Abstract
The human brain is characterized by highly dynamic patterns of functional connectivity. However, it is unknown whether this time-variant ‘connectome’ is related to the individual differences in the behavioural and cognitive traits described in the five-factor model of personality. To answer this question, inter-network time-variant connectivity was computed in n = 818 healthy people via a dynamical conditional correlation model. Next, network dynamicity was quantified throughout an ad-hoc measure (T-index) and the generalizability of the multi-variate associations between personality traits and network dynamicity was assessed using a train/test split approach. Conscientiousness, reflecting enhanced cognitive and emotional control, was the sole trait linked to stationary connectivity across several circuits such as the default mode and prefronto-parietal network. The stationarity in the ‘communication’ across large-scale networks offers a mechanistic description of the capacity of conscientious people to ‘protect’ non-immediate goals against interference over-time. This study informs future research aiming at developing more realistic models of the brain dynamics mediating personality differences.
Subject terms: Personality, Statistics
Introduction
Understanding how people differ in their cognitive, emotional, and behavioural dispositions is a central theme in psychology, psychiatry, and neurology. At the intersection across these disciplines, the ‘personality neuroscience’ field has emerged as a new academic ‘arena’ where researchers strive to reveal the brain mechanisms of personality traits1,2. So far, several neuroimaging studies have focused on linking personality measures with diverse structural and functional measures in single brain regions1,3–19. Others have investigated how variability in time-averaged (or static) functional connectivity relates to personality7,18–25.
However, no-one has yet assessed how the dynamic patterns of ‘communications’ across large-scale brain networks mediate personality differences. To offer new insights into the core neurological underpinnings of personality, we need to transition from static measures of connectivity to indices that resolve the temporal component of such connectivity patterns26,27. The most appropriate method to reveal the dynamicity (or its reverse, stationarity) in the connectivity between two ‘nodes’ (in this and other studies ‘a node’ is a ‘large-scale’ network) has been the subject of a fruitful debate28. Although different definitions exist28, a non-stationary connection can be quantified by the presence of frequent, long-lasting, or high excursions in the connection strength, despite the average value of the connection strength itself and the directionality of the excursion. In other words, a ‘connection’ between two ‘nodes’ (i.e., the amplitude of the correlation between two time-series) is ‘dynamic’ when it fluctuates frequently, for a long time, or to a large extent (or any combination of these elements). In contrast, a ‘stationary’ connection is one in which these connectivity patterns are minimal or infrequent.
At the cognitive level, the degree of dynamicity or stationarity in the human ‘connectome’ can be interpreted in different ways, depending on which ‘nodes’ are involved. For example, dynamic connections between networks that ‘feed’ external stimuli into the brain might result in ‘distractibility’, as, in this scenario, the rapidly changing information can ‘take the lead’ in driving the brain connections. Conversely, stationary connections between ‘top-down’ networks and ‘bottom-up’ sensory circuits can result in less ‘distractibility’, as, in this case, the incoming inputs may be ‘stabilized’ over-time against interference.
So far, different but partially related methods have been used to quantify the time-varying connectivity in the human brain, the most common being the ‘sliding’ window approach27–31. However, this method poses some limits on the temporal resolution and thus on the ability to characterize the relatively abrupt changes in the functional connectivity patterns that are commonly observed in resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rs-fMRI) data29. Here, we employed a dynamic conditional correlation (DCC) model that has been specifically developed for rs-fMRI datasets32. The DCC is based on a point-by-point volatility model (a generalized autoregressive conditional heteroscedastic – GARCH -model) and is robust against apparent changes in correlations caused by random noise30. These features make it an attractive tool for resolving the time-variant associations in datasets with low signal-to-noise ratio such as the rs-fMRI data.
We also sought to identify the time-resolved functional connectivity patterns that related to the cognitive and behavioural dispositions described in the five-factor model (FFM) of personality33,34. The FFM traits (i.e., neuroticism, extraversion, openness, agreeableness, and conscientiousness) have been empirically derived from a large body of epidemiological and psychosocial research35–38. The FFM traits also display sufficient universality across different demographic and cultural groups and predict important outcome measures such as educational or occupational success, risk to develop dementia, and longevity35–38.
Overall, this study was exploratory in its nature as there was (to the best of our knowledge) no other study assessing the relationship between time-variant connectivity measures and personality traits. However, we had some expectations regarding a specific effect of conscientiousness on such time-resolved connectivity indices. This is because we found, in the same sample of participants used here, that static (i.e., time-averaged) ‘connectomic’ measures (i.e., nodal strength, local clustering, and betweenness-centrality) positively related to conscientiousness, but not to any other FFM trait25. Hence, here, we tested whether conscientiousness was the sole FFM trait linked to time-resolved connectivity measures, over and above its effect on ‘static’ connectivity indices.
Nevertheless, it was reasonable to hypothesize that other FFM traits could have been linked to more or less dynamic connectivity patterns (e.g., openness could have been associated to more dynamic connectivity across sensory-related circuits).
Results
Participants
All data were drawn from n = 818 individuals from the Human Connectome Project (HCP) database, a large repository of behavioural and neuroimaging measures. The demographic and personality variables of the HCP sample are summarized in Table 1. Most participants were right-handed white Americans. Less than 10% had a Hispanic or Latino background.
Table 1.
Demographic variables | ||
Gender (males/females) | 367/451 | |
Age (years) | 28.7 ± 3.7 [22–37] | |
Handedness (Right/Left/Both) | 743/73/2 | |
Education (years) | 14.9 ± 1.8 [11–17] | |
Ethnicity (%) | Hispanic/Latino | 8.6% |
Not Hispanic/Latino | 90.5% | |
Unknown/Not Reported | 0.9% | |
Personality scores (NEO-FFI) | ||
Neuroticism | 16.3 ± 7.2 [0–43] | |
Extraversion | 30.7 ± 5.9 [11–47] | |
Openness | 28.3 ± 6.1 [12–45] | |
Agreeableness | 32.0 ± 5.0 [13–45] | |
Conscientiousness | 34.5 ± 5.9 [12–48] |
Potentially confounding variables such as age, sex, years of education, handedness, and intelligence scores were included as ‘nuisance’ variables in the statistical models testing for the relationship between the FFM personality traits and the time-resolved connectivity patterns.
Neuroimaging findings
Independent components analysis (ICA) of large-scale networks ‘nodes’
Brain networks were identified via group-ICA (dimensionality: n = 15) calculated by the HCP consortium and were characterized by a series of brain regions that have been reported in earlier studies (e.g., the sensory-motor circuit, the visual circuits, the default-mode network, the left and right fronto-parietal circuits, the salience network, etc.)39,40 (Fig. 1 and Supplementary Table 1 for a list of the anatomical regions belonging to each network ‘node’). Average connectivity strengths between nodes were also provided by the HCP consortium (‘netmats2’). Each of these large-scale networks was considered as a ‘node’ in the time-resolved connectivity analyses described in the Methods section.
Dynamical connectivity results independently of personality differences
First, the time-variant connectivity was estimated via the dynamic conditional correlation (DCC) model developed for rs-fMRI datasets32. A connection-wise index of non-stationarity (“T”-index) (initially introduced by Zalesky and colleagues) was computed for each connection (Fig. 2 for examples from real data)27. The higher the “T”-index, the more ‘dynamic’ (i.e., ‘non-stationary’) a connection is (see Methods section).
Second, the dynamicity of each connection (i.e. its “T”-index) was plotted against the static strength of the same connection (obtained from the HCP consortium and calculated using partial correlation) as shown in Fig. 3.
Independently of personality differences, the connections between visuo-parietal networks (occipito-parietal/occipito-frontal) and visual circuits (striate/extrastriate) that ‘feed’ external input into the brain were amongst the most dynamic (Fig. 3). Connections between occipito-frontal and sensory/motor networks and within sensory-motor circuits (i.e., sensory/motor-limbic-sensory/motor) also displayed a relatively high level of dynamicity (Fig. 3). In contrast, the connections across networks involved in cognitive controls (e.g., cingulate network, default mode network, hippocampal/para-hippocampal circuits) showed more stationary patterns of connectivity (Fig. 3).
Correlations between time-resolved indices of functional connectivity and personality traits
Next, we studied how the dynamicity of the connections across the nodes was related to individual differences in the FFM traits. To evaluate the generalizability of our findings, the initial sample of n = 818 participants was split into two sub-samples: a ‘training’ set (75% of participants, n = 613) and a ‘test’ set (25% of participants, n = 205). In the training set, we used multi-variate regression analyses (i.e., general linear models, GLMs) with the “T”-index as dependent variable, to explore the associations between the dynamicity of each connection and each FFM trait while accounting for potentially confounding effects driven by the remaining FFM traits as well as other ‘nuisance’ factors such as sex, age, education, handedness, and intelligence scores. Associations surviving a threshold of P < 0.05 (false-discovery-rate-FDR-corrected across 105 connections) were considered statistically significant. The associations discovered (i.e., the regression models estimated) in the ‘training’ dataset were used to assess the generalizability of our findings in the ‘test’ dataset (see Methods) using the relative root mean square error (RRMSE) as a criterion of merit41.
No significant associations, either positive or negative, were found between neuroticism (P’s > 0.2), extraversion (P’s > 0.6), openness (P’s > 0.2), and agreeableness (P’s > 0.9) scores and the dynamicity (“T”-index) of any connection. In contrast, negative associations were found between conscientiousness scores and the “T”-index across a series of networks including prefronto-parietal and prefronto-temporal networks, the default mode network, cingulate circuits, sensory-motor and limbic networks as well as posterior occipito-parietal circuits (P’s < 0.05, FDR) (Table 2, Fig. 4). Of note, we achieved good generalizability of our findings in all cases in which we found a significant effect in the GLM analyses (Table 2, Fig. 4) (median RRMSE = 0.14, maximum RRMSE = 0.20)41.
Table 2.
ICA node#1 | ICA node#2 | P value (FDR) | Effect size | RRMSE |
---|---|---|---|---|
Extra-striate | Cingulate | 0.012 | 0.154 | 0.167 |
Sensory/Motor-Limbic | Cingulate | 0.012 | 0.157 | 0.188 |
Occipito-Frontal | Extra-striate | 0.041 | 0.123 | 0.180 |
Occipito-Frontal | Fronto-Temporal | 0.041 | 0.125 | 0.168 |
Default-Mode | Right-Fronto-Parietal | 0.041 | 0.125 | 0.186 |
Default-Mode | Sensory/Motor-Limbic | 0.041 | 0.122 | 0.185 |
Extra-striate | Anterior Frontal | 0.041 | 0.131 | 0.154 |
Hippocampal-Cerebellar | Cingulate | 0.041 | 0.130 | 0.189 |
Sensory-Motor | Sensory/Motor-Limbic | 0.041 | 0.127 | 0.176 |
Occipito-Frontal | Striate | 0.046 | 0.114 | 0.177 |
Occipito-Frontal | Sensory-Motor | 0.046 | 0.118 | 0.145 |
Extra-striate | Occipito-Parietal | 0.046 | 0.119 | 0.173 |
Extra-striate | Hippocampal-Cerebellar | 0.046 | 0.114 | 0.175 |
Occipito-Parietal | Right-Fronto-Parietal | 0.046 | 0.116 | 0.195 |
Occipito-Parietal | Sensory-Motor | 0.046 | 0.114 | 0.149 |
Default-Mode | Striate | 0.046 | 0.113 | 0.142 |
Fronto-Temporal | Sensory/Motor-Limbic | 0.049 | 0.111 | 0.189 |
Effect sizes were estimated using partial correlations between the “T”-index and all independent variables in the model, ensuring full overlap between the estimated model coefficients and corresponding normalized effect sizes. Relative root mean square error (RRMSE) resulting from employing the estimated models on the test set is also reported. Model accuracy is considered excellent when RRMSE < 0.1, good when 0.1 < RRMSE < 0.2, fair when 0.2 < RRMSE < 0.3, and poor if RRMSE > 0.341.
To exclude that our main findings were not confounded by personality-driven differences in head movement during scanning, we also tested for associations between the average root-mean-square (RMS) displacement across runs and conscientiousness scores. No significant correlation was found between RMS and conscientiousness (Pearson’s R = 0.04, P = 0.24).
Discussion
We provide new evidence that people scoring high in conscientiousness display more stationary connectivity patterns across large-scale networks that have been implicated in cognitive control (i.e., prefronto-parietal, prefronto-temporal, default mode, anterior frontal, and cingulate networks), visuo-spatial and visuo-motor skills (striate, extra-striate, occipito-parietal, and occipito-frontal networks) and sensory/motor and emotional functions (sensory-motor and limbic networks). The relationship between conscientiousness and stationary connections was obtained using multi-variate statistical models that controlled for the remaining FFM traits (i.e., neuroticism, extraversion, openness, and agreeableness) and for variability in potentially confounding factors such as age, sex, handedness, education, and intelligence. Of note, the generalizability of our findings was supported by a ‘training’ and ‘test’ split validation approach.
We also found that, independently of personality differences, the connections across networks ‘feeding’ external inputs into the brain (occipito-parietal, occipito-frontal, striate, and extra-striate) and those regulating the motor output (i.e., sensory/motor-limbic-sensory/motor) displayed the highest levels of dynamicity. In contrast, the connections across networks involved in cognitive control (e.g., cingulate network, default mode network, anterior frontal circuits) showed more stationary connectivity patterns.
These data suggest that temporally resolved ‘connectomic’ indices are reliable markers that can be used as indicators of the cognitive and behavioural style across different people; for example, the ability of conscientious persons to efficiently pursue specific goals and maintain them consistent over time42–45. Furthermore, the current results support and extend our recent data showing that people scoring high in conscientiousness display increased nodal strength, local clustering, and betweenness-centrality across similar circuits25. Together, these findings mechanistically describe why conscientious people display a cognitive style that is resistant to the disruptive interference of incoming stimuli, which are often emotional in nature46.
This interpretation is in keeping with a theoretical framework positing that conscientiousness would contribute to the behavioural construct of ‘stability’, a meta-trait that would have evolved from the necessity to prevent the disruption of goals by interfering stimuli46. However, no significant results were obtained for the other ‘stability’ traits (i.e., low neuroticism and high agreeableness) which indicates that such effects do not necessarily extend to the meta-trait of ‘stability’. On the other hand, the temporal stationarity of the connectivity patterns in conscientious people can be interpreted in the context of previous studies showing that, across different samples and age-groups, conscientious people tend to display more ‘constant’ behavioural traits over time47–51.
The association between conscientiousness and stationary connectivity patterns was localized across networks that have been implicated in ‘top-down’ cognitive control, ‘bottom-up’ input-processing functions, and action generation. In particular, the fact that stationary interactions were found between prefronto-parietal and occipito-parietal/extra-striate circuits as well as between the default mode network and the sensory-motor, prefronto-parietal, and the visual striate networks is consistent with the notion that conscientious people maintain well the focus on non-immediate goals1,52,53.
At the same time, the stationary patterns of functional interactions between the cingulate cortices and sensory-motor limbic networks may help explaining why conscientious people successfully adjust their behavioural responses to different environmental contexts; for example, by delaying immediate gratification1,52,53. Stationarity in the patterns of ‘communication’ between memory circuits (i.e., hippocampal/cingulate networks) can also mediate a good performance during prospective memory tasks that require retrieval of complex sequences of planned actions54–56.
Strengths and limitations
The use of a large sample of participants (n = 818), state-of art analytical pipelines (i.e., DCC model), and the inclusion of a training/test-set for assessing the generalizability of our findings are the main methodological advantages of this study.
The fact that conscientiousness was the sole personality trait related to time-variant connectivity measures is consistent with our recent data showing that conscientiousness was the only FFM factor linked to heightened static ‘connectomic’ metrics such as nodal strength, local clustering, and betweenness-centrality25. Nevertheless, our earlier and current studies do not necessarily imply that other FFM traits do not have any brain functional correlate as there can be several reasons for null results including: (i) type II errors; (ii) non-linear relationships between FFM traits and time-variant metrics, and (iii) the fact that correlations between time-variant functional measures and personality traits might exist but could only be revealed by more narrow measures of personality (i.e., facets within each of the five factors) or other (e.g., task-related) functional measures.
Another issue regards the level of anatomical ‘granularity’ of the brain networks that is required to reveal the relationship between the time-resolved patterns of connectivity and personality differences. In other words, one could hypothesize that it is: 1) the interaction between large-scale brain networks that relates to the dynamicity or stability of functional connections, and/or 2) the more fine-grained interplay between individual areas within a network (e.g., anterior and posterior components of the default mode network) that is linked to the cognitive/behavioural differences described by the FFM of personality. Although either possibilities are equally plausible, non-mutually exclusive, and potentially interesting to study, we assumed here that a large-scale network approach would have retained a sufficient level of anatomical detail to reveal the relationship between time-variant connectivity and differences in the FFM traits. The use of large-scale circuits (n = 15 ICA-derived networks) also enabled us to compare the findings from this study to those reported in the previous one25.
Finally, in this study, as in many others using rs-fMRI, people’s performance during fixation (i.e., eye tracking data) was not monitored. However, eye tracking data in a limited group of participants (n = 132), for which the 3 Tesla MRI data were also available, was subsequently collected during MRI scanning at ultra-high field (7 Tesla). Hence, we assessed the potential impact of eye movements during scanning on our main outcome measure (T-index) and found that it was negligible (see supplementary material). Although this result does not exclude a priori whether participants’ ability to fixate affected our current findings (as the session with the eye tracking data at 7 T and the sessions at 3 T were on separate days); it showed in principle that the calculation of the T-index is not influenced by eye movements during rs-fMRI.
The potential confound of eye movements or closure during scanning can also be partially mitigated via reliable procedures that correct for head movements that are typically associated with fatigue or lapses in concentration during scanning. In our study, the noise related to head movements was removed via the HCP-specific ICA-FIX automated algorithm (which has ~99% sensitivity and specificity in de-noising HCP data57–59). Furthermore, we did not find any relationship between conscientiousness and head displacement during scanning which suggests negligible effects of personality differences on mediating compliance during rs-fMRI scanning. However, as before, we cannot completely exclude a priori any interaction between the de-noising procedures and the correct preservation of the personality specific characteristics in the rs-fMRI signal.
Conclusions
We found a negative association between conscientiousness scores and the dynamicity of the time-resolved functional connectivity patterns. Together with our recent results showing enhanced connectivity strength, local clustering, and betweenness centrality in cognitive networks in relation to conscientiousness, the current findings provide new mechanistic insights for the empirical observation that conscientious people are superb in maintaining long-term plans consistent over time.
Methods
Personality assessment
The FFM personality traits were assessed with the NEO five-factors inventory (NEO-FFI), which is composed by 60 items, 12 per each of the five factors33,60. For each item, participants reported their level of agreement on a 5-points Likert scale, from strongly disagree to strongly agree. This NEO instrument has been previously validated in the US and several other countries35. The recently discovered bug in the scoring of HCP agreeableness data was corrected prior to any further processing (personal communication on HCP mailing list on 03/09/2018 20:48 CEST). All data used in the present study are available for download from the Human Connectome Project (www.humanconnectome.org). Users must agree to data use terms for the HCP before being allowed access to the data and ConnectomeDB, details are provided at (https://www.humanconnectome.org/study/hcp-young-adult/data-use-terms). The HCP has implemented a two-tiered plan for data sharing, with different provisions for handling Open Access data and Restricted data (e.g., data related to family structure, age by year, handedness, etc). This study was carried out in compliance with the HCP restricted use data terms (https://www.humanconnectome.org/study/hcp-young-adult/document/wu-minn-hcp-consortium-restricted-data-use-terms). In particular, with reference to point 6 in the aforementioned document, local regulations at one of the PIs’ site (University of Rome Tor Vergata) do not require separate or individual ethics committee submission and/or approval.
MRI scanning protocol and pre-processing
Rs-fMRI data were acquired using a 3T scanner (Siemens AG, Erlangen, Germany)61. Four runs of approximately 14 minutes and 24 seconds each were obtained. Subjects lied within the scanner with open eyes while fixating a bright central cross projected on a dark background. Oblique axial acquisitions were alternated between phase encoding in a right-to-left direction in one run and phase encoding in a left-to-right direction in the other run. Gradient-echo echo-planar imaging used the following parameters: TR = 720 ms, TE = 33.1 ms, flip angle = 52°, FOV = 208 × 180 mm, Matrix 104 × 90, Slice thickness = 2.0 mm; 72 slices; 2.0 mm isotropic voxels, Multiband factor = 8, Echo spacing = 0.58 ms, BW = 2290 Hz/Px. This resulted in 4,800 rs-fMRI volumes in total per subject, subdivided in 4 runs of 1,200 volumes each. Structural (T1-weighted) images and field maps were also acquired to aid data pre-processing. Within the HCP consortium, each 1,200 brain volumes run of each subject’s rsfMRI data was minimally pre-processed according to the latest version (3.1) of the HCP pipeline62.
Each dataset was temporally de-meaned and had variance normalization applied according to Beckmann and colleagues {Beckmann, 2004 #280}. Group-principal component analysis (PCA) output was generated by MIGP (MELODIC’s Incremental Group-PCA) from n = 818 participants. This comprises the top 4,500 weighted spatial eigenvectors from a group-averaged PCA63. The MIGP output was then fed into group-independent component analysis (ICA) using FSL’s MELODIC tool64, applying spatial-ICA at dimensionality of 15. Successively, the ICA maps (dimensionality: 15) were dual-regressed into each subject’s 4D dataset to give a set of 15 time-courses of 4,800 time points per subject. Further details regarding data acquisition and pre-processing can be found in the HCP S900 Release reference manual available at https://www.humanconnectome.org/. Node maps (Fig. 1) as well as node- and subject-specific time-series were obtained from the HCP database for further processing. From the same database we also obtained connectivity matrices computed through partial correlation (‘netmats2’, https://db.humanconnectome.org/data/projects/HCP_1200) to be included in our analysis.
Estimation of time-variant functional connectivity
Dynamic connectivity estimation
To estimate DCC, we follow the methods outlined and the code distributed with the original publication32. GARCH volatility models assume that the conditional variance at time t is a linear combination of the past values of the conditional variance and of the past values of the squared process itself. The minimal univariate GARCH model takes the form:
1 |
where is a normal variable and the conditional variance is a function of previous time-step of the signal and of the variance itself:
2 |
where ω > 0, α ≥ 0, β ≥ 0 and α + β < 1. The algorithm employed in this paper for any two fMRI time-series (i.e., the DCC algorithm)65 consists of the following three steps: i) given that GARCH models account for volatility around a mean, all signals are de-trended using an autoregressive integrated moving average (ARIMA) model; ii) univariate GARCH models are fitted to each of the two time-series and standardized residuals are computed, and iii) an exponentially weighted moving average (EWMA)-type method is applied to the standardized residuals to compute a non-normalized version of the time-varying correlation matrix ,which is rescaled—see equations 19–24 in Lindquist et al.32—to obtain a conditional covariance. This procedure allows the estimation of time-varying, dynamical conditional covariance between the two components of which, in the case of two rs-fMRI time-series, can be employed as an estimate of dynamical connectivity, i.e. the DCC32 from the off-diagonal elements of the conditional covariance matric. The DCC which is an estimate of the association between the two time-series (i.e. connectivity) at the same temporal resolution as the original signals. All parameters are estimated using maximum likelihood methods.
After DCC estimation, for each DCC time-series (i.e., for each time-varying connection) we computed a non-stationarity index, term “T”-index” as explained in the following paragraph.
T-index for quantifying non-stationarity
The non-stationarity T-index used here to evaluate the non-stationarity of time-varying connectivity was inspired by the T-index originally developed by Zalesky et al.27. Such T-index is based on the rationale that the non-stationarity property of a time-resolved correlation depends on large and prolonged excursions from the median connectivity value over-time. The larger and/or more prolonged such excursions in connectivity are, the more “dynamical” (i.e., non-stationary) the connection is considered to be. The T-index is defined and calculated as follows: (see also supplementary information in Zalesky et al.)27: (a) an excursion is defined by two consecutive crossing points around the median value (see Fig. 5); (b) the excursion’s length τ is defined by the time difference between two consecutive crossing points; (c) the excursion’s magnitude h is defined as the absolute difference between the highest or lowest time series correlation and its median value, within the context of each specific excursion. Figure 5 graphically summarizes these definitions.
Formally, the T-index of dynamicity is defined as follow:
3 |
where N is the number of excursions.
To exclude potential confounding effects driven by differences in scan sessions (1st and 2nd half of the time-series), we conducted a series of additional analyses reported in supplementary material.
Moreover, to assess the impact of eye movements during scanning on the quantification of the T-index, we performed further analyses in a sub-group of people (n = 132) for which eye-tracking data were available during subsequent scanning at ultra-high-field (7 Tesla) (see supplementary material). Eye-tracking data (along with all other HCP data) are available as part of the 7 T data release at db.humanconnectome.org.
Statistical analyses
Generation of train-test samples
To test the associations between non-stationarity measures and personality traits, as well as to evaluate the generalizability of our findings, the initial sample of n = 818 participants was split into two matched sub-samples: a ‘training’ set (75% of participants, n = 613) and a ‘test’ set (25% of participants, n = 205). The matched train/test split was generated by random sampling (with replacement) and subsequent comparison between the resulting train and test sets through nonparametric Mann-Whitney-U tests until no significant differences in the median “T”-index (across subjects) for any 105 possible connections was found. This ensured that the train and test set had comparable dependent variable distributions across all possible connections.
Inference of associations between non-stationarity and personality
The ‘training’ sample was used to estimate the association between the “T”-index (i.e., non-stationarity measure) described above and the FFM personality traits. Specifically, general linear models (GLMs), including each of the FFM traits as well as age, sex, handedness, education, and intelligence scores as covariates of no interest, were fitted to each connection (dependent variable: non-stationarity index “T”). The resulting P-values were corrected for multiple comparisons across all possible 105 connections using a false discovery rate (FDR) procedure. Associations surviving a threshold of P < 0.05 (FDR-corrected) were considered statistically significant. The results were a set of FDR corrected P-values as well as multivariate regression coefficients for each connection. For each regression (i.e., for each connection), effect sizes were estimated using the absolute values of partial correlations between the “T”-index and all independent variables in the model. As partial correlation is defined through linear regression, this ensured full overlap between the estimated model coefficients and corresponding normalized effect sizes.
Generalizability to unseen test set
Finally, the ‘test’ sample (i.e., in an unseen group of subjects to which the model was completely ‘agnostic’) was employed to assess the generalizability of the multivariate models fitted on the ‘training’ set. To this end, the GLMs fitted in the training set were used to estimate the “T”-indices in the ‘test’ sample using the demographic variables and personality scores of the ‘test’ sample as inputs (i.e., the rs-fMRI data of the ‘train’ sample was not employed in this procedure). The similarity between ‘real’ “T”-indices (i.e., computed using rs-fMRI data from the ‘test’ sample) and ‘estimated’ time-variant indices (i.e., predicted using the GLM coefficients fitted on ‘training’ data only) was assessed through the relative root mean square error (RRMSE), a normalized version of the root mean squared error which is often used as a measure of the differences between predicted and observed values. Model accuracy can be considered excellent when RRMSE < 0.1, good when 0.1 < RRMSE < 0.2, fair when 0.2 < RRMSE < 0.3, and poor if RRMSE > 0.341. This ensured comparability of generalization capability across the personality traits. The image analysis pipeline is summarized in Fig. 6.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
Roberta Riccelli is funded by the University “Tor Vergata” of Rome, Italy, while Luca Passamonti is funded by the Medical Research Council (MRC) (MR/P01271X/1) at the University of Cambridge, UK. Antonio Terracciano is supported by the National Institute on Aging of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number R01AG053297 and R03AG051960. Iole Indovina is funded by the Italian Ministry of Health (PE-2013-02355372). Data collection and sharing for this project was provided by the MGH-USC Human Connectome Project (HCP; Principal Investigators: Bruce Rosen, M.D., Ph.D., Arthur W. Toga, Ph.D., Van J. Weeden, MD). The HCP project is supported by the National Institute of Dental and Craniofacial Research (NIDCR), the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) and the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NINDS) (Principal Investigators: Bruce Rosen, M.D., Ph.D., Martinos Center at Massachusetts General Hospital; Arthur W. Toga, Ph.D., University of Southern California, Van J. Weeden, MD, Martinos Center at Massachusetts General Hospital).
Author contributions
L.P. helped conceptualize and design the study, interpreted the results, drafted the manuscript, critically reviewed and revised the manuscript, and approved the final manuscript as submitted. R.R. led the data analysis, critically reviewed and revised the manuscript, and approved the final manuscript as submitted. I.I. provided methodological perspective on the study, critically reviewed the initial manuscript and approved the final manuscript as submitted. A.D. provided methodological perspective on the study, designed the figures and critically reviewed the initial manuscript. A.T. helped to conceptualize the study, critically reviewed the initial manuscript and approved the final manuscript as submitted. N.T. conceptualized, designed, and supervised all aspects of the study, including data analysis, and approved the final manuscript as submitted.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
is available for this paper at 10.1038/s41598-019-51469-2.
References
- 1.DeYoung CG. Personality Neuroscience and the Biology of Traits. Social and Personality Psychology Compass. 2010;4:1165–1180. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00327.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Corr, P. Understanding biological psychology. (Blackwell, 2006).
- 3.Canli T. Functional brain mapping of extraversion and neuroticism: learning from individual differences in emotion processing. Journal of personality. 2004;72:1105–32. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2004.00292.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Canli T, Sivers H, Whitfield SL, Gotlib IH, Gabrieli JD. Amygdala response to happy faces as a function of extraversion. science. 2002;296:2191. doi: 10.1126/science.1068749. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Cremers H, et al. Extraversion is linked to volume of the orbitofrontal cortex and amygdala. PloS one. 2011;6:e28421. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0028421. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Cremers HR, et al. Neuroticism modulates amygdala-prefrontal connectivity in response to negative emotional facial expressions. Neuroimage. 2010;49:963–70. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.08.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Dima D, Friston KJ, Stephan KE, Frangou S. Neuroticism and conscientiousness respectively constrain and facilitate short-term plasticity within the working memory neural network. Hum Brain Mapp. 2015;36:4158–63. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22906. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Fischer H, Wik G, Fredrikson M. Extraversion, neuroticism and brain function: A PET study of personality. Personality and Individual Differences. 1997;23:345–352. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(97)00027-5. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Hu X, et al. Voxel-based morphometry studies of personality: issue of statistical model specification–effect of nuisance covariates. Neuroimage. 2011;54:1994–2005. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Kapogiannis, D., Sutin, A., Davatzikos, C., Costa, P. & Resnick, S. The five factors of personality and regional cortical variability in the baltimore longitudinal study of aging. Hum Brain Mapp, 10.1002/hbm.22108 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 11.Krebs RM, Schott BH, Duzel E. Personality traits are differentially associated with patterns of reward and novelty processing in the human substantia nigra/ventral tegmental area. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;65:103–10. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.08.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Liu WY, et al. The Big Five of Personality and structural imaging revisited: a VBM - DARTEL study. Neuroreport. 2013;24:375–80. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e328360dad7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Lu F, et al. Relationship between personality and gray matter volume in healthy young adults: a voxel-based morphometric study. PloS one. 2014;9:e88763. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0088763. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Passamonti L, et al. Increased functional connectivity within mesocortical networks in open people. Neuroimage. 2015;104:301–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.09.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Rodrigo AH, et al. Linking trait-based phenotypes to prefrontal cortex activation during inhibitory control. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2016;11:55–65. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsv091. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Servaas MN, et al. Connectomics and neuroticism: an altered functional network organization. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40:296–304. doi: 10.1038/npp.2014.169. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Wright CI, et al. Neuroanatomical correlates of extraversion and neuroticism. Cerebral cortex. 2006;16:1809–19. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Indovina I, Riccelli R, Staab JP, Lacquaniti F, Passamonti L. Personality traits modulate subcortical and cortical vestibular and anxiety responses to sound-evoked otolithic receptor stimulation. J Psychosom Res. 2014;77:391–400. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2014.09.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Riccelli R, et al. Neuroticism modulates brain visuo-vestibular and anxiety systems during a virtual rollercoaster task. Hum Brain Mapp. 2017;38:715–726. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23411. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Berger, P., Bitsch, F., Nagels, A., Straube, B. & Falkenberg, I. Personality modulates amygdala and insula connectivity during humor appreciation: An event-related fMRI study. Soc Neurosci 1–13, 10.1080/17470919.2017.1403375 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 21.Angelides NH, Gupta J, Vickery TJ. Associating resting-state connectivity with trait impulsivity. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2017;12:1001–1008. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsx031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Deris N, Montag C, Reuter M, Weber B, Markett S. Functional connectivity in the resting brain as biological correlate of the Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales. Neuroimage. 2017;147:423–431. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.11.063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Markett S, Montag C, Melchers M, Weber B, Reuter M. Anxious personality and functional efficiency of the insular-opercular network: A graph-analytic approach to resting-state fMRI. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2016;16:1039–1049. doi: 10.3758/s13415-016-0451-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Pang Y, et al. Extraversion modulates functional connectivity hubs of resting-state brain networks. J Neuropsychol. 2017;11:347–361. doi: 10.1111/jnp.12090. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Toschi, N., Riccelli, R., Indovina, I., Terracciano, A. & Passamonti, L. Functional Connectome of the Five-Factor Model of Personality. PersonalityNeuroscience1 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 26.Vidaurre, D., Smith, S. M. & Woolrich, M. W. Brain network dynamics are hierarchically organized in time. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA, 10.1073/pnas.1705120114 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 27.Zalesky A, Fornito A, Cocchi L, Gollo LL, Breakspear M. Time-resolved resting-state brain networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2014;111:10341–6. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1400181111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Hindriks R, et al. Can sliding-window correlations reveal dynamic functional connectivity in resting-state fMRI? Neuroimage. 2016;127:242–256. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.11.055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Hutchison RM, et al. Dynamic functional connectivity: promise, issues, and interpretations. Neuroimage. 2013;80:360–78. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Robinson LF, et al. The Temporal Instability of Resting State Network Connectivity in Intractable Epilepsy. Hum Brain Mapp. 2017;38:528–540. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23400. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Hindriks R, et al. Corrigendum to ‘Can sliding-window correlations reveal dynamic functional connectivity in resting-state fMRI?’ [NeuroImage 127 (2016) 242–256] Neuroimage. 2016;132:115. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.02.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Lindquist MA, Xu Y, Nebel MB, Caffo BS. Evaluating dynamic bivariate correlations in resting-state fMRI: a comparison study and a new approach. Neuroimage. 2014;101:531–46. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.06.052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Costa, P. T. & McCrae, R. R. Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) professional manual. (Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, 1992).
- 34.Digman JM. Personality structure: Emergence of the five-factor model. Annual review of psychology. 1990;41:417–440. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.41.020190.002221. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 35.McCrae RR, Terracciano A. Universal features of personality traits from the observer’s perspective: data from 50 cultures. Journal of personality and social psychology. 2005;88:547. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.88.3.547. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Terracciano A, An Y, Sutin AR, Thambisetty M, Resnick SM. Personality Change in the Preclinical Phase of Alzheimer Disease. JAMA. Psychiatry. 2017 doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.2816. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Kern ML, Friedman HS. Do conscientious individuals live longer? A quantitative review. Health Psychol. 2008;27:505–12. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.27.5.505. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Sutin AR, Luchetti M, Stephan Y, Robins RW, Terracciano A. Parental educational attainment and adult offspring personality: An intergenerational life span approach to the origin of adult personality traits. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2017;113:144–166. doi: 10.1037/pspp0000137. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Raichle, M. E. The restless brain: how intrinsic activity organizes brain function. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci370 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 40.Toschi N, Duggento A, Passamonti L. Functional connectivity in amygdalar-sensory/(pre)motor networks at rest: new evidence from the Human Connectome Project. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2017;45:1224–1229. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13544. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Despotovic M, Nedic V, Despotovic D, Cvetanovic S. Evaluation of empirical models for predicting monthly mean horizontal diffuse solar radiation. Renewable and Sustainable Energy Reviews. 2016;56:246–260. doi: 10.1016/j.rser.2015.11.058. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Ozer DJ, Benet-Martinez V. Personality and the prediction of consequential outcomes. Annu Rev Psychol. 2006;57:401–21. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Roberts BW, Lejuez C, Krueger RF, Richards JM, Hill PL. What is conscientiousness and how can it be assessed? Dev Psychol. 2014;50:1315–30. doi: 10.1037/a0031109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Noftle EE, Robins RW. Personality predictors of academic outcomes: big five correlates of GPA and SAT scores. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2007;93:116–30. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.93.1.116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Sutin AR, et al. The five-factor model of personality and physical inactivity: A meta-analysis of 16 samples. Journal of Research in Personality. 2016;63:22–28. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2016.05.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.DeYoung CG. Cybernetic Big Five Theory. Journal of Research in Personality. 2015;56:33–58. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2014.07.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Donnellan MB, Conger RD, Burzette RG. Personality development from late adolescence to young adulthood: Differential stability, normative maturity, and evidence for the maturity‐stability hypothesis. Journal of personality. 2007;75:237–264. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2007.00438.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Lönnqvist J-E, Mäkinen S, Paunonen SV, Henriksson M, Verkasalo M. Psychosocial functioning in young men predicts their personality stability over 15 years. Journal of Research in Personality. 2008;42:599–621. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2007.08.006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Blonigen DM, Carlson MD, Hicks BM, Krueger RF, Iacono WG. Stability and change in personality traits from late adolescence to early adulthood: A longitudinal twin study. Journal of personality. 2008;76:229–266. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2007.00485.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Terracciano A, Stephan Y, Luchetti M, Sutin AR. Cognitive Impairment, Dementia, and Personality Stability Among Older Adults. Assessment. 2018;25:336–347. doi: 10.1177/1073191117691844. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Roberts BW, Caspi A, Moffitt TE. The kids are alright: growth and stability in personality development from adolescence to adulthood. Journal of personality and social psychology. 2001;81:670. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.81.4.670. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Eisenberg N, Duckworth AL, Spinrad TL, Valiente C. Conscientiousness: origins in childhood? Dev Psychol. 2014;50:1331–49. doi: 10.1037/a0030977. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.DeYoung CG, et al. Testing predictions from personality neuroscience. Brain structure and the big five. Psychol Sci. 2010;21:820–8. doi: 10.1177/0956797610370159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Arana JM, Meilan JJ, Perez E. The effect of personality variables in the prediction of the execution of different prospective memory tasks in the laboratory. Scand J Psychol. 2008;49:403–11. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2008.00671.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Cuttler C, Graf P. Personality predicts prospective memory task performance: an adult lifespan study. Scand J Psychol. 2007;48:215–31. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2007.00570.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Smith RE, Persyn D, Butler P. Prospective Memory, Personality, and Working Memory: A Formal Modeling Approach. Z Psychol. 2011;219:108–116. doi: 10.1027/2151-2604/a000055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Salimi-Khorshidi G, et al. Automatic denoising of functional MRI data: combining independent component analysis and hierarchical fusion of classifiers. Neuroimage. 2014;90:449–68. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Smith SM, et al. Resting-state fMRI in the Human Connectome Project. Neuroimage. 2013;80:144–68. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Griffanti L, et al. ICA-based artefact removal and accelerated fMRI acquisition for improved resting state network imaging. Neuroimage. 2014;95:232–47. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.03.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Terracciano A. The Italian version of the NEO PI-R: conceptual and empirical support for the use of targeted rotation. Personality and individual differences. 2003;35:1859–1872. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(03)00035-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Van Essen DC, et al. The Human Connectome Project: a data acquisition perspective. Neuroimage. 2012;62:2222–2231. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Glasser MF, et al. The minimal preprocessing pipelines for the Human Connectome Project. Neuroimage. 2013;80:105–124. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Smith SM, Hyvärinen A, Varoquaux G, Miller KL, Beckmann CF. Group-PCA for very large fMRI datasets. Neuroimage. 2014;101:738–749. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.07.051. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Beckmann CF, Smith SM. Probabilistic independent component analysis for functional magnetic resonance imaging. IEEE Trans Med Imaging. 2004;23:137–52. doi: 10.1109/TMI.2003.822821. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Engle R. Dynamic Conditional Correlation. Journal of Business & Economic Statistics. 2002;20:339–350. doi: 10.1198/073500102288618487. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.