Skip to main content
The Journal of Neuroscience logoLink to The Journal of Neuroscience
. 1998 Nov 15;18(22):9517–9528. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-22-09517.1998

Synaptic Integration in Striate Cortical Simple Cells

Judith A Hirsch 1, José-Manuel Alonso 1, R Clay Reid 2, Luis M Martinez 1
PMCID: PMC6792880  PMID: 9801388

Abstract

Simple cells in the visual cortex respond to the precise position of oriented contours (Hubel and Wiesel, 1962). This sensitivity reflects the structure of the simple receptive field, which exhibits two sorts of antagonism between on andoff inputs. First, simple receptive fields are divided into adjacent on and off subregions; second, within each subregion, stimuli of the reverse contrast evoke responses of the opposite sign: push–pull (Hubel and Wiesel, 1962;Palmer and Davis, 1981; Jones and Palmer, 1987; Ferster, 1988). We have made whole-cell patch recordings from cat area 17 during visual stimulation to examine the generation and integration of excitation (push) and suppression (pull) in the simple receptive field. The temporal structure of the push reflected the pattern of thalamic inputs, as judged by comparing the intracellular cortical responses to extracellular recordings made in the lateral geniculate nucleus. Two mechanisms have been advanced to account for the pull–withdrawal of thalamic drive and active, intracortical inhibition (Hubel and Wiesel, 1962; Heggelund, 1986; Ferster, 1988). Our results suggest that intracortical inhibition is the dominant, and perhaps sole, mechanism of suppression. The inhibitory influences operated within a wide dynamic range. When inhibition was strong, the membrane conductance could be doubled or tripled. Furthermore, if a stimulus confined to one subregion was enlarged so that it extended into the next, the sign of response often changed from depolarizing to hyperpolarizing. In other instances, the inhibition modulated neuronal output subtly, by elevating spike threshold or altering firing rate at a given membrane voltage.

Keywords: visual cortex, patch recording in vivo, simple cell, IPSP, EPSP, spiny stellate cell


Cortical sensitivity to patterned stimuli has its roots in the arrangement of synaptic inputs to simple cells, whose receptive fields are made of elongated, alternatingon and off subregions. Bright signals confined to an on subregion are excitatory, whereas dark ones reduce activity; that is, stimuli of the opposite contrast have a push–pull effect (Hubel and Wiesel, 1962; Movshon et al., 1978; Heggelund, 1981,1986; Palmer and Davis, 1981; Jones and Palmer, 1987; Ferster, 1988;Miller, 1994; Troyer et al., 1998; but see Debanne et al., 1998). Furthermore, when the receptive field is uniformly illuminated or darkened, simple cells respond poorly because their subregions have a mutually antagonistic relationship (Hubel and Wiesel, 1962). Thus, the output of the simple cell relies on the balance of excitation and suppression that various stimuli evoke.

We have used the technique of whole-cell recording (Hamill et al., 1981; Edwards et al., 1989; Blanton et al., 1989) in vivo(Pei et al., 1991; Ferster and Jagadeesh, 1992) to analyze the synaptic mechanisms that produce visually evoked responses in the receptive field. First, we examined the origins of excitatory and suppressive components of the responses to stimuli of reverse contrast flashed within a single subregion. Then we asked how excitatory and suppressive mechanisms interact when engaged together, as happens when a stimulus tilts away from the axis of the field to span neighboring subregions. Although suppression might result either from the removal of excitation or from active inhibition, the findings of these studies stress that active inhibition in the cortical receptive field is strong and modulates excitatory input both by means of hyperpolarization and increasing the membrane conductance (Hirsch et al., 1995).

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Anesthesia. Adult cats, 2.5–3.5 kg, were anesthetized with ketamine (10 mg/kg, i.m.) followed by thiopental sodium (20 mg/kg, i.v.), supplemented as needed. Lidocaine was applied topically at all incisions or points of pressure. Temperature (37–38°C), EKG, EEG, and expired CO2 (27–33 mmHg) were monitored throughout the experiment. Anesthesia was maintained by a continuous infusion of thiopental sodium (2–4 mg · kg−1 · hr−1, i.v.) adjusted as indicated by the EEG and EKG. After the completion of surgery, animals were paralyzed [vecuronium bromide (Norcuron) 0.2 mg · kg−1 · hr−1, i.v.] and artificially respired.

Surgery. An endotracheal tube was introduced through a tracheotomy before the animal was placed in a stereotaxic apparatus. Two cortical craniotomies were made; one centered on Horsley–Clark coordinates A6.5-L8.5 gave access to the lateral geniculate nucleus, and the other, centered on Horsley–Clark P3-L2, was enlarged to expose the longitudinal gyrus. Pupils were dilated with 1% atropine sulfate, and the nictitating membranes were retracted with 10% phenylephrine. Eyes were refracted and fitted with contact lenses to focus on a tangent screen. The position of the area centralis and the optic disk of each eye was determined with a fundus camera.

Acquisition of visually evoked responses. Intracellular and extracellular records were collected by a computer running the Discovery software package (Datawave Systems, Longmont, CO), intracellular records were normally sampled at 3–4 kHz. Each recording session was also stored on videotape at 11–22 kHz. An AT-vista board (Truevision, Indianapolis, IN), controlled by the same computer that received the data generated visual stimuli that were presented on a computer monitor (frame rate 100, 105, 128, or 140 Hz). Each cycle of the stimulus protocol consisted of light or dark squares at various contrasts (range, 30–70%) flashed singly for 29–39 msec in pseudorandom order, 16 times on a 16 × 16 grid (sparse noise; seeJones and Palmer, 1987). Grid spacing ranged from 0.4 to 0.85° and square size from 0.4 to 1.7°. Although there was no delay between sequential stimuli (as one was switched off another was flashed on), effective squares were usually separated by substantial intervals because the receptive fields were typically much smaller than the stimulus grid. Maps of the receptive fields were made by subtracting responses to dark stimuli from responses to bright stimuli and are plotted as shaded contours; each successive concentric line indicates a 10% reduction in the strength of response relative to the peak (spikes inclusive). Receptive fields with separate and adjacent onand off subregions were classified as simple (Hubel and Wiesel, 1962; for review, see Skottun et al., 1991). The termson and off are equivalent to the terms bright-excitatory and dark-excitatory used by others (DeAngelis et al., 1992).

Recording. Patch-pipette resistance was ≥12 MΩ when filled with internal solution, in mm; K gluconate 120; NaCl 5; CaCl2 1; MgCl2 1; EGTA 11; GTP 0.2; ATP 2; HEPES 40; and biocytin 1%, pH 7.3, 290 mOsm (Malinow and Tsein, 1990). For three cells, 10 mm QX-314 Br (courtesy of Astra) was included in the pipette. Initial seal resistances were 0.5–1.0 GΩ. Recordings were made with an Axopatch 200a amplifier and stored as described above; neither capacitance nor access resistance was compensated, so fast events were filtered. The bridge was balanced off-line. The voltage–current relationship was measured before and after each cycle of the stimulus protocol to monitor changes in the access and apparent input resistance, threshold for firing, and membrane time constant. The DC voltage changes produced by constant current injection during a given cycle of the protocol were taken from the responses to equivalent current pulses delivered before or after the cycle. This procedure served to separate drift in the recording circuit from real changes in the membrane potential that may have occurred over time. Because the access resistance often increased after rupture of the membrane (Edwards and Konnerth, 1992), the voltage in the records was sometimes divided (Stühmer et al., 1983). For this reason, as well as drift that can occur over the long time courses of the recording, we do not provide absolute resting potentials. Extracellular recordings in the lateral geniculate were amplified, filtered, and collected in parallel with the intracellular recordings.

Histology. After histological processing (Horikawa and Armstrong, 1988; Hirsch, 1995) labeled neurons were drawn using a camera lucida, or a computerized three-dimensional reconstruction system, (Microbrightfield, Cochester, VT). Reconstruction of the electrode tracks revealed that, with the exception of two dendritic recordings, our recordings came from the soma.

RESULTS

We have recorded from 21 simple cells in the adult cat striate cortex. Intracellular labeling allowed us to identify many of the cells from which we recorded; our sample included four layer 6 pyramids, four pyramids at the borders of layer 4, and nine spiny stellate cells in layer 4. Recordings lasted from 20 min to 2.75 hr, normally 30 min to 1 hr. The average time constant of the cells was 21 ± 4.2 msec (range, 15–24 msec). In addition, we have recorded extracellularly from fifteen cells in the lateral geniculate nucleus and measured their responses to the same set of visual stimuli used to study cortex.

Synaptic construction of the receptive field

Flash-evoked responses in the simple receptive field

Simple cells responded to the sparse-noise stimulus in a stereotyped way. Figure 1 shows the synaptic responses to light and to dark stimuli that fell over the peak of the off or the on subregion. The inset over each panel shows the stimulus position in the receptive field (theoff subregion is depicted as concentric bands that grow darker toward the peak; likewise, the on subregion is mapped by contours whose shade lightens as the strength of response increases). Each set of traces shows two individual responses to the stimulus and, beneath these, the averaged responses of all sixteen trials. A dark square presented to the middle of the offsubregion evoked an initial depolarization capped by a train of action potentials. The subsequent hyperpolarization and weak depolarization corresponded to the withdrawal of the dark square (cf. Ohzawa et al., 1996). Accordingly, a light square flashed at the same site evoked a hyperpolarization followed by a depolarizing wave. The responses evoked from the on subregion qualitatively mirrored those elicited from the off subregion. Superimposed on this basic structure, each simple cell had its own distinct behavior. For example, for the neuron illustrated here, responses to dark stimuli were stronger than responses to bright ones. Small variations aside, the push–pull response to squares flashed within a given subregion was common to all cells, provided the resting potential was near the threshold for firing.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

Push and pull in the simple cell response. Postsynaptic responses evoked by dark or bright stimuli that fell inside the off or the on subregion of a simple cell in upper layer 6. Each panel shows two individual responses to the stimulus with the average of all 16 as the boldtrace beneath. The position and sign of the stimulus is indicated in the receptive field map (the peak of the on subregion is light and that of the off subregions is dark; grid spacing is 0.4°) above each panel. Stimulus duration is marked by the bold bar under each trace in this and subsequent figures. A, A dark square flashed in theoff subregion elicited an initial depolarization and late hyperpolarization, top, whereas a bright square at the same site evoked a hyperpolarization and subsequent depolarization,bottom. B, Responses from theon subregion mirror those from theoff.

Comparison of the duration of thalamic and cortical responses

Simple cells receive the bulk of the input from the lateral geniculate nucleus (Lorente de Nó, 1944; Hubel and Wiesel, 1962;LeVay and Gilbert, 1976; Humphrey et al., 1985; Peters and Payne, 1993). To understand better how the primary afferents influence simple cells, we recorded extracellularly from the lateral geniculate during presentation of the sparse noise stimulus. Then we compared the histograms of thalamic activity with the intracellular cortical responses (Table 1). The cortical depolarization evoked by a stimulus of the appropriate sign (onset excitation) peaked while the relay cells were firing (onset activity). Likewise, withdrawal of a stimulus of the opposite sign produced an excitatory response in cortex (offset excitation) that followed the course of thalamic activity (offset activity). The complementary hyperpolarizing phases of the response also tracked thalamic firing, but had longer delays (2–10 msec) than the excitatory components, consistent with a synaptic relay through inhibitory interneurons (see next section of Results). In general, the cortical responses differed from the envelopes of thalamic activity in having somewhat longer durations. The prolongation of cortical responses may have been produced variously by the passive and active properties of the membrane (Rall, 1977; Hirsch, 1995), input from lagged relay cells (not included in our sample) (Mastronarde, 1987; Humphrey and Weller, 1988; Cai et al., 1997), and intracortical synaptic inputs (McGuire et al., 1984;Saint-Marie and Peters, 1985; Douglas et al., 1991; Peters and Payne, 1993; Ahmed et al., 1994; Hirsch, 1995; Cai et al., 1997).

Table 1.

Temporal substructure of cortical and thalamic responses to stimuli of reverse contrast

Simple cells                                              Relay cells
Push                            Pull                      Push        Pull
Onset excitation        Offset suppression    Onset suppression    Offset excitation    Onset activity    Offset activity
Latency Duration Peak Latency Duration Latency Duration Latency Duration Latency Duration Latency Duration
28  62 36  90 120 38 74 112 108 24 37 111  31
23  9 41  92 133 32 80 112 120 23 23 110  68
28  62 38  90  65 29 71 100  90 19 28  64 119
30  50 36  80 38 52  90 122 18 22  87  67
28  82 42 110 28 68  96 204 18 22  81  15
22  60 30  82 293 24 61  85 210 20 27  92  34
20  85 36 105  55 22 73  95 265 24 20 108  19
19  41 28  60 130 30 70 100 105 27 30 100  27
20 102 32 122 256 23 69  92 273 25 27 100  69
20  53 25  73 124 25 85 110 162 22 27  72  54
23.8  ± 4.2  66.6 ± 18.4 34.4 ± 5.5  90.4 ± 18.2 147 ± 84.6 28.9 ± 5.8 70.3 ± 9.2  99.2 ± 97.2 165.9 ± 67.9 22 ± 3.1 26.3 ± 4.9  92.5 ± 16.3  50.3 ± 31.8

The measurements of simple cell behavior came from responses evoked at the peak of the strongest subregion. Values for relay cells derived from responses driven from the center of the field. For cortical responses to stimuli of the appropriate sign (push), the latency, duration, and peak of the initial depolarization (onset excitation) are given; these values are followed by the latency and duration of the succeeding hyperpolarization (offset suppression). For responses to stimuli of the reverse contrast (pull), the latency and duration of the initial hyperpolarization (onset suppression) and offset excitation are provided. Spike histograms rather than intracellular voltage were used to evaluate thalamic responses. The entries in the last row of the table give the mean and SD for each of the 10 simple cells and 10 thalamic relay cells; all values are in milliseconds. (The thalamic neurons included in the table were presumed to be X cells, based on the size of their receptive fields.) Excitatory components are listed in normal typeface, and inhibitory ones are italicized.

Mechanisms of inhibition in the simple receptive field

Two distinct mechanisms, one passive and one active, are thought to contribute to the stimulus-evoked suppression. The idea of a passive contribution stemmed from the observation that cells in the lateral geniculate nucleus have high rates of spontaneous activity (Hubel and Wiesel, 1961) and are excitatory (LeVay and Gilbert, 1976; Hoffman and Stone, 1971; Ferster and Lindström, 1983; Hagihara et al., 1988). Thus, they could contribute a tonic depolarizing component to the resting potential of the simple cell membrane (Hubel and Wiesel, 1962). Presentation of a stimulus of the unmatched sign would lead to a dip in the transmembrane voltage of a simple cell as a consequence of silencing excitatory inputs from the thalamus. Active inhibition could be accounted for by cortical interneurons driven by thalamic afferents (Hubel and Wiesel, 1962; Sillito, 1975; Gilbert and Wiesel, 1979;Heggelund, 1986; Palmer and Davis, 1981; Jones and Palmer, 1987;Ferster, 1988).

We conducted experiments to examine the relative contributions of these active and passive mechanisms. Postsynaptic inhibition would be reflected by an increase in the membrane conductance, whereas the passive mechanism, withdrawal of excitation, would reduce conductance. Furthermore, the shape of a waveform produced by active process would shrink and then reverse sign as the membrane voltage neared and then moved below the reversal potential of the IPSP (EIPSP). By contrast, hyperpolarization would emphasize a withdrawal component because the membrane would be moved farther way from the reversal potential for excitation (EEPSP).

One series of experiments evaluated changes in the membrane resistance during the visually evoked hyperpolarization. For the spiny stellate cell drawn in Figure2A, the membrane conductance nearly doubled during the response to a stimulus of the inappropriate contrast. A dark stimulus presented to the onsubregion produced a robust hyperpolarization; Figure2B shows three individual trials of stimulus and the averaged response in bold. Figure 2C, bottomcompares the averaged response at the control voltage (bold line) with the averaged response to the same stimulus presented while the membrane was held at a relatively hyperpolarized level (dotted trace). These two responses, obtained at two different holding currents, allowed measurement of the change in conductance during the response, g(t) (Fig.2C, top). The conductance as a function of time was calculated as,

g(t)=K(IpIn)/(Vp(t)Vn(t))

where Ip is the amount of current injected through the electrode during the control (relatively depolarized) recording and In is the more hyperpolarized constant current,Vp(t) andVn(t) are the membrane voltages at these two holding currents, and t is the time before, during, and after stimulus presentation. The constant Knormalized the conductance so that g(0) = 100%. Figure3 charts the change in membrane conductance for three other cells. Typically, the change in conductance evoked was between 200 and 300%. The top two traces come from recordings made when QX-314 was present in the recording electrode. Because QX-314 blocks the slow, potassium-mediated IPSP (Otis et al., 1993), it is likely that that much of the conductance increase is caused by the fast, chloride-mediated IPSP.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2.

Visually evoked suppression is accompanied by an increase in the membrane conductance. A, Reconstruction of the cell, a spiny stellate cell in layer 4. B, A dark square flashed within the on subregion evoked a brisk hyperpolarization, as shown in three individual trials of the stimulus and the average of sixteen trials (bold).C, Top trace, Plot of conductance during the visual response: g(t)=K(IpIn)/(Vp(t) − Vn(t));g(t) is the normalized conductance at time t, IpIn is the difference between the values of constant current injected through the electrode, andVp(t) andVn(t) are the membrane voltages recorded while the membrane was held at the relatively positive and negative levels. The averaged responses obtained at the control and hyperpolarized levels that were used to calculateg(t) are shownbelow the graph. Grid spacing was 0.85°.

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3.

The time course of conductance increase during visually evoked suppression in three simple cells.A, B, Records from two layer 4 spiny stellate cells recorded with QX-314 in the electrode (the receptive field of cell B fell slightly outside of the stimulus grid). C, Record from a pyramid at the upper border of layer 4. Grid spacing: A, B, 0. 85°;C, 0. 4°.

We evaluated the relative importance of active and passive processes in generating the visually evoked suppression by determining the voltage dependence of the response to stimuli of the inappropriate contrast. The morphology of a spiny stellate cell in which the visually evoked suppression was reversed is illustrated in Figure4, top. The soma was located in the middle of layer 4; the axon projected to the lower half of layer 2+3 and to layer 6. Each panel in this figure shows somatic recordings made when the membrane was held at different levels of polarization. While the membrane was depolarized with current, the stimulus evoked a prominent hyperpolarization (Fig. 4B). When a moderate amount of hyperpolarizing current was injected, the initial response was slight if visible at all (Fig. 4C). Finally, the application of a stronger current changed the sign of the response from hyperpolarizing to depolarizing (Fig.4D). Of five cells tested, the IPSP was fully reversed in two. In the remaining cells, for suppression evoked by the flashed squares or by moving bars, the equilibrium potential was reached but the recordings ended before the next opportunity to inject stronger currents. All told, these results indicate that intracortical inhibition was the dominant force in generating suppressive responses evoked by stimuli of the opposite contrast; slight contributions made by the withdrawal of tonic excitation might have been present but masked by active processes.

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4.

Reversal of the visually evoked suppression. Reconstruction of the cell, a spiny stellate cell in layer 4 (A). Responses to a stimulus of the inappropriate polarity recorded while the membrane was held at three different levels of polarization. When the membrane was moderately depolarized, the initial response was hyperpolarizing (B). When membrane potential was stepped to more hyperpolarized levels, the response amplitude diminished (C), and then reversed (D). Recordings were actually collected in the order C, B, D; action potentials were suppressed with 10 mm QX-314. Grid spacing was 0.85°.

Interactions between and excitation and inhibition in simple cells

To examine the interactions between excitation and inhibition we used stimuli large enough to span the border between subregions, Figure5. The receptive field of the cell, a layer 6 pyramid (Fig. 5E), had a strong onsubregion and a weaker off subregion. A small stimulus confined to the off subregion evoked a strong depolarization (Fig. 5A, left column). The response to a larger stimulus that spread into a portion of the neighboringon subregion was hyperpolarizing (Fig. 5A,right column). These results indicated that the excitation evoked from the off subregion was overwhelmed by the inhibition elicited from the on subregion. Similar interactions were observed in eight cells recorded when the membrane potential was near the threshold for firing. A wiring diagram illustrating a circuit that could mediate this interaction is shown in Figure 5F. The large and small stimuli are sketched over the fields of two adjacent off-center relay cells that provide input to a pair of inhibitory and excitatory simple cells. The receptive fields of the two simple cells overlap but have an inverse arrangement of on and off subregions. The smaller size of the on subregion of the inhibitory simple cell indicates that this subregion is weaker than the off, a feature that would account for that fact that the cell is well driven by the large, dark stimulus.

Fig. 5.

Fig. 5.

Antagonism between subregions in the simple receptive field. A, Left, A small dark square confined to the off subregion evoked a depolarization. Right, A larger stimulus that spread into the on subregion suppressed the excitation driven through the off subregion. B, The averaged response to the large square (as in A) shown beside the averaged response to an identical stimulus flashed while the membrane was slightly hyperpolarized. The inhibition that dominated theright trace suppressed the excitation visible in theleft trace. C, Responses to large dark (left) and bright (right) squares that avoided the on subregion recorded while the membrane was at the hyperpolarized level. Left, The excitatory component of the response to the overlapping stimulus (i.e.,B, right) matched the excitation evoked by the dark stimulus that fell inside the off subregion.Right, The response to the bright stimulus shows that the membrane voltage had remained above the reversal potential for inhibition. D, Receptive fields and poststimulus time histograms from an extracellularly recorded, off center LGN X cell monitored at the same time as the cortical simple cell. The large spot (right) drove more spikes than the small one did (left), although the large spot fell mainly outside the center. (The subsample of the stimulus grid for the simple cell is shifted down two pixels from that for the relay cell.)E, The cell was a pyramid in layer 6. F, Possible circuit: the receptive field of superimposed inhibitory simple cell whose subregions are reversed compared with those of patched neuron, the fields of antecedent thalamic cells and relative stimulus placement. Grid spacing, 0.4°.

An alternative possibility to explain the suppressive response is that the larger square drove fewer thalamic spikes than the smaller one because the bigger stimulus invaded the suppressive surrounds of the relay cells. Hence, the response to the large spots might reflect this reduced excitatory input to the cortex. The records in panels Figures5BD provide two lines of evidence that reduction in thalamic drive does not account for our observations.

The average response to the large dark spot, recorded at the control level of membrane polarization, was plotted beside the averaged response to the same stimulus presented while the membrane was slightly hyperpolarized (Fig. 5B). At the hyperpolarized potential, the membrane was nearer EIPSP and farther from EEPSP, reducing inhibitory effects while enhancing excitatory currents. At this hyperpolarized potential, a strong excitatory component of the response was revealed. Thus, nearer the threshold for firing, although the net response to the overlapping stimulus was inhibitory, it did contain a strong excitatory component. A remaining question was whether or not the depolarization evoked by the large stimulus was really a reversed IPSP? Figure 5Cshows responses to large bright and dark squares that covered theoff subregion and spilled outside of the receptive field. The response to bright stimuli flashed within the offsubfield evoked a well defined hyperpolarization; the recording had remained above EIPSP.

As described earlier, we often place extracellular electrodes in the lateral geniculate nucleus in addition to the patch pipette in cortex. These recordings have provided additional evidence that the large spots did not lead to suppression of geniculate input. Figure 5Dshows the receptive field of an off center thalamic cell mapped with the small stimulus (left) and the large one (right). The receptive field of the relay cell was located at the upper border of the simple field. Plots of spike rate over time beneath each map show that the large spots were slightly more, rather than less, effective in driving thalamic activity. This occurred even though the large spot fell mainly outside of the center of the thalamic receptive field. In fact, we have not yet seen an instance in which the large spots evoke less activity than the small ones. Our sample contains over 10 similar records and includes examples from both sizes of stimulus grid we use [cells were presumed to be X cells based on the relative size of the receptive field compared with nearby neurons (Alonso et al., 1996; Reid and Alonso, 1996)]. Our results are in keeping with earlier studies that found that a substantial amount of the surround must be recruited to reduce the firing from the center (Hubel and Wiesel, 1961; Cleland and Lee, 1985; Bullier and Norton, 1987). Presumably, the suppressive mechanisms that regulate the response would shift from active inhibition to thalamic withdrawal when larger stimuli that cover more of the surround are used.

The forms of subfield interaction we have described so far can be explained simply by the hyperpolarizing effects of inhibition. Other more subtle responses are consistent with a weak shunt, or veto, mechanism. Figure 6Ashows responses evoked by square that fell within the boundaries of a single subregion. On the right are responses to a stimulus that edged from this subregion into an adjacent one. While the initial depolarizations evoked by the cross-border stimulus were often larger than the ones produced by the confined square, they often failed to produce action potentials. The top trace shows a suprathreshold response, but the threshold voltage was higher than for the two impulses that followed in the late phase of the response. By contrast, although the depolarizations evoked from the home subregion were smaller, they drove spikes or trains of spikes. A similar situation held for the recording shown in Figure 6, C andD; that is, the threshold for firing in the response to the overlapping stimulus was higher. This difference in threshold coincided with a difference in firing rate. During the initial phase of the response depicted in the left records, 9 spikes were produced by the overlapping stimulus and 21 by the confined square, for the cell on the left, the ratio was 17:25. The two plots inset beneath the traces were constructed from responses to pulses of depolarizing current injected through the electrode; they show that spike frequency increased with current strength. Hence, the dissociation between the synaptically induced voltage changes and firing rate is unlikely to reflect intrinsic properties of the neural membrane.

Fig. 6.

Fig. 6.

Alternate forms of subfield antagonism.A, B, Responses to stimuli confined to the home subregion evoked a depolarization that was smaller (A) but more effective in triggering spikes than the larger depolarization evoked by a stimulus that just crossed the border between subregions (B); same cell as in Figure 1. C, D, A second example of the slight dissociation between firing rate and threshold seen when comparing responses evoked within the home subregion (C) to those evoked by a stimulus that crossed the border between subregions; same cell as in Figure 3. Theinsets beneath the traces show that spike frequency increased monotonically with injection of direct depolarizing current pulses for each cell.

DISCUSSION

We have examined mechanisms of excitation and inhibition in the simple cells of the cat primary visual cortex. The temporal structure of the excitatory components of receptive field largely reflected thalamic drive, as judged by comparing thalamic firing patterns to the shape of the depolarizing components of the simple cell response. Active intracortical inhibition, on the other hand, appeared to play the dominant role in generating suppressive responses; suppression was accompanied by a substantial change in the membrane conductance and could be reversed when the membrane was hyperpolarized with current injected through the electrode. Thus, it seems that suppression must be mediated by inhibitory interneurons that contact cells whose subregions have the opposite polarity (Hubel and Wiesel, 1962; Palmer and Davis, 1981, Jones and Palmer, 1987; Ferster, 1988) rather than by reduction in the output of thalamocortical synapses. Furthermore, the dynamic range of inhibitory effects is wide. Inhibition can act powerfully, by summing with concomitant excitatory input to produce a net hyperpolarization, or it can act subtly by influencing firing threshold and rate.

Mechanisms of inhibition

Conductance changes

Our results show directly that inhibition in the simple receptive field is strong (Sillito, 1975, 1992; Heggelund, 1986; Ferster, 1986,1988; Bonds, 1989; DeAngelis et al., 1992; Volgushev et al., 1993;Nelson et al., 1994; Pei et al., 1994; Crook et al., 1997). We have found that the visually evoked suppression is accompanied by a twofold to threefold increase in the membrane conductance. It should be remembered, however, that our stimulus yielded modest responses compared with those that would be produced by oriented bars or gratings. Thus, conductance changes larger than those we have measured should occur routinely. Mechanistic models of normalization processes in cortex (Heeger, 1992; Carandini and Heeger, 1994; Carandini et al., 1997) such as the contrast gain control (Sclar and Freeman, 1982;Ohzawa et al., 1982) call for large, visually evoked changes in the membrane conductance, which, our results suggest exist.

The presence of a conductance increase was not unexpected (Bernander et al., 1991) and has been found in other systems. Blockade of spontaneous inputs to cortical pyramids increases the membrane resistance by 30–70% (Pare et al., 1998). In Purkinje cells, a train of spikes from just one presynaptic interneuron can reduce input resistance by a third (Häusser and Clark, 1997). Some earlier studies of the striate cortex did not detect changes in conductance during visual stimulation (Douglas et al., 1988; Berman et al., 1991; Ferster and Jagadeesh, 1992), although Ferster (1988) demonstrated an active cortical component of inhibitory interactions. These differences in measurement may have come about because some earlier tests were made from cells impaled with sharp electrodes, which themselves introduce a large resting leakage conductance (Staley et al., 1992) or from difficulties associated with the high access resistances involved in patch recording from tissue (Stühmer et al., 1983; Edwards and Konnerth, 1992).

Independence of excitation and inhibition

The visually evoked suppression could be reversed by current injection, suggesting that it is mediated by intracortical inhibition. A small part of an earlier study led to the conclusion that withdrawal of thalamic drive contributed strongly to visual responses (Ferster, 1988). In that study, thalamic afferents were electrically activated to produce a monosynaptic EPSP and disynaptic IPSP; at the resting potential, the shock-evoked response lacked a hyperpolarizing component so the resting potential was equated with EIPSP.Visually driven hyperpolarizations from rest were taken to reflect thalamic withdrawal because active inhibition was thought to be silent. There are two concerns with this interpretation. First, it seems that the resting potential was actually above EIPSP; cells fired spontaneously at that level, whereas EIPSP, by definition, falls below spike threshold. Second, recent work has shown that activating the primary afferents will disynaptically inhibit the geniculate (Lo and Sherman, 1994; Bal et al., 1995) via feedback from the perigeniculate (Friedlander et al., 1981; Dubin and Cleland, 1977) as well as the cortex. Thus, the absence of cortical hyperpolarization after shocks to the primary afferents, during the time when the geniculate would be silenced by the perigeniculate, suggests that withdrawal of thalamic input does not markedly influence the cortical membrane potential.

The results presented here indicate that inhibition in simple cells is postsynaptic and, therefore, mediated by synapses distinct from those that provide excitation. The excitation is laid out by thalamocortical inputs (Hubel and Wiesel, 1962; Tanaka, 1983, 1985; Alonso et al., 1996; Reid and Alonso, 1996; Ferster et al., 1996; Cai et al., 1997; Chung and Ferster, 1997; Table 1, this manuscript) and likely amplified by intracortical inputs (Saint-Marie and Peters, 1985;Douglas et al., 1991; Ahmed et al., 1994; Hirsch, 1995; Cai et al., 1997; Chung and Ferster, 1997). Inhibition is largely, or perhaps wholly, provided by inhibitory neurons apparently driven by stimuli of the reverse contrast. This separation of excitation and inhibition may give the cortex a wider dynamic range in which to negotiate its inputs (Troyer et al., 1998).

Interactions between excitation and inhibition

To understand the synaptic interactions that work to reduce responsiveness to suboptimal stimuli, we examined the patterns of excitation and inhibition evoked by stimuli that crossed the border between subregions. Our results indicate that inhibition regulates excitatory input in two ways, by means of hyperpolarizing the membrane or by effectively changing firing threshold or rate.

Net hyperpolarization

In many instances the inhibition driven through one subregion opposed the excitation driven through the other so strongly that the net response was hyperpolarizing. This observation bears on previous work that advanced the idea of cross-orientation inhibition. Several studies have reported that a cross-oriented bar presented in the simple receptive field led to inhibition (Bishop et al., 1973; Morrone et al., 1982; Pei et al., 1994). Our results indicate that subfield antagonism alone may account for the suppressive action of an orthogonal bar. It also follows that the structure of the receptive fields of the presynaptic inhibitory neurons need not be the precise mirror images of their postsynaptic targets (Fig. 5F, see circuit). That is, the interneurons can be driven to fire by the same stimuli that effectively suppress the cells they contact.

Modulation of firing behavior

A second, more subtle, action was revealed when the inhibition was too weak to hyperpolarize the membrane outright. Instead, the mild inhibition seemed to elevate slightly the level of somatic depolarization required to produce an action potential or to slow the rate of firing. In the cat auditory brainstem, intracellular responses to tonal stimuli also reveal a dissociation between somatic membrane voltage and spike threshold and rate (Rhode et al., 1983). Additionally, records from the turtle visual cortex show that temporal distribution of component IPSPs regulate the efficacy of depolarizing compound synaptic responses (Colombe and Ulinski, 1996; Mancilla and Ulinski, 1996). It is possible that the inhibition shunts somatic excitation before it spreads to the site where the full-blown spike is produced, presumably in the proximal axon (Colbert and Johnston, 1996). This scheme is plausible because excitatory synapses are normally distributed on the dendrites, whereas inhibitory synapses are dense around the soma and axon hillock (LeVay, 1973; Fairén et al., 1983; McGuire et al., 1984; Ahmed et al., 1994). Alternatively, the inhibition could elevate the threshold directly by increasing conductance at the zone of spike initiation (Colbert and Johnston, 1996). Although these effects are small at the level of a single cell, they may gain weight when propagated through the circuit. Different rates of firing influence the success of transmission across the synapse (Thomson and West, 1993; Allen and Stevens, 1994; Dobrunz and Stevens, 1997; Tsodyks and Markram, 1997) as well as the integration of inputs by the postsynaptic neuron (Bernander et al., 1991; Shadlen and Newsome, 1994; Colombe and Ulinski, 1996; Mancilla and Ulinski, 1996;Häusser and Clark, 1997; Mancilla et al., 1998).

To conclude, the spatial segregation of on andoff responses in the simple receptive field has given the opportunity to study excitatory and inhibitory inputs separately and to examine the mechanisms of their interaction when recruited in various balance. Our aim is to apply what we have learned from simple cells to studies of synaptic integration at higher cortical levels.

Footnotes

This work was supported by National Institutes of Health Grants EY09593 (J.A.H.) and EY05253 (T.N.W.), the Klingenstein Fund (J.A.H., R.C.R.), and the Human Frontiers Science Program Organization (L.M.M.). We are grateful to Torsten N. Wiesel for support and guidance during all phases of the project. Christine A. Gallagher, Kathleen McGowan, Johanna L. Kornblum, and Komal A. Desai provided superb technical support and drew the labeled cells, which Peter Peirce photographed precisely. We thank Sanford M. Simon for improving the manuscript, Matteo Carandini for advice in analyzing conductance changes, and S. Murray Sherman for helpful criticism of an earlier draft.

Correspondence should be addressed to: Judith A. Hirsch, Box 138, Laboratory of Neurobiology, The Rockefeller University, 1230 York Avenue, New York, NY 10021.

REFERENCES

  • 1.Ahmed B, Anderson JC, Douglas RJ, Martin KM, Nelson JC. Polyneuronal innervation of spiny stellate neurons in cat visual cortex. J Comp Neurol. 1994;341:39–49. doi: 10.1002/cne.903410105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Allen C, Stevens CF. An evaluation of causes of unreliability of synaptic transmission. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1994;9:10380–10383. doi: 10.1073/pnas.91.22.10380. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Alonso JM, Usrey WM, Reid RC. Precisely correlated firing in cells of the lateral geniculate nucleus. Nature. 1996;383:815–819. doi: 10.1038/383815a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Bal T, von Krosigk M, McCormick DA. Synaptic and membrane mechanisms underlying synchronized oscillations in the ferret lateral geniculate nucleus in vitro. J Physiol (Lond) 1995;483:641–663. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1995.sp020612. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Berman NJ, Douglas RJ, Martin KAC, Whitteridge D. Mechanisms of inhibition in cat visual cortex. J Physiol (Lond) 1991;440:697–722. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1991.sp018731. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Bernander O, Douglas RJ, Martin KM, Koch C. Synaptic background activity influences spatiotemporal integration in single pyramidal cells. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1991;88:11569–11573. doi: 10.1073/pnas.88.24.11569. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Bishop PO, Coombs JS, Henry GH. Receptive fields of simple cells in the cat striate cortex. J Physiol (Lond) 1973;231:31–60. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1973.sp010218. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Blanton M, LoTurco JJ, Kriegstein AR. Whole-cell recording from neurons in slices of reptilian and mammalian cerebral cortex. J Neurosci Methods. 1989;30:203–210. doi: 10.1016/0165-0270(89)90131-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Bonds AB. Role of inhibition in the specification of orientation selectivity of cells in the cat striate cortex. Vis Neurosci. 1989;2:41–55. doi: 10.1017/s0952523800004314. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Bullier J, Norton TT. Comparison of receptive-field properties of X and Y ganglion cells with X and Y lateral geniculate cells in the cat. J Neurophysiol. 1987;42:274–291. doi: 10.1152/jn.1979.42.1.274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Cai D, DeAngelis GC, Freeman RD. Spatiotemporal receptive field organization in the lateral geniculate nucleus of cats and kittens. J Neurophysiol. 1997;78:1045–1061. doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.78.2.1045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Carandini M, Heeger D. Summation and division by neurons in visual cortex. Science. 1994;264:1333–1336. doi: 10.1126/science.8191289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Carandini M, Heeger DJ, Movshon JA. Linearity and normalization in simple cells of the macaque primary visual cortex. J Neurosci. 1997;17:8621–8644. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-21-08621.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Chung S, Ferster D. The size of thalamic input to simple cells of the visual cortex. Soc Neurosci Abstr. 1997;23:2059. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Cleland BG, Lee BB. A comparison of visual responses of cat lateral geniculate nucleus neurones with those of ganglion cells afferent to them. J Physiol (Lond) 1985;369:249–268. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1985.sp015899. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Colbert CM, Johnston D. Axonal action-potential initiation and Na+ channel densities in the soma and axon initial segment of subicular pyramidal neurons. J Neurosci. 1996;16:6676–6686. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-21-06676.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Colombe JB, Ulinski PS. Temporal integration windows for neurons in the feedforward pathways of visual cortex. Soc Neurosci Abstr. 1996;22:284. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Crook JM, Kisvarday ZF, Eysel UT. GABA-induced inactivation of functionally characterized sites in cat striate cortex: effects on orientation tuning and direction selectivity. Vis Neurosci. 1997;14:141–158. doi: 10.1017/s095252380000883x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.DeAngelis GC, Robson JG, Ohzawa I, Freeman RD. Organization of suppression in receptive fields of neurons in cat visual cortex. J Neurophysiol. 1992;68:144–163. doi: 10.1152/jn.1992.68.1.144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Debanne D, Shulz D, Fregnac Y. Activity dependent on and off responses in cat visual cortical receptive fields. J Physiol (Lond) 1998;508:523–548. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.1998.00523.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Dobrunz LE, Stevens CF. Heterogeneity of release probability, facilitation, and depletion at central synapses. Neuron. 1997;18:995–1008. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80338-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Douglas RJ, Martin KAC, Whitteridge D. Selective responses of visual cortical cells do not depend on shunting inhibition. Nature. 1988;332:642–644. doi: 10.1038/332642a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Douglas RJ, Martin KAC, Whitteridge D. An intracellular analysis of the visual responses of neurones in cat visual cortex. J Physiol (Lond) 1991;440:659–696. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1991.sp018730. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Dubin MW, Cleland BG. Organization of visual inputs to interneurons of lateral geniculate nucleus of the cat. J Neurophysiol. 1977;40:410–427. doi: 10.1152/jn.1977.40.2.410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Edwards FA, Konnerth T. Patch-clamping cells in sliced tissue preparations. In Methods in Enzymology. 1992;207:208–222. doi: 10.1016/0076-6879(92)07015-g. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Edwards FA, Konnerth A, Sakmann B, Takahashi T. A thin slice preparation for patch clamp recordings from neurones of the mammalian central nervous system. Pflügers Arch. 1989;414:600–612. doi: 10.1007/BF00580998. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Fairén A, DeFelipe J, Regidor J. Nonpyramidal neurons: general account in cerebral cortex: cellular components of the cerebral cortex, Vol 1, (Peters A, Jones EG, eds) pp 201–253. Plenum; New York: 1983. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Ferster D. Orientation selectivity of synaptic potentials in neurons of cat primary visual cortex. J Neurosci. 1986;6:1284–1301. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.06-05-01284.1986. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Ferster D. Spatially opponent excitation and inhibition in simple cells of the cat visual cortex. J Neurosci. 1988;8:1172–1180. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.08-04-01172.1988. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Ferster D, Jagadeesh B. EPSP-IPSP interactions in cat visual cortex studied with in vivo whole-cell patch recording. J Neurosci. 1992;14:1262–1274. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.12-04-01262.1992. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Ferster D, Lindström S. An intracellular analysis of geniculo-cortical connectivity in area 17 of the cat. J Physiol (Lond) 1983;342:181–215. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1983.sp014846. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Ferster D, Chung S, Wheat H. Orientation selectivity of thalamic input to simple cells of cat visual cortex. Nature. 1996;380:249–252. doi: 10.1038/380249a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Friedlander MJ, Lin CS, Stanford LR, Sherman SM. Morphology of functionally identified neurons in lateral geniculate nucleus of the cat. J Neurophysiol. 1981;46:80–129. doi: 10.1152/jn.1981.46.1.80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Gilbert CD, Wiesel TN. Morphology and intracortical projections of functionally identified neurons in cat visual cortex. Nature. 1979;280:120–125. doi: 10.1038/280120a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Hagihara K, Tsumoto T, Sato H, Hata Y. Actions of excitatory amino acid antagonists on geniculo-cortical transmission in the cat’s visual cortex. Exp Brain Res. 1988;69:407–416. doi: 10.1007/BF00247586. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Hamill OP, Marty A, Neher E, Sakmann B, Sigworth FJ. Improved patch clamp technique for high resolution current recording from cells and cell free patches. Pflügers Arch. 1981;391:85–100. doi: 10.1007/BF00656997. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Hausser M, Clark BA. Tonic synaptic inhibition modulates neuronal output pattern and spatiotemporal synaptic integration. Neuron. 1997;19:665–678. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80379-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Heeger DJ. Normalization of cell responses in cat visual cortex. Vis Neurosci. 1992;9:181–198. doi: 10.1017/s0952523800009640. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Heggelund P. Receptive field organization of simple cells in cat striate cortex. Exp Brain Res. 1981;42:82–98. doi: 10.1007/BF00235733. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Heggelund P. Quantitative studies of enhancement and suppression zones in the receptive fields of simple cells in cat striate cortex. J Physiol (Lond) 1986;373:293–310. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1986.sp016048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Hirsch JA. Synaptic integration in layer 4 of the ferret visual cortex. J Physiol (Lond) 1995;481:247–262. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1995.sp020577. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Hirsch JA, Alonso JM, Reid RC. Two dimensional spatial maps of excitation and inhibition in the simple receptive field. Soc Neurosci Abstr. 1995;21:21. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Hoffman KP, Stone J. Conduction velocity of afferents to cat visual cortex: a correlation with cortical receptive field properties. Brain Res. 1971;32:460–466. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(71)90340-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Horikawa K, Armstrong WE. A versatile means of labeling: injection of biocytin and its detection with avidin conjugates. J Neurosci Methods. 1988;25:1–11. doi: 10.1016/0165-0270(88)90114-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Hubel DH, Wiesel TN. Integrative action in the cat’s lateral geniculate body. J Physiol (Lond) 1961;155:385–398. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1961.sp006635. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Hubel DH, Wiesel TN. Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat’s visual cortex. J Physiol (Lond) 1962;160:106–154. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1962.sp006837. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Humphrey AL, Weller RE. Structural correlates of functionally distinct X-cells in the lateral geniculate nucleus of the cat. J Comp Neurol. 1988;268:448–468. doi: 10.1002/cne.902680312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Humphrey AL, Sur M, Ulrich DJ, Sherman SM. Projection patterns of individual X and Y cell axons from the lateral geniculate nucleus to cortical areas 17 and 18. J Comp Neurol. 1985;233:159–189. doi: 10.1002/cne.902330203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Jones JP, Palmer LA. The two-dimensional spatial structure of simple receptive fields in cat striate cortex. J Neurophysiol. 1987;58:1187–1211. doi: 10.1152/jn.1987.58.6.1187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.LeVay S. Synaptic patterns in the visual cortex of the cat and monkey: Electron microscopy of Golgi preparations. J Comp Neurol. 1973;150:53–86. doi: 10.1002/cne.901500104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.LeVay S, Gilbert CD. Laminar patterns of geniculocortical projection in the cat. Brain Res. 1976;113:1–19. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(76)90002-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Lo FS, Sherman SM. Feedback inhibition in the cat’s lateral geniculate nucleus. Exp Brain Res. 1994;100:365–368. doi: 10.1007/BF00227207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Lorente de Nó R. Cerebral cortex: architecture, intracortical connections, motor projections. In: Fulton JF, editor. Physiology of the nervous system. Oxford UP; London: 1944. pp. 291–325. [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Malinow R, Tsien RW. Presynaptic enhancement shown by whole-cell recordings of long-term potentiation in hippocampal slices. Nature. 1990;346:177–180. doi: 10.1038/346177a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Mancilla JG, Ulinski PS. Temporal structure of compound postsnaptic potentials in visual cortex. In: Bower J, editor. Computational neuroscience: trends in research. Academic; San Diego: 1996. pp. 227–232. [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Mancilla JG, Fowler M, Ulinski PS. Responses of regular spiking and fast spiking cells in turtle visual cortex to light flashes. Vis Neurosci. 1998;15:979–993. doi: 10.1017/s0952523898155190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Mastronarde DN. Two classes of single-input X-cells in cat lateral geniculate nucleus. I. Retinal inputs and the generation of receptive-field properties. J Neurophysiol. 1987;57:357–380. doi: 10.1152/jn.1987.57.2.357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.McGuire B, Hornung JP, Gilbert CD, Wiesel TN. Patterns of synaptic input of layer 4 of the cat striate cortex. J Neurosci. 1984;4:3021–3033. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.04-12-03021.1984. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Miller KD. A model for the development of simple cell receptive fields and the ordered arrangement of orientation columns through activity-dependent competition between ON- and OFF-center inputs. J Neurosci. 1994;14:409–4041. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-01-00409.1994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Morrone MC, Burr DC, Maffei L. Functional implications of cross-orientation of cortical visual cells. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1982;216:335–354. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1982.0078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Movshon JA, Thompson ID, Tolhurst DJ. Spatial summation in the receptive fields of simple cells in the cat’s striate cortex. J Physiol (Lond) 1978;283:53–77. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1978.sp012488. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Nelson S, Toth L, Sheth B, Sur M. Orientation selectivity of cortical neuron during intracellular blockade of inhibition. Science. 1994;265:74–77. doi: 10.1126/science.8047882. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Ohzawa I, Sclar G, Freeman R. Contrast gain control in the cat visual cortex. Nature. 1982;298:266–268. doi: 10.1038/298266a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Ohzawa I, DeAngelis GC, Freeman RD. Encoding of binocular disparity by simple cells in the cat’s visual cortex. J Neurophysiol. 1996;75:1779–1805. doi: 10.1152/jn.1996.75.5.1779. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Otis TS, De Koninck Y, Mody I. Characterization of synaptically elicited GABAb responses using patch-clamp recordings in rat hippocampal slices. J Physiol (Lond) 1993;463:391–407. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1993.sp019600. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Palmer LA, Davis TL. Receptive-field structure in cat striate cortex. J Neurophysiol. 1981;46:260–276. doi: 10.1152/jn.1981.46.2.260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Pare D, Shink E, Gaudreau H, Destexhe A, Lang E. Impact of spontaneous synaptic activity on the resting properties of cat neocortical pyramidal neurons in vivo. J Neurophysiol. 1998;79:1450–1460. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.79.3.1450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Pei X, Vidyasagar TR, Volgushev M, Creutzfeldt OD. Whole-cell recording and conductance measurements on cat visual cortex in vivo. NeuroReport. 1991;2:485–488. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199108000-00019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Pei X, Vidyasagar TR, Volgushev M, Creutzfeldt OD. Receptive field analysis and orientation selectivity of postsynaptic potentials of simple cells in cat visual cortex. J Neurosci. 1994;14:7130–7140. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-11-07130.1994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Peters A, Payne BR. Numerical relationships between geniculocortical afferents and pyramidal cell modules in primary visual cortex. Cereb Cortex. 1993;3:69–78. doi: 10.1093/cercor/3.1.69. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Rall W. Handbook of physiology. The nervous system. Cellular biology of neurons. Sect 1, Vol 1, Part 1, pp 39–97. American Physiology Society; Bethesda, MD: 1977. Core conductor theory and cable properties of neurons. [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Reid RC, Alonso JM. The precision of monosynaptic connections from the thalamus to visual cortex. Nature. 1996;378:281–284. doi: 10.1038/378281a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Rhode WS, Smith PH, Oertel D. Physiological response properties of cells labeled intracellularly with horseradish peroxidase in cat dorsal cochlear nucleus. J Comp Neurol. 1983;213:426–447. doi: 10.1002/cne.902130407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Saint Marie RL, Peters A. The morphology and synaptic connections of spiny stellate neurons in monkey visual cortex (area 17): a Golgi-electron microscopic study. J Comp Neurol. 1985;233:213–235. doi: 10.1002/cne.902330205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Sclar G, Freeman RD. Orientation selectivity in the cat’s striate cortex is invariant with stimulus contrast. Exp Brain Res. 1982;46:457–461. doi: 10.1007/BF00238641. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Shadlen MN, Newsome WT. Noise, neural codes and cortical organization. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1994;4:569–579. doi: 10.1016/0959-4388(94)90059-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Sillito AM. The contribution of inhibitory mechanisms to the receptive field properties of neurons in the striate cortex of the cat. J Physiol (Lond) 1975;250:305–329. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1975.sp011056. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Sillito AM. GABA-mediated inhibitory processes in the function of the geniculo-striate system. Prog Brain Res. 1992;90:349–384. doi: 10.1016/s0079-6123(08)63622-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Skottun BC, DeValois RL, Grosof DH, Movshon JA, Albrecht DG, Bonds AB. Classifying simple and complex cells on the basis of response modulation. Vision Res. 1991;31:1079–1086. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(91)90033-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Staley KJ, Otis TS, Mody I. Membrane properties of dentate gyrus granule cells: comparison of sharp microelectrode and whole-cell recordings. J Neurophysiol. 1992;67:1346–1358. doi: 10.1152/jn.1992.67.5.1346. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Stühmer A, Roberts M, Almers W. The loose patch clamp. In: Sakmann B, Neher E, editors. Single channel recording. Plenum; New York: 1983. pp. 123–132. [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Tanaka K. Cross-correlation analysis of geniculostriate neuronal relationships in cats. J Neurophysiol. 1983;49:1303–1318. doi: 10.1152/jn.1983.49.6.1303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Tanaka K. Organization of geniculate inputs to visual cortical cells in the cat. Vision Res. 1985;25:357–364. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(85)90060-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Thomson AM, West DC. Fluctuation in pyramid-pyramid excitatory postsynaptic potentials modified by presynaptic firing pattern and postsynaptic membrane potential using paired intracellular recordings in rat neocortex. Neuroscience. 1993;54:329–346. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(93)90256-f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Troyer TW, Krukowski A, Priebe NJ, Miller KD. Contrast-invariant orientation tuning in cat visual cortex: feedforward tuning and correlation-based intracortical connectivity. J Neurosci. 1998;18:5908–5927. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-15-05908.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Tsodyks MV, Markram H. The neural code between neocortical pyramidal neurons depends on neurotransmitter release probability. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1997;94:719–723. doi: 10.1073/pnas.94.2.719. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Volgushev M, Pei X, Vidyasagar TR, Creutzfeldt OD. Excitation and inhibition in orientation selectivity of cat visual cortex neurons revealed by whole-cell recordings in vivo. Vis Neurosci. 1993;10:1151–1155. doi: 10.1017/s0952523800010257. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from The Journal of Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Society for Neuroscience

RESOURCES