Abstract
Context
Although the consequences of severe iodine deficiency are beyond doubt, the effects of mild to moderate iodine deficiency in pregnancy on child neurodevelopment are less well established.
Objective
To study the association between maternal iodine status during pregnancy and child IQ and identify vulnerable time windows of exposure to suboptimal iodine availability.
Design
Meta-analysis of individual participant data from three prospective population-based birth cohorts: Generation R (Netherlands), INMA (Spain), and ALSPAC (United Kingdom); pregnant women were enrolled between 2002 and 2006, 2003 and 2008, and 1990 and 1992, respectively.
Setting
General community.
Participants
6180 mother-child pairs with measures of urinary iodine and creatinine concentrations in pregnancy and child IQ. Exclusion criteria were multiple pregnancies, fertility treatment, medication affecting the thyroid, and preexisting thyroid disease.
Main Outcome Measure
Child nonverbal and verbal IQ assessed at 1.5 to 8 years of age.
Results
There was a positive curvilinear association of urinary iodine/creatinine ratio (UI/Creat) with mean verbal IQ only. UI/Creat <150 µg/g was not associated with lower nonverbal IQ (−0.6 point; 95% CI: −1.7 to 0.4 points; P = 0.246) or lower verbal IQ (−0.6 point; 95% CI: −1.3 to 0.1 points; P = 0.082). Stratified analyses showed that the association of UI/Creat with verbal IQ was only present up to 14 weeks of gestation.
Conclusions
Fetal brain development is vulnerable to mild to moderate iodine deficiency, particularly in the first trimester. Our results show that potential randomized controlled trials investigating the effect of iodine supplementation in women with mild to moderate iodine deficiency on child neurodevelopment should begin supplementation not later than the first trimester.
This meta-analysis of individual participant data showed that a lower urinary iodine/creatinine ratio during pregnancy was associated with lower verbal IQ up to the 14th week of gestation.
Iodine is an essential trace element required for the production of thyroid hormones; optimal thyroid hormone availability is important for normal fetal brain development (1, 2). During pregnancy, there is a higher demand for maternal iodine intake (3, 4). This is due to (i) the increased maternal thyroid hormone synthesis required to ensure adequate thyroid hormone availability to the fetus, (ii) greater urinary iodine loss due to an increased glomerular filtration rate, and (iii) placental transfer of iodine to the fetus to facilitate fetal thyroid hormone production. Although severe iodine deficiency is no longer common in Europe, mild to moderate iodine deficiency is still common, especially in pregnant women (5). Severe iodine deficiency in pregnancy results in a higher risk of goiter, hypothyroidism, and mental retardation in the offspring (6). However, the consequences of mild to moderate iodine deficiency in pregnancy on child neurodevelopment are less well established (4).
Mild to moderate iodine deficiency or low iodine intake during pregnancy has been associated with adverse child neurodevelopmental outcomes in some (7–13) but not all studies (14–16). Differences in results between studies may be related to methodological differences (e.g., measurement of iodine status, selected reference group, and available data on confounders), the age at assessment of the neurodevelopmental outcome of interest, the timing of the iodine measurements, and the relative severity of iodine deficiency in the population. Although the main focus in the literature has been on the effects of iodine deficiency, some studies have suggested adverse effects of supplemental intake or excess iodine on either maternal thyroid function (17, 18), fetal thyroid function (19, 20), or child neurodevelopment (10, 15, 16).
International health authorities have similar recommendations to ensure optimal iodine status in pregnancy (21–23). It is universally recognized that any necessary iodine supplementation should be commenced before or as early as possible in pregnancy to achieve adequate iodine intake, owing to the susceptibility of the fetal brain to iodine deficiency (23). However, whether the effect of iodine on child cognition varies during different stages of pregnancy is unknown. We therefore assessed the association between maternal iodine status in pregnancy and child IQ across three cohorts of differing iodine status and investigated potential effect modification by gestational age.
Material and Methods
Study design and populations
This study was embedded in three cohort studies: Generation R (Netherlands), the INfancia y Medio Ambiente Project (INMA; Spain, three regions), and the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC; United Kingdom). The study designs have been described elsewhere (24–27); the ALSPAC study website contains details of all the data that are available through a fully searchable data dictionary and variable search tool (28). For the current study, mother-child pairs were included if a measure of urinary iodine and creatinine concentration during pregnancy and child IQ scores were available. Exclusion criteria were multiple pregnancies, fertility treatment, medication affecting the thyroid, and preexisting thyroid disease. Ethical approval was obtained from the Medical Ethical Committee of the Erasmus Medical Center (Generation R); the Ethical Committee of the Municipal Institute of Medical Investigation and the ethical committees of the hospitals involved in the study (INMA); and the ALSPAC Ethics and Law Committee and local research ethics committees; approval was given by participants and/or parents or guardians of the children by a signed informed consent form.
Maternal iodine status
Urinary iodine concentration (UIC) and creatinine concentration were measured in spot urine samples stored at −20°C after collection. As part of this study, additional urine samples were analyzed for iodine and creatinine concentrations, and existing measurements from each cohort (7, 19, 29) were also included. The additional measurements were performed in the same laboratories where the existing measurements were performed. The laboratories were registered with EQUIP and used certified reference materials (Seronorm Urine levels one and two; Nycomed, Norway) for the verification of results. In Generation R, UIC was measured by the Sandell-Kolthoff method. In the INMA, UIC was measured using paired-ion reversed-phase, high-performance liquid chromatography with electrochemical detection at a silver working electrode (Waters Chromatography, Milford, MA). In the ALSPAC, UIC was measured on a dynamic reaction cell inductively coupled plasma mass spectrometer. Urinary creatinine concentration was determined by the Jaffe rate method in all cohorts. More information on the measurement methods and the variability between assays can be found in an online repository (30).
In a subset of women, repeated measures of urinary iodine and creatinine were available; we used the earliest available sample as an indicator of iodine status. The urinary iodine/creatinine ratio (UI/Creat) was used as a measure of iodine status. Because of possible contamination of UIC by the use of iodine-containing test strips in ALSPAC (31), UIC >500 µg/L and/or UI/Creat >700 µg/g was excluded from the analyses in this cohort (N = 363). These cutoffs were based on previous work in ALSPAC and from other studies of pregnant women in the United Kingdom (7, 32, 33). We grouped women’s results by UI/Creat as follows: (i) <150 µg/g, (ii) 150 to <500 µg/g, and (iii) ≥500 µg/g; according to World Health Organization classification, these groups broadly relate to iodine deficiency, sufficiency, and excess, respectively.
Maternal thyroid function
TSH and free thyroxine (FT4) were measured according to different methodologies between cohorts, which are described in detail elsewhere (34–36). For the analysis, FT4 and TSH concentrations were logarithmically transformed, and cohort-specific SD scores were calculated with a mean of 0 and an SD of 1 based on the data of thyroid peroxidase antibody (TPOAb)-negative women (TPOAb measurements were available in Generation R and ALSPAC). TPOAb titers ≥60 IU/mL and ≥6 IU/mL were considered positive in Generation R and ALSPAC, respectively. These cutoffs were determined by the assay manufacturers.
Nonverbal and verbal IQ scores
In Generation R, nonverbal IQ was assessed at a median age of 5.9 years using a subset of the Snijders Oomen Nonverbal Intelligence Test (2.5-7-Revised) (37), and verbal IQ was estimated by the short form of the McArthur Communicative Development Inventory (38) at a median age of 1.5 years. In the INMA, nonverbal and verbal IQ scores were assessed at a median age of 4.6 years using the McCarthy Scales of Children’s Abilities (39). In the ALSPAC, nonverbal and verbal IQ scores were assessed at a median age of 8.6 years using the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, third UK edition (40). Except for verbal IQ ascertainment in Generation R, which involved a parental questionnaire, all other measurements were performed by psychologists or trained staff. To homogenize the different scores, raw cohort-specific scores were standardized to a mean of 100 and an SD of 15. Children with IQ scores <50 or >150 (n = 3) were considered outliers and were excluded from the analyses. Suboptimal IQ was defined as an IQ score <85.
Potential confounding variables
Information on maternal age, educational level (low, middle, high), ethnicity/country of birth (cohort-specific categories), parity (zero, one, two or more), prepregnancy body mass index, and smoking during pregnancy (never smoked, smoked in the beginning or until pregnancy confirmed, continued smoking) was collected by questionnaires administered during pregnancy. Gestational age at urine sampling was defined using ultrasonography or last menstrual period. Child sex and age at time of the IQ assessment was obtained during the study visits.
Statistical analyses
UI/Creat was not normally distributed and was therefore transformed using the natural logarithm; back-transformed values are shown in plots for better interpretation. We studied the associations of UI/Creat with child nonverbal and verbal IQ scores by using one-step and two-step approaches. In the one-step approach, data from the cohorts were pooled, and we performed standard multivariable linear regression models with and without a quadratic term to investigate the possible nonlinear nature of the associations. Nonlinearity was also investigated by using ordinary least squares linear regression models with restricted cubic splines with three knots. With ANOVA, we tested the null hypothesis that child mean IQ was similar across the full range of the natural logarithm of UI/Creat. The decision to use linear regression models instead of multilevel models for the one-step analyses was made because we found no difference between multilevel models with random intercepts and/or slope per cohort vs standard linear regression correcting for cohort (e.g., cohort-specific variable ethnicity/country of birth) when assessed using the Akaike information criterion and log-likelihood tests. In the two-step approach we first studied the associations of UI/Creat <150 µg/g and UI/Creat ≥500 µg/g with child IQ by using linear regression models in each cohort separately. In these analyses, the reference group consisted of women with UI/Creat of 150 to 500 µg/g. We then combined the cohort-specific effect estimates using random-effects meta-analyses.
Potential effect modification according to gestational age was analyzed by adding a product interaction term between UI/Creat and gestational age to the one-step approach models. Because of the known constraints of statistical power for interaction analyses, a P value <0.15 for interaction terms was used to screen for potential relevant modification (41). We further quantified potential relevant differences by performing stratified analyses by tertiles of gestational age (≤12 weeks, >12 to ≤14 weeks, and >14 weeks). We also studied associations with suboptimal IQ (score <85) by combining cohort-specific estimates from logistic regression models into random-effects meta-analyses.
Sensitivity analyses were designed to study (i) the associations of UI/Creat with verbal IQ score in mother-child pairs from the INMA and ALSPAC only, as verbal IQ was assessed at a preschool age in Generation R; (ii) the association between UI/Creat and maternal TSH and FT4 SD scores within the ±4 SD range around the mean, as TSH and FT4 values outside this range were considered outliers (n = 19 for TSH; n = 5 for FT4); and (iii) whether the association between UI/Creat and IQ score could potentially be explained by maternal thyroid function by adjusting for FT4 and TSH in the models.
Heterogeneity between cohorts was assessed using the Cochran Q test and the I2 statistic (42). All models were adjusted for potential confounding variables. However, because of collinearity between maternal ethnicity/country of birth, child age at IQ ascertainment, and cohort, we adjusted for maternal ethnicity/country of birth only in the one-step approach models.
We applied inverse probability weighting to take into account the potential differential loss to follow-up (30) [i.e., to account for selection bias that potentially arises when only the population with available data on iodine status and child IQ is included compared with a full initial cohort recruited at pregnancy (43)]. Briefly, we used information available for all participants at recruitment to predict the probability of participation in the study and used the inverse of these probabilities as weights in the analyses so that results would be representative of the initial populations of the cohorts. In addition, missing values in potential confounding variables were imputed using chained equations (44). A total of 25 data sets were generated. A P value <0.05 was defined as statistically significant. Statistical analyses were performed with STATA (version 14.0; StataCorp, College Station, TX) and R statistical software (version 3.3.2, package rms).
Results
The final study population consisted of 6180 mother-child pairs (Fig. 1). The median UIC (UI/Creat) was 159 µg/L (214 µg/g) in Generation R (adequate intake), 128 µg/L (152 µg/g) in the INMA (mild deficiency), and 96 µg/L (124 µg/g) in the ALSPAC (moderate deficiency) (Table 1). Iodine status was determined at a median [interquartile range (IQR)] gestational age of 13.1 (12.1, 14.8) weeks, 13.0 (12.4, 14.1) weeks, and 12.0 (8.0, 16.0) weeks in Generation R, the INMA, and the ALSPAC, respectively.
Table 1.
Generation R (n = 1931) | INMA (n = 1269) | ALSPAC (n = 2980) | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
n | Values | n | Values | n | Values | |
Offspring neurodevelopment, no. (%) | ||||||
Suboptimal nonverbal IQa | 1540 | 175 (11.4) | 1269 | 216 (17.0) | 2979 | 479 (16.1) |
Suboptimal verbal IQa | 1618 | 279 (17.2) | 1269 | 211 (16.6) | 2977 | 480 (16.1) |
Female sex, no. (%) | 1931 | 963 (49.9) | 1268 | 632 (49.8) | 2980 | 1514 (50.8) |
Iodine status | 1931 | 1269 | 2980 | |||
UI/Creat, µg/g, median (IQR) | 214 (143–308) | 152 (96–258) | 124 (82–199) | |||
UI/Creat <150 µg/g, no. (%) | 531 (27.5) | 623 (49.1) | 1831 (61.4) | |||
UI/Creat >500 µg/g, no. (%) | 97 (5.0) | 52 (4.1) | 81 (2.7) | |||
UIC, µg/L, median (IQR) | 159 (90–275) | 128 (75–213) | 96 (57–153) | |||
Gestational age at urine sampling, wk | 1931 | 1267 | 2980 | |||
Median (IQR) | 13.1 (12.1–14.8) | 13.0 (12.4–14.1) | 12.0 (8.0–16.0) | |||
Range (min–max) | 6.1–30.5 | 8.6–39.4 | 1.0–42.0 | |||
>20th week of gestation, no. (%) | 66 (3.4) | 130 (10.2) | 211 (7.1) | |||
Maternal thyroid function | ||||||
TSH, mIU/L, median (IQR) | 1719 | 1.29 (0.79–1.95) | 1227 | 1.25 (0.85–1.80) | 1102 | 0.97 (0.64–1.38) |
FT4, pmol/L, median (IQR) | 1728 | 14.6 (12.9–16.5) | 1229 | 10.6 (9.7–11.6) | 1108 | 16.2 (14.9–17.7) |
TPOAb positivity, no. (%) | 1737 | 98 (5.6) | NA | NA | 1111 | 146 (13.1) |
Gestational age, wk, mean (SD) | 1733 | 13.3 (1.9) | 1228 | 13.2 (1.4) | 1118 | 10.3 (2.7) |
Educational level, no. (%) | 1835 | 1265 | 2888 | |||
Low | 154 (8.4) | 270 (21.3) | 573 (19.8) | |||
Middle | 760 (41.4) | 525 (41.5) | 1810 (62.7) | |||
High | 921 (50.2) | 470 (37.2) | 505 (17.5) | |||
Maternal ethnicity/country of birth, no. (%) | 1803 | 1266 | 2877 | |||
Spanish | NA | 1184 (93.5) | NA | |||
Latin-American | NA | 56 (4.4) | NA | |||
European/other | NA | 26 (2.1) | NA | |||
Dutch | 1012 (53.2) | NA | NA | |||
Indonesian | 69 (3.6) | NA | NA | |||
Cape Verdean | 58 (3.1) | NA | NA | |||
Moroccan | 115 (6.0) | NA | NA | |||
Surinamese | 154 (8.1) | NA | NA | |||
Turkish | 170 (9.0) | NA | NA | |||
Other, non-Western | 150 (7.9) | NA | NA | |||
Other, Western | 174 (9.1) | NA | NA | |||
White | NA | NA | 2841 (98.7) | |||
Nonwhite | NA | NA | 36 (1.3) | |||
Maternal age, y, mean (SD) | 1931 | 30.5 (4.8) | 1257 | 31.6 (3.9) | 2980 | 28.6 (4.5) |
Parity, no. (%) | 1931 | 1267 | 2877 | |||
0 | 1121 (58.1) | 727 (57.4) | 1346 (46.8) | |||
1 | 564 (29.2) | 458 (36.1) | 992 (34.5) | |||
≥2 | 246 (12.7) | 82 (6.5) | 539 (18.7) | |||
Smoking during pregnancy, no. (%) | 1744 | 1254 | 2926 | |||
Never | 1319 (75.6) | 870 (69.4) | 2434 (83.2) | |||
In the beginning of pregnancy | 168 (9.6) | 168 (13.4) | 125 (4.3) | |||
Continued | 257 (14.7) | 216 (17.2) | 367 (12.5) | |||
Prepregnancy BMI, kg/m2, median (IQR) | 1694 | 22.6 (20.8–25.2) | 1269 | 22.5 (20.8–25.0) | 2713 | 22.2 (20.5–24.4) |
Values are based on unimputed data.
Abbreviations: BMI, body mass index; IQR, interquartile range; NA, not applicable.
Suboptimal is defined as an IQ score <85.
Nonverbal IQ
Using pooled data in the one-step approach, we observed a positive linear association between the UI/Creat and mean nonverbal IQ score [Fig. 2(a) and (30)], although this association was not statistically significant. Using the two-step approach in which we combined cohort-specific effect estimates using random-effects meta-analysis, neither UI/Creat <150 µg/g nor UI/Creat ≥500 µg/g was associated with nonverbal IQ score (−0.6 point; 95%CI: −1.7 to 0.4 points; P = 0.246 and −1.1 points, 95%CI: −4.2 to 2.0 points; P = 0.478) [Fig. 2(b) and 2(c)]. UI/Creat was not associated with suboptimal nonverbal IQ (30).
Verbal IQ
Using the one-step approach, we observed a positive curvilinear association between UI/Creat and verbal IQ score [Fig. 3(a) and (30)]. There was a positive linear association when measures in preschool children from Generation R were excluded. Using the two-step approach, neither UI/Creat <150 µg/g nor UI/Creat ≥500 µg/g was associated with verbal IQ score (−0.6 point, 95% CI: −1.3 to 0.1 points; P = 0.082 and −0.6 point, 95% CI: −2.6 to 1.4 points; P = 0.552, respectively) [Fig 3(b) and 3(c) or suboptimal verbal IQ score (30)].
Effect modification according to gestational age
The continuous association of UI/Creat with nonverbal IQ score did not differ according to gestational age at measurement (P for interaction term = 0.306). By contrast, we identified possible effect modification by gestational age in the association with verbal IQ (P for interaction term = 0.078). Stratification by tertile of gestational age showed a positive curvilinear association of UI/Creat with mean child verbal IQ score, with an overall effect of ∼5 IQ points during the first 12 weeks of pregnancy [Fig. 4(a) and (30)]. Furthermore, there was a positive linear association between UI/Creat and mean child verbal IQ score during the 12th to 14th weeks of pregnancy, with an overall effect of ∼3 IQ points [Fig. 4(b)]. This association was no longer present after the 14th week of pregnancy [Fig. 4(c)].
Iodine status and thyroid function
UI/Creat was not associated with TSH (0.007, 95 CI%: −0.044 to 0.058; P = 0.789) or with FT4 (−0.044, 95 CI%: −0.092 to 0.005; P = 0.079). The association did not change between UI/Creat and child nonverbal or verbal IQ score after adjustment for TSH and/or FT4; there was also no sign of effect modification by TSH or FT4 (data not shown). There was no association of UI/Creat with TSH and FT4 in TPOAb-negative women only.
Discussion
This meta-analysis of individual participant data showed that a lower UI/Creat during pregnancy was associated with lower verbal IQ score. The association of UI/Creat with verbal IQ score was only seen up to the start of the second trimester (up to the 14th week of gestation). In contrast, we observed no associations between IQ score and UI/Creat <150 µg/g or >500 µg/g.
Only a few of the previous single-center studies (i.e., the Generation R and ALSPAC cohort studies) focused on child nonverbal IQ score (7, 14). They found no association between UI/Creat <150 µg/g and nonverbal IQ score. It was suggested that iodine deficiency in the Generation R cohort may not have been severe enough for an association to be identified (14). After combining these two cohorts of contrasting iodine status with a third mildly deficient population (INMA), there was still no effect of iodine deficiency on nonverbal IQ score.
Our meta-analysis using predefined cutoffs showed that UI/Creat <150 µg/g was not associated with lower verbal IQ score. The estimates we found for the ALSPAC contrasted with the strong negative association of maternal UI/Creat <150 µg/g in the first trimester (defined as ≤13 weeks’ gestation) with child verbal IQ score found in a previously published study from that cohort (fully adjusted: −2.9, 95CI%: −5.0 to −0.8; P = 0.006) (7). However, there are a few important differences between the studies. Compared with the previous publication, the ALSPAC data in our study included a larger number of mother-child pairs (2980 vs 958), fewer iodine-deficient women [1831 (61.4%) vs 646 (67.4%)], a study population with a higher UI/Creat [median UI/Creat (IQR): 124 (82 to 199) vs 110 (74 to 170)], and most notably, a higher number of mothers with iodine status measured after the first trimester [1135 (38%) vs 0 (0%); median gestational age (IQR): 12 (8 to 16) weeks vs 10.0 (9 to 12) weeks]. In addition, we adjusted our analysis for a more stringent selection of variables. Comparison between study populations and additional analysis of the association between UI/Creat <150 µg/g and a verbal IQ score in the bottom quartile in the whole cohort and in samples from ≤13 weeks’ gestation are described in an online repository (30).
The importance of iodine status in the preconceptional stage for child IQ has recently been shown (45). In early pregnancy, the fetus is fully dependent on the placental transfer of thyroid hormone to support the crucial processes of brain development (2). There is a need for optimal iodine supply from the initiation of conception, implying that sufficient intrathyroidal iodine stores at the preconception stage may well be critical. Indeed, our results suggest that the fetus is particularly sensitive to suboptimal iodine status in the early stages of pregnancy (e.g., ≤14 weeks of gestation) for optimal development of verbal IQ. Effects on verbal IQ could possibly be explained by the impact of mild iodine deficiency, via thyroid hormone, on the auditory system (13, 46). In our study, we did not find evidence that the association between UI/Creat and verbal IQ was mediated via maternal thyroid function. Possible explanations could be that urinary iodine excretion is a highly volatile and crude measurement of individual iodine status and/or a crude marker of thyroidal iodine availability. Alternatively, it is also possible that the effects are (in part) mediated via fetal thyroid function.
This study confirms that low iodine status is associated with a reduction in verbal IQ scores, putting these children at potential risk for poorer academic achievement (47). Furthermore, our findings may have implications on a national level (e.g., by negatively affecting economic growth) (48). However, there is still inconclusive evidence that supplementation in pregnant women with mild to moderate iodine deficiency is beneficial for child neurodevelopment (11, 15, 16, 49–53). A recent randomized placebo-controlled trial showed no benefit on children’s nonverbal or verbal IQ score with daily supplementation with 200 µg of iodine (as potassium iodide) in women with mild iodine deficiency (52). In addition to the already mentioned limitations of that trial (54), our results provide an explanation for the null finding; the trial randomly assigned women at up to 14 weeks of gestation, whereas we showed that maternal iodine status is particularly important in the first trimester. Although our study needs replication, it suggests that the trial might have missed a critical period of vulnerability in women with iodine deficiency. Our results clearly suggest that additional randomized controlled trials should start with iodine supplementation early in the first trimester or preferably even before pregnancy.
The strengths of our study are as follows: A consistent approach to the analysis and harmonization of potential confounding variables across cohorts optimized comparisons; advanced statistical methods were used to overcome selection bias due to loss to follow-up and missing data; and UI/Creat was used as a marker of iodine status. The latter has been shown to be a more valid measure of iodine excretion when used in groups of the same age and sex (55), though we recognize that a single measure may not be reflective of overall iodine status in an individual. A limitation of the study is that the assessment of IQ was performed with different tools at different ages. Nevertheless, the tools measured the same construct (nonverbal or verbal IQ), and the standardization of IQ scores in each cohort facilitated comparison of results across cohorts. Sensitivity analysis in older children only (e.g., excluding children from Generation R, thus reducing the age range at which verbal IQ was assessed) confirmed the association between UI/Creat and verbal IQ score. Another limitation is that UIC was measured in different laboratories using different assays; it is known that urinary iodine measurements vary between laboratories (56). We used laboratories that were registered with EQUIP, and the use of certified reference materials enabled us to ensure the accuracy of the results.
In conclusion, this study confirms that iodine status in pregnancy is associated with child IQ scores, and results indicate that the development of verbal IQ of the fetus is particularly vulnerable to suboptimal iodine concentration during early pregnancy up until the start of the second trimester. As such, our results suggest that iodine supplementation after the first 14 weeks of pregnancy could be outside the critical period during which iodine availability affects fetal brain development. However, further studies should replicate these data and investigate the effects of iodine supplementation.
Acknowledgments
The Generation R study, Netherlands, is conducted by the Erasmus Medical Center in close collaboration with the Faculty of Social Sciences of the Erasmus University Rotterdam, the Municipal Health Service Rotterdam area, Rotterdam, and the Stichting Trombosedienst & Artsenlaboratorium Rijnmond (STAR-MDC), Rotterdam. The Generation R Study is supported by the Erasmus Medical Center, Rotterdam, the Erasmus University Rotterdam, the Netherlands Organization for Health Research and Development (ZonMw), the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO), and the Ministry of Health, Welfare and Sport. A grant from the Sophia Children’s Hospital Research Funds supports the neurodevelopmental work on thyroid. R.P.P. is supported by a ZonMw VIDI Grant, project number 1717331.
The INMA study, Spain, was funded by grants from UE (FP7-ENV-2011 cod 282957 and HEALTH.2010.2.4.5-1) and Spain: Instituto de Salud Carlos III (Red INMA G03/176; CB06/02/0041; FIS-FEDER: PI041436, PI05/1079, PI06/0867, PI081151, FIS-and PS09/00090, PI11/01007, PI11/02591, PI11/02038, PI13/1944, PI13/2032, PI14/00891, PI14/01687, and PI16/1288; Miguel Servet-FEDER CP11/00178, CP15/00025, and CPII16/00051, MS13/00054), Generalitat Valenciana: FISABIO (UGP 15-230, UGP-15-244, and UGP-15-249), Generalitat de Catalunya-CIRIT 1999SGR 00241, Fundació La marató de TV3 (090430), Department of Health of the Basque Government (2005111093 and 2009111069), and the Provincial Government of Gipuzkoa (DFG06/004 and DFG08/001).
The ALSPAC study, United Kingdom, is supported by the UK Medical Research Council and Wellcome (Grant ref: 102215/2/13/2) and the University of Bristol, which provides core support for the ALSPAC. We are extremely grateful to all the families who took part in this study, the midwives for their help in recruiting them, and the whole ALSPAC team, which includes interviewers, computer and laboratory technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers, managers, receptionists, and nurses. A comprehensive list of grants funding is available on the ALSPAC website (www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac/external/documents/grant-acknowledgements.pdf). The existing iodine measurements in ALSPAC were funded from (i) the NUTRIMENTHE project, which received a research grant from the European Community’s 7th Framework Programme (FP7/2008–2013) under grant agreement 212652; and (ii) a PhD studentship that was funded by Wassen International and the Waterloo Foundation (2009 to 2012).
Financial Support: This study was supported in part by the EUthyroid Project: European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme under grant agreement No. 634453 (to D.L., T.I.M.K., S.C.B., M.D., J.S., M.P.R., M.G., and R.P.P.).
Author Contributions: D.L. did the analyses, interpreted the data, and was involved in writing the manuscript. T.I.M.K., S.C.B., M.D., and M.P.R. contributed to the data analyses, interpretation of the data, and writing of the report. H.T., M.M., M.D., S.L., M.E., J.M.I., and J.S. helped with interpretation of the data and contributed to the writing of the manuscript. A.E.v.H. and Y.B.d.R. helped with the acquisition of data and contributed to the writing of the manuscript. M.G. and R.P.P. supervised the analyses, contributed to the writing of the manuscript, and directed the project.
Disclosure Summary: The authors have nothing to disclose.
Glossary
Abbreviations:
- ALSPAC
Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children
- FT4
free thyroxine
- INMA
INfancia y Medio Ambiente Project
- IQR
interquartile range
- TPOAb
thyroid peroxidase antibody
- UIC
urinary iodine concentration
- UI/Creat
urinary iodine/creatinine ratio
References
- 1. Lavado-Autric R, Ausó E, García-Velasco JV, Arufe M del C, Escobar del Rey F, Berbel P, Morreale de Escobar G. Early maternal hypothyroxinemia alters histogenesis and cerebral cortex cytoarchitecture of the progeny. J Clin Invest. 2003;111(7):1073–1082. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Bernal J. Thyroid hormones and brain development. Vitam Horm. 2005;71:95–122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Trumpff C, De Schepper J, Tafforeau J, Van Oyen H, Vanderfaeillie J, Vandevijvere S. Mild iodine deficiency in pregnancy in Europe and its consequences for cognitive and psychomotor development of children: a review. J Trace Elem Med Biol. 2013;27(3):174–183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Zimmermann MB. Iodine deficiency. Endocr Rev. 2009;30(4):376–408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Iodine Global Network. Global scorecard of iodine nutrition 2017. Available at: www.ign.org/cm_data/IGN_Global_Map_PW_30May2017_1.pdf Accessed 8 May 2018.
- 6. Zimmermann MB, Jooste PL, Pandav CS. Iodine-deficiency disorders. Lancet. 2008;372(9645):1251–1262. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Bath SC, Steer CD, Golding J, Emmett P, Rayman MP. Effect of inadequate iodine status in UK pregnant women on cognitive outcomes in their children: results from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC). Lancet. 2013;382(9889):331–337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Hynes KL, Otahal P, Hay I, Burgess JR. Mild iodine deficiency during pregnancy is associated with reduced educational outcomes in the offspring: 9-year follow-up of the gestational iodine cohort. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2013;98(5):1954–1962. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. van Mil NH, Tiemeier H, Bongers-Schokking JJ, Ghassabian A, Hofman A, Hooijkaas H, Jaddoe VWV, de Muinck Keizer-Schrama SM, Steegers EAP, Visser TJ, Visser W, Ross HA, Verhulst FC, de Rijke YB, Steegers-Theunissen RPM. Low urinary iodine excretion during early pregnancy is associated with alterations in executive functioning in children. J Nutr. 2012;142(12):2167–2174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Abel MH, Caspersen IH, Meltzer HM, Haugen M, Brandlistuen RE, Aase H, Alexander J, Torheim LE, Brantsæter A-L. Suboptimal maternal iodine intake is associated with impaired child neurodevelopment at 3 years of age in the Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study. J Nutr. 2017;147(7):1314–1324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Murcia M, Espada M, Julvez J, Llop S, Lopez-Espinosa M-J, Vioque J, Basterrechea M, Riaño I, González L, Alvarez-Pedrerol M, Tardón A, Ibarluzea J, Rebagliato M. Iodine intake from supplements and diet during pregnancy and child cognitive and motor development: the INMA Mother and Child Cohort Study. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2018;72(3):216–222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Abel MH, Ystrom E, Caspersen IH, Meltzer HM, Aase H, Torheim LE, Askeland RB, Reichborn-Kjennerud T, Brantsæter AL. Maternal iodine intake and offspring attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: results from a large prospective cohort study. Nutrients. 2017;9(11):1239. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Hynes KL, Otahal P, Burgess JR, Oddy WH, Hay I. Reduced educational outcomes persist into adolescence following mild iodine deficiency in utero, despite adequacy in childhood: 15-year follow-up of the gestational iodine cohort investigating auditory processing speed and working memory. Nutrients. 2017;9(12):1354. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Ghassabian A, Steenweg-de Graaff J, Peeters RP, Ross HA, Jaddoe VW, Hofman A, Verhulst FC, White T, Tiemeier H. Maternal urinary iodine concentration in pregnancy and children’s cognition: results from a population-based birth cohort in an iodine-sufficient area. BMJ Open. 2014;4(6):e005520. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Murcia M, Rebagliato M, Iñiguez C, Lopez-Espinosa M-J, Estarlich M, Plaza B, Barona-Vilar C, Espada M, Vioque J, Ballester F. Effect of iodine supplementation during pregnancy on infant neurodevelopment at 1 year of age. Am J Epidemiol. 2011;173(7):804–812. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Rebagliato M, Murcia M, Alvarez-Pedrerol M, Espada M, Fernández-Somoano A, Lertxundi N, Navarrete-Muñoz E-M, Forns J, Aranbarri A, Llop S, Julvez J, Tardón A, Ballester F. Iodine supplementation during pregnancy and infant neuropsychological development: INMA Mother and Child Cohort Study. Am J Epidemiol. 2013;177(9):944–953. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Shi X, Han C, Li C, Mao J, Wang W, Xie X, Li C, Xu B, Meng T, Du J, Zhang S, Gao Z, Zhang X, Fan C, Shan Z, Teng W. Optimal and safe upper limits of iodine intake for early pregnancy in iodine-sufficient regions: a cross-sectional study of 7190 pregnant women in China. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2015;100(4):1630–1638. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Sang Z, Wei W, Zhao N, Zhang G, Chen W, Liu H, Shen J, Liu J, Yan Y, Zhang W. Thyroid dysfunction during late gestation is associated with excessive iodine intake in pregnant women. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2012;97(8):E1363–E1369. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Medici M, Ghassabian A, Visser W, de Muinck Keizer-Schrama SMPF, Jaddoe VWV, Visser WE, Hooijkaas H, Hofman A, Steegers EAP, Bongers-Schokking JJ, Ross HA, Tiemeier H, Visser TJ, de Rijke YB, Peeters RP. Women with high early pregnancy urinary iodine levels have an increased risk of hyperthyroid newborns: the population-based Generation R Study. Clin Endocrinol (Oxf). 2014;80(4):598–606. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Connelly KJ, Boston BA, Pearce EN, Sesser D, Snyder D, Braverman LE, Pino S, LaFranchi SH. Congenital hypothyroidism caused by excess prenatal maternal iodine ingestion. J Pediatr. 2012;161(4):760–762. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Andersson M, de Benoist B, Delange F, Zupan J; WHO Secretariat. Prevention and control of iodine deficiency in pregnant and lactating women and in children less than 2-years-old: conclusions and recommendations of the Technical Consultation. Public Health Nutr. 2007;10(12A):1606–1611. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Alexander EK, Pearce EN, Brent GA, Brown RS, Chen H, Dosiou C, Grobman WA, Laurberg P, Lazarus JH, Mandel SJ, Peeters RP, Sullivan S. 2017 Guidelines of the American Thyroid Association for the diagnosis and management of thyroid disease during pregnancy and the postpartum. Thyroid. 2017;27(3):315–389. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Lazarus J, Brown RS, Daumerie C, Hubalewska-Dydejczyk A, Negro R, Vaidya B. 2014 European thyroid association guidelines for the management of subclinical hypothyroidism in pregnancy and in children. Eur Thyroid J. 2014;3(2):76–94. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Guxens M, Ballester F, Espada M, Fernández MF, Grimalt JO, Ibarluzea J, Olea N, Rebagliato M, Tardón A, Torrent M, Vioque J, Vrijheid M, Sunyer J; INMA Project. Cohort Profile: the INMA--INfancia y Medio Ambiente--(Environment and Childhood) Project. Int J Epidemiol. 2012;41(4):930–940. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Kooijman MN, Kruithof CJ, van Duijn CM, Duijts L, Franco OH, van IJzendoorn MH, de Jongste JC, Klaver CCW, van der Lugt A, Mackenbach JP, Moll HA, Peeters RP, Raat H, Rings EHHM, Rivadeneira F, van der Schroeff MP, Steegers EAP, Tiemeier H, Uitterlinden AG, Verhulst FC, Wolvius E, Felix JF, Jaddoe VWV. The Generation R Study: design and cohort update 2017. Eur J Epidemiol. 2016;31(12):1243–1264. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Boyd A, Golding J, Macleod J, Lawlor DA, Fraser A, Henderson J, Molloy L, Ness A, Ring S, Davey Smith G. Cohort Profile: the ‘children of the 90s’--the index offspring of the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. Int J Epidemiol. 2013;42(1):111–127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Fraser A, Macdonald-Wallis C, Tilling K, Boyd A, Golding J, Davey Smith G, Henderson J, Macleod J, Molloy L, Ness A, Ring S, Nelson SM, Lawlor DA. Cohort Profile: the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children: ALSPAC mothers cohort. Int J Epidemiol. 2013;42(1):97–110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Executive ALSPAC. Access data and samples. Available at:www.bris.ac.uk/alspac/researchers/data-access/data-dictionary/. Accessed 12 March 2018.
- 29. Murcia M, Rebagliato M, Espada M, Vioque J, Santa Marina L, Alvarez-Pedrerol M, Lopez-Espinosa M-J, León G, Iñiguez C, Basterrechea M, Guxens M, Lertxundi A, Perales A, Ballester F, Sunyer J; INMA Study Group. Iodine intake in a population of pregnant women: INMA Mother and Child Cohort Study, Spain. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2010;64(12):1094–1099. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Levie D, Korevaar TIM, Bath SC, Murcia M, Dineva M, Llop S, Espada M, van Herwaarden AE, de Rijke YB, Ibarluzea JM, Sunyer J, Tiemeier H, Rayman MP, Guxens M, Peeters RP. Data from: Association of maternal iodine status with child IQ: a meta-analysis of individual participant data. RePub Repository 2018. Accessed 27 November 2018. https://repub.eur.nl/pub/112369/. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 31. Pearce EN, Lazarus JH, Smyth PP, He X, Smith DF, Pino S, Braverman LE. Urine test strips as a source of iodine contamination. Thyroid. 2009;19(8):919. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Pearce EN, Lazarus JH, Smyth PPA, He X, Dall’amico D, Parkes AB, Burns R, Smith DF, Maina A, Bestwick JP, Jooman M, Leung AM, Braverman LE. Perchlorate and thiocyanate exposure and thyroid function in first-trimester pregnant women. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2010;95(7):3207–3215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Bath SC, Pop VJM, Furmidge-Owen VL, Broeren MAC, Rayman MP. Thyroglobulin as a functional biomarker of iodine status in a cohort study of pregnant women in the United Kingdom. Thyroid. 2017;27(3):426–433. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Korevaar TIM, Muetzel R, Medici M, Chaker L, Jaddoe VWV, de Rijke YB, Steegers EAP, Visser TJ, White T, Tiemeier H, Peeters RP. Association of maternal thyroid function during early pregnancy with offspring IQ and brain morphology in childhood: a population-based prospective cohort study. Lancet Diabetes Endocrinol. 2016;4(1):35–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Julvez J, Alvarez-Pedrerol M, Rebagliato M, Murcia M, Forns J, Garcia-Esteban R, Lertxundi N, Espada M, Tardón A, Riaño Galán I, Sunyer J. Thyroxine levels during pregnancy in healthy women and early child neurodevelopment. Epidemiology. 2013;24(1):150–157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Nelson SM, Haig C, McConnachie A, Sattar N, Ring SM, Smith GD, Lawlor DA, Lindsay RS. Maternal thyroid function and child educational attainment: prospective cohort study. BMJ. 2018;360:k452. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Tellegen PJ, Winkel M, Wijnberg-Williams BJ, Laros JA. Snijders-Oomen Nonverbal Intelligence Test. SON-R 21/2-7 Manual and Research Report. Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger B.V.; 1998. Available at:www.testresearch.nl/sonr/sonr257manual.pdf. Accessed 25 May 2018. [Google Scholar]
- 38. Fenson L, Pethick S, Renda C, Cox JL, Dale PS, Reznick JS. Short-form versions of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories. Appl Psycholinguist. 2000;21(1):95–116. [Google Scholar]
- 39. McCarthy D. McCarthy Scales of Children’s Abilities. New York, NY:Psychological Corporation; 1972. [Google Scholar]
- 40. Wechsler D, Golombok S, Rust J. WISC-III UK Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children. Sidcup, UK: Psychological Corporation; 1992. [Google Scholar]
- 41. Selvin S. Statistical Analysis of Epidemiologic Data. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 1996. [Google Scholar]
- 42. Higgins JP, Thompson SG. Quantifying heterogeneity in a meta-analysis. Stat Med. 2002;21(11):1539–1558. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Weisskopf MG, Sparrow D, Hu H, Power MC. Biased exposure-health effect estimates from selection in cohort studies: are environmental studies at particular risk? Environ Health Perspect. 2015;123(11):1113–1122. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Sterne JAC, White IR, Carlin JB, Spratt M, Royston P, Kenward MG, Wood AM, Carpenter JR. Multiple imputation for missing data in epidemiological and clinical research: potential and pitfalls. BMJ. 2009;338(jun29 1):b2393. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Robinson SM, Crozier SR, Miles EA, Gale CR, Calder PC, Cooper C, Inskip HM, Godfrey KM. Preconception maternal iodine status is positively associated with IQ but not with measures of executive function in childhood. J Nutr. 2018;148(6):959–966. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Melse-Boonstra A, Mackenzie I. Iodine deficiency, thyroid function and hearing deficit: a review. Nutr Res Rev. 2013;26(2):110–117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ, Ridder EM. Show me the child at seven II: childhood intelligence and later outcomes in adolescence and young adulthood. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2005;46(8):850–858. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Lynn R, Vanhanen T. National IQs: a review of their educational, cognitive, economic, political, demographic, sociological, epidemiological, geographic and climatic correlates. Intelligence. 2012;40(2):226–234. [Google Scholar]
- 49. Berbel P, Mestre JL, Santamaría A, Palazón I, Franco A, Graells M, González-Torga A, de Escobar GM. Delayed neurobehavioral development in children born to pregnant women with mild hypothyroxinemia during the first month of gestation: the importance of early iodine supplementation. Thyroid. 2009;19(5):511–519. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Velasco I, Carreira M, Santiago P, Muela JA, García-Fuentes E, Sánchez-Muñoz B, Garriga MJ, González-Fernández MC, Rodríguez A, Caballero FF, Machado A, González-Romero S, Anarte MT, Soriguer F. Effect of iodine prophylaxis during pregnancy on neurocognitive development of children during the first two years of life. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2009;94(9):3234–3241. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Santiago P, Velasco I, Muela JA, Sánchez B, Martínez J, Rodriguez A, Berrio M, Gutierrez-Repiso C, Carreira M, Moreno A, García-Fuentes E, Soriguer F. Infant neurocognitive development is independent of the use of iodised salt or iodine supplements given during pregnancy. Br J Nutr. 2013;110(5):831–839. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Gowachirapant S, Jaiswal N, Melse-Boonstra A, Galetti V, Stinca S, Mackenzie I, Thomas S, Thomas T, Winichagoon P, Srinivasan K, Zimmermann MB. Effect of iodine supplementation in pregnant women on child neurodevelopment: a randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. Lancet Diabetes Endocrinol. 2017;5(11):853–863. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Zhou SJ, Skeaff SA, Ryan P, Doyle LW, Anderson PJ, Kornman L, Mcphee AJ, Yelland LN, Makrides M. The effect of iodine supplementation in pregnancy on early childhood neurodevelopment and clinical outcomes: results of an aborted randomised placebo-controlled trial. Trials. 2015;16(1):563. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. Bath SC. Iodine supplementation in pregnancy in mildly deficient regions. Lancet Diabetes Endocrinol. 2017;5(11):840–841. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Knudsen N, Christiansen E, Brandt-Christensen M, Nygaard B, Perrild H. Age- and sex-adjusted iodine/creatinine ratio: a new standard in epidemiological surveys? Evaluation of three different estimates of iodine excretion based on casual urine samples and comparison to 24 h values. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2000;54(4):361–363. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56. Ittermann T, Johner S, Below H, Leiterer M, Thamm M, Remer T, Völzke H. Interlaboratory variability of urinary iodine measurements. Clin Chem Lab Med. 2018;56(3):441–447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]