Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2020 Oct 1.
Published in final edited form as: Ann Epidemiol. 2019 Aug 21;38:28–34.e2. doi: 10.1016/j.annepidem.2019.08.007

Marital Status and Cognitive Impairment in the United States: Evidence from the National Health and Aging Trends Study

Hui Liu 1, Yan Zhang 1, Sarah A Burgard 2, Belinda L Needham 3
PMCID: PMC6812624  NIHMSID: NIHMS1538114  PMID: 31591027

Abstract

Purpose

We provide population-based longitudinal evidence of marital status differences in the risk of cognitive impairment and dementia in the United States.

Methods

Data were from the longitudinal National Health and Aging Trends Study (NHATS) 2011–2018. The sample included 7,508 respondents age 65 and over who contributed 25,897 person-year records. We estimated discrete-time hazard models to predict the risk of dementia and cognitive impairment, not dementia (CIND) as well as impairment in three major cognitive domains: memory, orientation, and executive function.

Results

Relative to their married counterparts, divorced and widowed elders had higher odds of dementia and CIND, as well as higher odds of impairment in each of the cognitive domains. Never-married elders had higher odds of impairment in memory and orientation than their married counterparts, but did not differ significantly in the odds of impaired executive function, dementia, or CIND. Cohabiting elders did not differ significantly from married respondents on any measure of cognitive impairment. We found no gender differences in the associations between marital status and the measures of cognitive impairment.

Conclusions

Marital status is a potentially important but overlooked social risk/protective factor for cognitive impairment. Divorced and widowed older adults are particularly vulnerable to cognitive impairment.

Keywords: cognitive impairment, dementia, marital status, divorce, widowhood

Introduction

With the rapid aging of the U.S. population, dementia and cognitive impairment have emerged as serious and growing public health concerns [1,2]. Dementia is a stage of severe cognitive impairment that is associated with disability, premature death, and increased need for medical and personal care [1,36]. In 2018, about 5.7 million people in the United States were living with dementias and the annual estimated cost of dementia care would reach $277 billion [3]. In recent decades, researchers have devoted serious efforts to identifying risk factors for dementia and designing preventive strategies. These efforts have focused predominantly on proximate behavioral and biological factors. Although the etiology of cognitive impairment and dementia extends beyond these factors [1,2], there is less research on social risk factors for dementia [1,712]. For example, while a number of studies have shown that married people are healthier, both mentally and physically, and live longer than unmarried people [1328], it is unclear whether marital status is associated with the risk of cognitive impairment, particularly in the U.S. population. In this study, we assess marital status as a potential social risk/protective factor, examining its association with cognitive impairment and progression of dementia among older adults in the United States. Given prior evidence of gender differences in the association between marital status and health [17,19,21,29,30], we also consider whether the associations of marital status with cognitive outcomes differ for women and men.

A small group of studies based on regional and community samples outside the United States have explored basic patterns of marital status differences in dementia, and revealed mixed evidence [3138]. For example, a study of Swedish adults found that unmarried men and women had a significantly higher risk of developing dementia than their married counterparts [31]. An earlier study among a cohort from southwestern France found that never-married older adults had a higher risk of dementia and Alzheimer’s disease (AD), the most common type of dementia, than their married and cohabiting counterparts, but that neither the risk of dementia nor the risk of AD was elevated among divorced and widowed older adults [32]. In contrast, a study of the Korean population found that being divorced, widowed, or single was associated with a greater risk of AD [36]. Further, a study of the Chinese population found that never-married and widowed Chinese men had greater odds of cognitive impairment than married Chinese men, but did not identify significant marital status differences among Chinese women [37]. A meta-analysis of 15 studies using data from 812,047 participants (all from outside the United States) found that both never-married and widowed people had a higher risk of dementia (42% and 20% higher, respectively) than married people [38].

The United States has witnessed remarkable changes in marriage in the past decades, accompanied by rapid population aging. Divorce rates among older Americans doubled between 1990 and 2010 [39]. Over 1/3 of American marriages end in divorce by age 55 and over in 2009 [40]. Surprisingly, research on cognitive impairment in the United States, mostly based on regional samples, either ignored marital status or simply included it as a covariate. For example, a recent study of the Framingham Heart Study Offspring cohort in Massachusetts examined a range of lifestyle-related risk factors for dementia and found widowhood was related to increased risk of dementia [41]. Studies of older residents aged 70–89 in Olmsted County Minnesota found that being never married and previously married (widowed or divorced) were related to higher risk of mild cognitive impairment for men but not for women [42,43]. A study of 1,221 married older couples aged 65 and older in a rural county in northern Utah reported that among subjects whose spouse had dementia, men were at greater risk of developing dementia than women [44]. Nevertheless, nationally representative studies on marital status differences in cognitive impairment and dementia in the United States are limited.

Data and Sample

Data were drawn from the National Health and Aging Trends Study (NHATS) 2011–2018, which was conducted by the Johns Hopkins University Bloomberg School of Public Health in collaboration with the University of Michigan. NHATS gathers information, through annual in-person interviews, from a nationally representative sample of Medicare beneficiaries age 65 and older who live in communities, residential care, or nursing homes within the contiguous United States (i.e., excluding Alaska, Hawaii, and Puerto Rico) in order to foster research that will reduce disability, maximize health and independent functioning, and enhance quality of life at older ages [45]. NHATS used Medicare’s enrollment database as the sampling frame and oversampled older persons and Black individuals [45]. In 2011, 8,245 respondents age 65 and above completed the initial (Wave 1) interview (71% response rate). Respondents have been re-interviewed annually to document changes over time, with the most recently released follow-up being the 2018 wave.

We excluded nursing home residents in the analysis because they were not eligible for the NHATS sample person (SP) interview where most of our analytic variables were derived. We further restricted the analysis to respondents who had complete data on cognitive measures and other key variables. The final sample includes 7,508 respondents (3,135 men and 4,373 women), who contributed 25,897 person-year records (10,614 person-years for men and 15,283 person-years for women). In our baseline sample, 5,527 respondents had normal cognition, 982 had CIND, and 999 had dementia. In terms of marital status, the baseline sample included 3,609 married, 151 cohabiting, 2,542 widowed, 913 divorced, and 293 never married respondents. Table A1 in the Appendix provides detailed frequencies for marital status and cognitive status, including transitions across waves.

Measures

Cognitive impairment and dementia

NHATS respondents completed a series of performance-based tests that measured their cognitive status. These cognitive tests evaluated three key domains of cognitive functioning: memory (immediate and delayed 10-word recall), orientation (date, month, year, and day of the week; naming the president and vice president), and executive function (clock drawing test) [4648]. Following previous studies, we defined impairment as a cognitive score of 1.5 standard deviations below the mean or lower [4648]. The cutoff points for the specific domains of impairment were <=3 for orientation (range=0–8) and memory (range=0–20), and <=1 for executive functioning (range=0–5). We analyzed cognitive impairment (1=yes, 0=no) in these three domains separately. Following revised criteria for diagnosing Alzheimer’s disease and related dementia [48,49], we also created an overall measure of cognitive impairment/dementia by combining the three domains and categorizing respondents into three groups: 1) dementia, defined as impairment in at least two domains; 2) cognitive impairment, not dementia (CIND), defined as impairment in only one domain; and 3) normal cognition, defined as impairment in no domain. These cognitive test criteria yield prevalence rates of dementia that align with rates calculated from reports of diagnosis [46].

For those respondents who could not participate in the study by themselves, the NHATS survey was completed by a proxy (a spouse or adult child). About half of the proxies said the sample person could participate in the cognitive tests, and in those cases, the sample respondent completed the tests. For respondents who were unable to complete the cognitive tests (1.66%), cognitive status was measured via the proxy’s report of AD diagnosis or their responses to the Ascertain Dementia 8 (AD8), one of the most frequently used information-based instruments for assessing early memory loss, temporal orientation, judgment, and function [50,51]. In these cases, the sample person was categorized as having dementia if the proxy reported that the sample person had been diagnosed with dementia or if the AD8 score met the criteria for likely dementia (score ≥2).

Marital status

Marital status was measured as a time-varying covariate reflecting marital status at the time of the survey, with five categories: married (reference), cohabiting, divorced/separated, widowed, and never married.

Other covariates

Age was categorized into six groups: 65–69 (reference), 70–74, 75–79, 80–84, 85–89, and 90 and older. Race/ethnicity was self-reported and included four categories: non-Hispanic white (reference), non-Hispanic black, Hispanic, and other. Education included four categories: less than high school (reference), high school degree or equivalent, some college, and college graduate. Nativity indicated whether the respondent was born in the United States (1=yes, 0=no). Proxy-report indicated whether dementia status was reported by a proxy (1=proxy-report, 0=self-report). Age was measured as a time-varying covariate; all other covariates were time-invariant based on Wave 1 data.

Statistical Methods

To compare the risk of cognitive impairment across marital status groups, we estimated discrete-time hazard models. Specifically, we created person-year record files and then estimated two types of models: binary logit models to predict cognitive impairment in each of the specific domains and multinomial logit models to predict overall dementia/CIND risk. A respondent contributes an observation for each wave at which they were interviewed, up to the onset of impairment or right censoring (i.e., loss to follow-up or death). The discrete-time hazard model is specified as:

logh(tij)h0(tij)=j=18αjDij+XiB1+ZijB2 (1)

where h(tij) indicates the discrete-hazard (i.e., conditional probability) of the onset of impairment for individual i at wave j. h0(tij) indicates the discrete-hazard of baseline cognitive status for individual i at wave j. j=18αjDij represents the set of intercepts for the eight periods/years of NHATS 2011–2018, one per period. Xi indicates the vector of time-invariant covariates and Zij indicates the vector of time-varying covariates including marital status. B1 and B2 are corresponding coefficient vectors. We conducted two models: Model 1 estimated the main effects of marital status and Model 2 added the interaction terms for marital status categories by gender. All covariates were included in both models. All analyses were weighted using the wave-specific weight.

Results

Table 1 shows the descriptive statistics of unweighted frequencies and weighted proportions for all analyzed variables for the total sample as well as by marital status. The prevalence of CIND was significantly higher among widowed (8.39%) and divorced (6.50%) respondents than among married respondents (5.27%). The prevalence of dementia was higher among widowed (6.31%), never married (5.59%) and divorced (3.59%) respondents than among married respondents (2.70%). The prevalence of impairment in orientation was higher among widowed (6.44%) and never-married (5.39%) respondents than among married respondents (2.65%). The prevalence of impairment in executive function was higher among divorced (4.52%) and widowed (5.38%) respondents than among married respondents (3.26%). Finally, the prevalence of impairment in memory was higher among divorced (5.67%), widowed (9.31%) and never-married (7.00%) respondents than among married respondents (4.17%). Cohabiting and married respondents did not have significantly different prevalence of cognitive impairment.

Table 1.

Descriptive Statistics of Person-Period Files (unweighted frequencies and weighted proportions), NHATS 2011–18

Variables Total Married Cohabiting Divorced & Separated Widowed Never married

N % N % N % N % N % N %
Cognitive impairment
 Normal cognition 22,440 89.86 11,431 92.02 462 93.07 2,833 89.91* 6,937 85.30* 777 88.07*
 CIND 2,044 6.26 814 5.27 33 3.95 244 6.50* 873 8.39* 80 6.34
 Dementia 1,413 3.88 480 2.70 20 2.98 140 3.59* 693 6.31* 80 5.59*
 Impairment in orientation a. 1,623 3.87 522 2.65 22 1.90 153 3.23 838 6.44* 88 5.39*
 Impairment in executive function b. 1,569 4.08 569 3.26 21 2.63 191 4.52* 705 5.38* 83 5.14
 Impairment in memory c. 2,299 5.84 789 4.17 34 3.57 250 5.67* 1,113 9.31* 113 7.00*
Male 10,614 42.74 7,320 56.07 285 53.30 1,122 34.01* 1,555 18.91* 332 37.69*
Age groups
 65–69 2,820 16.34 1,731 19.36 112 27.85* 480 19.92 360 6.91* 137 19.54
 70–74 6,362 31.48 3,691 35.01 173 37.80 1,079 39.61* 1,141 19.25* 278 35.46
 75–79 6,051 23.29 3,310 24.29 107 19.98 795 22.73 1,643 22.18* 196 20.62
 80–84 5,224 15.60 2,373 13.63 62 8.14* 523 11.75 2,100 22.55* 166 12.91
 85–89 3,471 9.17 1,235 6.21 45 4.82 237 4.26* 1,849 18.11* 105 8.42
 90+ 1,969 4.12 385 1.52 16 1.42 103 1.74 1,410 11.01* 55 3.05
Race/ethnicity
 White 19,399 84.60 10,264 87.29 395 83.67 1,928 76.22* 6,246 84.18* 566 75.97*
 Black 4,742 7.18 1,589 4.60 72 5.04 1,018 13.55* 1,756 8.70* 307 14.97*
 Hispanics 1,130 5.15 538 4.88 23 5.16 168 6.30 344 4.72 57 8.47
 Others 626 3.07 334 3.23 25 6.13 103 3.93 157 2.40 7 0.58*
Education
 Less than high school 4,966 15.99 1,849 12.78 103 19.94 686 17.13* 2,088 21.12* 240 20.69*
 High school 8,953 34.77 4,064 32.19 165 32.57 1,038 32.57 3,431 42.25* 255 28.03
 Some college 8,548 34.47 4,623 36.94 190 35.36 1,121 37.12 2,357 28.65* 257 29.87
 College above 3,430 14.77 2,189 18.09 57 12.13 372 13.18* 627 7.98* 185 21.41
Proxy report 506 1.66 197 1.38 8 1.52 42 1.00 213 2.02* 46 5.58
Born in US 23,692 90.72 11,590 90.17 486 93.99 2,920 90.80 7,863 92.01 833 87.05

N of respondents 7,508 100.00 3,609 100.00 151 100.00 913 100.00 2,542 100.00 293 100.00
N of person-periods 25,897 100.00 12,725 100.00 515 100.00 3,217 100.00 8,503 100.00 937 100.00

Note:

*

Difference between married and the specific unmarried group is significant at p < .05.

a.

N of person-periods=30,625.

b.

N of person-periods=30,266.

c.

N of person-periods=28,730.

Table 2 presents the estimated odds ratios of cognitive impairment from the discrete-time hazard models. The results of Model 1 show that both divorced and widowed respondents had significantly higher odds of all types of cognitive impairment—CIND, dementia, and impairment in each specific domain—after controlling for all covariates. Specifically, compared to their married counterparts, those who were divorced had 29% higher odds of CIND (OR=1.29, 95% CI=1.11,1.48), 42% higher odds of dementia (OR=1.42, 95% CI=1.11,1.80), 23% higher odds of orientation impairment (OR=1.23, 95% CI=1.01,1.50), 45% higher odds of executive function impairment (OR=1.45, 95% CI=1.18, 1.78), and 42% higher odds of memory impairment (OR=1.42, 95% CI=1.23, 1.65). The widowed group had 25% higher odds of CIND than the married group (OR=1.25, 95% CI=1.04, 1.50), as well as 35% higher odds of dementia (OR=1.35, 95% CI=1.10, 1.65), 31% higher odds of orientation impairment (OR=1.31, 95% CI=1.09, 1.58), 23% higher odds of executive function impairment (OR=1.23, 95% CI=1.02, 1.48), and 39% higher odds of memory impairment (OR=1.39, 95% CI=1.22, 1.57). Never-married respondents had 66% and 45% higher odds of impairment in orientation (OR=1.66, 95% CI=1.08, 2.56) and memory (OR=1.45, 95% CI=1.09, 1.93), respectively, than their married counterparts, but did not have significantly different odds of CIND, dementia, or executive function impairment. Cohabiting respondents did not differ significantly from married respondents in any of the cognitive outcomes. The results of Model 2 (Table 2) show no significant gender interactions, suggesting that the associations between marital status and cognitive outcomes did not vary by gender.

Table 2.

Adjusted Odds Ratios from Discrete-time Hazard Models, NHATS 2011–18

Cognitive Impairment Base Category: Normal Cognition Impairment in Separate Domains

CIND Dementia Orientation Executive Function Memory

Model 1
Marital status (ref: Married)
 Cohabiting 0.76 (0.46–1.23) 1.16 (0.65–2.06) 0.75 (0.49–1.13) 0.88 (0.48–1.60) 0.94 (0.61–1.45)
 Divorced 1.29*** (1.11–1.48) 1.42** (1.11–1.80) 1.23* (1.01–1.50) 1.45*** (1.18–1.78) 1.42*** (1.23–1.65)
 Widowed 1.25* (1.04–1.50) 1.35** (1.10–1.65) 1.31** (1.09–1.58) 1.23* (1.02–1.48) 1.39*** (1.22–1.57)
 Never married 1.13 (0.74–1.73) 1.31 (0.78–2.19) 1.66* (1.08–2.56) 1.37 (0.88–2.13) 1.45* (1.09–1.93)
Model 2
Marital status (ref: Married)
 Cohabiting 0.71 (0.25–2.00) 0.68 (0.25–1.83) 0.83 (0.32–2.18) 1.10 (0.45–2.67) 0.88 (0.40–1.90)
 Divorced 1.42** (1.10–1.84) 1.24 (0.88–1.75) 1.13 (0.83–1.54) 1.31 (0.97–1.78) 1.51** (1.16–1.97)
 Widowed 1.38** (1.09–1.74) 1.22 (0.95–1.55) 1.28 (0.98–1.66) 1.13 (0.89–1.44) 1.34** (1.11–1.61)
 Never married 1.19 (0.68–2.08) 1.01 (0.53–1.91) 1.75 (0.97–3.16) 1.27 (0.75–2.14) 1.39 (0.97–1.99)
Male 1.63*** (1.26–2.10) 0.97 (0.75–1.25) 1.11 (0.87–1.42) 1.22 (0.94–1.57) 1.30** (1.07–1.58)
Marital status × male
 Cohabiting × male 1.12 (0.31–4.03) 2.28 (0.82–6.33) 0.84 (0.24–2.98) 0.68 (0.22–2.10) 1.12 (0.40–3.15)
 Divorced × male 0.83 (0.52–1.31) 1.29 (0.75–2.23) 1.21 (0.74–2.00) 1.21 (0.78–1.87) 0.86 (0.57–1.28)
 Widowed × male 0.80 (0.58–1.10) 1.21 (0.84–1.75) 1.06 (0.74–1.51) 1.20 (0.85–1.69) 1.13 (0.88–1.44)
 Never married × male 0.94 (0.49–1.78) 1.90 (0.85–4.24) 0.84 (0.45–1.59) 1.15 (0.66–1.99) 1.10 (0.67–1.82)

N of respondents 7,508 7,508 7,508 7,508
N of person-periods 25,897 30,625 30,266 28,730
***

p<.001

**

p<.01

*

p<.05

Note: All models control for gender, age, race/ethnicity, education, proxy report, and born in the U.S.

Sensitivity analysis

We conducted a series of sensitivity analyses to test the robustness of the results. First, we excluded all cases with any cognitive impairment at the baseline survey to eliminate the influence of the baseline association between marital status and cognition. The resulting analysis focused on the incidence of impairment across waves (results shown in Appendix A, Table A2). In a second set of models, we excluded proxy cases (i.e., we restricted the analysis to self-reporting respondents) to determine whether including proxy cases had introduced bias in the estimates (results shown in Appendix A, Table A3). Finally, the results of additional analyses (not shown but available upon request) showed that, on average, those who were lost to follow-up were older, less educated, and more likely to be unmarried and cognitively impaired than those who had complete information, suggesting that the primary sample excluded the most vulnerable respondents, and therefore, the main estimates of cognitive impairment might be overly conservative. Thus, in a third set of models we restricted the analysis to respondents who had complete follow-up information across all waves to test whether loss to follow-up had introduced bias in the estimates (results shown in Appendix A, Table A4). All sensitivity test results were similar to the findings reported in the paper, although the significance levels of some effects declined due to smaller sample sizes.

Discussion

This study provides population-based evidence of marital status differences in cognitive impairment and dementia in the United States. A number of studies have shown that married people are healthier (both mentally and physically) and live longer than unmarried people [17,19,24,25,27], but few of these studies have examined cognitive health. Our analysis of nationally representative longitudinal data from the NHATS extends the evidence of this long- observed marital advantage to cognitive impairment and dementia, which are emerging public health concerns in the context of rapid population aging. Results suggest that marital status is a potentially important but understudied social risk/protective factor for cognitive impairment and the development of dementia, and that there is significant heterogenity in cognitive health across marital status.

We found that divorced and widowed respondents were the most disadvantaged in cognitive health in late life. Compared to their married counterparts, divorced and widowed older adults had significantly higher odds of impairment in all examined cognitive domains as well as in overall cognitive impairment and dementia. These results are consistent with our expectations as well as with the general literature suggesting that married people enjoy better health than unmarried people, in particular when compared to those who were previously married [19,21,23,24,26]. The results are also consistent with the previous studies conducted outside the United States, which found significantly higher risks of dementia among the divorced and widowed than among the married [32,37,39]. Further, this study found that the cognitive disadvantage experienced by divorced and widowed people relative to married people occurred in all the assessed domains of cognition (i.e., memory, orientation, and executive function), suggesting that limitations in each specific domain of cognition contribute to overall cognitive disadvantage.

Never-married individuals also had higher odds of impairment in memory and orientation than their married counterparts, but did not differ in the domain of executive function or the overall odds of either cognitive impairment or dementia. Although the specific reasons for this variation across cognitive outcomes remain unclear, this finding is consistent with previous studies that found mixed evidence for health differences between never-married and married individuals, with some suggesting moderate differences [1820] and others suggesting no differences [52]. In fact, there is some evidence that in recent decades, the never married have become more like the married in terms of self-rated overall health [19]. Nevertheless, our sample of never-married respondents is small relative to the samples of divorced and widowed respondents, which may have limited statistical power and increased the risk of Type 2 error.

This is one of the first studies to compare the risk of cognitive impairment among cohabitors and married people. Previous studies, based primarily on European data, have combined married and cohabiting respondents in studies of the association between marital status and dementia risk [34]. We found no significant difference in cognitive impairment between cohabitors and married respondents, which is consistent with previous literature suggesting that cohabitation and marriage tend to be similar in predicting health outcomes among older people [51], although the meanings and functions of these two types of unions differ for younger people [52]. Nevertheless, we note that the sample of cohabitors is the smallest of all the marital status groups in the NHATS. Among the cohabitors in the focal sample, only 20 had dementia and 33 had CIND across the entire study period. Therefore, the findings for this specific group should be interpreted with caution.

Next, we hypothesized that the associations of marital status with cognitive impairment and dementia may vary by gender for several reasons. For example, the prevalence of dementia appears to differ for men and women, although the evidence is mixed, with some studies suggesting that women suffer more memory problems, faster rates of cognitive decline, and a higher risk of dementia [5355], and others suggesting that the incidence of vascular dementia is higher for men than women in all age groups [56]. In addition, within traditional marriages, women tend to take on more responsibilities for maintaining social connections to family and friends and are more likely to provide physical care and emotional support for their spouse —factors that promote health and may also reduce the risk of cognitive impairment for married men [17,19,21,29,30]. Contrary to our expectations, we found no gender differences in the links between marital status and cognitive impairment. Although prior studies of other health outcomes suggest that men tend to receive more health benefits from marriage than women, and women are more psychologically and physiologically vulnerable to marital stress than men [17,26,29], the current results suggest that being divorced or widowed is related to a higher risk of cognitive impairment for both men and women. Future studies should investigate the specific pathways by which divorce and widowhood affect cognitive health for older men and women, and whether the specific pathways vary by gender, even if the overall pattern of association does not.

Being married is associated with multiple advantages that may translate to cognitive health and reduced dementia risk. For example, married people have access to greater economic resources than unmarried people through pooled income and health insurance via a spouse’s employment [14,16,57]. These economic resources available to married individuals and, to a lesser degree, cohabitors may enhance overall health status and cognitive capacity through providing better nutrition and care in the event of illness, and allowing the purchase of medical treatment and other health-enhancing products [14,16]. Having a spouse is also an important source of social support (e.g., love, advice, and care) and enlarges individuals’ networks by connecting them with, for example, the spouse’s friends and family. A growing number of studies suggest that social engagement (i.e., degree of participation in a community or society) and a larger network size may reduce the risk of dementia by improving cognitive reserves, which strengthen the ability to cope with neuropathological damage [38,58]. Conversely, divorce and widowhood may undermine health and increase stress [17,21,59,60], which is pathogenic and associated with a higher risk of cognitive impairment and dementia [61,62]. Future research should investigate these specific mechanisms through which marital status is linked to cognitive impairment. Such research will be necessary for the development of specific evidence-driven interventions and policy guidelines that promote cognitive well-being and reduce dementia risk among vulnerable groups such as the divorced and widowed.

This study has several limitations. First, the measures of cognitive impairments are based on cognitive tests and proxy reports rather than clinical diagnosis. Although previous research has demonstrated that using the NHATS cognitive tests yields prevalence estimates of dementia that align with estimates calculated from reports of diagnosis [46], the issue of potential misclassification cannot be ignored. Second, the analytical sample is relatively small for some of the unmarried groups, such as the never married and cohabitors. Thus, future studies should use datasets with larger sample sizes to confirm or refute the current findings. Third, although we controlled for basic demographic covariates in the analysis, there are other important confounding psychosocial and neurological factors that may influence cognition and marital status, which should be considered in future analyses. Finally, although we developed research hypotheses based on causal implications from previous studies, the current analysis focused primarily on documenting general associations rather than determining causality. Future studies should use a clearly defined causal estimand with the exposure and outcome mechanism specified via the guidance of causal theories to better understand the causal process underlying the association between marital status and cognitive impairment.

Conclusions

The study results suggest that being divorced or widowed at older ages may be a risk factor for cognitive impairment and progression to dementia for both men and women, highlighting that divorced and widowed older adults may be particularly vulnerable to dementia. The number of divorced and widowed older adults in the United States continues to grow as people live longer and their marital histories become more complex. Therefore, it is important to further explore the complex life-course characteristics of marital relationships (e.g., marital histories, marital quality, and marital duration) that might contribute to the risk of cognitive impairment and dementia so that effective interventions can be implemented to reduce those risks. The current findings imply that routine medical dementia screening and cognition-promoting intervention strategies should be customized according to marital status in order to address key pathways to reduce the risk of cognitive impairment and dementia.

Supplementary Material

1

List of abbreviations

NHATS

National Health and Aging Trends Study

AD

Alzheimer’s disease

CIND

Cognitive impairment, not dementia

AD8

Ascertain Dementia 8

Appendix A

Table A1.

Unweighted Frequencies of Marital Status and Cognitive Status at the Baseline Survey and Subsequent Follow-Up

Men (n=3,135) Women (n=4,373) Total (N=7,508)
Baseline marital status
 Married 2,124 1,485 3,609
 Cohabiting 93 58 151
 Divorced 331 582 913
 Widowed 477 2,065 2,542
 Never married 110 183 293
Marital transitions across waves
 Transition to widowhood in Waves 2–8 192 266 458
 Transition to divorce in Waves 2–8 33 19 52
 Transition to (re)marriage in Waves 2–8 23 11 34
Baseline cognitive status
 Normal cognition 2,306 3,221 5,527
 CIND 459 523 982
 Dementia 370 629 999
 Impairment in orientation 281 475 756
 Impairment in executive function 326 399 725
 Impairment in memory 471 658 1,129
Cognitive transitions across waves
 Normal cognition to CIND waves 2–8 448 614 1,062
 Normal cognition/CIND to dementia waves 2–8 150 264 414

Table A2.

Adjusted Odds Ratios from Discrete-time Hazard Models (Sample with Baseline Cognitive Impairments Excluded), NHATS 2011–18

Cognitive Impairment Base Category: Normal Cognition Impairment in Separate Domains

CIND Dementia Orientation Executive Function Memory

Model 1
Marital status (ref: Married)
 Cohabiting 0.85 (0.43–1.68) 1.90 (0.89–4.07) 0.72 (0.30–1.73) 0.85 (0.37–1.95) 1.26 (0.65–2.44)
 Divorced 1.31** (1.08–1.58) 2.00** (1.27–3.17) 1.31 (0.91–1.88) 1.76** (1.26–2.46) 1.69*** (1.34–2.12)
 Widowed 1.17 (0.95–1.43) 1.47* (1.03–2.10) 1.34* (1.03–1.73) 1.30 (0.97–1.74) 1.51*** (1.22–1.87)
 Never married 1.11 (0.69–1.78) 0.64 (0.26–1.58) 1.46 (0.83–2.58) 0.73 (0.39–1.39) 1.34 (0.88–2.04)
Model 2
Marital status (ref: Married)
 Cohabiting 0.99 (0.27–3.63) 0.60 (0.18–1.98) 0.46 (0.10–2.19) 1.41 (0.43–4.66) 0.72 (0.24–2.16)
 Divorced 1.44* (1.05–1.98) 1.59 (0.82–3.05) 1.12 (0.72–1.74) 1.65* (1.02–2.68) 1.72** (1.16–2.58)
 Widowed 1.26 (0.97–1.63) 1.21 (0.84–1.75) 1.22 (0.87–1.70) 1.17 (0.83–1.65) 1.50** (1.13–1.98)
 Never married 1.12 (0.62–2.01) 0.54 (0.18–1.57) 1.75 (0.74–4.10) 0.38* (0.15–0.96) 1.30 (0.78–2.18)
Male 1.49** (1.13–1.97) 0.80 (0.56–1.16) 1.14 (0.80–1.63) 1.17 (0.81–1.69) 1.42* (1.07–1.89)
Marital status × male
 Cohabiting × male 0.78 (0.15–4.00) 5.37* (1.26–22.86) 1.97 (0.30–12.75) 0.31 (0.06–1.50) 2.22 (0.51–9.65)
 Divorced × male 0.82 (0.49–1.37) 1.62 (0.64–4.11) 1.37 (0.68–2.74) 1.10 (0.60–2.00) 0.94 (0.53–1.69)
 Widowed × male 0.84 (0.60–1.18) 1.51 (0.86–2.64) 1.25 (0.77–2.03) 1.28 (0.79–2.07) 1.01 (0.69–1.48)
 Never married × male 1.03 (0.45–2.32) 1.43 (0.28–7.37) 0.44 (0.10–1.97) 3.62* (1.05–12.42) 1.07 (0.45–2.53)

N of respondents 5,527 5,527 5,527 5,527
N of person-periods 23,916 26,171 25,768 25,179
***

p<.001

**

p<.01

*

p<.05

p<.1

Note: All models control for gender, age, race/ethnicity, education, proxy report, and born in the U.S.

Table A3.

Adjusted Odds Ratios from Discrete-time Hazard Models (Sample of Self-Reports Only), NHATS 2011–18

Cognitive Impairment Base Category: Normal Cognition Impairment in Separate Domains

CIND Dementia Orientation Executive Function Memory
Model 1
Marital status (ref: Married)
 Cohabiting 0.77 (0.47–1.26) 0.90 (0.46–1.77) 0.73 (0.48–1.12) 0.85 (0.45–1.60) 0.86 (0.54–1.39)
 Divorced 1.29*** (1.12–1.49) 1.36* (1.03–1.80) 1.23 (0.99–1.52) 1.42** (1.13–1.78) 1.39*** (1.20–1.62)
 Widowed 1.27* (1.06–1.53) 1.35** (1.08–1.67) 1.33** (1.10–1.61) 1.22* (1.01–1.49) 1.36*** (1.19–1.55)
 Never married 1.19 (0.78–1.84) 1.50 (0.89–2.53) 1.66* (1.04–2.66) 1.37 (0.83–2.26) 1.46* (1.06–2.03)
Model 2
Marital status (ref: Married)
 Cohabiting 0.72 (0.25–2.03) 0.80 (0.29–2.15) 0.87 (0.33–2.28) 1.15 (0.47–2.82) 0.92 (0.42–2.00)
 Divorced 1.44** (1.11–1.85) 1.29 (0.88–1.89) 1.16 (0.85–1.58) 1.30 (0.94–1.80) 1.54** (1.17–2.02)
 Widowed 1.40** (1.10–1.78) 1.25 (0.96–1.64) 1.31 (1.00–1.71) 1.14 (0.90–1.44) 1.33** (1.10–1.62)
 Never married 1.31 (0.74–2.32) 1.21 (0.65–2.28) 1.74 (0.91–3.32) 1.32 (0.74–2.37) 1.49 (0.98–2.26)
Male 1.63*** (1.25–2.11) 1.07 (0.82–1.39) 1.13 (0.88–1.45) 1.25 (0.97–1.60) 1.35** (1.10–1.64)
Marital status × male
 Cohabiting × male 1.11 (0.31–4.01) 1.24 (0.34–4.50) 0.75 (0.21–2.64) 0.58 (0.18–1.84) 0.91 (0.32–2.61)
 Divorced × male 0.82 (0.52–1.29) 1.09 (0.60–1.97) 1.14 (0.70–1.86) 1.18 (0.76–1.85) 0.78 (0.51–1.19)
 Widowed × male 0.81 (0.58–1.11) 1.17 (0.79–1.71) 1.04 (0.73–1.50) 1.18 (0.85–1.64) 1.10 (0.86–1.41)
 Never married × male 0.84 (0.44–1.62) 1.64 (0.74–3.64) 0.87 (0.43–1.78) 1.05 (0.59–1.86) 0.96 (0.56–1.66)

N of respondents 7,242 7,284 7,279 7,269
N of person-periods 25,391 30,048 29,680 28,229
***

p<.001

**

p<.01

*

p<.05

p<.1

Note: All models control for gender, age, race/ethnicity, education, and born in the U.S.

Table A4.

Adjusted Odds Ratios from Discrete-time Hazard Models (Sample of Complete Follow-up Data across Waves), NHATS 2011–18

Cognitive Impairment Base Category: Normal Cognition Impairment in Separate Domains

CIND Dementia Orientation Executive Function Memory
Model 1
Marital status (ref: Married)
 Cohabiting 0.81 (0.37–1.77) 1.33 (0.47–3.76) 0.89 (0.44–1.79) 0.94 (0.31–2.88) 0.98 (0.52–1.87)
 Divorced 1.19 (0.91–1.56) 2.05*** (1.36–3.10) 1.01 (0.64–1.57) 1.90*** (1.33–2.71) 1.26 (0.92–1.72)
 Widowed 1.32 (1.00–1.73) 1.29 (0.84–1.99) 1.52* (1.09–2.10) 1.29 (0.94–1.77) 1.35* (1.04–1.75)
 Never married 1.06 (0.62–1.81) 0.74 (0.22–2.51) 2.44** (1.32–4.50) 1.52 (0.75–3.05) 1.23 (0.66–2.30)
Model 2
Marital status (ref: Married)
 Cohabiting 0.46 (0.11–1.93) 0.65 (0.16–2.72) 1.21 (0.34–4.37) 0.84 (0.23–3.11) 1.14 (0.38–3.44)
 Divorced 1.26 (0.84–1.91) 2.18** (1.33–3.57) 0.89 (0.49–1.62) 1.65* (1.01–2.68) 1.58 (0.99–2.51)
 Widowed 1.55** (1.13–2.14) 1.33 (0.86–2.05) 1.77** (1.18–2.65) 1.28 (0.88–1.86) 1.54** (1.12–2.10)
 Never married 1.10 (0.57–2.13) 0.59 (0.16–2.14) 2.86* (1.29–6.34) 1.19 (0.46–3.06) 1.41 (0.69–2.89)
Male 1.73*** (1.26–2.37) 0.92 (0.59–1.44) 1.45 (0.96–2.18) 1.27 (0.86–1.90) 1.51* (1.10–2.06)
Marital status × male
 Cohabiting × male 2.29 (0.49–10.73) 3.24 (0.52–20.12) 0.61 (0.16–2.37) 1.18 (0.43–3.21) 0.78 (0.18–3.39)
 Divorced × male 0.94 (0.48–1.83) 0.83 (0.32–2.15) 1.39 (0.63–3.04) 1.34 (0.67–2.68) 0.60 (0.29–1.27)
 Widowed × male 0.59* (0.38–0.90) 0.84 (0.34–2.06) 0.63 (0.31–1.27) 0.89 (0.58–1.38) 0.77 (0.50–1.19)
 Never married × male 1.01 (0.40–2.55) 2.77 (0.58–13.24) 0.71 (0.26–1.93) 1.70 (0.60–4.86) 0.77 (0.32–1.87)

N of respondents 2,478 2,478 2,478 2,478
N of person-periods 15,257 18,147 17,654 17,013
***

p<.001

**

p<.01

*

p<.05

p<.1

Note: All models control for gender, age, race/ethnicity, education, proxy report, and born in the U.S.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  • 1.Blazer DG, Yaffe K, Liverman CT. Cognitive aging: Progress in understanding and opportunities for action. Washington, DC: National Academies Press; 2015. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Ray S, Davidson S. Dementia and cognitive decline: A review of the evidence. 2014; Retrieved from https://www.ageuk.org.uk/Documents/EN-GB/For-professionals/Research/Cognitive_decline_and_dementia_evidence_review_Age_UK.pdf?dtrk=true
  • 3.Langa KM, Chernew ME, Kabeto MU, Herzog AR, Ofstedal MB, Willis RJ, et al. National estimates of the quantity and cost of informal caregiving for the elderly with dementia. J Gen Intern Med 2001;16(11):770–778. 10.1111/j.1525-1497.2001.10123.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Obisesan TO, Gillum RF. Cognitive function, social integration and mortality in a US national cohort study of older adults. BMC Geriatr 2009;9(1):33 10.1186/1471-2318-9-33 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Alzheimer’s Association. 2017 Alzheimer’s disease facts and figures. Alzheimers Dement 2017;13(4):325–373. 10.1016/j.jalz.2017.02.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Langa KM, Larson EB, Crimmins EM, Faul JD, Levine DA, Kabeto MU, et al. A comparison of the prevalence of dementia in the United States in 2000 and 2012. JAMA Intern Med 2017;177(1):51–58. http://doi: 10.1001/jamainternmed.2016.6807 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Orrell M, Butler R, Bebbington P. Social factors and the outcome of dementia. Int J Geriatr Psych 2000; 15(6):515–520. ;2-U [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Sharp ES, Gatz M. The relationship between education and dementia: An updated systematic review. Alz Dis Assoc Dis 2011;25(4):289–304. http://doi:10.1097/WAD.0b013e318211c83c [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Egan M, Hobson S, Fearing VG. Dementia and occupation: A review of the literature. Can J Occup Ther 2006;73(3):132–140. 10.2182/cjot.05.0015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Zunzunegui MV, Alvarado BE, Del Ser T, Otero A. Social networks, social integration, and social engagement determine cognitive decline in community-dwelling Spanish older adults. J Gerontol B-Psychol 2003;58(2):S93–S100. 10.1093/geronb/58.2.S93 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Pillai JA, Verghese J. Social networks and their role in preventing dementia. Indian J Psychiat 2009;51(Suppl1):S22–S28 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Shankar A, Hamer M, McMunn A, Steptoe A. Social isolation and loneliness: Relationships with cognitive function during 4 years of follow-up in the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing. Psychosm Med 2013;75(2):161–170. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31827f09cd [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Sbarra DA. Marriage protects men from clinically meaningful elevations in C-reactive protein: Results from the National Social Life, Health, and Aging Project (NSHAP). Psychosm Med 2009;71(8):828. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Waite LJ, Gallagher M. The case for marriage: Why married people are happier, healthier, and better off financially. New York, NY: Broadway Books; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Burman B, Margolin G. Analysis of the association between marital relationships and health problems: An interactional perspective. Psychol Bull 1992;112(1):39–63 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Ross CE, Mirowsky J, Goldsteen K. The impact of family on health: The decade in review. J Marriage Fam 1990;52:1059–1078. doi: 10.2307/353319 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Liu H, Waite LJ. Bad marriage, broken heart? Age and gender differences in the link between marital quality and cardiovascular risks among older adults. J Health Soc Behav 2014;55(4): 403–423. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Liu H Till death do us part: marital status and U.S. mortality trends, 1986–2000. J Marriage Fam 2009;71(5): 1158–1173. 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2009.00661.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Liu H, Umberson D. The times they are a changin’: Marital status and health differentials from 1972 to 2003. J Health Soc Behav 2008;49:239–253. doi: 10.1177/002214650804900301 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Liu H, Zhang Z. Disability trends by marital status among older Americans, 1997–2010: An Examination by gender and race. Popul Res Policy Rev 2013;32:103–127. 10.1007/s11113-012-9259-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Williams K, Umberson D. Marital status, marital transitions, and health: A gendered life course perspective. J Health Soc Behav 2004;45:81–98. doi: 10.1177/002214650404500106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Kiecolt-Glaser JK, Newton TL. Marriage and health: His and hers. Psychol Bull 2001;127:472–503. 10.1037/0033-2909.127.4.472 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Zhang Z, Hayward MD. Gender, the marital life course, and cardiovascular disease in late midlife. J Marriage Fam 2006;68:639–657. 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2006.00280.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Hughes ME, Waite LJ. Marital biography and health at mid-life. J Health Soc Behav 2009;50(3):344–358. doi: 10.1177/002214650905000307 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Carr D, Springer KW. Advances in families and health research in the 21st century. J Marriage Fam 2010;72(3):743–761. 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2010.00728.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Umberson D, Thomeer M, Williams K. Family status and mental health: Recent advances and future directions In Aneshensel CS, Phelan JC & Bierman A (Eds.), Handbook of the sociology of mental health (2nd ed.) (pp. 405–431). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Springer; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Carr D, Freedman VA, Cornman JC, Schwarz N. Happy marriage, happy life? Marital quality and subjective well-being in later life. J Marriage and Fam 2014;76(5):930–948. doi: 10.1111/jomf.12133 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Carr D, Cornman JC, Freedman VA. Marital quality and negative experienced well-being: An assessment of actor and partner effects among older married persons. J Gerontol B-Psychol 2015;71(1):177–187. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbv073. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Simon RW. Revisiting the Relationships among Gender, Marital Status, and Mental Health. Am J Sociol 2002. January 11;107(4):1065–96. doi: 10.1086/339225 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Umberson D Gender, marital status and the social control of health behavior. Soc Sci Med 1992. January 8;34(8):907–17. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(92)90259-S [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Sundstrom A, Westerlund O, Kotyrlo E. Marital status and risk of dementia: a nationwide population-based prospective study from Sweden. BMJ open 2016;6(1):e008565 10.1136/bmjopen-2015-008565 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Helmer C, Damon D, Letenneur L, Fabrigoule C, Barberger-Gateau P, Lafont S, et al. Marital status and risk of Alzheimer’s disease: a French population-based cohort study. Neurology 1999;53(9):1953–1953. 10.1212/WNL.53.9.1953 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Sundstrom A, Westerlund O, Mousavi-Nasab H, Adolfsson R, Nilsson L. The relationship between marital and parental status and the risk of dementia. Int Psychogeriatr 2014;26(5):749–757. 10.1017/S1041610213002652 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Hakansson K, Rovio S, Helkala EL, Vilska A, Winblad B, Soininen H, et al. Association between mid-life marital status and cognitive function in later life: population based cohort study. BMJ 2009;339:b2462 10.1136/bmj.b2462 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.van Gelder BM, Tijhuis M, Kalmijn S, Giampaoli S, Nissinen A, Kromhout D. Marital status and living situation during a 5-year period are associated with a subsequent 10-year cognitive decline in older men: the FINE Study. J Gerontol B-Psychol 2006;61(4):213–219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Bae JB, Kim YJ, Han JW, Kim TH, Park JH, Lee SB. Incidence of and risk factors for Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment in Korean elderly. Dement Geriatr Cogn 2015;39(1–2): 105–115. doi: 10.1159/000366555. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Feng L, Ng XT, Yap P, Li J, Lee TS, Hakansson K, et al. Marital status and cognitive impairment among community-dwelling Chinese older adults: The role of gender and social engagement. Dement Geriatr Cogn 2014;4(3):375–384. doi: 10.1159/000358584. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Sommerlad A, Ruegger J, Singh-Manoux A, Lewis G, Livingston G. Marriage and risk of dementia: Systematic review and meta-analysis of observational studies. J Neurol Neurosur Ps jnnp-2017. 10.1136/jnnp-2017-316274. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Brown SL, Lin IF. The gray divorce revolution: Rising divorce among middle-aged and older adults, 1990–2010. J Gerontol B-Psychol 2012;67(6):731–741. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbs089 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Kreider RM, Ellis R. Number, timing, and duration of marriages and divorces: 2009. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Commerce, Economics and Statistics Administration, U.S. Census Bureau; 2011 [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Li J, Ogrodnik M, Kolachalama VB, Lin H, Au R. Assessment of the mid-life demographic and lifestyle risk factors of dementia using data from the Framingham Heart Study offspring cohort. J Alzheimers Dis 2018;63(3): 1119–1127. doi: 10.3233/JAD-170917 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Pankratz VS, Roberts RO, Mielke MM, Knopman DS, Jack CR, Geda YE, Rocca WA, Petersen RC. Predicting the risk of mild cognitive impairment in the Mayo Clinic Study of Aging. Neurology 2015;84(14):1433–1442. 10.1212/WNL.0000000000001437 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Roberts RO, Geda YE, Knopman DS, Cha RH, Pankratz VS, Boeve BF, Tangalos EG, Ivnik RJ, Rocca WA, Petersen RC. The incidence of MCI differs by subtype and is higher in men: the Mayo Clinic Study of Aging. Neurology 2012;78(5):342–351. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182452862 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Norton MC, Smith KR, Østbye T, Tschanz JT, Corcoran C, Schwartz S, Piercy KW, Rabins PV, Steffens DC, Skoog I, Breitner JC. Greater Risk of Dementia When Spouse Has Dementia? The Cache County Study: [See editorial comments by Dr. Peter P. Vitaliano, pp 976–978]. J Am Geriatr Soc 2010;58(5):895–900. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2010.02806.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Kasper JD, Freedman VA. National Health and Aging Trends Study user guide: Rounds 1–6 final release. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University School of Public Health; 2017. Retrieved from https://www.nhats.org/scripts/documents/NHATS_User_Guide_R1_R6_2017_Revised_12_12_17.pdf [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Kasper JD, Freedman VA, Spillman BC. Classification of persons by dementia status in the National Health and Aging Trends Study. Technical Paper #5. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University School of Public Health; 2013. Retrieved from https://www.nhats.org/scripts/documents/NHATS_Dementia_Technical_Paper_5_Jul2013.pdf [Google Scholar]
  • 47.NHATS Data Collection Procedures Round 1. 2011. Retrieved from https://www.nhats.org/scripts/documents/NHATS_Round1_Data_Collection_Procedures_version2.pdf
  • 48.Morris JC. Revised criteria for mild cognitive impairment may compromise the diagnosis of Alzheimer Disease dementia. Arch Neurol 2012;69(6):700–708. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2011.3152 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.McKhann GM, Knopman DS, Chertkow H, Hyman BT, Jack CR Jr, Kawas CH, et al. The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic gudielines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement 2011;7(3):263–269. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Nonaka K, Hasebe M, Koike T, Suzuki H, Fukaya T, Fujiwara Y. Differences in health outcome among married, never-married, and separated or divorced elderly In Japan. Innovation in Aging 2017;1(suppl_1):379–379. doi: 10.1093/geroni/igx004.1378 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Brown SL, Bulanda JR, Lee GR. Transitions into and out of cohabitation in later life. J Marriage Fam 2012;74(4):774–793. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2012.00994.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.King V, Scott ME. A comparison of cohabiting relationships among older and younger adults. J Marriage Fam 2005;67(2):271–285. doi: 10.1111/j.0022-2445.2005.00115.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Lin KA, Choudhury KR, Rathakrishnan BG, Marks DM, Petrella JR, Doraiswamy PM, et al. Marked gender differences in progression of mild cognitive impairment over 8 years. Alzheimer’s Dement (New York, N Y) 2015;1(2):103–10. doi: 10.1016/j.trci.2015.07.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Laws KR, Irvine K, Gale TM. Sex differences in cognitive impairment in Alzheimer’s disease. World J Psychiatry 2016;January 11;6(1):54—65. doi: 10.5498/wjp.v6.i1.54 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Azad NA, Al Bugami M, Loy-English I. Gender differences in dementia risk factors. Gend Med 2007;4(2):120–9. doi: 10.1016/S1550-8579(07)80026-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Ruitenberg A, Ott A, van Swieten JC, Hofman A, Breteler MM. Incidence of dementia: does gender make a difference? Neurobiol Aging 2001;22(4):575–80. doi: 10.1016/S0197-4580(01)00231-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Goldman N, Korenman S, Weinstein R Marital status and health among the elderly. Soc Sci Med 1995;40(12):1717–1730. 10.1016/0277-9536(94)00281-W [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Stern Y Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet Neurol 2012;11(11): 1006–1012. 10.1016/S1474-4422(12)70191-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Umberson D, Williams K, Powers DA, Liu H, Needham B. You Make Me Sick: Marital Quality and Health Over the Life Course. J Health Soc Behav 2006. January 10;47(1): 1–16. doi: 10.1177/002214650604700101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Liu H Marital dissolution and self-rated health: age trajectories and birth cohort variations. Soc Sci Med 2012;74(7):1107–16. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.11.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Rothman SM, Mattson MP. Adverse stress, hippocampal networks, and Alzheimer’s disease. Neuromolecular Med 2010;12(1):56–70. doi: 10.1007/s12017-009-8107-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Johansson L, Guo X, Hällström T, Norton MC, Waern M, Östling S, et al. Common psychosocial stressors in middle-aged women related to longstanding distress and increased risk of Alzheimer’s disease: a 38-year longitudinal population study. BMJ Open 2013;January 11;3(9):e003142. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2013-003142 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

1

RESOURCES