Abstract
There are multiple well-established evidence-based treatments for posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). However, recent clinical trials have shown that combat-related PTSD in military populations is less responsive to evidence-based treatments than PTSD in most civilian populations. Traumatic death of a close friend or colleague is a common deployment-related experience for active duty military personnel. When compared with research on trauma and PTSD in general, research on traumatic loss suggests that it is related to higher prevalence and severity of PTSD symptoms. Experiencing a traumatic loss is also related to the development of prolonged grief disorder, which is highly comorbid with depression. This study examined the relationship between having traumatic loss-related PTSD and treatment response to Cognitive Processing Therapy in active duty military personnel. Participants included 213 active duty service members recruited across two randomized clinical trials. Results showed that service members with primary traumatic loss-related PTSD (n = 44) recovered less from depressive symptoms than those with different primary traumatic events (n = 169). Tests of mediation found that less depression recovery suppressed recovery from PTSD symptoms in those with traumatic loss-related PTSD. These findings suggest that evidence-based treatments for PTSD should better accommodate loss and grief in military populations.
Although there are well-established evidence-based treatments for PTSD, recent studies have found that military service members with combat-related PTSD do not respond as robustly to those treatments as their civilian counterparts. In fact, about one half of active duty military members retain a PTSD diagnosis after completing evidence-based treatment for PTSD (Cigrang et al., 2017; Foa et al., 2018; Resick et al., 2015, 2017) compared with less than 20% of civilian women and men (Galovski, Blain, Mott, Elwood, & Houle, 2012; Resick et al., 2002, 2012).
In order to improve these response rates and better serve military service members with PTSD, the factors that contribute to this discrepancy in recovery rates need to be explored. One factor that may contribute to less robust recovery in active duty and veteran populations is the uniqueness of warzone exposure among combatants and the frequency and types of traumatic events endorsed by service members. Previous research by the STRONG STAR Consortium (Litz et al., 2018) showed that reports of common deployment-related traumatic events can be grouped into at least six distinct categories, each with unique peri-event and post-event response patterns: traumatic loss, life threat to self, life threat to others, moral injury by self, moral injury by others, and aftermath of battle. Experiencing the traumatic loss of a close friend or colleague is a very prevalent and distressing category of trauma that service members often encounter. For example, in a study conducted by Hoge et al. (2004), 86% - 87% of active duty service members deployed to Iraq and 43% of service members deployed to Afghanistan knew someone who was killed or seriously injured during combat. Litz and colleagues found that 20% of 999 active duty military assessed for participation in a PTSD clinical trial endorsed a traumatic loss as the traumatic event that bothers them most, defined by the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 4th ed. (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association, 2000). This is not surprising given the high risk of danger coupled with the magnified and highly interdependent nature of military life during combat deployments. Losing a service member from one’s own unit can be similar to losing a close family member. Service members are also at risk for losing unit members even after returning home from suicide (U.S. Department of Veteran Affairs, 2016).
Experiencing a traumatic loss appears to be associated with increased risk of PTSD. Although the overall risk of developing PTSD following trauma exposure is approximately 9% (Breslau et al., 1998), research in civilian samples has found higher rates of PTSD development related to traumatic loss, ranging from 22% - 35% (Amick-McMullan, Kilpatrick, & Resnick, 1991; Boelen, Keijser, & Smid, 2015; Green et al., 2001). Additionally, Breslau and colleagues found that the event that was most often reported as a most distressing traumatic event (31% of all PTSD cases) was the sudden, unexpected death of a loved one. Finally, in a large sample of bereaved civilians, those who experienced violent loss of loved ones had significantly higher symptom levels of PTSD than persons bereaved by nonviolent losses (Boelen et al., 2015).
Compared with civilians (e.g., Boelen et al., 2015), less is known about the mental and behavioral health impact of traumatic loss in military samples. A recent study (Litz et al., 2018) found that treatment-seeking service members with traumatic loss index events reported higher levels of reexperincing symptoms, avoidance, and guilt as compared with service members with life threat events. Similarly, Simon et al. (2018) examined the relationship between loss, complicated grief, and PTSD symptoms in service members and veterans. They found that losing a fellow service member (endorsed by 41% of the sample) was associated with a higher prevalence of complicated grief, which was then related to significantly greater PTSD severity, functional impairment, trauma-related guilt, and lifetime suicide attempts. Simon and colleagues recommended additional research on the topic and proposed that clinical severity of disorders with partially overlapping symptoms, such as depression, may serve as a clinical severity marker for those with PTSD who were exposed to a death. They in turn called for treatment outcome research to attend to this subpopulation of those with PTSD.
Depression and PTSD
PTSD and depression are highly comorbid and, as Simon and colleagues (2018) discussed, have overlapping symptoms. Epidemiological studies show 48% of individuals with a lifetime diagnosis of PTSD have comorbid depression (Kessler, Sonnega, Bromet, Hughes, & Nelson, 1995) with similar rates (44% for males and 56% for females) in veterans (Kulka et al., 1990). Although there have been no population comorbidity studies for post-9/11 military populations, a large (N = 309) study of U.S. veterans who had deployed to Iraq, Afghanistan, and surrounding locations, found 39% of those with current PTSD also having current major depression (Kimbrel, Meyer, DeBeer, Gulliver, & Morissette, 2016). It appears that comorbid depression impacts the natural recovery process following a trauma. One longitudinal study of civilians found that, following a traumatic event, higher symptoms of depression at each time point (measured at 1 week, 1 month, and 4 months) was related to a worsening of PTSD symptoms at the next time point (King, King, McArdle, Shalev, & Doron-LaMarca, 2009). Follow-up data from a large epidemiological study found that, among survivors of a large earthquake who had PTSD, those with comorbid major depression (MDD) were less likely to recover from PTSD 3 years later (26.4% recovery rate) than those without MDD (47.4% recovery rate; Tural, Önder, & Aker, 2012).
Treatment studies show similar findings. For example, Haagen, ter Heide, Mooren, Knipscheer, and Kleber (2017) found that the presence and severity of a comorbid depressive disorder predicted poorer PTSD treatment outcome in a sample of refugees engaged in Eye Movement Desensitization and Reprocessing (EMDR) therapy. Reduction in PTSD symptoms over the course of Cognitive Processing Therapy (CPT) was also correlated with reduction in symptoms of depression (Galovski et al., 2016; Liverant, Suvak, Pineles, & Resick, 2012), and sudden improvements in depression during treatment were related to better PTSD treatment response (Keller, Feeny, & Zoellner, 2014). To our knowledge, only one treatment study has examined the relationship between depression and PTSD recovery in a veteran sample, finding that reduction in symptoms of depression preceded reduction in symptoms of PTSD in veterans engaged in CPT in a residential setting (Schumm, Dickstein, Walter, Owens, & Chard, 2015).
Traumatic Loss and Depression
Depression and PTSD are separate constructs from the abnormal sequelae of traumatic loss, namely traumatic grief (interchangeably used here and in the literature with prolonged grief disorder [PGD]; Boelen, van den Bout, & de Keijser, 2003; Jordan & Litz, 2014; Prigerson et al., 1996). However, the symptoms of PGD and depression have considerable overlap and are highly comorbid. In a confirmatory factor analysis study, Boelen and van den Bout (2005) found that traumatic grief was highly correlated with symptoms of depression (r = .78) and that several core symptoms (feelings of worthlessness, self-blame, and experiencing a lack of meaning in the world) could not separate depression from traumatic grief in an initial factor analysis. In a study examining the mediating variables in the relationship between violent (vs. nonviolent) loss and PTSD, PGD, and depression (Boelen et al., 2015), the same combination of mediators were found for the relationship between violent loss and both PGD and depression (i.e., unrealness, negative cognitions about the self and the future, catastrophic misinterpretations, depressive avoidance). In comparison, anxious avoidance uniquely mediated the relationship between violent loss and PTSD. Although not everyone with a traumatic loss goes on to develop PGD, there is a higher risk of developing PGD among individuals who experience violent, sudden, or unexpected loss than among those who experience loss from forseeable and natural causes (Boelen et al., 2015; Lobb, Kristjanson, & Aoun, 2010). Thus, if PTSD treatment outcomes are associated with outcomes on symptoms of depression (Galovski et al., 2016; Haagen et al., 2017; Keller et al., 2014; Liverant et al., 2012) and depression is highly comorbid with PGD, those with traumatic loss (who are at risk for experiencing symptoms of traumatic grief, and, therefore, symptoms of depression) would have a poorer prognosis for PTSD recovery following treatment.
To our knowledge, no studies have examined whether the presence of Criterion-A events that entail traumatic loss are associated with worse outcomes from evidence-based treatments among service members with PTSD, nor whether symptoms of depression affect the relationship between traumatic loss and PTSD symptom improvement with treatment. The current study examined the relationship between experiencing a traumatic loss index event (i.e., the event reported as currently most distressing) and treatment response to CPT in an active duty military sample. Additionally, this study examined whether symptoms of depression mediated that association. Given the literature suggesting that military service members with traumatic loss experience more severe baseline PTSD symptoms than those with life threat index traumas (Litz et al., 2018) and that loss of a fellow service member is related to higher levels of psychopathology than other types of loss (Simon et al., 2018), we hypothesized that participants who experienced a traumatic loss as their index trauma would experience less robust recovery from PTSD compared to participants with other types of index traumas. Given the research suggesting the negative effects of depression on PTSD recovery overall, we further hypothesized that the relationship between traumatic loss and PTSD recovery would be mediated by smaller changes in the traumatic loss group in symptoms of depression from baseline to posttreatment.
Method
Participants and Procedures
Data for this study was taken from two large randomized clinical trials (MASKED FOR REVIEW; MASKED FOR REVIEW) which were approved by all appropriate Institutional Review Boards (MASKED FOR REVIEW). Participants were 213 active duty U.S. military personnel who had experienced at least one deployment, met diagnostic criteria for PTSD according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disoders, Fourth Edition, Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR; American Psychiatric Association, 2000) during the initial assessment, and who completed either individual (n = 83) or group (n = 130) CPT (Resick, Monson, & Chard, 2016) as part of two large randomized clinical trials. See (MASKED FOR REVIEW) and (MASKED FOR REVIEW) for a full description of these studies. Table 1 lists the demographics and descriptive statistics on the main study variables for the overall sample and for the groups as derived from the mutually exclusive categories described below.
Table 1.
Demographic Characteristics and Descriptive Statistics for Main Study Variables
Characteristic | Total Sample (N = 213) |
Primary Index Event |
χ2, t | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Traumatic Loss (n = 44) |
Other Event (n = 169) |
||||
Age | 33.95 (7.21) | 32.86 (7.01) | 33.49 (6.73) | 0.46 | 0.64 |
Male | 195 (92%) | 44 (100%) | 151 (89%) | 5.12 | 0.023 |
Married/Cohabitating | 158 (74%) | 32 (73%) | 126 (75%) | 0.06 | 0.81 |
Race/Ethnicity | 0.64 | 0.88 | |||
Black | 54 (26%) | 12 (27%) | 42 (25%) | ||
Hispanic | 41 (19%) | 7 (16%) | 34 (20%) | ||
White | 98 (46%) | 20 (46%) | 78 (46%) | ||
Other | 20 (9%) | 5 (9%) | 15 (9%) | ||
Education | 1.05 | 0.57 | |||
High school or less | 55 (26%) | 14 (32%) | 41 (24%) | ||
Some college/ Associate degree | 143 (67%) | 27 (61%) | 116 (69%) | ||
College/Graduate degree | 15 (7%) | 3 (7%) | 12 (7%) | ||
Months in service | 135.64 (75.62) | 133.83 (75.96) | 142.59 (74.69) | 0.68 | 0.49 |
Enlisted Rank | 206 (97%) | 42 (96%) | 164 (97%) | 0.28 | 0.64 |
Typical Duty | 0.62 | 0.74 | |||
Combat Arms | 74 (35%) | 17 (39%) | 57 (34%) | ||
Combat Support | 47 (22%) | 8 (18%) | 39 (23%) | ||
Combat Service Support | 92 (43%) | 19 (43%) | 73 (43%) | ||
Number of deployments | 10.76 | 0.01 | |||
1 | 51 (24%) | 5 (11%) | 46 (27%) | ||
2 | 77 (36%) | 24 (55%) | 53 (31%) | ||
3 | 52 (24%) | 7 (16%) | 45 (27%) | ||
4+ | 33 (16%) | 8 (18%) | 25 (15%) | ||
Main Study Measures (M, SD) | |||||
PCL-S (Baseline) | 55.19 (10.41) | 55.61 (12.21) | 55.08 (9.93) | 0.30 | 0.76 |
PCL-S (Follow-up) | 45.27 (15.40) | 44.93 (15.63) | 45.36 (15.38) | 0.16 | 0.87 |
BDI-II (Baseline) | 28.94 (11.13) | 27.59 (11.48) | 29.29 (11.04) | 0.90 | 0.37 |
BDI-II (Follow-up) | 21.48 (14.14) | 23.75 (15.46) | 20.89 (13.26) | 1.20 | 0.23 |
Note. PCL-S = PTSD Checklist – Stressor Specific; BDI-II = Beck Depression Inventory-II. Tests for all categorical variables are with df = 1–3. Tests for variables presented with means and standard deviations are with df = 211.
The majority of participants were male (92%), had multiple deployments (76% at least two deployments), married (74%), and had at least some higher education (74% at least “some college”). This sample was somewhat diverse in ethnicity (52% White, 26% African American, 18% Hispanic) and military occupation specialty (35% combat arms, 22% combat support, 43% combat services support). On average, participants were 34 years old, mid-level enlisted soldiers (79% E-4 to E-6) with approximately 11 years of service. Participants were assessed on the main study variables prior to receiving treatment and 2 weeks after completion of treatment. All participants included in the current study provided complete data.
Measures
The PTSD Checklist, Stressor Specific Version (PCL-S; Weathers, Litz, Herman, Huska, & Keane, 1993) is a 17-item, self-report scale measuring how much an individual is bothered by re-experiencing, avoidance and arousal symptoms on a scale from 1 (not at all) to 5 (extremely). Higher scores reflect greater PTSD symptom severity. A review of the psychometric properties of the PCL-S reported acceptable test-retest reliability, internal consistency, convergent validity, and sensitivity to change (Wilkins, Lang, & Norman, 2011). Coefficient alpha for PCL-S total scores was excellent at both baseline (α = .84) and follow-up assessments (α = .94).
The Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II; Beck, Steer, & Brown, 1996) is a 21-item, self-report measure of the severity of depressive symptoms. Each item represents a unique affective or somatic symptom related to depression and is composed of four statements that reflect symptom severity. The statements are scaled from 0 (no disturbance) to 3 (maximal disturbance). Scores on all items are summed to obtain a total severity score. The BDI-II has demonstrated excellent test-retest reliability, concurrent validity, and criterion-based validity in a variety of samples (e.g., Beck, Steer, Ball, & Ranieri, 1996; Wang, & Gorenstein, 2013). Coefficient alpha for BDI-II total scores was excellent at both baseline (α = .90) and follow-up assessments (α = .92).
The Life Events Checklist for DSM 5 (LEC-5; Weathers et al., 2013) includes a list of 16 potentially traumatic life events that are commonly associated with PTSD symptoms and designed to facilitate PTSD diagnosis. For each potentially traumatic life event, respondents rate their experience of that event on a nominal scale (1 = happened to me, 2 = witnessed it, 3 = learned about it, 4 = part of my job, 5 = not sure, and 6 = doesn’t apply). The LEC-5 was used in the current study to assist in the identification the participant’s Index Event (i.e., the traumatic event that has bothered them the most in the past month).
The Deployment Risk and Resilience Inventory-2 (DRRI-2; King, King, Vogt, Knight, & Samper, 2006) is a comprehensive assessment tool, consisting of 17 separate scales, designed to identify key deployment-related risk factors in service members and veterans. Two of the DRRI-2 scales, Combat Experiences and Aftermath of Battle were used in the current study to assist in the identification of the participant’s Index Event. Items for these scales are rated on a 6-point Likert scale 1 (Almost none of the time) to 6 (Daily or almost daily) with higher scores indicating more exposure to military-related traumatic events during deployment.
Coding of Index Events
Index events were identified using a combination of self-report event checklists and a structured clinical interview. After participants completed the LEC-5 and the DRRI-2 Combat Experiences and Aftermath of Battle Scales, Masters or Doctoral level clinical interviewers used this information and a structured clinical interview to guide participants to identify the event that has bothered them the most within the past month. Event descriptions were written, verbatim, by the trained interviewers. The written interview answers were used to categorize all index events. Thus, while participants often endorsed multiple traumatic experiences (both military and non-military related) only index events were coded and analyzed for this study.
Index events were categorized in two ways, using the typology established by Stein et al. (2012). First, they were categorized nonexclusively, meaning they could be assigned up to three categories if their selected index event included multiple types of experiences. For example, if a service member described their index event during the structured interview by reporting that they were injured by an improvised explosive device (IED) while riding in a vehicle on a convoy, and their friend was killed in that same IED explosion, the event would be coded as follows: life threat to self + traumatic loss. With this coding structure, approximately half of the index events (46%) were assigned multiple codes. Next, index events were coded again, this time using mutually exclusive categories. Index events were only given one category, that which was determined by the coder to be the most salient part of the experience, following the coding rules created by Stein et al (2012).
For both exclusive and nonexclusive coding, two independent raters, blind to the hypotheses of the study, coded the index events. One rater coded all of the events and the second coded a random 20% of the events. High interrater reliability was established between the coders for both the exclusive ratings (Cohen’s kappa = 0.80) and the nonexclusive ratings (Cohen’s kappa ranging from .77 to .86), indicating that coders not only agreed on which categories were present, but also agreed on which category was primary when participants provided complex and multidimensional index events. A final meeting was called to settle any disagreements, and the results of this final meeting provided the data used in the analyses reported hereafter. Also, an independent auditor who was not a part of the original coding team, but is an author on this paper, verified that the coders’ ratings were consistent with the coding scheme. The auditor reviewed the ratings for all 213 participants and made no changes to the final ratings provided by the coding team.
When index events were categorized using mutually exclusive coding, the distribution of index events were as follows: life threat to self (n = 46, 22%), traumatic loss (n = 44, 21%), aftermath of violence (n = 38,18%), moral injury by other (n = 36, 17%), life threat to others (n = 35, 16%), and moral injury by self (n = 14, 6%). Participants were then separated into two groups (Traumatic Loss vs. Other Events), first using exclusive categories (n = 44 for traumatic loss, n = 169 other), and then using nonexclusive categories (n = 55 traumatic loss, n = 158 other). For the nonexclusive categorizations, seven of the 11 participants with secondary traumatic loss were categorized as experiencing aftermath of violence as their primary index event, whereas the remaining four were categorized as primarily experiencing moral injury by others.
Data Analysis
We began by examining the degree to which the primary traumatic loss group (using mutually exclusive codes) differed from those with other primary index events on key demographic variables. We also examined whether the other index events differed in terms of changes in depression and changes in PTSD severity in order to justify combining those groups in our later analyses. Then, to address our first prediction that traumatic loss would be associated with less recovery over the course of CPT treatment, we computed difference scores (Baseline – Follow-up) for the PCL-S between the two assessment time points and tested whether the Traumatic Loss and Other Primary Index Events differed in changes in PTSD severity using a between groups t-test; the resulting test term is analytically equivalent to the group*time interaction term from a repeated measures ANOVA when using complete data, and has the added benefit of allowing us to use a single variable in the subsequent mediation analysis.
To address our second prediction that changes in PCL-S scores would be mediated by changes in depression, we similarly computed difference scores for the BDI-II. We then used these difference scores as a mediator of the traumatic loss/PTSD relationship using the PROCESS macro (Hayes, 2017), using bias-corrected bootstrapping to provide the strongest possible confidence interval for the examined indirect effect (Preacher & Hayes, 2008). The test for mediation via changes in BDI-II scores controlled for treatment type (i.e., individual vs. group) given previously reported differences between the two treatment formats (Resick et al., 2017), gender, and number of previous deployments. Though our primary aim was to determine whether primary index traumatic loss (n = 44) was implicated in these relationships, we conducted an identical second set of analyses using the nonexclusive codes (n = 55 traumatic loss, n = 158 other) to determine the degree to which having traumatic loss as the most salient (determined by coders) component of one’s experience was important. Finally, we followed up the mediation results by examining differences between the groups’ changes in depression.
Results
Preliminary Analyses
As can be seen in Table 1, the group with traumatic loss as the primary component of their index event differed significantly only in terms of gender composition and number of prior deployments. With respect to gender, fewer females (n = 0) than expected experienced primary traumatic loss (standardized residual = −1.98), consistent with higher overall rates of male participants and gender-specific limitations in combat-oriented roles that may limit the occasions to experience such losses. While no women experienced a traumatic loss as their primary index event, two women had traumatic loss index events when using the collapsed primary and secondary categorization. This quasi-complete separation of gender within our primary traumatic loss variable makes it difficult to completely control for the effect of gender in the model. A separate set of analyses were conducted for males only and the same pattern of results reported hereafter were found. Female service members were included in this and subsequent analyses because we felt it was important to note their lack of experiencing traumatic loss as their primary index event.With respect to the number of prior deployments, only one category had more or fewer participants than expected by chance; significantly more participants with traumatic loss as the primary component of their index event than expected by chance reported having two prior deployments (n = 24, standardized residual = 2.00). Additionally, there were no differences in changes in depression (F(4,164) = 2.20, p = .07, η2 = .05) or changes in PTSD severity (F(4,164) = .76, p =.071, η2 = .01) among the other index event groups, indicating that it was appropriate to pool them together as a single group for subsequent analyses.
Main Analyses
Participants’ PCL-S scores improved overall (mean change score = 9.92, SD = 13.87) and in both the primary traumatic loss group (mean change score = 10.69, SD = 11.82) and other primary index event group (mean change score = 9.73, SD = 14.38). The difference in change between groups was not significant (t(211) = .41, p = .69, d = .07). Similar results were found when these analyses were re-run using nonexclusive categories, (t(211) = 0.22, p = .83, d = .03). Thus, our first hypothesis that having a traumatic loss would be associated with less improvement from baseline to posttreatment PCL-S scores was not supported.
The failure to find a direct association between traumatic loss and improvement in PCL-S scores does not preclude an indirect effect of a third variable. When the indirect and direct effects have opposite signs, they can in essence cancel each other out in a phenomenon known as “inconsistent mediation” (Hayes, 2017; MacKinnon, Fairchild, & Fritz, 2007). As illustrated in the top model in Figure 1, the mediation analysis revealed that the indirect effect was significant (b = −3.00, SE = 1.44, 95% CI −5.92, −0.23) within the overall model of changes in PCL-S scores (F(7,204) = 28.24, p < .001). Having a traumatic loss as the primary component of one’s index event predicted smaller improvements in depression (b = −4.40, t(206) = −1.98, p = .04), and less improvement in depression predicted smaller improvements in PCL-S scores (b = 0.68, t(205) = 12.45, p < .001).
Figure 1.
Mediation models for (A) individuals with primary traumatic loss (n = 44) and (B) individuals with either primary or secondary traumatic loss (n = 55). Change scores were calculated as pre-treatment minus post-treatment. Covariates were included in models of both the mediator and the outcome variables, but are omitted for clarity. PTSD = posttraumatic stress disorder.
Results of the mediation analysis coupled with the lack of apparent association between traumatic loss and PCL-S change suggest this is an example of inconsistent mediation. In fact, the mediation model estimates the direct effect as being significant and opposite in sign from the indirect effect (b = 3.75, t(205) = 2.14, p = .03), indicating that when changes in depression symptoms are not accounted for, experiencing a traumatic loss is associated with more recovery from PTSD following treatment rather than less. Thus, for those with traumatic loss index events, lack of change in symptoms of depression suppresses recovery from PTSD. However, these effects were present only for individuals who experienced traumatic loss as the primary component of their index event. This model predicted 15% of the variance in changes in depression and 50% of the variance in changes in PTSD scores. An identical second set of analyses was conducted using the nonexclusive codes (n = 55 traumatic loss, n = 158 other). As seen in the bottom model in Figure 1, neither the total effect, b = 0.89, t(205) = 0.42, p = .67, direct effect, b = 2.92, t(205) = 1.80, p = .07, nor the indirect effect, b = −2.02, SE = 1.28, 95% CI [−4.57, 0.48], was significant in this model in the overall model of changes in PCL-S scores within the overall model of changes in PCL-S scores (F(7,204) = 28.60, p < .001).
Alternative Explanatory Model
The hypothesized mediation model necessarily leads to inconsistent mediation, but there is an alternative plausible model. The alternative model is that changes in PTSD predict changes in depression rather than vice versa, and that changes in PTSD and exposure to a traumatic loss are uncorrelated predictors. We examined this model and found that both traumatic loss (b = −5.19, t(206) = −3.10, p = .002, β = −.16) and changes in PTSD (b = 0.64, t(206) = 12.50, p < .001, β = .68) predicted changes in depression, accounting for 46% of the variance. This model is more parsimonious, as it requires estimating one fewer path and it does not require positing a hidden, positive relationship between traumatic loss and improvement in PTSD. Choosing between them is a matter of theory rather than statistics.
Exploratory Analyses
The a-path in the top model in Figure 1 indicates that when mutually exclusive codes were used, the Traumatic Loss group had a decrease in BDI-II scores that was 4.40 points smaller than the Other Index Event group, but that path does not indicate whether either, neither, or both groups actually exhibited a significant decrease in depression from baseline to posttreatment. To better understand the observed suppression effect, we conducted exploratory repeated measures t tests on the BDI-II pre-and post-treatment scores separately for both the primary traumatic loss group and the other index event group. Results showed that the traumatic loss group exhibited a nonsignificant decrease in depression symptoms throughout treatment (mean change = 3.84 points, t(43) = 2.02, p = .053, d = .30). In contrast, those with other index events experienced a significant decrease in depressive symptoms over the course of treatment (mean change = 8.40 points, t(168) = 8.43, p < .001, d = .64). A t-test on the depression difference scores showed that the difference in changes in depression was significant t(211) = 2.09, p = .04, d = .35). Given this result, the suppression effect appears to be driven by the fact that individuals with traumatic loss as the primary component of their index event showed less global improvement in depression over the course of treatment. We conducted supplemental item-level analyses to determine whether any particular depressive symptoms were responsible for the differences between groups. None of the individual symptoms changed differently over time between the groups, suggesting that observed differences in changes in depression were due to global improvement in depression as opposed to any particular symptoms.
Discussion
The current study examined the relationship between traumatic loss and treatment response among active duty military personnel who received CPT. We hypothesized that traumatic loss would be associated with less robust recovery from PTSD and that less change in depression would mediate the relationship between traumatic loss and PTSD recovery. Our hypotheses were partially supported. Contrary to our first hypothesis, when not accounting for the effects of depression, it appears that there are no differences in PTSD recovery between those with primary traumatic loss index events and the comparison group. However, consistent with our second hypothesis, those with primary traumatic loss index events experienced less reduction in their symptoms of depression than those with other primary index events, and reductions in symptoms of depression were associated with reductions in PTSD symptoms from baseline to posttreatment. In other words, those who recovered less from depression recovered less from PTSD, similar to previous treatment studies (Galovski et al., 2016; Liverant et al., 2012). Specifically, depression symptoms appear to be suppressing recovery of PTSD for those with primary traumatic loss index events, significantly more so than for those with other types of index events. Importantly, this was only true for those who experienced a traumatic loss that was determined to be the primary (i.e., most salient) part of their index event.
These findings are consistent with cognitive-behavioral theory. Core symptoms of depression include loss of interest and engagement in activities (i.e., depressive avoidance), chronic fatigue, as well as feelings of worthlessness and excessive generalized guilt. All of these symptoms are represented on the BDI-II and represent malleable risk factors for intervention. Depressive avoidance and lack of engagement in activities tend to be broad in scope, but would likely include avoidance of trauma reminders. In turn, this would impede progress in PTSD treatments because successful treatment relies on approaching rather than avoiding situations, memories, and emotions. Additionally, when individuals experience a general sense of guilt or worthlessness, they may believe that they do not deserve to experience joy or to recover from PTSD. Furthermore, they may believe that recovering from PTSD will mean that they have forgotten or disrespected their lost loved one. In this way, excessive guilt, feelings of worthlessness, or lack of joy can serve to reinforce avoidance of accepting the reality that their loved one is “really gone.”
These findings have important implications for improving trauma-focused therapy for those with primary traumatic loss. For these individuals, it may be important to focus on symptoms of depression, particularly those symptoms that contribute to avoidance and depressive cognitions. Potential areas to target include reducing depressive avoidance and increasing motivation by facilitation of interest in activities through values-based behavioral activation; decreasing fatigue and improving sleep quality through cognitive-behavioral therapy for insomnia; and reducing isolation by building skills in seeking social support. Additionally, if maladaptive cognitions that are specifically related to depressive avoidance are present (e.g., “If I experience joy, it means I don’t miss my friend,” “I should not do things I enjoy because my friend is unable to do so,” “If I recover from PTSD, it means my friend is really gone”), the clinician should emphasize the facilitation of processing natural sadness and grief and support the patient in moving towards positive and meaningful activities that function to “honor” or “remember” their loved ones.
Given the results of inconsistent mediation, the alternative model, in which a traumatic loss-related index event and changes in PTSD symptoms are uncorrelated predictors of changes in depression over the course of CPT treatment, is also plausible. Though it has been shown that traumatic loss is associated with an increased risk of developing PTSD (Amick-McMullan et al.,1991; Boelen et al., 2015; Green et al., 2001) and higher PTSD severity compared with life threat events (Litz et al., 2018), there are no known previous studies examining the relationship between traumatic loss and PTSD treatment response compared with other types of trauma. However, this alternative model is generally inconsistent with the larger literature on the relationship between depression and PTSD recovery, which suggests that symptoms and diagnosis of depression predict less natural (King et al, 2009; Tural et al, 2012) and treatment led PTSD recovery (Haagen et al., 2017), and that improvements in depression predict reduction in PTSD symtpoms throughout PTSD treatment (Keller et al., 2014; Schumm et al., 2015).
This alternative model would imply that lack of recovery from depression is predicted by lack of recovery from PTSD, but does not provide any explaination for the higher rates of PTSD non-recovery in military populations. Thus, it would be impossible to speculate on potentially effective adjustments to evidence-based PTSD treatments to improve outcomes for service members and veterans. Time-lagged research is needed on the relationship between traumatic loss, depression, and PTSD recovery in order to empirically parse apart these two models.
Limitations and Future Directions
To our knowledge, the current study is the first to identify a suppression effect of depression on the recovery of PTSD in individuals with traumatic loss. The importance of these findings is elevated when considering the frequency of traumatic loss in the military coupled with recent data showing that military service members do not respond as well to first-line PTSD treatments as do their civilian counterparts (Steenkamp, Litz, Hoge, & Marmar, 2015). The primary limitation to this study is the lack of assessment of traumatic grief, because our current findings do not allow us to tease apart the overlap of depression and traumatic grief as a suppressing mechanism. Future research should focus on separating the role of traumatic grief from depression in this model. The second limitation to this study is that symptoms of depression and PTSD were measured simultaneously; thus, it is not possible to determine the exact trajectory of change of depression and PTSD from pre-to post-treatment and alternative explanatory models are also plausible. Session-by-session assessment of symptoms is needed to determine the points during treatment at which those with traumatic loss index events differ from those with other primary traumatic experiences. This would enable us to understand the specific components of treatment that should be tailored for a traumatic loss population. Additionally, in light of studies showing that women experience greater gains from evidence-based treatments for PTSD (e.g. Galovski, Blain, Chappuis, & Fletcher, 2013; Sripada et al, 2017), the lack of women experiencing traumatic loss as their primary index event might have influenced our results, though we think it is noteworthy in itself that none of the female service members reported this as their primary index event and would encourage future investigators to examine if this is the case in a broader sample of female service members as women take on combat roles in greater numbers. Finally, this study used indirect categorization of index events by trained coders, rather than by the participant. Replication of this study using categories created by participant rating may yield different results. However, the high degree of inter-rater reliability between coders implies clear and meaningful categories were able to be derived using the current methodology.
In conclusion, it is of utmost importance to provide our military service members and veterans with gold-standard, evidence-based mental health care for PTSD. Traumatic loss is a significant risk for our military personnel and veterans, and the presence of traumatic loss may inhibit recovery from PTSD. The current study provides preliminary data to assist providers and clinical researchers in understanding the potential role of traumatic loss and depression in PTSD recovery and continuing to make trauma-related treatment more effective for this population. As future research provides greater understanding about the unique challenges facing this group, we must continue to test and adapt treatment to best meet the needs of our military service members.
Acknowledgments
Funding for this work was made possible by the U.S. Department of Defense through the U.S. Army Medical Research and Materiel Command, Congressionally Directed Medical Research Programs, Psychological Health and Traumatic Brain Injury Research Program awards W81XWH-08–02-109 (Alan Peterson) W81XWH-08–02-0114 (Brett Litz), and W81XWH-08–02-0116 (Patricia Resick).
The views expressed herein are solely those of the authors and do not reflect an endorsement by or the official policy or position of the U.S. Army, the Department of Defense, the Department of Veterans Affairs, or the U.S. Government.
References
- American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Washington, DC: Author. [Google Scholar]
- Amick-McMullan A, Kilpatrick DG, & Resnick HS (1991). Homicide as a risk factor for PTSD among surviving family members. Behavior Modification, 15, 545–559. 10.1177/01454455910154005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beck AT, Steer RA, Ball R, & Ranieri WF (1996). Comparison of Beck Depression Inventories–IA and –II in psychiatric outpatients. Journal of Personality Assessment, 67, 588–597. 10.1207/s15327752jpa6703_13 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beck AT, Steer RA, & Brown GK (1996). Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory-II. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
- Benjamini Y, & Hochberg Y (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, Series B. 57, 289–300. Retreived from https://www.jstor.org/stable/2346101 [Google Scholar]
- Boelen PA, Keijser J, & Smid G (2015). Cognitive–behavioral variables mediate the impact of violent loss on post-loss psychopathology. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 7, 382–390. 10.1037/tra0000018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boelen PA, & van den Bout J (2005). Complicated grief, depression, and anxiety as distinct postloss syndromes: A confirmatory factor analysis study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 2175–2177. 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.11.2175 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boelen PA, van den Bout J, & de Keijser J (2003). Traumatic grief as a disorder distinct from bereavement-related depression and anxiety: A replication study with bereaved mental health care patients. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 1339–1341. 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.7.1339 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Breslau N, Kessler RC, Chilcoat HD, Schultz LR, Davis GC, & Andreski P (1998). Trauma and posttraumatic stress disorder in the community: The 1996 Detroit Area Survey of Trauma. American Journal of Psychiatry, 55, 626–632. 10.1001/archpsyc.55.7.626. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cigrang JA, Rauch SA, Mintz J, Brundige AR, Mitchell JA, Najera E, … Peterson AL; for the STRONG STAR Consortium. (2017). Moving effective treatment for posttraumatic stress disorder to primary care: A randomized controlled trial with active duty military. Families, Systems, & Health, 35, 450–462. 10.1037/fsh0000315 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Foa EB, McLean CP, Zang Y, Rosenfield D, Yadin E Yarvis JS, … Peterson Alan L.; for the STRONG STAR Consortium. (2018). Effect of prolonged exposure therapy delivered over 2 weeks vs. 8 weeks vs. present-centered therapy on PTSD symptom severity in military personnel: A randomized clinical trial. Journal of the American Medical Association, 319, 354–364. 10.1001/jama.2017.21242 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galovski TE, Blain LM, Chappuis C, & Fletcher T (2013). Sex differences in recovery from PTSD in male and female interpersonal assault survivors. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 51, 247–255. 10.1016/j.brat.2013.02.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galovski TE, Blain LM, Mott JM, Elwood L, & Houle T (2012). Manualized therapy for PTSD: Flexing the structure of cognitive processing therapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 80, 968–981. 10.1037/a0030600 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Galovski TE, Harik JM, Blain LM, Farmer C, Turner D, & Houle T (2016). Identifying patterns and predictors of PTSD and depressive symptom change during cognitive processing therapy. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 40, 617–626. 10.1007/s10608-016-9770-4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Green BL, Krupnick JL, Stockton P, Goodman L, Corcoran C, & Petty R (2001). Psychological outcomes associated with traumatic loss in a sample of young women. American Behavioral Scientist, 44, 817–837. 10.1177/00027640121956511 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Haagen JFG, ter Heide FJJ, Mooren TM, Knipscheer JW, & Kleber RJ (2017). Predicting post-traumatic stress disorder treatment response in refugees: Multilevel analysis. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 56, 69–83. 10.1111/bjc.12121 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hayes AF (2017). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York, NY: Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
- Hoge CW, Castro CA, Messer SC, McGurk D, Cotting DI, & Koffman RL (2004). Combat duty in Iraq and Afghanistan, mental health problems, and barriers to care. New England Journal of Medicine, 351, 13–22. 10.1056/NEJMoa040603 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jordan AH, & Litz BT (2014). Prolonged grief disorder: Diagnostic, assessment, and treatment considerations. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 45, 180–187. 10.1037/a0036836 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Keller SM, Feeny NC, & Zoellner LA (2014). Depression sudden gains and transient depression spikes during treatment for PTSD. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 82, 102–111. 10.1037/a0035286 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kessler RC, Sonnega A, Bromet E, Hughes M, & Nelson CB (1995). Posttraumatic stress disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey. Archives of General Psychiatry, 52, 1048–1060. 10.1001/archpsyc.1995.03950240066012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kimbrel NA, Meyer EC DeBeer BB, Gulliver SB, & Morissette SB (2016). A 12-month prospective study of the effects of PTSD-depression comorbidity on suicidal behavior in Iraq/Afghanistan-era veterans, Psychiatry Research, 243(30), 97–99. 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.06.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- King DW, King LA, McArdle JJ, Shalev AY, & Doron-LaMarca S (2009). Sequential temporal dependencies in associations between symptoms of depression and posttraumatic stress disorder: An application of bivariate latent difference score structural equation modeling. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 44, 437–464. 10.1080/00273170903103308 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- King LA, King DW, Vogt DS, Knight J, & Samper RE (2006). Deployment risk and resilience: A collection of measures for studying deployment related experiences in military-personnel and veterans. Military Psychology, 18, 89–120. Doi: 10.1207/s15327876mp1802_1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kulka RA, Schlenger WA, Fairbanks JA, Hough RL, Jordan BK, Marmar CR, ... Cranston AS (1990). Trauma and the Vietnam War generation: Report of findings from the National Vietnam Veterans Readjustment Study. New York, NY: Brunner/Mazel. [Google Scholar]
- Litz BT, Contractor AA, Rhodes C, Dondanville KA, Jordan AH, Resick PA, … Peterson AL; for the STRONG STAR Consortium. (2018). Distinct trauma types in military service members seeking treatment for posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 31, 286–295. 10.1002/jts.22276 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liverant GI, Suvak MK, Pineles SL, & Resick PA (2012). Changes in posttraumatic stress disorder and depressive symptoms during cognitive processing therapy: Evidence for concurrent change. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 80, 957–967. DOI: 10.1037/a0030485 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lobb EA, Kristjanson LJ, & Aoun SM (2010). Predictors of complicated grief: A systematic review of empirical studies. Death Studies, 34, 673–698. 10.1080/07481187.2010.496686 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacKinnon DP, Fairchild AJ, & Fritz MS (2007). Mediation analysis. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 593–614. 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085542 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Preacher KJ, & Hayes AF (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 879–891. 10.3758/BRM.40.3.879 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prigerson H, Bierhals AJ, Kasl SV, Reynolds CF, Shear K, Newsom JT, & Jacobs S (1996). Complicated grief as a disorder distinct from bereavement-related depression and anxiety: A replication study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 153, 1484–1486. 10.1176/ajp.153.11.1484 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Resick PA, Monson CM, & Chard KM (2016). Cognitive processing therapy for PTSD: A comprehensive manual. New York, NY: Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
- Resick PA, Nishith P, Weaver TL, Astin MC, & Feuer CA (2002). A comparison of cognitive-processing therapy with prolonged exposure and a waiting condition for the treatment of chronic posttraumatic stress disorder in female rape victims. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 867–879. 10.1037/0022-006X [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Resick PA, Wachen JS, Dondanville KA, Pruiksma KE, Yarvis JS, Peterson AL, & Mintz J; and the STRONG STAR Consortium. (2017). Effect of group vs individual cognitive processing therapy in active-duty military seeking treatment for posttraumatic stress disorder: A randomized clinical trial. JAMA Psychiatry, 74, 28–36. 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.2729 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Resick PA, Wachen JS, Mintz J, Young-McCaughan S, Roache JD, Borah AM, … Peterson AP; on behalf of the STRONG STAR Consortium. (2015). A randomized clinical trial of group cognitive processing therapy compared with group present-centered therapy for PTSD among active duty military personnel, Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 83, 1058–1068. 10.1037/ccp0000016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Resick PA, Williams LF, Suvak MK, Monson CM, & Gradus JL (2012). Long-term outcomes of cognitive-behavioral treatments for posttraumatic stress disorder among female rape survivors. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 80, 201–210. 10.1037/a0026602 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schumm JA, Dickstein BD, Walter KH, Owens GP, & Chard KM (2015). Changes in posttraumatic cognitions predict changes in posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms during cognitive processing therapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 83, 1161–1166. 10.1037/ccp0000040 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simon NM, O’Day EB, Hellberg SN, Hoeppner SS, Charney ME, Robinaugh DJ, … Rauch SAM (2018). The loss of a fellow service member: Complicated grief in post-9/11 service members and veterans with combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Neuroscience Research, 96, 5–15. 10.1002/jnr.24094 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sripada RK, Pfeiffer PN, Rampton J, Ganoczy D, Rauch SAM, Polusny MA, & Bohnert KM (2017). Predictors of PTSD Symptom Change Among Outpatients in the U.S. Department of Veterans Affairs Health Care System. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 30(1), 45–53. 10.1002/jts.22156 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steenkamp MM, Litz BT, Hoge CW, & Marmar CR (2015). Psychotherapy for military-related PTSD: A review of randomized clinical trials. Journal of the American Medical Association, 314, 489–500. 10.1001/jama.2015.8370 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stein NR, Mills MA, Arditte K, Mendoza C, Borah AM, Resick PA, & Litz BT; and the STRONG STAR Consortium. (2012). A scheme for categorizing traumatic military events. Behavior Modification, 36, 785–805. 10.1177/0145445512446945 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tural U, Önder E, & Aker T (2012). Effect of depression on recovery from PTSD. Community Mental Health Journal, 48, 161–166. 10.1007/s10597-010-9359-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- U.S. Department of Veteran Affairs. (2016). Suicide among veterans and other Americans 2001–2014. Retrieved from https://www.mentalhealth.va.gov/docs/2016suicidedatareport.pdf
- Wang YP & Gorenstein C (2013). Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory-II: A comprehensive review. Revista Brasileira de Psiquiatria, 35, 416–431. 10.1590/1516-4446-2012-1048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weathers FW, Blake DD, Schnurr PP, Kaloupek DG, Marx BP, & Keane TM (2013). The Life Events Checklist for DSM-5 (LEC-5). Instrument available from the National Center for PTSD; at www.ptsd.va.gov. [Google Scholar]
- Weathers F, Litz B, Herman D, Huska J, & Keane T (1993, October). The PTSD Checklist (PCL): Reliability, validity, and diagnostic utility. Paper presented at: 9th Annual Meeting of the International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies, San Antonio, TX. [Google Scholar]
- Wilkins CK, Lang AJ, & Norman SB (2011). Synthesis of the psychometric properties of the PTSD Checklist (PCL) Military, Civilian, and Specific Versions. Depression and Anxiety, 28, 596–606. 10.1002/da.20837 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]