Abstract
Background
Environmental lead exposure is associated with cognitive impairment in healthy children, with deficits seen in intelligence quotient (IQ), attention, and behavior. Neurocognitive dysfunction is also a well-described complication among children with chronic kidney disease (CKD). The objective was to evaluate the association between blood lead levels (BLL) and performance on neurocognitive assessments in a cohort of children with CKD.
Methods
Cross-sectional study of children with mild to moderate CKD from the Chronic Kidney Disease in Children (CKiD) multicenter prospective cohort study. The primary exposure was BLL. The primary outcome was performance on age-specific neurocognitive assessments evaluating IQ, executive functioning, attention, hyperactivity, and behavior. Multivariable linear regression was used to evaluate the association between BLL and neurocognitive performance, adjusted for key sociodemographic and clinical variables.
Results
A total of 412 subjects were included with median age 15.4 years, median estimated GFR 39 mL/min/1.732, median BLL1.2 mcg/dL, and median IQ score 99. In multivariable linear regression, higher BLL was associated with significantly lower IQ score (− 2.1 IQ points for every 1-mcg/dL increase in BLL, p = 0.029). Higher BLL was associated with worse scores on the Conners’ Continuous Performance Test II Variability T-Score, a measure of inattention (+ 1.8 T-Score points for every 1-mcg/dL increase in BLL, p = 0.033).
Conclusions
Low-level lead exposure is associated with significantly lower IQ and more inattention in children with CKD, a population already at high risk for neurocognitive dysfunction. Universal screening for elevated BLL should be considered for all children with CKD at age 12–24 months.
Keywords: Chronic kidney disease, Neurocognition, Lead, Heavy metals, Children
Introduction
Environmental lead exposure is a well-known cause of neurocognitive dysfunction in children [1–10]. Increasing evidence has demonstrated that even low-level lead exposure can have significant effects on cognition and behavior. In 1991, the Centers for Disease Control (CDC) lowered the blood lead level (BLL) of concern from 30 to 10 μg/dL in response to studies that showed that BLLs as low as 10 μg/dL were associated with adverse neurocognitive outcomes [11]. More recent studies have demonstrated neurocognitive abnormalities including impairments in academic achievement, verbal skills, impulsivity, attention, and behavior even among children with peak BLLs less than 10 μg/dL [2, 7, 12–18]. Therefore, the US Environmental Protection Agency considers no BLL safe for children [19]. Children with BLLs requiring intervention are now identified using the reference value corresponding to the 97.5th percentile of BLL distribution in US children age 1–5 years from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES), which is currently 5 μg/dL [20].
Children with chronic kidney disease (CKD) are already at high risk for neurocognitive dysfunction. Impairments in intelligence quotient (IQ), verbal abilities, memory, attention, inhibitory control, and executive function have all been reported in this population [21–24]. Multiple factors have been associated with neurocognitive dysfunction in children with CKD including severity of kidney disease, hypertension, and psychosocial factors such as depression, stress, and school absences [21, 23–30].
Despite the known contributions of lead and kidney disease to cognition, to our knowledge, there are no prior studies evaluating the contribution of lead toxicity to neurocognitive dysfunction in children with CKD. Given that children with CKD are already vulnerable to neurocognitive deficits, we hypothesized that these children would be at even higher risk of lead-associated neurotoxicity. Using a large cohort of children with CKD, we sought to determine if lead is associated with neurocognitive dysfunction in this population.
Methods
Study population
The Chronic Kidney Disease in Children (CKiD) study is a prospective cohort study of CKD in children being conducted at 46 centers in North America. Eligibility criteria for enrollment include age 1 to 16 years and estimated glomerular filtration rate (eGFR) of 30 to 90 mL/min/1.73 m2, calculated using the bedside CKiD equation [31]. Exclusion criteria include solid organ, bone marrow/stem cell transplant, cancer, or HIV. Full details of the CKiD protocol have been described previously [32].
Measures
BLLs were measured by high-resolution inductively coupled plasma mass spectrometry at the University of California, Santa Cruz, Environmental Toxicology Laboratory (Smith DR). Samples were analyzed on an Element XR inductively coupled plasma mass spectrometer (Thermo Scientific, West Palm Beach, FL, USA) using standardized protocols including confirmation that storage materials were not contaminated with background lead. No samples were below the analytical limit of detection (< 0.1 μg/dL). Accuracy was assessed using National Institute of Standards and Technology (NIST; Gaithersburg, MD, USA) standard reference materials (SRMs). Analyses using SRMs reflecting blood lead levels of 1.6 μg/dL and 25.3 μg/dL had percentage relative standard deviations (%RSDs) of 4.6 and 5.5, respectively. We assessed reproducibility by (a) analyzing replicate samples at intervals throughout the same analytic run, (b) analyzing samples in triplicate in the same run, and (c) analyzing replicate samples in separate runs. Percentage RSD for all reproducibility determinations was < 2.5%.
Participants had BLL measured at visit 2, visit 4, or visit 6, generally corresponding to 2, 4, or 6 years after study entry, respectively. For participants with more than one BLL measured, the BLL obtained most proximal to neurocognitive testing was used (defined as concurrent BLL). All BLLs were obtained prior to neurocognitive testing. A battery of age-specific neurocognitive assessments was administered at visit 3, 5, 7, or 9 after study entry. The last available neurocognitive results were used in order to evaluate long-term effects of lead toxicity on neurocognition.
Full-scale IQ was measured using age-appropriate assessments (Mullen Scales of Early Learning for subjects 12–29 months, Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence-Third Edition for subjects 30 months to 5 years, and Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence for subjects 6–18 years). Attention regulation was assessed with the Conners’ Kiddies Continuous Performance Test (K-CPT) (subjects 4–5 years) or Conners’ Continuous Performance Test-II (CPT-II) (subjects 6 years and older). The CPT-II is a computerized measure of attention that requires the individual to touch the mouse or space bar in conjunction with visual stimuli that are presented at the rate of about one per second over approximately 14 min. Scores include omission and commission errors, correct detection rate, response variability, and reaction time. Both the K-CPT and the CPT-II provide estimates of sustained attention and inhibitory control. Executive functioning was assessed with the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System Tower Subset (subjects > 6 years) and the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Functions (BRIEF) (parent-reported for preschool version BRIEF-P for subjects 2–5 years and BRIEF for subjects 6–18 years, or self-reported adult version BRIEF-A for subjects 18 years and older). The BRIEF is a parent or self-completed scale that provides ratings on metacognition, behavior regulation, and overall executive functioning. Behavioral symptoms were assessed using the parental rating scales of the Behavior Assessment System for Children Second Edition (BASC-2). Components of the BASC-2 include externalizing and internalizing problem composite scores, adaptive skills, and scales of hyperactivity and attention. All of the tasks were administered/supervised by a licensed psychologist.
Data analysis
Demographic and clinical characteristics of the study population were assessed using means with standard deviations (SD) for normally distributed continuous variables, medians with interquartile ranges (IQR) for non-normally distributed continuous variables, or frequencies with percentages for categorical variables. To assess the association between lead and neurocognitive performance, multiple linear regression was performed for each neurocognitive measure, with test score as the dependent variable and BLL in μg/dL as a continuous variable as the main explanatory variable. Based on prior studies of neurocognitive dysfunction in children with CKD, we a priori defined a set of covariates to include in the analyses. Key sociodemographic variables included age, sex, race, poverty (defined based on income and family size using the 2009 Poverty Guidelines published by the United States Department of Health and Human Services) [33], and maternal education (categorized as high school education, some college, or college and more). Maternal education was included because this has previously been shown to be associated with cognition in children with CKD [23]. CKD-related variables included CKD stage, CKD duration, glomerular versus non-glomerular diagnosis, hypertension (defined as systolic or diastolic blood pressure ≥ 95th percentile for age/sex/height or self-reported hypertension with use of an anti-hypertensive medication), proteinuria (categorized as significant if urine protein:creatinine ratio was > 0.2 and < 2 mg protein/mg creatinine or nephrotic if urine protein/creatinine ratio was ≥ 2 mg protein/mg creatinine), and anemia (defined as hemoglobin < 5th percentile for age/sex/race). Given the high rate of prematurity and abnormal birth history in the CKiD cohort, a composite variable of abnormal birth history was used to include any subject with premature birth, low birth weight, or was small for gestational age. Time-varying covariate data such as age, eGFR, and laboratory studies were reported from date of neurocognitive testing. Time-fixed covariate data such as CKD diagnosis, race, ethnicity, income, and maternal education were reported from the study entry visit. Analyses were conducted using Stata, version 15.1 (Statacorp, College Station, Texas). A p value of < 0.05 was the threshold for statistical significance.
Results
Sample characteristics
The study sample included 412 participants from the CKiD cohort who had at least one BLL measurement. All subjects had BLL measured prior to neurocognitive testing. Mean time between BLL measurement and neurocognitive testing was2.3 years (SD 1.4). Median BLL was 1.2 μg/dL (range 0.1 to 5.1 μg/dL). Sociodemographic and clinical variables are summarized in Table 1. Median age was 15.4 years, 60% were male, 68% were Caucasian, 14% were Hispanic ethnicity, and 23% met criteria for poverty. Median estimated GFR was39.0 mL/min/1.73 m2, median CKD duration was 13.6 years, 84% had non-glomerular CKD diagnosis, 31% had abnormal birth history, 42% had hypertension, 18% had nephrotic-range proteinuria, and 37% were anemic.
Table 1.
Variable |
N = 412 subjects N (%) or median (IQR) |
---|---|
Age, years | 15.4 (11.9, 18.6) |
Male sex | 249 (60%) |
Race | |
Caucasian | 282 (68%) |
African American | 68 (17%) |
Other | 21 (5%) |
More than one excluding African American | 23 (6%) |
More than one including African American | 18 (4%) |
Hispanic | 56 (14%) |
Povertya | |
Yes | 93 (23%) |
No | 310 (75%) |
Missing | 9 (2%) |
Maternal education | |
High school | 161 (39%) |
Some college | 110 (27%) |
College and more | 129 (31%) |
Missing | 12 (3%) |
Lead (mcg/dL) | 1.2 (0.8, 1.8) |
Estimated GFR, mL/min/1.73 m2 | 39.0 (27.0, 54.3) |
CKD Stage | |
Stage 1 | 4 (1%) |
Stage 2 | 64 (16%) |
Stage 3 | 213 (52%) |
Stage 4 | 126 (31%) |
Stage 5 | 3 (1%) |
Missing | 2 (0.5%) |
CKD duration, years | 13.6 (9.9, 16.5) |
Etiology of CKD | |
Non-glomerular | 344 (84%) |
Glomerular | 68 (17%) |
Abnormal birth history (premature, small for gestational age, or low-birth weight) | 126 (31%) |
Hypertension (SBP or DBP ≥ 95th percentile or self-reported hypertension plus antihypertensive use) | 175 (42%) |
Proteinuria | |
None (UPC < 0.2) | 113 (27%) |
Significant (UPC 0.2 to < 2) | 202 (49%) |
Nephrotic (UPC ≥ 2) | 76 (18%) |
Missing | 21 (5%) |
Hemoglobin, g/dL | 12.7(11.5, 13.8) |
Anemia (hemoglobin < 5th percentile for age, sex, race) | |
No | 237 (58%) |
Yes | 152 (37%) |
Missing | 23 (6%) |
Based on 2009 poverty guidelines, http://aspe.hhs.gov/poverty/09poverty.shtml
GFR glomerular filtration rate, CKD chronic kidney disease, UPC urine protein to creatinine ratio, mg protein/mg creatinine
Results of neurocognitive testing are summarized in Table 2. Mean IQ score was 98 (SD 16). Eighteen percent of subjects scored more than one SD below the normative mean value for IQ. A total of 27–39% of subjects scored more than one SD above the normative mean values for performance on the K-CPT or CPT-II (with higher scores reflective of worse ratings of performance on attention and inhibitory control). Thirty percent of subjects scored more than one SD above the normative mean on the BRIEF (with higher scores reflective of worse performance on assessments of executive functioning). A total of 24–32% of subjects scored more than one SD above the normative mean on the BASC-2 measures of overall behavior, hyperactivity, and attention (with higher scores reflective of more impaired performance), and 22% scored more than one SD below the normative mean on the adaptive composite (with higher scores reflective of more intact performance on assessments of adaptability).
Table 2.
Neurocognitive variable | N | Mean (SD) | N (%) at riska |
---|---|---|---|
Full-Scale IQ | 409 | 98 (16) | 75 (18) |
K-CPT or CPT-II | |||
Errors of omission | 352 | 53 (16) | 123 (35) |
Errors of commission | 355 | 52 (11) | 140 (39) |
Hit reaction | 355 | 47 (12) | 97 (27) |
Correct detection | 355 | 52 (11) | 120 (34) |
Variability scaled score | 354 | 51 (12) | 138 (39) |
BRIEF Global Executive Composite | 394 | 53 (12) | 118 (30) |
D-KEFS Tower Total Achievement Score | 357 | 10 (3) | 24 (7) |
BASC-2 (parent report) | |||
Behavioral Symptoms Summary | 369 | 49 (10) | 89 (24) |
Externalizing Problems Summary | 367 | 49 (10) | 87 (24) |
Internalizing Problems Summary | 368 | 49 (9) | 92 (25) |
Adaptive Behavior Summary | 368 | 49 (11) | 80 (22) |
Hyperactivity Clinical Scale | 372 | 50 (10) | 101 (27) |
Attention Clinical Scale | 372 | 51 (10) | 118 (32) |
Percent of subjects > 1 or < 1 standard deviation below normative mean. Please note that the directionality of the scores is different for each measure, with higher scores on several tests reflecting more intact performance and higher scores on other tests reflecting more impaired performance. Specifically, IQ scores are reported as standard scores with means of 100 and standard deviation of 15; higher scores reflect more intact performance. CPT and BRIEF scores are reported as T-score with a mean of 50 and SD of 10; higher scores reflect more impaired abilities. D-KEFS scores are reported as scaled scores, with a mean of 10 and SD of 3; higher scores reflect more intact performance. BASC-2 scores are reported as T-scores with a mean of 50 and SD 10. Higher scores reflect more impaired performance, except for the adaptive composite where higher scores reflect more intact performance
Association between blood lead levels and neurocognitive results
Table 3 shows results of multivariable linear regression analysis evaluating the association of BLL and full-scale IQ score. Every 1-μg/dL increase in BLL was associated with a 2.1 lower IQ score (95% CI − 3.9 to − 0.2), after adjustment for both sociodemographic and CKD-related variables. Other variables that were significantly associated with lower IQ score included female sex, lower maternal education, abnormal birth history, and nephrotic-range proteinuria. Other markers of CKD severity including CKD stage, duration, glomerular diagnosis, hypertension, and anemia were not associated with IQ score.
Table 3.
Coefficient | P value | 95% CI | |
---|---|---|---|
Lead, mcg/dL | −2.1 | 0.029* | −3.9, −0.2 |
Female sex | −4.2 | 0.008* | −7.3, −1.1 |
Age | 0.3 | 0.24 | −0.2, 0.9 |
Non-white race | −2.3 | 0.18 | −5.7, 1.0 |
Poverty | −3.8 | 0.05 | −7.7, 0.04 |
Maternal education | |||
High school | Ref | ||
Some college | 4.7 | 0.013* | 1.0, 8.4 |
College and more | 11.6 | < 0.001* | 7.9, 15.2 |
CKD stage | 0.9 | 0.50 | −1.7, 3.4 |
CKD duration | 0.01 | 0.96 | −0.5, 0.5 |
Glomerular CKD diagnosis | −1.0 | 0.70 | −6.3, 4.2 |
Abnormal birth history | −6.6 | < 0.001* | −9.8, −3.4 |
Proteinuria | |||
None | Ref | ||
Significant | −1.0 | 0.60 | −4.6, 2.7 |
Nephrotic | −5.1 | 0.042* | −10.1, −0.2 |
Hypertension | 0.1 | 0.97 | −2.9, 3.1 |
Anemia | −2.2 | 0.21 | −5.6, 1.2 |
p < 0.05
Table 4 shows results of multivariable linear regression analysis evaluating the association of BLL and CPT variability score. Higher variability score is a marker of problems with sustained attention and attention regulation. Every 1-μg/dL increase in BLL was associated with a 1.8 increase (i.e., worse performance) in the CPT variability T-score (95% CI 0.2 to 3.5). Of other sociodemographic or clinical variables, only longer CKD duration was associated with variability score, with longer CKD duration associated with slightly better scores on attention.
Table 4.
Coefficient | P value | 95% CI | |
---|---|---|---|
Lead, mcg/dL | 1.8 | 0.033* | 0.2, 3.5 |
Female sex | 1.3 | 0.38 | −1.5, 4.1 |
Age | 0.2 | 0.51 | −0.3, 0.7 |
Non-white race | 0.4 | 0.82 | −2.7, 3.5 |
Poverty | 3.2 | 0.08 | −0.3, 6.7 |
Maternal education | |||
High school | Ref | ||
Some college | 0.2 | 0.90 | −3.1, 3.5 |
College and more | −0.5 | 0.77 | −3.8, 2.8 |
CKD Stage | −0.8 | 0.51 | −3.1, 1.5 |
CKD duration | −0.5 | 0.024* | −1.0, −0.1 |
Glomerular CKD diagnosis | −2.5 | 0.29 | −7.3, 2.2 |
Abnormal birth history | 2.9 | 0.05 | −0.01, 5.9 |
Hypertension | −1.0 | 0.46 | −3.7, 1.7 |
Proteinuria | |||
None | Ref | ||
Significant | 0.9 | 0.60 | −2.4, 4.2 |
Nephrotic | 2.6 | 0.26 | −1.9, 7.1 |
Anemia | 0.1 | 0.96 | −3.0, 3.1 |
p < 0.05
In univariable analyses, higher BLL was associated with poorer performance on ratings of the global executive composite score from the BRIEF (β = 1.7, p = 0.019) and worse scores on parental ratings of total behavior problems (β = 1.8, p = 0.004), externalizing problems composite (β = 2.1, p =0.001), adaptive skills composite (β = − 3.1, p = < 0.001), hyperactivity scale (β = 1.7, p = 0.012), and attention scale (β =2.2, p = 0.001) on the BASC-2. These associations did not remain statistically significant after adjusting for other sociodemographic and clinical variables.
Discussion
In this cohort of children with CKD, we found that higher BLL was associated with significantly lower IQ score and poorer performance on assessments of attention, adjusted for known sociodemographic and clinical contributors to neurocognitive dysfunction. Notably, the median BLL in this population was only 1.2 μg/dL, well below the current national reference value for BLL of 5 μg/dL. Although we do not have a control group in this study and the sample size is relatively small, the point estimate for the magnitude of the impact of lead on IQ in this population of children with CKD is of greater magnitude in comparison to historical data from healthy children. In this study, we have identified a potentially modifiable risk factor for neurocognitive dysfunction in this high-risk population.
The mechanism of lead exposure leading to neurocognitive dysfunction is likely multifactorial [34, 35]. Children who are exposed to environmental lead may be at higher risk of toxicity because a greater proportion of ingested lead is absorbed from the gastrointestinal tract, and the developing nervous system may be more susceptible to neurotoxins [34, 36]. Lead has a long half-life in the body due to sequestration in bone, so BLL can underestimate the true burden of lead exposure. This may explain why childhood lead exposure has long-term neurologic consequences [34] and may contribute to the neurologic vulnerability in children with CKD.
Neurocognitive dysfunction is a well-described complication among children and adults with CKD. Deficits have been demonstrated across a range of neurocognitive domains including IQ, memory, attention, inhibitory control, and executive functioning [21–24, 37]. In the CKiD study, 21–40% of children with mild to moderate CKD were identified as being at risk for some form of neurocognitive dysfunction [23, 38]. Neurocognitive deficits are also common in adults with CKD, especially among adults with advanced CKD and end-stage kidney disease [39–42]. Longer duration of disease, increased severity of disease, and younger age at CKD onset have been associated with a higher risk of neurocognitive dysfunction among children with CKD [23, 25, 43]. Hypertension, including ambulatory hypertension and increased blood pressure variability, has also been identified as a potential contributor to neurocognitive dysfunction in this population [24, 28, 44].
There have been several large cohort studies of healthy children evaluating the impact of environmental lead exposure on IQ. In a cohort of 148 children, Bellinger et al. found that higher BLL at 24 months was associated with lower full-scale IQ at age 10 years; for every 10-μg/dL increase in BLL, IQ declined by 5.8 points [7]. Canfield et al. reported a cohort of 172 children who had BLLs measured at multiple time points between ages 6 and 60 months. For every 10-μg/dL increase in lifetime average blood lead concentration, IQ declined by4.6 points. In a subgroup of 101 children whose maximum BLL was consistently below 10 μg/dL, every 1-μg/dL increase in lifetime average lead concentration was associated with a 1.4 decline in IQ, providing further evidence that even low-level lead exposure can have significant effects on neurocognition [13]. In 2005, Lanphear et al. published a study on low-level lead effects on IQ among 1333 children pooled from seven international population-based longitudinal cohort studies. As BLL increased from 2.4 to 30 μg/dL, there was a 6.9 point decline in IQ (95% CI 4.2–9.4). However, as lead increased from 2.4 to 10 μg/dL, IQ declined by 3.9 points (95% CI 2.4–5.3), indicating that the magnitude of lead-associated decline in IQ was greater at lower level lead exposure [14]. A study of over 1000 New Zealand children showed that for every 5-μg/dL increase in BLL as a child, there was a 1.61 point decline in IQ score as an adult (95% CI 0.7−2.5) [45]. Environmental lead exposure has also been associated with increased risk of attention deficit and hyper-activity disorder (ADHD) [46–50].
In our study, we found that for every 1-μg/dL increase in BLL, IQ score decreased by 2.1 points, in comparison to a range of 0.3–1.4 reported in the cohorts of healthy children described above. Although we cannot make direct comparisons to these previously published cohorts, and we understand the statistical limitations inherent in studying a relatively smaller cohort of children selected for a different observational purpose, there is a suggestion that the magnitude of the effect of lead on IQ may be higher in this population of children with CKD. We hypothesize that children with “neurologic vulnerability” due to chronic diseases such as CKD may be more susceptible to the toxic effects of lead (and perhaps other toxins) compared to healthy children. We also found that female sex was associated with lower IQ. Although this has not been described in studies of healthy children, this association has been found in previous studies on neurocognition in the CKiD cohort [23, 28]. There is not an obvious biological explanation for this finding; it is possible that there is some residual confounding by diagnosis. We plan to further explore the effect of sex on neurocognition in future studies in children with CKD. We also found that longer duration of CKD was associated with slightly better CPT variability scores. However, the effect size was small and may not be clinically significant.
In addition to the CKD population, the impact of lead on cognition may extend to other groups of children with chronic disease in whom neurocognitive dysfunction is also common. Long-term neurocognitive deficits have been described in a variety of pediatric chronic diseases including cancer, sickle cell disease, congenital heart disease, and type 1 diabetes mellitus [51]. In the example of sickle cell disease, there have been prior reports of peripheral neuropathy occurring in the context of lead exposure. The authors postulated that children with sickle cell disease may have an increased risk of developing neuropathy after exposure to lead [52, 53]. Given the high risk of neurocognitive dysfunction in other children with chronic diseases, it is important to characterize the contribution of lead, as it would be a modifiable risk factor for adverse neurocognitive outcomes.
There are several limitations to this study. First, all patients in this study had CKD and there was no control group of healthy children. This was a cross-sectional study; therefore, causality cannot be determined. In addition, there was variability in the age at which BLL was measured, age at neurocognitive testing, and interval between the time of BLL measurement and neurocognitive testing. However, lead is known to have a very long half-life, and neurocognitive deficits persist for many years. We used concurrent BLL for patients with multiple lead measurements, as this is reflective of chronic lead exposure and has previously been highly correlated with IQ [14]. Finally, prenatal exposure to lead can impact neurocognitive outcomes, and data on maternal lead exposure was not available in this dataset.
Conclusion
Very low-level environmental lead exposure is associated with significantly lower IQ and problems with attention regulation in children with CKD. The impact of lead neurotoxicity may be greater in children with CKD compared to healthy children. The American Academy of Pediatrics (AAP) and the CDC currently recommend that asymptomatic children should be screened for elevated BLL according to federal, local, and state requirements. Targeted screening is recommended for children who live in communities with ≥ 25% of housing built before 1960 or ≥ 5% of children aged 12–24 months with BLL ≥ 5 μg/dL [54]. Based on the results of this study, we would suggest that clinicians consider universal lead screening for all children with CKD at age 12–24 months. In addition, children with CKD who are found to have elevated BLL should be followed closely for neurocognitive dysfunction. Interventions for treating lead exposure should follow the AAP Recommendations on Medical Management of Childhood Lead Exposure and Poisoning [55]. Future studies should focus on evaluating the association of lead and neurocognition in other populations of children with chronic diseases who may also be at higher risk of neurologic complications. Although the neurocognitive complications associated with CKD and other chronic childhood diseases may not be preventable, environmental lead exposure may be a modifiable risk factor that could lead to improvements in long-term neurocognitive outcomes.
Acknowledgments
Data in this manuscript were collected by the Chronic Kidney Disease in Children prospective cohort study (CKiD) with clinical coordinating centers (Principal Investigators) at Children’s Mercy Hospital/University of Missouri-Kansas City (Bradley Warady, MD) and Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia (Susan Furth, MD, PhD), Central Biochemistry Laboratory (George Schwartz, MD) at the University of Rochester Medical Center, and data coordinating center (Alvaro Muñoz, PhD and Derek Ng, PhD) at the Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health. The CKiD study is supported by grants from the National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Diseases, with additional funding from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, and the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (U01-DK-66143, U01-DK-66174, U24-DK-082194, U24-DK-66116). The CKiD website is located at https://statepi.jhsph.edu/ckid. This study was also supported by K23ES016541.
Funding The CKiD study is supported by grants from the National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Diseases, with additional funding from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, and the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (U01-DK-66143, U01-DK-66174, U24-DK-082194, U24-DK-66116). The CKiD website is located at https://statepi.jhsph.edu/ckid. This study was also supported by K23ES016541.
Footnotes
Conflict of interest The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
Ethical approval The CKiD protocol has been reviewed and approved by the institutional review boards of each participating center. All participants and/or guardians provided written informed consent.
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Baghurst PA, Mcmichael AJ, Wigg NR, Vimpani GV, Robertson EF, Roberts RJ, Tong SL (1992) Environmental exposure to Lead and Childrens intelligence at the age of 7 years - the port-Pirie cohort study. N Engl J Med 327:1279–1284. 10.1056/Nejm199210293271805 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Bellinger D, Sloman J, Leviton A, Rabinowitz M, Needleman HL, Waternaux C (1991) Low-level Lead-exposure and Childrens cognitive function in the preschool years. Pediatrics 87:219–227 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Liu JH, Liu XC, Wang W, McCauley L, Pinto-Martin J, Wang YJ, Li LD, Yan CH, Rogan WJ (2014) Blood Lead concentrations and Children’s behavioral and emotional problems acohort study. JAMA Pediatr 168:737–745. 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2014.332 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Mendelsohn AL, Dreyer BP, Fierman AH, Rosen CM, Legano LA, Kruger HA, Lim SW, Barasch S, Au L, Courtlandt CD (1999) Low-level lead exposure and cognitive development in early childhood. J Dev Behav Pediatr 20:425–431. 10.1097/00004703-199912000-00004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Mendelsohn AL, Dreyer BP, Fierman AH, Rosen CM, Legano LA, Kruger HA, Lim SW, Courtlandt CD (1998) Low-level lead exposure and behavior in early childhood. Pediatrics 101:e10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Needleman HL, Schell A, Bellinger D, Leviton A, Allred EN (1990) The long-term effects of exposure to low-doses of Lead in childhood - an 11-year follow-up report. N Engl J Med 322:83–88. 10.1056/Nejm199001113220203 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Bellinger DC, Stiles KM, Needleman HL (1992) Low-level lead exposure, intelligence and academic achievement: a long-term follow-up study. Pediatrics 90:855–861 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Bellinger D, Leviton A, Allred E, Rabinowitz M (1994) Pre- and postnatal lead exposure and behavior problems in school-aged children. Environ Res 66:12–30. 10.1006/enrs.1994.1041 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Needleman HL, Riess JA, Tobin MJ, Biesecker GE, Greenhouse JB (1996) Bone lead levels and delinquent behavior. JAMA 275:363–369 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Wright JP, Dietrich KN, Ris MD, Hornung RW, Wessel SD, Lanphear BP, Ho M, Rae MN (2008) Association of prenatal and childhood blood lead concentrations with criminal arrests in early adulthood. PLoS Med 5:e101 10.1371/journal.pmed.0050101 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Fadrowski JJ, Navas-Acien A, Tellez-Plaza M, Guallar E, Weaver VM, Furth SL (2010) Blood lead level and kidney function in US adolescents: the third National Health and nutrition examination survey. Arch Intern Med 170:75–82. 10.1001/archinternmed.2009.417 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Bellinger DC, Needleman HL (2003) Intellectual impairment and blood lead levels. N Engl J Med 349:500–502; author reply 500–502. 10.1056/NEJM200307313490515 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Canfield RL, Henderson CR Jr, Cory-Slechta DA, Cox C, Jusko TA, Lanphear BP (2003) Intellectual impairment in children with blood lead concentrations below 10 microg per deciliter. N Engl J Med 348:1517–1526. 10.1056/NEJMoa022848 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Lanphear BP, Hornung R, Khoury J, Yolton K, Baghurst P, Bellinger DC, Canfield RL, Dietrich KN, Bornschein R, Greene T, Rothenberg SJ, Needleman HL, Schnaas L, Wasserman G, Graziano J, Roberts R (2005) Low-level environmental lead exposure and children’s intellectual function: an international pooled analysis. Environ Health Perspect 113:894–899. 10.1289/ehp.7688 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Schwartz J (1994) Low-level lead exposure and children’s IQ: a meta-analysis and search for a threshold. Environ Res 65:42–55. 10.1006/enrs.1994.1020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Chiodo LM, Jacobson SW, Jacobson JL (2004) Neurodevelopmental effects of postnatal lead exposure at very low levels. Neurotoxicol Teratol 26:359–371. 10.1016/j.ntt.2004.01.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Rocha A, Trujillo KA (2019) Neurotoxicity of low-level lead exposure: history, mechanisms of action, and behavioral effects in humans and preclinical models. Neurotoxicology 73:58–80. 10.1016/j.neuro.2019.02.021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Surkan PJ, Zhang A, Trachtenberg F, Daniel DB, McKinlay S, Bellinger DC (2007) Neuropsychological function in children with blood lead levels <10 microg/dL. Neurotoxicology 28:1170–1177. 10.1016/j.neuro.2007.07.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.EPA report on the environment, https://www.epa.gov/roe/. Accessed 22 Jan 2019
- 20. [22 Jan 2019];CDC response to Advisory Committee on Childhood Lead Poisoning Prevention Recommendations in “Low Level Lead Exposure Harms Children: A Renewed Call of Primary Prevention”. http://www.cdc.gov/nceh/lead/ACCLPP/activities.htm. Accessed.
- 21.Gerson AC, Butler R, Moxey-Mims M, Wentz A, Shinnar S, Lande MB, Mendley SR, Warady BA, Furth SL, Hooper SR (2006) Neurocognitive outcomes in children with chronic kidney disease: current findings and contemporary endeavors. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev 12:208–215. 10.1002/mrdd.20116 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Gipson DS, Wetherington CE, Duquette PJ, Hooper SR (2004) The nervous system and chronic kidney disease in children. Pediatr Nephrol 19:832–839. 10.1007/s00467-004-1532-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Hooper SR, Gerson AC, Butler RW, Gipson DS, Mendley SR, Lande MB, Shinnar S, Wentz A, Matheson M, Cox C, Furth SL, Warady BA (2011) Neurocognitive functioning of children and adolescents with mild-to-moderate chronic kidney disease. Clin J Am Soc Nephrol 6:1824–1830. 10.2215/CJN.09751110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Ruebner RL, Laney N, Kim JY, Hartung EA, Hooper SR, Radcliffe J, Furth SL (2016) Neurocognitive dysfunction in children, adolescents, and young adults with CKD. Am J Kidney Dis 67:567–575. 10.1053/j.ajkd.2015.08.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Slickers J, Duquette P, Hooper S, Gipson D (2007) Clinical predictors of neurocognitive deficits in children with chronic kidney disease. Pediatr Nephrol 22:565–572. 10.1007/s00467-006-0374-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Drew DA, Weiner DE (2014) Cognitive impairment in chronic kidney disease: keep vascular disease in mind. Kidney Int 85: 505–507. 10.1038/ki.2013.437 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Kalaitzidis RG, Karasavvidou D, Tatsioni A, Balafa O, Pappas K, Spanos G, Pelidou SH, Siamopoulos KC (2013) Risk factors for cognitive dysfunction in CKD and hypertensive subjects. Int Urol Nephrol 45:1637–1646. 10.1007/s11255-013-0450-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Lande MB, Gerson AC, Hooper SR, Cox C, Matheson M, Mendley SR, Gipson DS, Wong C, Warady BA, Furth SL, Flynn JT (2011) Casual blood pressure and neurocognitive function in children with chronic kidney disease: a report of the children with chronic kidney disease cohort study. Clin J Am Soc Nephrol 6:1831–1837. 10.2215/CJN.00810111 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Lande MB, Batisky DL, Kupferman JC, Samuels J, Hooper SR, Falkner B, Waldstein SR, Szilagyi PG, Wang H, Staskiewicz J, Adams HR (2017) Neurocognitive function in children with primary hypertension. J Pediatr 180:148–155e141. 10.1016/j.jpeds.2016.08.076 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Brouhard BH, Donaldson LA, Lawry KW, McGowan KR, Drotar D, Davis I, Rose S, Cohn RA, Tejani A (2000) Cognitive functioning in children on dialysis and post-transplantation. Pediatr Transplant 4:261–267 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Schwartz GJ, Munoz A, Schneider MF, Mak RH, Kaskel F, Warady BA, Furth SL (2009) New equations to estimate GFR in children with CKD. J Am Soc Nephrol 20:629–637. 10.1681/ASN.2008030287 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Furth SL, Cole SR, Moxey-Mims M, Kaskel F, Mak R, Schwartz G, Wong C, Munoz A, Warady BA (2006) Design and methods of the chronic kidney disease in children (CKiD) prospective cohort study. Clin J Am Soc Nephrol 1:1006–1015. 10.2215/CJN.01941205 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.2009 HHS Poverty Guidelines, https://aspe.hhs.gov/2009-hhs-poverty-guidelines. Accessed 22 Jan 2019
- 34.Lidsky TI, Schneider JS (2003) Lead neurotoxicity in children: basic mechanisms and clinical correlates. Brain 126:5–19 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Schneider JS, Huang FN, Vemuri MC (2003) Effects of low-level lead exposure on cell survival and neurite length in primary mesencephalic cultures. Neurotoxicol Teratol 25:555–559 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Wright RO, Tsaih SW, Schwartz J, Wright RJ, Hu H (2003) Association between iron deficiency and blood lead level in a longitudinal analysis of children followed in an urban primary care clinic. J Pediatr 142:9–14. 10.1067/mpd.2003.mpd0344 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Bugnicourt JM, Godefroy O, Chillon JM, Choukroun G, Massy ZA (2013) Cognitive disorders and dementia in CKD: the neglected kidney-brain axis. J Am Soc Nephrol 24:353–363. 10.1681/ASN.2012050536 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Hooper SR, Gerson AC, Johnson RJ, Mendley SR, Shinnar S, Lande MB, Matheson MB, Gipson DS, Morgenstern B, Warady BA, Furth SL (2016) Neurocognitive, social-behavioral, and adaptive functioning in preschool children with mild to moderate kidney disease. J Dev Behav Pediatr 37:231–238. 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000267 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Yaffe K, Ackerson L, Kurella Tamura M, Le Blanc P, Kusek JW, Sehgal AR, Cohen D, Anderson C, Appel L, Desalvo K, Ojo A, Seliger S, Robinson N, Makos G, Go AS, Chronic Renal Insufficiency Cohort Investigators (2010) Chronic kidney disease and cognitive function in older adults: findings from the chronic renal insufficiency cohort cognitive study. J Am Geriatr Soc 58: 338–345. 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2009.02670.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Harciarek M, Williamson JB, Biedunkiewicz B, Lichodziejewska-Niemierko M, Debska-Slizien A, Rutkowski B (2012) Risk factors for selective cognitive decline in dialyzed patients with end-stage renal disease: evidence from verbal fluency analysis. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 18:162–167. 10.1017/S1355617711001445 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Kalirao P, Pederson S, Foley RN, Kolste A, Tupper D, Zaun D, Buot V, Murray AM (2011) Cognitive impairment in peritoneal dialysis patients. Am J Kidney Dis 57:612–620. 10.1053/j.ajkd.2010.11.026 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Post JB, Morin KG, Sano M, Jegede AB, Langhoff E, Spungen AM (2012) Increased presence of cognitive impairment in hemodialysis patients in the absence of neurological events. Am J Nephrol 35: 120–126. 10.1159/000334871 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Mendley SR, Matheson MB, Shinnar S, Lande MB, Gerson AC, Butler RW, Warady BA, Furth SL, Hooper SR (2015) Duration of chronic kidney disease reduces attention and executive function in pediatric patients. Kidney Int 87:800–806. 10.1038/ki.2014.323 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Lande MB, Mendley SR, Matheson MB, Shinnar S, Gerson AC, Samuels JA, Warady BA, Furth SL, Hooper SR (2016) Association of blood pressure variability and neurocognition in children with chronic kidney disease. Pediatr Nephrol 31:2137–2144. 10.1007/s00467-016-3425-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Reuben A, Caspi A, Belsky DW, Broadbent J, Harrington H, Sugden K, Houts RM, Ramrakha S, Poulton R, Moffitt TE (2017) Association of Childhood Blood Lead Levels with Cognitive Function and Socioeconomic Status at age 38 years and with IQ change and socioeconomic mobility between childhood and adulthood. JAMA 317:1244–1251. 10.1001/jama.2017.1712 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Ha M, Kwon HJ, Lim MH, Jee YK, Hong YC, Leem JH, Sakong J, Bae JM, Hong SJ, Roh YM, Jo SJ (2009) Low blood levels of lead and mercury and symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity in children: a report of the children’s health and environment research (CHEER). Neurotoxicology 30:31–36. 10.1016/j.neuro.2008.11.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Hong SB, Im MH, Kim JW, Park EJ, Shin MS, Kim BN, Yoo HJ, Cho IH, Bhang SY, Hong YC, Cho SC (2015) Environmental lead exposure and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptom domains in a community sample of south Korean school-age children. Environ Health Perspect 123:271–276. 10.1289/ehp.1307420 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Kim S, Arora M, Fernandez C, Landero J, Caruso J, Chen A (2013) Lead, mercury, and cadmium exposure and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children. Environ Res 126:105–110. 10.1016/j.envres.2013.08.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Kim Y, Cho SC, Kim BN, Hong YC, Shin MS, Yoo HJ, Kim JW, Bhang SY (2010) Association between blood lead levels (<5 mug/dL) and inattention-hyperactivity and neurocognitive profiles in school-aged Korean children. Sci Total Environ 408:5737–5743. 10.1016/j.scitotenv.2010.07.070 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Park JH, Seo JH, Hong YS, Kim YM, Kang JW, Yoo JH, Chueh HW, Lee JH, Kwak MJ, Kim J, Woo HD, Kim DW, Bang YR, Choe BM (2016) Blood lead concentrations and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in Korean children: a hospital-based case control study. BMC Pediatr 16:156 10.1186/s12887-016-0696-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Compas BE, Jaser SS, Reeslund K, Patel N, Yarboi J (2017) Neurocognitive deficits in children with chronic health conditions. Am Psychol 72:326–338. 10.1037/amp0000042 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Erenberg G, Rinsler SS, Fish BG (1974) Lead neuropathy and sickle cell disease. Pediatrics 54:438–441 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Anku VD, Harris JW (1974) Peripheral neuropathy and lead poisoning in a child with sickle-cell anemia. Case report and review of the literature. J Pediatr 85:337–340 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Council On Environmental Health (2016) Prevention of childhood Lead toxicity. Pediatrics 138(1). 10.1542/peds.2016-1493 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.American Academy of Pediatrics Recommendations on Medical Management of Childhood Lead Exposure and Poisoning. https://www.pehsu.net/_Library/facts/medical-mgmnt-childhood-lead-exposure-June-2013.pdf. Accessed 21 June 2019