30 times’, ‘31-40 times’, ‘41-50 times’, ‘51-60 times’, ‘61-70 times’, ‘71-80 times’, ‘81-90 times’, and ‘91+ times’. Although the measure has not yet been validated for novel nicotine products such as vape pens, it has been employed in prior studies to compare the frequency with which at-risk populations use multiple addictive substances including cigarettes (Ames, Xie, Shono, & Stacy, 2017), alcohol (Ames, Grenard, & Stacy, 2013; Ames, Xie, Shono, & Stacy, 2017; Grenard, Ames, & Stacy, 2012), marijuana (Ames, Xie, Shono, & Stacy, 2017; Grenard, Ames, & Stacy, 2012), and methamphetamine (Ames, Grenard, & Stacy, 2013; Grenard, Ames, & Stacy, 2012). The measure was selected so that the use of cigarettes, e-cigarettes, and cigars could be compared on an equal scale. It was administered once per year over a period of three years.

Analyses

The analytic dataset contained 1,060 students nested within 29 schools. Calculating the intra-class correlation revealed that between 2.0% and 5.8% of the variance in the use of cigarettes, e-cigarettes, and cigars could be attributed to random effects at the school level. To account for similarities between students attending the same school, a clustering variable was created that indicated the school each student attended. This variable was integrated into all statistical models. Standard errors robust to non-normality and non-independence of observations were estimated for each model.

An analysis was performed to determine whether students who did not complete the second and third assessments shared common characteristics. Students with missing data at the second assessment were more likely to be male (64.0% vs 47.7%, p <.001). No statistically significant differences were detected in the ethnicity of the students (70.4% Hispanic vs 76.3% Hispanic, p = .498), their age at the first assessment (17.5 vs 17.4, p = .176) or their use of cigarettes (34.3% vs 24.8%, p = .057), e-cigarettes (37.6% vs 32.4%, p = .392), or cigars (26.3% vs 18.5%, p = .063) in the past year at the first assessment. The same pattern persisted at the third assessment. Students with missing data were more likely to be male (62.8% vs 46.8%, p <.001). Differences in the ethnicity of the students (72.5% Hispanic vs 76.1% Hispanic, p = .809), their initial age (17.5 vs 17.5, p = .375), and their initial use of cigarettes (32.6% vs 24.5%, p = .057), e-cigarettes (34.7% vs 32.9%, p = .875), and cigars (24.1% vs 18.6%, p = .158) were not statistically significant. Based on these findings, full-information maximum likelihood was employed to account for missing data (Graham, 2012; Little & Rubin, 2002). Missing responses in the use of NTPs were assumed to be missing at random conditional on the reported use of these products throughout the three-year study. Covariates that might influence the identification of latent subgroups, such as gender, were excluded from the initial RMLPA model (Nylund-Gibson & Masyn, 2016).

The RMLPA model included nine continuous variables representing the use of cigarettes, e-cigarettes, and cigars in each year of the three-year study. To select the most plausible number of latent subgroups, a series of RMLPA models were tested with between two and seven profiles. Each model used 5,000 random sets of starting values, 20 iterations at the initial stage, and 100 final stage optimizations. The mean and variance of the first subgroup was fixed to zero to delineate students who never used NTPs for the entirety of the three-year study. The best fitting model was selected (Collins & Lanza, 2010; Henson, Reise, & Kim, 2007; Nylund, Asparouhov, & Muthén, 2007; Tofighi & Enders, 2007) by evaluating the log-likelihood, Akaike’s Information Criterion (AIC; Akaike, 1987), Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC; Schwarz, 1978), sample size adjusted Bayesian Information Criterion (SSA-BIC; Sclove, 1987), entropy (Celeux & Soromenho, 1996), and average posterior probabilities (APP; Nagin & Odgers, 2010). Using the three-step method (Asparouhov & Muthén, 2014; Vermunt, 2010), a multinomial logistic regression was conducted in which membership in a specific latent subgroup was the dependent variable. The independent variables included in the model were gender (female = 0 vs male =1), ethnicity (non-Hispanic = 0 vs Hispanic =1), age, exposure to NTP advertising outside the retail environment, frequency of visits to TROs, and a TOMA assessment of whether TROs prompted spontaneous associations related to NTPs.

RESULTS

Descriptive Statistics

The sample was 50.7% male and 75.2% Hispanic (see Table 1). The mean age was 17.5 years (SD = 0.9). A sizable portion of the students reported weekly exposure to television commercials (40.4%), online ads (31.8%), newspaper and magazine ads (30.9%), and radio spots (19.8%) for NTPs. Students also reported weekly visits to gas stations (65.3%), convenience stores (48.1%), and liquor stores (43.9%). Nearly one-sixth of the students (16.4%) spontaneously associated an image of one of these stores with an NTP. Compared to students who did not exhibit the same associative memories, these students reported greater use of cigarettes (1.8 vs 0.9, p <.001), e-cigarettes (2.1 vs 0.9, p <.001), and cigars (1.2 vs 0.6, p = .002) in the past year at the first assessment on a scale that ranged from 0 (‘0 times’) to 11 (‘91+ times’). A similar pattern was observed when comparing the use of cigarettes (1.9 vs 0.9, p <.001), e-cigarettes (2.6 vs 1.0, p <.001), and cigars (1.0 vs 0.7, p = .133) in the past year at the first assessment between students who spontaneously associated two or more TROs with an NTP to those who associated one or fewer TROs with an NTP.

Table 1.

Descriptive statistics for longitudinal cohort of alternative high school students (n = 1060) assessed over a three-year period

Cases
n (%)
Complete Cases
n (%)
First Assessment
Gender
Male 534 (50.7%) 1054 (99.4%)
Female 520 (49.3%)
Ethnicity
Hispanic 777 (75.2%) 1033 (97.5%)
Non-Hispanic 256 (24.8%)
Age
Less than 17 Years Old 316 (29.9%) 1057 (99.7%)
17 Years Old 440 (41.6%)
Greater than 17 Years Old 301 (28.5%)
Weekly Exposure to Advertising for NTPs
Television Commercials 395 (40.4%) 978 (92.3%)
Online Ads 311 (31.8%) 979 (92.4%)
Newspaper and Magazine Ads 302 (30.9%) 978 (92.3%)
Radio Spots 189 (19.4%) 975 (92.0%)
Weekly TRO Visits
Gas Stations 676 (65.3%) 1035 (97.6%)
Convenience Stores 493 (48.1%) 1026 (96.8%)
Liquor Stores 453 (43.9%) 1033 (97.5%)
Associative Memories Related to NTPs
One Spontaneous Association 72 (7.2%) 996 (94.0%)
Two Spontaneous Associations 51 (5.1%)
Three Spontaneous Associations 41 (4.1%)
NTPs Used in the Past Year
Cigarettes 267 (26.4%) 1011 (95.4%)
E-Cigarettes 337 (33.3%) 1012 (95.5%)
Cigars 198 (19.9%) 997 (94.1%)
Only One Product 171 (17.3%) 990 (93.4%)
Two or More Products 241 (24.3%) 990 (93.4%)
Second Assessment
NTPs Used in the Past Year
Cigarettes 221 (25.1%) 882 (83.2%)
E-Cigarettes 243 (27.7%) 878 (82.8%)
Cigars 167 (19.0%) 879 (82.9%)
Only One Product 145 (16.7%) 870 (82.1%)
Two or More Products 189 (21.8%) 870 (82.1%)
Third Assessment
NTPs Used in the Past Year
Cigarettes 175 (21.3%) 820 (77.4%)
E-Cigarettes 169 (20.6%) 820 (77.4%)
Cigars 143 (17.5%) 818 (77.2%)
Only One Product 96 (11.9%) 806 (76.0%)
Two or More Products 148 (18.3%) 807 (76.1%)

Note: Students were permitted to skip survey questions they did not want to answer. Consequently, the number of complete cases from the original cohort of 1,060 students varied for each measure. NTPs = Nicotine and tobacco products. TROs = Gas stations, convenience stores, and liquor stores that sell nicotine and tobacco products.

Within the full cohort, the use of only one product (17.3%) was less common than the use of two or more products (24.3%) at the first assessment. At the second assessment, a statistically significant decrease in e-cigarette use was observed (33.3% vs 25.1%, p = .004) while changes in the use of cigarettes (26.4% vs 27.7%, p = .937) and cigars (19.9% vs 19.0%, p = .682) from the first assessment were not statistically significant. An observed decrease in the use of two or more products was also not statistically significant (24.3% vs 21.8%, p = .313). A comparison between product use at the first assessment and the third assessment revealed a statistically significant decline in the use of e-cigarettes (33.3% vs 20.6%, p <.001) and cigarettes (26.4% vs 21.3%, p = .042) but not cigars (19.9% vs 17.5%, p = .475). The use of two or more products also declined (24.3% vs 18.3%, p <.001).

Identifying Latent Subgroups

The log-likelihood, AIC, BIC, and SSA-BIC steadily decreased with each RMLPA model (see Table 2) leading to the selection of six latent subgroups. A model with seven subgroups was tested but the solution converged to a local maximum (Goodman, 1974). APP and entropy confirmed that the model with six subgroups had good separation with excellent categorization (Nagin & Odgers, 2010).

Table 2.

Summary of information used to identify latent subgroups of alternative high school students (n = 1060) that repeatedly used multiple nicotine and tobacco products over a three-year period

Two Subgroups
Three Subgroups
Four Subgroups
Five Subgroups
Six Subgroups
Parameters 19 29 39 49 59
Log-likelihood −2297.547 −1662.729 −1485.865 −1291.637 −1157.255
AIC 4633.094 3383.457 3049.731 2681.274 2432.510
BIC 4727.232 3527.142 3242.961 2924.051 2724.834
SSA-BIC 4666.885 3435.033 3119.092 2768.420 2537.441
Entropy 1.000 0.980 0.979 0.975 0.971
APP 1.000 0.967 0.936 0.918 0.872

Note: AIC = Akaike’s Information Criterion, BIC = Bayesian Information Criterion, SSA-BIC = Sample size adjusted Bayesian Information Criterion, and APP = Average Posterior Probabilities.

Nearly half (45.9%) of the students were classified as nonusers (see Figure 1) who did not experiment with cigarettes, e-cigarettes, or cigars at any point during the three-year study. More than a third (37.9%) were designated experimenters who used cigarettes, e-cigarettes, and cigars between zero and ten times each year. The next three subgroups were labeled light users who favored cigarettes (4.8%), e-cigarettes (4.2%), or cigars (4.5%). In a single year, each of these subgroups used all three products between 40 and 160 times. The final subgroup was designated moderate users (2.7%) who collectively used all three products more than 100 times per year.

Figure 1. Visual depiction of latent subgroups of alternative high school students (n = 1060) that repeatedly used multiple nicotine and tobacco products over a three-year period.

Figure 1.

Note: Each bar represents the estimated mean use of a single product in a single year within a latent subgroup. Three bars are present in each year to depict the frequency with which multiple nicotine and tobacco products are used.

Independent Variables Associated with Subgroup Membership

A multinomial logistic regression that utilized nonusers as the reference group (see Table 3) revealed that males were more likely to be light e-cigarette users vs nonusers (Odds Ratio [OR] = 3.50, 95% Confidence Intervals [95% CI] = 1.58-7.77, p = .002). Hispanics were less likely to be light cigarette users vs nonusers (OR =0.19, 95% CI = 0.08-0.47, p <.001) or moderate users vs nonusers (OR = 0.09, 95% CI = 0.03-0.26, p <.001). Students who were older at the first assessment were more likely to be light cigarette users vs nonusers (OR = 1.53, 95% CI = 1.12-2.08, p = .007) or light e-cigarette users vs nonusers (OR = 1.59, 95% CI = 1.13-2.24, p = .008). Exposure to tobacco advertising outside the retail environment was not a statistically significant independent variable in most instances except when comparing light e-cigarette users to nonusers (OR = 1.47, 95% CI = 1.06-2.03, p = .021).

Table 3.

Odds ratios for multinomial logistic regression depicting the association between measures administered in the first year of the study and membership in subgroups of alternative high school students (n = 1060) that repeatedly used multiple nicotine and tobacco products over a three-year period

Experimenter
vs. Nonuser
Light Cigarette
User vs. Nonuser
Light E-Cigarette
User vs. Nonuser
Light Cigar
User vs. Nonuser
Moderate
User vs. Nonuser
OR [95% CI] OR [95% CI] OR [95% CI] OR [95% CI] OR [95% CI]






Gender (Male) 0.95 [0.68, 1.32] 0.97 [0.53, 1.79] 3.50 [1.58, 7.77] 1.93 [0.77, 4.86] 2.16 [0.91, 5.14]
Ethnicity (Hispanic) 0.91 [0.61, 1.34] 0.19 [0.08, 0.47] 0.33 [0.11, 1.01] 0.54 [0.21, 1.38] 0.09 [0.03, 0.26]
Age 1.02 [0.88, 1.18] 1.53 [1.12, 2.08] 1.59 [1.13, 2.24] 1.05 [0.60, 1.84] 1.11 [0.60, 2.06]
Advertising Exposure 1.14 [0.96, 1.36] 1.03 [0.77, 1.37] 1.47 [1.06, 2.03] 1.24 [0.89, 1.74] 1.22 [0.73, 2.05]
TRO Visits 1.41 [1.24, 1.61] 1.92 [1.38, 2.68] 2.12 [1.56, 2.88] 2.46 [1.92, 3.16] 3.96 [2.07, 7.56]
Associative Memories 1.25 [1.06, 1.47] 1.62 [1.20, 2.20] 1.92 [1.31, 2.81] 1.44 [1.03, 2.03] 2.73 [1.89, 3.94]

Note: Advertising Exposure = Exposure to newspaper and magazine ads, television commercials, radio spots, and online ads for nicotine and tobacco products, TRO Visits = Frequency of visits to gas stations, convenience stores, and liquor stores, and Associative Memories = Spontaneous associations with gas stations, convenience stores, and liquor stores that are related to the use of nicotine and tobacco products.

The frequency with which students visited TROs was associated with membership in each subgroup. For each increase in the original scale, such as when a student reported visiting TROs ‘Two or three times a month’ instead of ‘Once a month’, the odds were 1.41 times greater that the student would be an experimenter vs a nonuser (95% CI = 1.24-1.61, p <.001), 1.92 times greater that the student would be a light cigarette user vs a nonuser (95% CI = 1.38-2.68, p <.001), 2.12 times greater that the student would be a light e-cigarette user vs a nonuser (95% CI = 1.56-2.88, p <.001), 2.46 times greater that the student would be a light cigar user vs a nonuser (95% CI = 1.92-3.16, p <.001), and 3.96 times greater that the student would be a moderate user vs a nonuser (95% CI = 2.07-7.56, p <.001).

A similar pattern was observed for students who reported spontaneous associations with TROs that were related to NTPs. For each associative memory linked to an NTP, the odds were 1.25 times greater that the student would be an experimenter vs a nonuser (95% CI = 1.06-1.47, p = .007), 1.44 times greater that the student would be a light cigar user vs a nonuser (95% CI = 1.03-2.03, p = .034), 1.62 times greater that the student would be a light cigarette user vs a nonuser (95% CI = 1.20-2.20, p = .002), 1.92 times greater that the student would be a light e-cigarette user vs a nonuser (95% CI = 1.31-2.81, p = .001), and 2.73 times greater that the student would be a moderate user vs a nonuser (95% CI = 1.89-3.94, p <.001).

DISCUSSION

Tobacco Retail Outlets and the Repeated Use of Nicotine and Tobacco Products

The current study used RMLPA to identify latent subgroups of AHS students that used cigarettes, e-cigarettes, and cigars over a three-year period. While prior research has employed latent class analysis to denote the existence of subgroups that use multiple NTPs at one point in time (Gilreath, et al., 2016; Haardörfer, et al., 2016), this investigation extends these findings by revealing that the use of NTPs in these subgroups may reoccur year after year. A particularly novel finding is that TROs may function as environmental cues that prompt spontaneous associations linked to the repeated use of NTPs. Beyond serving as locations for obtaining NTPs, the sight of TROs may bring to mind implicit memories which initiate a behavioral sequence that culminates in the use of one or more products. This mechanism may partially explain the findings from a recent meta-analysis (Finan, et al., 2018) indicating that TRO density influences youth cigarette use. Given the broad range of products currently being promoted in TROs (Ribisl, et al., 2017), future studies may find that TRO density is also associated with the use of e-cigarettes (Pérez, et al., 2017) and cigars (Cantrell, et al., 2016).

The repeated use of NTPs has major health implications. Although some researchers argue that the consistent use of e-cigarettes may save lives (Levy, et al., 2018), the potential benefits are moot if more harmful products are still used with enough regularity to reveal clinical sequelae in the future. Moreover, if the use of NTPs are essentially equated in habit formation then occurrences of use of one product could propel the use of all products leading to greater levels of nicotine addiction (Timberlake, 2008; Sung, Wang, Yao, Lightwood, & Max, 2018) and a greater likelihood of relapse during cessation attempts (Messer, et al., 2015).

Limitations

Although the findings presented are compelling several limitations must be noted. RMLPA was used to identify latent subgroups within a sample of AHS students who are themselves an at-risk subgroup within the general youth population in the United States (Johnson & Taliaferro, 2012). In addition, a comparison between the sample and school-level demographics reported by the California Department of Education in 2014 revealed statistically significant differences in the gender (50.7% male vs 59.7% male, p <.001) and ethnicity (75.2% Hispanic vs 71.0% Hispanic, p = .005) of the longitudinal cohort. These differences hinder the generalizability of the findings. Future studies should consider applying RMLPA to other longitudinal datasets to confirm whether the same subgroups exist elsewhere and whether those subgroups exhibit comparable patterns of behavior. It may also be informative to use alternative statistical techniques to examine the use of NTPs such as dual-trajectory and triple-trajectory latent growth mixture models (Cho, et al., 2018).

Another limitation is that the current investigation focused on whether or not TROs prompted spontaneous associations related to NTPs but did not delineate the precise source of those associations. Randomized experiments indicate advertising for e-cigarettes influences associative memories (Pokhrel, et al., 2016; Pokhrel, Herzog, Fagan, Unger, & Stacy, 2018). However, the repeated purchase of NTPs at TROs, whether legally or illegally, may also independently or synergistically shape associative memories. Understanding these neurocognitive mechanisms and determining the extent to which they influence both explicit and implicit cognition could be critical to the development of policies that reduce youth use of NTPs.

The current investigation was also limited to self-report measures. Among smokers, self-report measures have been known to produce imprecise estimates of cigarette use (Shiffman, 2009; Stanton, McClelland, Elwood, Ferry, & Silva, 1996; Wang, Shiffman, Griffith, & Heitjan, 2012). Recall bias may have similarly resulted in inaccurate reports of the use of cigarettes, e-cigarettes, and cigars in the past year which could have influenced the identification of the latent subgroups. The measures utilized were also restricted to three NTPs. Future studies may benefit by broadening the range of products used as indicators of the latent subgroups. Given the recent legalization of marijuana in California, the body of evidence documenting the co-occurring use of cannabis and tobacco (Agrawal, Budney, & Lynskey, 2012), and the disproportionately high rates of marijuana use among AHS students (Johnson, McMorris, & Kubik, 2013) it may also be insightful to distinguish between subgroups of youth who separately or simultaneously use multiple substances.

Reducing the Potency and Prevalence of Tobacco Retail Outlets

By understanding the effect of TROs on the use of NTPs, policymakers can begin to develop strategies that disrupt the link between environmental cues and unwanted behaviors.One possible mechanism for achieving this is to enact comprehensive marketing restrictions (Henriksen, 2012). Restrictions would have the practical effect of reducing cues in retail environments frequented by youth (Henriksen, Feighery, Schleicher, Haladjian, & Fortmann, 2004; Sanders-Jackson, Parikh, Schleicher, Fortmann, & Henriksen, 2015) which may cause fewer students to spontaneously associate TROs with NTPs. This form of habit disruption (Wood & Neal, 2016) may partially explain decreases in smoking observed after the implementation of marketing restrictions required by the 1998 Master Settlement Agreement (Iwasaki, Tremblay, & Tremblay, 2006).

An alternative strategy is to decrease the prevalence of TROs (Ashe, Jernigan, Kline, & Galaz, 2003; Cohen & Anglin, 2009; Finan, et al., 2018; Institute of Medicine, 2007). This could be achieved by placing fees on tobacco licenses issued to retailers (Henriksen, 2012), passing moratoriums on the provision of new licenses once a certain threshold is reached (US Department of Health and Human Services, 2000), or by enacting localized restrictions under the authority granted by the Family Smoking Prevention and Tobacco Control Act (Lange, Hoefges, & Ribisl, 2015). Collectively, these policies might create an environment in which a student is able to walk home from school without being prompted to enter a store promoting addictive, carcinogenic products.

  • Prior research shows that tobacco retail outlets (TROs) influence youth tobacco use

  • TROs prompt associations in memory linked to the repeated use of tobacco products

  • Policies to reduce tobacco use should consider the effect of TROs on at-risk youth

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank Sandy Asad, Sara J Asad, Melissa Garrido, Sarah Z Gonzalez, Hannah Jornacion, and Brenda Lisa Lucero for their tireless efforts recruiting and tracking alternative high school students. Additional thanks to Jerry Grenard for critical help refining the central concepts.

Role of Funding Sources

Research reported in this publication was supported by the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHHD) and the Food and Drug Administration (FDA) Center for Tobacco Products (R01HD077560). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NICHHD or the FDA. Neither the NICHHD nor the FDA had a role in the design of the study, the collection, analysis, or interpretation of the data, the writing of the manuscript, or the decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Footnotes

Conflict of Interest

None.

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

1

Abbreviations: AHS = Alternative High School, AIC = Akaike’s Information Criterion, APP = Average Posterior Probabilities, BIC = Bayesian Information Criterion, NTP = Nicotine and Tobacco Product, POS = Point of Sale, RMLCA = Repeated Measures Latent Class Analysis, RMLPA = Repeated Measures Latent Profile Analysis, SSA-BIC = Sample size adjusted Bayesian Information Criterion, TOMA = Top of Mind Awareness, TRO = Tobacco Retail Outlets

References

  1. Agrawal A, Budney A, & Lynskey M (2012). The co-occurring use and misuse of cannabis and tobacco: A review. Addiction, 107(7), 1221–1233. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2012.03837.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Akaike H (1987). Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika, 52, 317–332. doi: 10.1007/BF0229435 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  3. Ames S, Grenard J, & Stacy A (2013). Dual process interaction model of HIV-risk behaviors among drug offenders. AIDS and behavior, 17(3), 914–925. doi: 10.1007/s10461-012-0140-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Ames S, Grenard J, Thush C, Sussman S, Wiers R, & Stacy A (2007). Comparison of indirect assessments of association as predictors of marijuana use among at-risk adolescents. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 15(2), 204–218. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.15.2.218 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Ames S, Xie B, Shono Y, & Stacy A (2017). Adolescents at risk for drug abuse: A 3-year dual-process analysis. Addiction, 112(5), 852–863. doi: 10.1111/add.13742 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Ashe M, Jernigan D, Kline R, & Galaz R (2003). Land use planning and the control of alcohol, tobacco, firearms, and fast food restaurants. American Journal of Public Health, 93(9), 1404–1408. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.93.9.1404 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Asparouhov T, & Muthén B (2014). Auxiliary variables in mixture modeling: Three-step approaches using Mplus. Structural Equation Modeling, 21(3), 329–341. doi: 10.1080/10705511.2014.915181 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  8. Axelrod J (1968). Attitude measures that predict purchase. Journal of Advertising Research, 8(1), 3–17. [Google Scholar]
  9. Axelrod J (1980). Advertising wearout. Journal of Advertising Research, 20(5), 13–18. [Google Scholar]
  10. Backinger C, Fagan P, O’Connell M, Grana R, Lawrence D, Bishop J, & Gibson J (2008). Use of other tobacco products among U.S. adult cigarette smokers: Prevalence, trends and correlates. Addictive Behaviors, 33(3), 472–489. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.10.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Barnett E, Spruijt-Metz D, Unger J, Rohrbach L, Sun P, & Sussman S (2013). Bidirectional associations between future time perspective and substance use among continuation high-school students. Substance Use & Misuse, 48(8), 574–580. doi: 10.3109/10826084.2013.787092 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Beleva Y, Pike J, Miller S, Xie B, Ames S, & Stacy A (2018). Share of advertising voice at the point-of-sale and its influence on at-risk students’ use of alternative tobacco products. Nicotine & Tobacco Research. doi: 10.1093/ntr/nty152 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Booker C, Harding S, & Benzeval M (2011). A systematic review of the effect of retention methods in population-based cohort studies. BMC Public Health, 11, 249. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-11-249 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Cantrell J, Pearson J, Anesetti-Rothermel A, Xiao H, Kirchner T, & Vallone D (2016). Tobacco retail outlet density and young adult tobacco initiation. Nicotine & Tobacco Research, 18(2), 130–137. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntv036 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Celeux G, & Soromenho G (1996). An entropy criterion for assessing the number of clusters in a mixture model. Journal of Classification, 13(2), 195–212. doi: 10.1007/BF01246098 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  16. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2014, January 21). 2011 Youth tobacco survey. Retrieved from Smoking & Tobacco Use: http://www.cdc.gov/tobacco/data_statistics/surveys/yts/index.htm [Google Scholar]
  17. Chang C, Corey C, Rostron B, & Apelberg B (2015). Systematic review of cigar smoking and all cause and smoking related mortality. BMC Public Health, 15, 390. doi: 10.1186/s12889-015-1617-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Chassin L, Presson C, Pitts S, & Sherman S (2000). The natural history of cigarette smoking from adolescence to adulthood in a midwestern community sample: Multiple trajectories and their psychosocial correlates. Health Psychology, 19(3), 223–231. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.19.3.223 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Cho J, Goldenson N, Stone M, McConnell R, Barrington-Trimis J, Chou C, …Leventhal A (2018). Characterizing polytobacco use trajectories and their associations with substance use and mental health across mid-adolescence. Nicotine & Tobacco Research, 20(suppl_1), S31–S38. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntx270 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Cohen J, & Anglin L (2009). Outlet density: A new frontier for tobacco control. Addiction, 104(1), 2–3. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2008.02389.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Collins L, & Lanza S (2010). Latent Class and Latent Transition Analysis: With Applications in the Social, Behavioral, and Health Sciences. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. [Google Scholar]
  22. Costello D, Dierker L, Jones B, & Rose J (2008). Trajectories of smoking from adolescence to early adulthood and their psychosocial risk factors. Health Psychology, 27(6), 811–818. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.27.6.811 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Delnevo C, Giovenco D, & Miller Lo E (2017). Changes in the mass-merchandise cigar market since the Tobacco Control Act. Tobacco Regulatory Science, 3(2 Suppl 1), S8–S16. doi: 10.18001/TRS.3.2(Suppl1).2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Duncan T, Duncan S, Stryker L, Li F, & Alpert A (2006). An Introduction to Latent Variable Growth Curve Modeling. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [Google Scholar]
  25. El-Toukhy S, Sabado M, & Choi K (2017). Trends in tobacco product use patterns among U.S. youth, 1999–2014. Nicotine & Tobacco Research. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntx128 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Federal Trade Commission. (2018). Federal trade commission cigarette report. Washington, DC: Federal Trade Commission. [Google Scholar]
  27. Feighery E, Henriksen L, Wang Y, Schleicher N, & Fortmann S (2006). An evaluation of four measures of adolescents' exposure to cigarette marketing in stores. Nicotine & Tobacco Research, 8(6), 751–759. doi: 10.1080/14622200601004125 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Feighery E, Ribisl K, Shleicher N, & Halvorson S (2001). Cigarette advertising and promotional strategies in retail outlets: Results of a statewide survey in California. Tobacco Control, 10(1), 184–188. doi: 10.1136/tc.10.2.184 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Finan L, Lipperman-Kreda S, Abadi M, Grube J, Kaner E, Balassone A, & Gaidus A (2018). Tobacco outlet density and adolescents’ cigarette smoking: A meta-analysis. Tobacco Control. doi: 10.1136/tobaccocontrol-2017-054065 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Frigon A, & Krank M (2009). Self-coded indirect memory associations in a brief school-based intervention for substance use suspensions. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 23(4), 736–742. doi: 10.1037/a0017125 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Galton F (1880). Psychometric Experiments. Brain, 2, 149–162. [Google Scholar]
  32. Gentzke A, Creamer M, Cullen K, Ambrose B, Willis G, Jamal A, King B (2019). Vital signs: Tobacco product use among middle and high school students - United States, 2011–2018. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 68(6), 157–164. doi: 10.15585/mmwr.mm6806e1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Gibson W (1959). Three multivariate models: Factor analysis, latent structure analysis, and latent profile analysis. Psychometrika, 24, 229–252. doi: 10.1007/BF02289845 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  34. Gilreath T, Leventhal A, Barrington-Trimis J, Unger J, Cruz T, Berhane K, … McConnell R (2016). Patterns of alternative tobacco product use: Emergence of hookah and e-cigarettes as preferred products amongst youth. Journal of Adolescent Health, 58(2), 181–185. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2015.10.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Goniewicz M, Knysak J, Gawron M, Kosmider L, Sobczak A, Kurek J, … Benowitz N (2014). Levels of selected carcinogens and toxicants in vapour from electronic cigarettes. Tobacco Control, 23(2), 133–139. doi: 10.1136/tobaccocontrol-2012-050859 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Goodman L (1974). Exploratory latent structure analysis using both identifiable and unidentifiable models. Biometrika, 61, 215–231. [Google Scholar]
  37. Graham J (2012). Missing Data: Analysis and Design. New York, NY: Springer. [Google Scholar]
  38. Graham J, Flay B, Johnson C, Hansen W, Grossman L, & Sobel J (1984). Reliability of self-report measures of drug use in prevention research: Evaluation of the Project SMART questionnaire via the test-retest reliability matrix. Journal of Drug Education, 14, 175–193. doi: 10.2190/CYV0-7DPB-DJFA-EJ5U [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Grenard J, Ames S, & Stacy A (2012). Deliberative and spontaneous cognitive processes associated with HIV risk behavior. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 36(1), 95–107. doi: 10.1007/s10865-012-9404-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Gruber A (1969). Top-of-mind awareness and share of families: An observation. Journal of Marketing Research, 6(2), 227–231. doi: 10.2307/3149678 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  41. Haardörfer R, Berg C, Lewis M, Payne J, Pillai D, McDonald B, & Windle M (2016). Polytobacco, marijuana, and alcohol use patterns in college students: A latent class analysis. Addictive Behaviors, 59, 58–64. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2016.03.034 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Hall E, Zuniga R, Cartier J, Anglin M, Danila B, Ryan T, & Mantius K (2003). Staying in Touch: A Fieldwork Manual of Tracking Procedures for Locating Substance Abusers in Follow up Studies. Los Angeles, CA: UCLA Integrated. [Google Scholar]
  43. Hanewinkel R, Isensee B, Sargent J, & Morgenstern M (2011). Cigarette advertising and teen smoking initiation. Pediatrics, 127(2), e271–e278. doi: 10.1542/peds.2010-2934 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Henriksen L (2012). Comprehensive tobacco marketing restrictions: Promotion, packaging, price and place. Tobacco Control, 21(2), 147–153. doi: 10.1136/tobaccocontrol-2011-050416 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Henriksen L, Feighery E, Schleicher N, Haladjian H, & Fortmann S (2004). Reaching youth at the point of sale: Cigarette marketing is more prevalent in stores where adolescents shop frequently. Tobacco Control, 13, 315–318. doi: 10.1136/tc.2003.006577 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Henriksen L, Schleicher N, Feighery E, & Fortmann S (2010). A longitudinal study of exposure to retail cigarette advertising and smoking initiation. Pediatrics, 126(2), 232–238. doi: 10.1542/peds.2009-3021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Henson J, Reise S, & Kim K (2007). Detecting mixtures from structural model differences using latent variable mixture modeling: A comparison of relative model fit statistics. Structural Equation Modeling, 14(2), 202–226. doi: 10.1080/10705510709336744 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  48. Hull C (1943). Principles of Behavior: An Introduction to Behavior Theory. New York, NY: Appleton-Century-Crofts. [Google Scholar]
  49. Institute of Medicine. (2007). Ending the Tobacco Problem: A Blueprint for the Nation. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. [Google Scholar]
  50. Iwasaki N, Tremblay C, & Tremblay V (2006). Advertising restrictions and cigarette smoking: Evidence from myopic and rational addiction models. Contemporary Economic Policy, 24(3), 370–381. doi: 10.1093/cep/byj024 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  51. James W (1890). Habit. New York, NY: Henry Holt and Company. [Google Scholar]
  52. Johnson K, McMorris B, & Kubik M (2013). Comparison of health-risk behaviors among students attending alternative and traditional high schools in Minnesota. The Journal of School Nursing, 29(5), 343–352. doi: 10.1177/1059840512469409 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Johnson K, & Taliaferro L (2012). Health behaviors and mental health of students attending alternative high schools: A review of the research literature. Journal for Specialists in Pediatric Nursing, 17, 79–97. doi: 10.1111/j.1744-6155.2011.00311.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Keller K (1993). Conceptualizing, measuring, and managing customer-based brand equity. Journal of Marketing, 57(1), 1–22. doi: 10.2307/1252054 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  55. Keller K (2012). Strategic Brand Management: Building, Measuring, and Managing Brand Equity (4th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall. [Google Scholar]
  56. Kelly A, Masterman P, & Marlatt G (2005). Alcohol-related associative strength and drinking behaviours: Concurrent and prospective relationships. Drug and Alcohol Review, 24(6), 489–498. doi: 10.1080/09595230500337675 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Krank M, Schoenfeld T, & Frigon A (2010). Self-coded indirect memory associations and alcohol and marijuana use in college students. Behavior Research Methods, 42(3), 733–738. doi: 10.3758/BRM.42.3.733 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Landrine H, Klonoff E, & Alcaraz R (1996). Asking age and identification may decrease minors’ access to tobacco. Preventive Medicine, 25(3), 301–306. doi: 10.1006/pmed.1996.0060 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Lange T, Hoefges M, & Ribisl K (2015). Regulating tobacco product advertising and promotions in the retail environment: A roadmap for states and localities. Journal of Law, Medicine & Ethics, 43(3), 878–896. doi: 10.1111/jlme.12326 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Lazarsfeld P (1950). The logical and mathematical foundation of latent structure analysis and The interpretation and mathematical foundation of latent structure analysis In Stouffer S, Guttman L, Suchman E, Lazarsfeld P, Star S, & Clausen J, Measurement and Prediction (pp. 362–472). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. [Google Scholar]
  61. Lazarsfeld P, & Henry N (1968). Latent Structure Analysis. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin. [Google Scholar]
  62. Levy D, Borland R, Lindblom E, Goniewicz M, Meza R, Holford T, … Abrams D (2018). Potential deaths averted in USA by replacing cigarettes with e-cigarettes. Tobacco Control, 27(1), 18–25. doi: 10.1136/tobaccocontrol-2017-053759 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Levy D, Stark C, & Squire L (2004). Intact conceptual priming in the absence of declarative memory. Psychological Science, 15(10), 680–686. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00740.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Lipperman-Kreda S, Grube J, & Friend K (2012). Local tobacco policy and tobacco outlet density: Associations with youth smoking. Journal of Adolescent Health, 50(6), 547–552. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2011.08.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Little R, & Rubin D (2002). Statistical Analysis with Missing Data (2nd ed.). New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. [Google Scholar]
  66. Lovato C, Watts A, & Stead L (2011). Impact of tobacco advertising and promotion on increasing adolescent smoking behaviours. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, 3, CD003439. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD003439.pub2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Marynak K, Gentzke A, Wang T, Neff L, & King B (2018). Exposure to electronic cigarette advertising among middle and high school students - United States, 2014–2016. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 67(10), 294–299. doi: 10.15585/mmwr.mm6710a3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. McCarthy W, Mistry R, Lu Y, Patel M, Zheng H, & Dietsch B (2009). Density of tobacco retailers near schools: Effects on tobacco use among students. American Journal of Public Health, 99(11), 2006–2013. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2008.145128 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. McCuller W, Sussman S, Holiday K, Craig S, & Dent C (2002). Tracking procedures for locating high-risk youth. Evaluation & the Health Professions, 25(3), 345–362. doi: 10.1177/0163278702025003006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Messer K, Vijayaraghavan M, White M, Shi Y, Chang C, Conway K, … Pierce J (2015). Cigarette smoking cessation attempts among current US smokers who also use smokeless tobacco. Addictive Behaviors, 51, 113–199. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2015.06.045 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Miller S, & Berry L (1998). Brand salience versus brand image: Two theories of advertising effectiveness. Journal of Advertising Research, 38(5), 77–83. [Google Scholar]
  72. Miller S, Pike J, Chapman J, Xie B, Hilton B, Ames S, & Stacy A (2017). The prevalence and marketing of electronic cigarettes in proximity to at-risk youth: An investigation of point-of-sale practices near alternative high schools. Journal of Child & Adolescent Substance Abuse, 26(2), 119–124. doi: 10.1080/1067828X.2016.1230529 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Miller S, Pike J, Stacy A, Xie B, & Ames S (2017). Negative affect in at-risk youth: Outcome expectancies mediate relations with both regular and electronic cigarette use. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 31(4), 457–464. doi: 10.1037/adb0000272 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Muthén B (2001a). Latent variable mixture modeling In Marcoulides G, & Schumacker R, New Developments and Techniques in Structural Equation Modeling (pp. 1–33). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [Google Scholar]
  75. Muthén B (2001b). Second-generation structural equation modeling with a combination of categorical and continuous latent variables: New opportunities for latent In Collins L, & Sayer A, New Methods for the Analysis of Change (pp. 291–322). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. [Google Scholar]
  76. Nagin D (1999). Analyzing developmental trajectories: A semiparametric group-based approach. Psychological Methods, 4(2), 139–157. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.4.2.139 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Nagin D, & Odgers C (2010). Group-based trajectory modeling in clinical research. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 6, 109–138. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131413 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. National Cancer Institute. (2008). The role of the media in promoting and reducing tobacco use. Tobacco Control Monograph No. 19. Bethesda, MD: US Department of Health and Human Services, National Institutes of Health, National Cancer Institute. [Google Scholar]
  79. Neal D, Wood W, Wu M, & Kurlander D (2011). The pull of the past: When do habits persist despite conflict with motives? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37, 1428–1437. doi: 10.1177/0146167211419863 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Nonnemaker J, Rostron B, Hall P, MacMonegle A, & Apelberg B (2014). Mortality and economic costs from regular cigar use in the United States, 2010. American Journal of Public Health, 104(9), e86–e91. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2014.301991 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Nylund K, Asparouhov T, & Muthén B (2007). Deciding on the number of classes in latent class analysis and growth mixture modeling: a Monte Carlo simulation study. Structural Equation Modeling, 14(4), 535–569. doi: 10.1080/10705510701575396 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  82. Nylund-Gibson K, & Masyn K (2016). Covariates and mixture modeling: Results of a simulation study exploring the impact of misspecified effects on class enumeration. Structural Equation Modeling, 23(6), 782–797. doi: 10.1080/10705511.2016.1221313 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  83. Pérez A, Chien L, Harrell M, Pasch K, Obinwa U, & Perry C (2017). Geospatial associations between tobacco retail outlets and current use of cigarettes and e-cigarettes among youths in Texas. Journal of Biometrics & Biostatistics, 8(5), 375. doi: 10.4172/2155-6180 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Pisinger C, & Dossing M (2014). A systematic review of health effects of electronic cigarettes. Preventive Medicine, 69, 248–260. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2014.10.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Pokhrel P, Fagan P, Herzog T, Chen Q, Muranaka N, Kehl L, & Unger J (2016). E-cigarette advertising exposure and implicit attitudes among young adult non-smokers. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 163, 134–140. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2016.04.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Pokhrel P, Herzog T, Fagan P, Unger J, & Stacy A (2018). E-cigarette advertising exposure, explicit and implicit harm perceptions, and e-cigarette use susceptibility among non-smoking young adults. Nicotine & Tobacco Research. doi: 10.1093/ntr/nty030 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Pokhrel P, Sussman S, & Stacy A (2014). Relative effects of social self-control, sensation seeking, and impulsivity on future cigarette use in a sample of high-risk adolescents. Substance Use & Misuse, 49(4), 343–351. doi: 10.3109/10826084.2013.841241 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Ribisl K, D’Angelo H, Feld A, Schleicher N, Golden S, Luke D, & Henriksen L (2017). Disparities in tobacco marketing and product availability at the point of sale: Results of a national study. Preventive Medicine, 105, 381–388. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2017.04.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Robertson L, McGee R, Marsh L, & Hoek J (2015). A systematic review on the impact of point-of-sale tobacco promotion on smoking. Nicotine & Tobacco Research, 17(1), 2–17. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntu168 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Salemink E, & Wiers R (2014). Alcohol-related memory associations in positive and negative affect situations: Drinking motives, working memory capacity, and prospective drinking. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 28(1), 105–113. doi: 10.1037/a0032806 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Sanders-Jackson A, Parikh N, Schleicher N, Fortmann S, & Henriksen L (2015). Convenience store visits by US adolescents: Rationale for healthier retail environments. Health & Place, 34, 63–66. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2015.03.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Schacter D (1985). Priming of old and new knowledge in amnesic patients and normal subjects. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 444, 41–53. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1985.tb37578.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Schwarz G (1978). Estimating the dimension of a model. Annals of Statistics, 6, 461–464. doi: 10.1214/aos/1176344136 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  94. Sclove S (1987). Application of model-selection criteria to some problems in multivariate analysis. Psychometrika, 52, 333–343. doi: 10.1007/BF02294360 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  95. Scott C (2004). A replicable model for achieving over 90% follow-up rates in longitudinal studies of substance abusers. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 74(1), 21–36. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2003.11.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Shiffman S (2009). How many cigarettes did you smoke? Assessing cigarette consumption by global report, Time-Line Follow-Back, and ecological momentary assessment. Health Psychology, 28(5), 519–526. doi: 10.1037/a0015197 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Shimamura A, & Squire L (1984). Paired-associate learning and priming effects in amnesia: A neuropsychological study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113(4), 556–570. doi: 10.1007/s12160-012-9461-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Shono Y, Ames S, & Stacy A (2016). Evaluation of internal validity using modern test theory: Application to word association. Psychological Assessment, 28(2), 194–204. doi: 10.1037/pas0000175 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Shono Y, Edwards M, Ames S, & Stacy A (2018). Trajectories of cannabis-related associative memory among vulnerable adolescents: Psychometric and longitudinal evaluations. Developmental Psychology. doi: 10.1037/dev0000510 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Sjoerds Z, Luigjes J, Van Den Brink W, Denys D, & Yücel M (2014). The role of habits and motivation in human drug addiction: A reflection. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 5, 1–5. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Soneji S, Barrington-Trimis J, Wills T, Leventhal A, Unger J, Gibson L, …Sargent J (2017). Association between initial use of e-cigarettes and subsequent cigarette smoking among adolescents and young adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of the American Medical Association Pediatrics, 171(8), 788–797. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2017.1488 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Stacy A (1997). Memory activation and expectancy as prospective predictors of alcohol and marijuana use. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106(1), 61–73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Stacy A, Ames S, & Knowlton B (2004). Neurologically plausible distinctions in cognition relevant to drug use etiology and prevention. Substance Use & Misuse, 39(10-12), 1571–1623. doi: 10.1081/JA-200033204 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Stacy A, Flay B, Sussman S, Brown K, Santi S, & Best J (1990). Validity of alternative self-report indices of smoking among adolescents. Psychological Assessment, 2, 442–446. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.2.4.442 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  105. Stanton W, McClelland M, Elwood C, Ferry D, & Silva P (1996). Prevalence, reliability and bias of adolescents’ reports of smoking and quitting. Addiction, 91(11), 1705–1714. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.1996.9111170511.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Sung H, Wang Y, Yao T, Lightwood J, & Max W (2018). Polytobacco use and nicotine dependence symptoms among US adults, 2012–2014. Nicotine & Tobacco Research, 20(suppl_1), S88–S98. doi: 10.1093/ntr/nty050 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Taylor S, & Fiske S (1978). Salience, attention, and attribution: Top of the head phenomena. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 11, 249–288. doi: 10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60009-X [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  108. Timberlake D (2008). A latent class analysis of nicotine-dependence criteria and use of alternative tobacco. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 69(5), 709–717. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2008.69.709 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Tofighi D, & Enders C (2007). Identifying the correct number of classes in growth mixture models In Samuelsen KM, Advances in Latent Variable Mixture Models (pp. 317–341). Greenwhich, CT: Information Age Publishing. [Google Scholar]
  110. US Department of Health and Human Services. (2000). Reducing tobacco use: a report of the Surgeon General. Atlanta, GA: US Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion, Office on Smoking and Health. [Google Scholar]
  111. US Department of Health and Human Services. (2014). The health consequences of smoking—50 years of progress: a report of the Surgeon General. Atlanta, GA: US Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion, Office on Smoking and Health. [Google Scholar]
  112. US Department of Health and Human Services. (2016). E-cigarette use among youth and young adults: a report of the Surgeon General. Atlanta, GA: US Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion, Office on Smoking and Health. [Google Scholar]
  113. Vaidya C, Gabrieli J, Keane M, & Monti L (1995). Perceptual and conceptual memory processes in global amnesia. Neuropsychology, 9(4), 12. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.9.4.580 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Vermunt J (2010). Latent class modeling with covariates: Two improved three-step approaches. Political Analysis, 18(4), 450–469. doi: 10.1093/pan/mpq025 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  115. Wang H, Shiffman S, Griffith S, & Heitjan D (2012). Truth and memory: Linking instantaneous and retrospective self-reported cigarette consumption. The Annals of Applied Statistics, 6(4), 1689–1706. doi: 10.1214/12-AOAS557 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Wiers R, & Stacy A (2006). Implicit cognition and addiction: An introduction In Wiers R, & Stacy A (Eds.), Handbook of Implicit Cognition and Addiction (pp. 363–378). Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications. [Google Scholar]
  117. Wiers R, Bartholow B, van den Wildenberg E, Thush C, Engels R, Sher K, … Stacy A (2007). Automatic and controlled processes and the development of addictive behaviors in adolescents: A review and a model. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior Special Issue: Adolescents, Drug Abuse and Mental Disorders, 86(2), 263–283. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2006.09.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Wood W (2017). Habit in personality and social psychology. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 21(4), 389–403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Wood W, & Neal D (2007). A new look at habits and the habit-goal interface. Psychological Review, 114(4), 843–863. doi: 10.1177/1088868317720362 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Wood W, & Neal D (2016). Habit-based behavior change interventions. Behavioral Science & Policy, 2, 71–78. [Google Scholar]
  121. Wood W, & Rünger D (2016). Psychology of habit. Annual Review of Psychology, 67, 289–314. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033417 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Xie B, Palmer P, Yan L, Lin C, & Johnson C (2013). Developmental trajectories of cigarette use and associations with multi-layered risk factors in Chinese early adolescents. Nicotine & Tobacco Research, 15(10), 1673–1681. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntt035 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Yin H, & Knowlton B (2006b). Addiction and learning in the brain In Wiers R, & Stacy A, Handbook of Implicit Cognition and Addiction (pp. 167–183). Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications [Google Scholar]
  124. Yin H, & Knowlton B (2006a). The role of the basal ganglia in habit formation. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7, 464–476. doi: 10.1038/nrn1919 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES