Skip to main content
APA Open Access logoLink to APA Open Access
. 2019 Sep 26;128(8):823–839. doi: 10.1037/abn0000460

The Latent Structure of Interpersonal Problems: Validity of Dimensional, Categorical, and Hybrid Models

Leon P Wendt 1,*, Aidan G C Wright 2, Paul A Pilkonis 3, Tobias Nolte 4, Peter Fonagy 4, P Read Montague 5, Cord Benecke 6, Tobias Krieger 7, Johannes Zimmermann 8
Editor: Angus MacDonald III
PMCID: PMC6816327  PMID: 31556632

Abstract

Interpersonal problems are key transdiagnostic constructs in psychopathology. In the past, investigators have neglected the importance of operationalizing interpersonal problems according to their latent structure by using divergent representations of the construct: (a) computing scores for severity, agency, and communion (“dimensional approach”), (b) classifying persons into subgroups with respect to their interpersonal profile (“categorical approach”). This hinders cumulative research on interpersonal problems, because findings cannot be integrated both from a conceptual and a statistical point of view. We provide a comprehensive evaluation of interpersonal problems by enlisting several large samples (Ns = 5,400, 491, 656, and 712) to estimate a set of latent variable candidate models, covering the spectrum of purely dimensional (i.e., confirmatory factor analysis using Gaussian and nonnormal latent t-distributions), hybrid (i.e., semiparametric factor analysis), and purely categorical approaches (latent class analysis). Statistical models were compared with regard to their structural validity, as evaluated by model fit (corrected Akaike’s information criterion and the Bayesian information criterion), and their concurrent validity, as defined by the models’ ability to predict relevant external variables. Across samples, the fully dimensional model performed best in terms of model fit, prediction, robustness, and parsimony. We found scant evidence that categorical and hybrid models provide incremental value for understanding interpersonal problems. Our results indicate that the latent structure of interpersonal problems is best represented by continuous dimensions, especially when one allows for nonnormal latent distributions.

Keywords: interpersonal problems, factor mixture modeling, nonnormal factor distribution, confirmatory factor analysis, latent class analysis

General Scientific Summary

This study suggests that interpersonal problems are best characterized by continuous differences in severity and interpersonal style (i.e., agency and communion), and not by “types” of people.


Interpersonal problems are key constructs relevant to understanding and characterizing psychopathology that cut across traditional diagnostic categories (American Psychiatric Association, 2013; Hopwood, Wright, Ansell, & Pincus, 2013). For example, interpersonal problems show significant associations with a broad range of psychopathological constructs, such as transdiagnostic dimensions (Girard et al., 2017), maladaptive personality traits (Williams & Simms, 2016; Wright et al., 2012), and core dimensions of personality functioning (Dowgwillo, Roche, & Pincus, 2018; Pincus, 2018). Research on pathoplasticity suggests that mental disorders are embedded in diverse interpersonal dynamics, in which symptoms and interpersonal styles have reciprocal effects on their respective expression and maintenance (Cain, Pincus, & Grosse Holtforth, 2010; Dawood, Dowgwillo, Wu, & Pincus, 2018; Erickson et al., 2016; Przeworski et al., 2011). Treatment research has demonstrated that interpersonal problems can be reduced by psychotherapy (e.g., Horowitz, Rosenberg, & Bartholomew, 1993; McFarquhar, Luyten, & Fonagy, 2018; Quilty, Mainland, McBride, & Bagby, 2013) indicating overall symptomatic change (e.g., Altenstein-Yamanaka, Zimmermann, Krieger, Dörig, & Grosse Holtforth, 2017; Dammann et al., 2016; Huber, Henrich, & Klug, 2007; Renner et al., 2012). Interpersonal problems predict worse treatment outcome (Boswell, Cain, Oswald, McAleavey, & Adelman, 2017; Gomez Penedo, Constantino, Coyne, Bernecker, & Smith-Hansen, 2018; Gomez Penedo, Constantino, Coyne, Westra, & Antony, 2017; McEvoy, Burgess, & Nathan, 2014; Shapiro et al., 1994), possibly explained by weaker therapeutic alliance (Constantino et al., 2010; Zilcha-Mano et al., 2015) and poorer quality of cognitive-emotional processing (Altenstein-Yamanaka et al., 2017). Also, the optimal therapeutic intervention might be dependent on the specific problem content (Gomez Penedo et al., 2017, 2018; Krieg & Tracey, 2016; Newman, Jacobson, Erickson, & Fisher, 2017).

The major conceptual foundation of research on interpersonal problems is the Interpersonal Circumplex (IPC; Gurtman & Pincus, 2003; Wiggins, 1991), which delineates interpersonal problems along the dimensions of agency and communion (see Figure 1). However, although this conceptual foundation is well-grounded and established (Acton & Revelle, 2002; Alden, Wiggins, & Pincus, 1990; Boudreaux, Ozer, Oltmanns, & Wright, 2018; Tracey, Rounds, & Gurtman, 1996), investigators adopt distinct scoring procedures across studies, including, but not limited to, dimensional (i.e., computing scores for severity, agency, and communion) and categorical approaches (i.e., classifying persons into subgroups according to their interpersonal profile). In the following, we argue that the way in which researchers conceptualize, operationalize (i.e., score measures), and statistically model interpersonal problems should match their underlying latent structure, namely, it should correspond to what exactly is being measured. Notably, a measurement can be considered valid when the empirical evidence supports that the test scores reflect the target construct adequately (Kane, 2013). Dimensional and categorical scoring procedures of interpersonal problems summarize the information at hand in distinct ways, often based on data reduction techniques, such as factor analysis (i.e., dimensional approach; Altenstein-Yamanaka et al., 2017; Ansell, Grilo, & White, 2012; Barrett & Barber, 2007; Blomquist, Ansell, White, Masheb, & Grilo, 2012; Dinger et al., 2015; Locke et al., 2017; Luo, Nuttall, Locke, & Hopwood, 2018; Miller, Price, Gentile, Lynam, & Campbell, 2012; Puschner, Kraft, & Bauer, 2004; Quilty et al., 2013; Ruiz et al., 2004; Wilson, Revelle, Stroud, & Durbin, 2013) and latent class analysis/cluster analysis (i.e., categorical approach; Cain et al., 2010, 2012; Cooper & Anderson, 2019; Dawood, Thomas, Wright, & Hopwood, 2013; Grosse Holtforth et al., 2014; Hopwood, Clarke, & Perez, 2007; Leihener et al., 2003; Przeworski et al., 2011; Salzer et al., 2008, 2010, 2013; Simon, Cain, Wallner Samstag, Meehan, & Muran, 2015; Wright et al., 2013a; Zilcha-Mano et al., 2015).1

Figure 1.

Figure 1

The octant scales of interpersonal problems can be arranged in a circular structural representation, in accordance with the Interpersonal Circumplex (Wiggins, 1991).

Generally, the adequacy of a scoring method depends on whether its implicit structural assumptions are met. Categorical and dimensional approaches to interpersonal problems are mutually exclusive in their structural assumptions and therefore, cannot be equally valid. Failure in meeting those assumptions dilutes the psychometric properties of the measurement and affects the quality of inferences, for instance, by losing statistical power (e.g., median splits) or by researchers being misled to draw false conclusions (Markon, Chmielewski, & Miller, 2011; Morey et al., 2012; Preacher, Rucker, MacCallum, & Nicewander, 2005). To illustrate this, imagine that the latent structure of interpersonal problems was truly three-dimensional, yet a hypothetical finding was based on the categorical approach (i.e., calculating latent classes). In this scenario, the latent classes simply captured blends of the dimensions, in other words, dimensional information on individual differences would have become confounded within classes. How could the statistical association between an interpersonal type and another psychological construct be interpreted under such circumstances? The answer is unclear, because it would be indistinguishable to what degree the association could be attributed to the influence of any one of those three dimensions, let alone additive or interactive effects between them. In addition, empirical results from studies using incompatible operationalizations of the construct cannot be integrated with each other. This is true from a conceptual, but also from a statistical point of view (i.e., meta-analytic integration). As a result of this ambiguity, knowledge on interpersonal problems can hardly be accumulated. The coexistent use of different scoring procedures may amplify information burden and impede comprehension (Kane, 2013). A comprehensive structural analysis of interpersonal problems is needed to resolve those issues and move the field forward—ever more so given the widespread use of the construct as cited above.

Interpersonal problems are commonly measured by the Inventory of Interpersonal Problems (IIP; Alden et al., 1990; Horowitz, Rosenberg, Baer, Ureño, & Villaseñor, 1988) and the Circumplex Scales of Interpersonal Problems (CSIP; Boudreaux et al., 2018). Both measures assess interpersonal dysfunction on eight octant scales, each denoting a set of social difficulties (see Figure 1). In the following, we will outline the two most commonly used structural models of interpersonal problems. For the fully dimensional approach, a three-factorial solution has been well established (Acton & Revelle, 2002; Alden et al., 1990; Boudreaux et al., 2018; Hopwood & Good, 2018; Monsen, Hagtvet, Havik, & Eilertsen, 2006; Tracey et al., 1996; Wilson et al., 2013). The IPC (Gurtman & Pincus, 2003; Wiggins, 1991) provides the predominant factor rotation, locating the octants within a circular array, as displayed in Figure 1. As a result of the circular representation, the octant scores represent blends of the underlying dimensions. The two orthogonal substantive dimensions of Agency and Communion form the main axes of this circular arrangement. Agency (also: power, control, or dominance) is a bipolar continuum of agentic versus submissive interpersonal tendencies, with high levels indicating an assertive and low levels capturing a nonassertive style. Communion (also: solidarity, friendliness, warmth, love, or affiliation) describes a bipolar continuum of communal versus detached interpersonal tendencies, with high levels associated with a warm and low levels associated with a cold style. Some circumplex instruments, such as the IIP and the CSIP, reflect a third dimension that captures the level of severity irrespective of style (Tracey et al., 1996; Wilson et al., 2013). This third dimension denotes a general factor that is reflected by an individual’s mean across all subscales and has been explained to capture overall interpersonal distress (or “elevation” in terms of the Structural Summary Method; Gurtman, 1992; Zimmermann & Wright, 2017). All things combined, the circumplex model predicts a sinusoidal pattern of endorsement on the octants (Figure 2a). Sinusoidal curves are assumed to vary in amplitude, elevation, and (angular) location, depending on the individual’s unobserved continuous factor scores.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

(a)-(c) Predicted octant scores by candidate models (from left to right: CFA-PC, 5-Class LCA, and 3-Factor 5-Class SP-FA) in Sample 1 (N = 5,400). Dashed lines indicate the predicted pattern for the average individual in class k. Gray lines represent 15 simulated observations. In contrast to our approach to use indicators that were standardized in reference to population norms, the models plotted in this figure were based on z-transformed octant scores to facilitate visualization. Line strengths indicate relative class proportions.

The categorical approach to interpersonal problems proposes that the covariance between octants can be sufficiently explained by k unobserved interpersonal types (i.e., latent classes). Categorical models estimate a specific pattern of endorsements on the octants for each latent class, respectively. Individuals are assumed to belong to one of the k estimated latent classes and are predicted to show the class’ prototypical pattern (i.e., interpersonal prototypes, Figure 2b). Although the categorical approach to interpersonal problems does not presume a circular structural model, the resulting octant profiles are usually summarized and evaluated by circumplex statistics in a subsequent analytic step (i.e., elevation, amplitude, and angular location; Gurtman & Balakrishnan, 1998; Wright, Pincus, Conroy, & Hilsenroth, 2009) to locate latent classes in the circular space (e.g., Cain et al., 2010, 2012; Dawood et al., 2013; Grosse Holtforth et al., 2014; Hopwood et al., 2007; Lo Coco, Gullo, Scrima, & Bruno, 2012; Przeworski et al., 2011; Salzer et al., 2013; Slaney, Pincus, Uliaszek, & Wang, 2006; Wright, Pincus, Conroy, & Elliot, 2009, 2013a; Zilcha-Mano et al., 2015).

Another structural representation of interpersonal problems can be attempted by factor mixture modeling, comprising a spectrum of latent variable models between categorical and dimensional hybrid mixtures (Hallquist & Wright, 2014). Semi-Parametric Factor Analysis (SP-FA) is one such approach, assuming that the covariation between octants can be explained by a mixture of three latent factors and a k-fold categorical latent variable. More specifically, SP-FA identifies k clusters in which individuals are concentrated in the three-dimensional space. The latent classes can be conceptualized as latent subpopulations that shape a joint multimodal distribution (i.e., k-modal).2 For example, SP-FA could identify a latent class of individuals characterized by high severity, high agency, and low communion. Hence, the hybrid approach describes individuals both in terms of dimensional scores and class membership (Figure 2c).

We argue that the current practices to operationalize interpersonal problems impede scientific progress for two major reasons: (a) inferences in the research literature may be affected by limited validity and (b) empirical results cannot be integrated conceptually and statistically. Our investigation attempts to resolve those issues by comparing the dimensional, categorical, and hybrid approaches with regard to model fit (structural validity) and prediction (concurrent validity) in four large samples. We aim to facilitate the conceptual and statistical integration of future findings and promote a cumulative science in this important domain of functioning.

Method

Samples

An overview of the samples and measures used in this study is displayed in Table 1. Descriptive statistics, measure’s internal consistencies and correlation matrices for each sample are reported in the online supplemental materials (Tables S1–S4).

Table 1. Samples and Measures.

No. Sample description N Age M (SD) Interpersonal measure External criterion variables
Note. DSM-5 = Diagnostic and Statistical Manual for Mental Disorders-Fifth Edition; IIP = Inventory of Interpersonal Problems; CSIP = Circumplex Scales of Interpersonal Problems; α = Cronbach’s α; ω = McDonald’s omega.
1 Treatment-seeking individuals, Germany (Benecke et al., 2011; Henkel et al., 2017) 5,400 37.4 (11.8) IIP-64, German translation (Horowitz, Strauss, & Kordy, 2000), octant scales internal consistencies ω =.71–.87, standardized by population norms Symptom Checklist 90 Revised (SCL-90-R; Schmitz et al., 2000) General Severity Index
Somatization
Obsessive-compulsiveness
Interpersonal sensitivity
Depression
Anxiety
Hostility
Paranoid ideation
Phobic anxiety
Psychoticism
2 Outpatients and healthy controls, United Kingdom (Euler et al., 2019) 491 31.5 (10.7) IIP-32 (Horowitz, Alden, Wiggins, & Pincus, 2000), octant scales’ internal consistencies ω = .66–.85, standardized by population norms Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI; Derogatis, 1993) General Severity Index
Baratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11; Patton, Stanford, & Barratt, 1995) Somatization
Empathy Quotient (EQ; Baron-Cohen & Wheelwright, 2004) Obsessive-compulsiveness
The Other as Shamer Scale (OAS; Goss, Gilbert, & Allan, 1994) Interpersonal sensitivity
Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS; Gratz & Roemer, 2004) Depression
Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (SPQ; Raine, 1991) Anxiety
Posttraumatic Stress Checklist Scale; (Blanchard, Jones-Alexander, Buckley, & Forneris, 1996) Hostility
Paranoid ideation
Phobic anxiety
Psychoticism
Cognitive impulsiveness
Empathy
External shame
Emotional dysregulation
Schizotypal personality
Posttraumatic stress
3 Clinical and community samples subsetted for individuals that meet one DSM-5 mental disorder diagnosis, United States; Girard et al. (2017) 656 36.0 (10.5) IIP-64 (Alden, Wiggins, & Pincus, 1990), octant scales’ internal consistencies α = .76–.90, standardized by population norms The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (SCID-I; First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, 1997) and The Structured Interview for DSM-IV Personality (SIDP-IV; Pfohl, Blum, & Zimmerman, 1997) Total diagnoses count
Axis-I diagnoses
Axis-II diagnoses
Internalizing-fear
Internalizing-distress
Externalizing-antagonism
Externalizing-disinhibition
Detachment
Thought disorder
4 Undergraduate students at the University of Pittsburgh, United States; Boudreaux, Ozer, Oltmanns, and Wright (2018) 712 18.7 (1.7) CSIP (Boudreaux et al., 2018), octant scales’ internal consistencies α = .78–.89 z-transformed Big-Five-Inventory−2 (BFI-2; Soto & John, 2017) Negative emotionality
Personality Inventory for DSM-5 (PID-5–100; Maples et al., 2015) Agreeableness
Conscientiousness
Extraversion
Open-mindedness
Negative affect
Antagonism
Disinhibition
Detachment
Psychoticism

Sample 1

Data from 5,400 treatment-seeking participants (66% women; mean age of 37.40, SD = 11.80) were collected at 12 psychoanalytic training institutions belonging to the German Psychoanalytic Society (Benecke et al., 2011; Henkel et al., 2019). Included were participants who underwent assessment and passed entry diagnostics for an outpatient psychotherapy, irrespective of a subsequent beginning of therapy. The mean (standardized) IIP total score was z = 0.2.

Sample 2

The second sample (Euler et al., 2019) was recruited in Greater London via the Personality and Mood Disorder Research Consortium. The sample (N = 491) consisted of 302 healthy community participants and 189 outpatients referred from National Health Service specialist personality disorder clinical services. Participants’ age was M = 31.53 (SD = 10.74) and 65% were female. We expected a bimodal distribution on psychopathology markers, because outpatients and control group participants were subjected to different sampling processes (i.e., selecting for extreme values). As expected, outpatients were more severely distressed in terms of the IIP total score (z = 1.73) than the control group (z = 0.41).

Sample 3

The full sample consisted of 825 participants from five clinical and community samples collected at the University of Pittsburgh, as described in Girard et al. (2017). Clinical samples were derived from outpatient clinics excluding patients with a lifetime history of psychotic disorders or medical conditions of the central nervous system. One of the samples excluded participants with bipolar disorder. For our analysis, we took a subset of participants that met the criteria for a least one mental disorder diagnosis (N = 656). In this sample, the average age was M = 35.95 (SD = 10.47) and 66% were female. The sample of Axis I + II diagnosed participants was more interpersonally distressed than the national norm, z = 0.80 (IIP total). Diagnostic criteria were rated by mental health professionals or trained interviewers on the base of semistructured interviews.

Sample 4

We used the second sample from Boudreaux et al. (2018), consisting of 757 undergraduate students (average age was 18.7 years, SD = 1.7) that were enrolled at the University of Pittsburgh. Gender was not recorded for most participants because of an administration error (of those who that recorded: 123 women, 77 men). Octant scores were z-transformed because population norms were not available for the here used measure of interpersonal problems.

Measures

Most of the instruments used in this investigation were constructed and validated for clinical populations (except for the Big-Five-Inventory 2 that assesses normal-range variation in personality; Soto & John, 2017). Apart from the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM–IV (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual for Mental Disorders-Fourth Edition; First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, 1997) and the Structured Interview for DSM–IV Personality (Pfohl, Blum, & Zimmerman, 1997) all instruments were administered as self-report. Whereas interpersonal problems were measured by all samples, each data set yielded its own distinct pool of external variables.

The Inventory of Interpersonal Problems (IIP)

Interpersonal problems were measured with different versions and translations of the IIP (Alden et al., 1990), as indicated in the sample descriptions in Table 1. Measures of interpersonal problems assess problematic interpersonal behaviors that are performed excessively or inhibited strongly. With regard to the IIP, the distress associated with such behaviors is rated, ranging from not at all (0) to extremely (4) on a 5-point scale. Items are aggregated to obtain octant scores named Domineering/Controlling (e.g., “I am too aggressive towards other people”), Vindictive/Self-Centered (e.g., “It is hard for me to feel good about another person’s happiness”), Cold/Distant (e.g., “It is hard for me to feel close to other people”), Socially Inhibited (e.g., “It is hard for me to introduce myself to new people”), Nonassertive (e.g., “It is hard for me to confront people with problems that come up”), Overly Accommodating (e.g., “I let other people take advantage of me too much”), Self-Sacrificing (e.g., “I am overly generous to other people”), and Intrusive/Needy (e.g., “I open up to people too much”).

The Circumplex Scales of Interpersonal Problems (CSIP)

The CSIP (Boudreaux et al., 2018) consists of 64 items that are rated on a 4-points Likert-type scale. Respondents indicate to what degree the given statements (e.g., “Bossing around other people too much”) are experienced as a problem, ranging from not a problem (0) to serious problem (3). The CSIP assesses the interpersonal octants, as described earlier. The measure was recently introduced as an alternative instrument to the IIP and demonstrated very good convergent and discriminant validity with its counterpart, although having marginal overlap in wording and content.

Symptom-Checklist-90-Revised (SCL-90–R) and Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI)

We used the German translation for the SCL-90–R (Schmitz et al., 2000) and the BSI (i.e., short form of the SCL-90–R; Derogatis, 1993) to assess psychological distress in terms of symptom severity on a 5-point scale ranging from not at all (0) to extremely (4). The scales include Somatization (e.g., “Trouble getting your breath”), Obsessive-Compulsion (e.g., “Having to check and double-check what you do”), Interpersonal Sensitivity (e.g., “Others are unsympathetic”), Depression (e.g., “Feeling Blue”), Anxiety (e.g., “Heart pounding/racing”), Hostility (e.g., “Urges to harm someone”), Phobic Anxiety (e.g., “Afraid on the street”), Paranoid Ideation (e.g., “Having beliefs that others do not share”), and Psychoticism (e.g., “You should be punished for your sins”). The General Severity Index (GSI) is the mean from all subscales and captures global symptom severity (Urbán et al., 2014).

Barratt’ Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11)

The BIS-11 (Patton, Stanford, & Barratt, 1995) measures impulsiveness with 30 items, assessing the frequency of impulsive behavior on a 4-point scale ranging from rarely/never (1) to almost always/always (4). Our study used only the Attentional Impulsiveness scale, which denotes the inability to focus or concentrate (e.g., “I often have extraneous thoughts when thinking”).

Empathy Quotient (EQ)

The EQ (Baron-Cohen & Wheelwright, 2004) contains 40-items (and 20 filler items) to measure empathy, as defined by the ability to perceive and understand the intentions of others. The instrument showed a multidimensional factor structure including Cognitive Empathy (e.g., “I can tell if someone is masking their true emotions”), Emotional Reactivity (e.g., “Seeing people cry doesn’t really upset me”; reversed), and Social Skills (e.g., “I find it hard to know what to do in social situations”; reversed). Items are rated on a 4-point scale ranging from strongly disagree (0) to strongly agree (3).

Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (SPQ)

The SPQ (Raine, 1991) was used to assess Schizotypal Personality by 74 items that relate to the DSM–III–R (APA, 1987) diagnostic criteria of Schizotypal Personality Disorder: ideas of reference, excessive social anxiety, odd beliefs or magical thinking, unusual perceptual experiences, odd or eccentric behavior, no close friends, odd speech, constricted affect, and suspiciousness. Schizotypal traits are generally related to discomfort in social interactions and a reduced capacity for interpersonal relations (APA, 2013). Items are rated as present or absent (0 = No, 1 = Yes). Factor analyses have shown a 3-dimensional structure (Badcock & Dragović, 2006) including Cognitive-Perceptual Dysfunction (e.g., “I sense some person or force”), Interpersonal Deficits (e.g., “I tend to keep in the background”), and Disorganization (e.g., “I am an odd, unusual person”).

The “Other as Shamer” Scale (OAS)

The OAS (Goss, Gilbert, & Allan, 1994) includes 18 items to assess shame experiences that occur from perceived negative evaluations by others. Items (e.g., “Others are critical or punishing when I make a mistake”) are rated on a 5-point frequency scale ranging from never (0) to almost always (4). The construct has shown a three-factor structure (Being Seen as Inferior, Being Seen as Empty or Trivial, and Being Observed Doing Mistakes). Shame is associated with aggression/hostility or withdrawal from social interactions (Smart Richman & Leary, 2009).

Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Strategies Scale (DERS)

The DERS (Gratz & Roemer, 2004) assesses dysregulation of mainly negative emotional states among adults by 36 items on a 5-point scale (1 = almost never, 5 = almost always). Multidimensional facets include Nonacceptance of Emotional Responses (e.g., “When I’m upset, I become angry with myself for feeling that way”), Difficulty Engaging in Goal-Directed Behavior (e.g., “When I’m upset, I have difficulties getting work done”), Impulse Control Difficulties (e.g., “I experience my emotions as overwhelming and out of control”), Lack of Emotional Awareness (e.g., “I pay attention to how I feel”; reversed), Limited Access to Emotion Regulation Strategies (e.g., “When I’m upset, I believe that I will remain that way for a long time”), and Lack of Emotional Clarity (e.g., “I am confused about how I feel”).

Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Checklist (PCL)

The PCL (Blanchard, Jones-Alexander, Buckley, & Forneris, 1996) assesses the 17 symptoms of posttraumatic stress disorder postulated in DSM–IV. Symptomatic distress is rated for the last month on a 5-point scale ranging from not at all (1) to extremely (5). The latent structure of posttraumatic stress disorder has been subject to debates (Blevins, Weathers, Davis, Witte, & Domino, 2015; Elhai et al., 2011). The PCL is assumed to be constituted by four highly correlated factors including Reexperiencing (e.g., “Flashbacks”), Avoidance (e.g., “Avoiding thoughts of trauma”), Hyperarousal (e.g., “Hypervigilance”), and Dysphoria (or Emotional Numbing, e.g., “Restricted Affect”).

Structured Clinical Interview for DSM–IV Axis-I disorders (SCID-I)

The SCID-I (First et al., 1997) was used to assess Axis-I mental disorders as operationalized by the DSM–IV. Moderate to excellent interrater agreement was reported for Axis I disorders (Lobbestael, Leurgans, & Arntz, 2011).

Structured Interview for DSM–IV Personality (SIDP-IV)

The SIDP-IV (Pfohl et al., 1997) was administered to assess DSM–IV personality disorders (i.e., Axis-II). Good interrater reliability (i.e., intraclass correlation) was reported (Jane, Pagan, Turkheimer, Fiedler, & Oltmanns, 2006).

Mental disorder diagnoses assessed with SCID-I and SIDP-IV were aggregated to dimensional diagnosis counts based on syndrome clusters from (a) the Hierarchical Taxonomy of Psychopathology (HiTOP; internalizing-fear, internalizing-distress, externalizing-antagonism, externalizing-disinhibition, thought disorder, and detachment; Kotov et al., 2017) and (b) Axis-I + II disorders from DSM–5 (APA, 2013). HiTOP related syndromal clusters are based on the empirically observed covariation of mental disorders. Yet, the psychometric properties of diagnoses counts are not clear.

Big-Five-Inventory - 2 (BFI-2)

Personality domains and facets were assessed by the BFI-2 (Soto & John, 2017), consisting of 60 items (four items per facet). Respondents endorse short statements on a 5-point, Likert-type scale ranging from disagree strongly (1) to agree strongly (5). For the current sample, Boudreaux et al. (2018) reported strong internal consistencies and test-retest reliability at the domain level (α = .76–.84) and the facet level (average α was .76, average retest reliability was .73).

Personality Inventory for DSM–5 (PID-5–100)

Maladaptive personality domains and facets were measured using a short form of the PID-5 (Maples et al., 2015), that consists of 100 items (four items per facet). Respondents rate on a 4-point scale whether the presented statements apply to themselves (e.g., “I don’t get as much pleasure out of things as others seem to.”) ranging from very false or often false (0) to very true or often true (3). Maladaptive personality dimensions achieved strong internal consistencies at the domain level (α = .81–.89) and the facet level (Mdn = .81).

Latent Variable Models

Octant scores served as indicators to fit latent dimensional, categorical, and hybrid models to data collected from four large samples. All samples had acceptable levels of skewness and kurtosis for the octant scales (< |1|). Nonetheless, all models were estimated with maximum likelihood and robust standard errors (MLR). Mean structures were included in all estimated models to permit for direct comparisons. We provide an overview on the different models and their parameterization in the online supplemental materials (see Table S5 see Figures S1–S3 for structural notations).

Latent class analysis (LCA; note this is sometimes referred to as latent profile analyses when dimensional indicators are used, as was done here) was used to estimate fully categorical latent structures. In LCA, the pattern of covariation among the observed variables is presumed to arise from latent classes that are characterized by different patterns of means on the observed variables; in this case, different profiles of octants scores. Individual deviations from the expected pattern are modeled as random error, and such error variances are set to equivalence across classes. In the current investigation, the optimal number of classes in LCA was determined by exploratory analyses. We used four decision heuristics, including the Bootstrapped Likelihood-Ratio Test (BLRT), the Vuong-Lo-Mendel-Rubin Test (VLMR), the small-sample corrected Akaike’s information criterion (AICC; Burnham & Anderson, 2004), and the Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC; Schwarz, 1978). Multiple candidate models were selected when the stopping rules supported different solutions. We limited the extraction of classes to a maximum of 15 and only considered solutions viable when the smallest class comprised at least 5% of the total sample. Those criteria were liberal boundaries that would exclude fundamentally impractical solutions and prevent overfitting.

For the different variants in dimensional models (i.e., more restrictive vs. less restrictive, normal vs. nonnormal latent distribution, purely factorial vs. factor mixture) we specified a set of candidates that were collectively based on three-dimensions, as suggested by past research (Acton & Revelle, 2002). The factorial part is based on specifying agency and communion as orthogonal factors that show a circular pattern of factor loadings. A third, general factor loads equally on all octants. In contrast to common bifactor models, we let the general factor correlate freely with group factors. This has conceptual reasons, namely, that the general factor is regarded as assessing the degree of overall distress, and the group factors the tendency to experience some problems more than others (in other words: the most prevalent interpersonal style). Freeing those correlations allows for the possibility that different problem contents may be associated with different levels of distress (e.g., submissive problems might be more disturbing than agentic problems).

In our most restrictive CFA-PC (also known as the “perfect circumplex” solution; Gurtman & Pincus, 2003) the factor loadings on the group factors are specified in a way that induces a fixed correlational pattern upon octants characterized by two conditions: equal spacing and equal communalities. Adjacent octants are restricted to have equal spacing in between, as is reflected in the factor loadings (i.e., “equal spacing” condition). Style dimensions are restricted to be measured with identical reliability (i.e., “equal communalities” condition). Finally, the latent factor distributions are predicated on the Gaussian distribution (as is customary in standard CFA). The CFA-PC was selected as a candidate model, because it directly corresponds to the commonly used simple scoring procedure for the IPC domains (Locke, 2010):

Agency=PA+(NO×.71)+(BC×.71)(FG×.71)(JK×.71)HI
Communion=LM+(NO×.71)+(JK×.71)(BC×.71)(FG×.71)DE
GeneralInterpersonalDistress=(PA+BC+DE+FG+HI+JK+LM+NO)8

In our less restrictive CFA-QC (i.e., the “quasi-circumplex” solution), both spacing and communalities are free to deviate from equality. CFA-QC was included as a candidate model, because previous research suggested that the fit of IPC-based models can be significantly improved by relaxing those assumptions, without sacrificing validity (Acton & Revelle, 2002; Gurtman & Pincus, 2000).

We included additional dimensional models that allowed for deviations from normality and retained the strict conditions for a perfect circumplex. This approach can be applied by skew-t-CFA (Asparouhov & Muthén, 2016), allowing the estimation of flexible nonnormal distributions for the latent continuous factors (i.e., skewed, t-shaped or both). For the skew-t-CFA approach, additional parameters comprise multivariate degrees of freedom and factor-specific skew. When the model does not converge, a simpler approach can be used that does not include the t-distribution (i.e., skew-CFA) or the skew parameters (i.e., t-CFA). In CFA and skew-t-CFA individuals are still assumed to stem from one single population.

This is different for the hybrid approach, as implemented by factor mixture modeling, namely, Semi-Parametric Factor Analysis (SP-FA; Hallquist & Wright, 2014). Like in fully dimensional approaches, individuals are presumed to vary in three continuous dimensions (following the perfect circumplex). However, a latent k-fold categorical variable identifies locations in the three-dimensional space in which individuals concentrate in clusters (i.e., to form latent classes). Those latent classes are characterized by distinct patterns on the factor-specific means. Each class is assumed to be normally distributed and factor variances are fixed to be equal in all classes. The dimensional part of the hybrid model was confirmatory and deciding upon the optimal number of latent classes was data-driven by consulting the AICC and BIC. The maximum number of extracted classes was limited to nine and solutions that included latent classes comprising less than 5% of the total sample were not considered. Supported solutions were selected as candidate models. We also considered including nonparametric factor analysis (NP-FA; Hallquist & Wright, 2014) as a second hybrid modeling approach, however, it was not identified.

Model Evaluation

Structural validity of models was assessed by relative fit indices to compare nonnested models, corrected Akaike’s information criterion (AICC) and the Bayesian information criterion (BIC). Tests of model fit quantify the degree to which the assumption of local independence is valid (i.e., a common assumption of latent variable models). Local independence states that the covariance between indicators ought to be fully explained by the latent variables included in the model, in other words, indicators ought to be uncorrelated conditioned on the latent variables. AICC and BIC evaluate model fit but differ in the degree to which model parsimony (e.g., the number of free parameters) is weighted (Dziak, Coffman, Lanza, & Li, 2012; Vrieze, 2012). The BIC more heavily penalizes additional parameters (i.e., weighs parsimony more) and, therefore, when the AICC and BIC disagree in practice, the BIC always favors a more parsimonious model relative to the AICC.

Concurrent validity of candidate models was evaluated as follows: we estimated individual factor scores and class memberships to investigate criterion-oriented validity of competing approaches, which was defined as the utility of the models in predicting conceptually relevant external variables. Those variables were chosen to capture a broad range of psychopathology and personality-related variation. For this purpose, multiple linear regressions were estimated to predict continuous outcomes (e.g., measures of symptom load) from multiple latent scores (i.e., factor scores and/or class memberships). The adjusted coefficient of determination (R2) was used to estimate the amount of variance explained in external variables. The relative importance of predictors is indicated by the unique variance explained in external variables (ΔR2), corresponding to the squared part correlation between predictor and criterion. We dummy-coded the categorical variable denoting latent class membership.3 We explored possible interactions between latent dimensions, although previous research has indicated that interactions among IPC factors are not often significant (Wilson et al., 2013).

Several goodness-of-fit indices were calculated for factor analytic models: The comparative fit index (CFI), the Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI), the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), and standardized root mean square residual (SRMR). We further calculated statistical indices that are useful for the psychometric evaluation of factor analytic models, for which multiple common sources of variance are present, for example, one general factor and two group factors (Rodriguez, Reise, & Haviland, 2016).4 We calculated Explained Common Variance (ECV) to estimate the degree to which the octant scores have one single common source of variance, such that the measure could be considered essentially unidimensional. ECV reflects the percent of common variance that can be attributed to the general factor with values closer to 1 indicating stronger unidimensionality. Coefficient H was calculated to estimate the degree to which latent factors would likely replicate across samples. Values of H greater than .60 are recommended. Factor determinacy (FD) was calculated to evaluate whether factor score estimates could be considered trustworthy to reflect true individual differences. Values of FD greater than .90 indicate trustworthy factor scores. Standardized residuals between the model-implied and the observed covariance matrices were investigated to identify localized areas of strain (i.e., misfit), with values greater than |2.56| indicating local misfit.

Software Packages

Statistical analyses were executed using R (R Core Team, 2017), the Lavaan Package (Rosseel, 2012), the MplusAutomation Package (Hallquist & Wiley, 2018), and Mplus Version 8 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998–2017).

We estimated LCA and SP-FA models beginning with 500 random starts and doubling the number of random starts when needed to replicate the log-likelihood at least 10 times. Multiple regressions with continuous outcome were estimated by Ordinary Least Squares (OLS). Robust regression analysis was employed by the Robustbase Package (Maechler et al., 2016) to screen for discrepancies with OLS estimates.

Results

Model Estimation

LCA models for up to 15 latent classes were estimated and mostly converged (nonconvergence occurred in Sample 3 for k = 15 and in Sample 4 for k ≥ 8). The entropy statistic never went below .811 indicating acceptable class separation. Categorical solutions were not considered for subsequent analytic steps when the smallest latent class comprised less than 5% of the total sample, which was the case in Sample 1 for k ≥ 10, Sample 2 k ≥ 9, Sample 3 k ≥ 7, Sample 4 k ≥ 5. VLMR favored less complex class solutions, generally (Sample 1 k = 5, Sample 2 and 3 k = 3, Sample 4 k = 2). BLRT, AICC, and BIC favored the most complex solutions, respectively (Sample 1 k = 9, Sample 2 k = 8, Sample 3 k = 6, Sample 4 k = 4). Notably, the optimal number of classes was not robust between decision criteria and not robust between samples. Further details on the estimated LCA models can be found in the online supplemental materials (Tables S6–S9).

The most restrictive CFA-PC model (i.e., equal spacing and communalities, latent normal distributions) yielded acceptable fit to the data, CFI = .938–.957, TLI = .928–.950, SRMR = .059–.078 (Samples 1–4). The RMSEA suggested shortcomings in fit, RMSEA = .075–.111, p < .001. Across samples, standardized factor loadings were statistically significant, λGENERAL = .57–.82, λAGENCY = |.56|–|.64|, λCOMMUNION = |.49|–|.54| (highest loading for the marker octants). ECV indicated that the general factor accounted for roughly 75% (and the group factors for 25%) of the common variance, ECV = .69–.76 (Samples 1–4). This shows that, although the general factor was the dominant source of variance, the construct should not be considered essentially unidimensional. Coefficient H indicated that the general factor was defined well and the style dimensions were defined moderately well by the indicators used, HGENERAL = .88–.89, HAGENCY = .50–.63, HCOMMUNION = .49–.58 (Samples 1–4), such that those latent variables would likely replicate across samples. Factor determinacy indicated that factor scores could be trusted to reflect true individual variation, FDGENERAL = .96–.97, FDAGENCY = .87–.92, FDCOMMUNION = .85–.90 (Samples 1–4). The latent correlation between agency and the general factor was estimated to be negative (Sample 1 r = −.283, Sample 2 r = −.292, Sample 3 r = −.267, Sample 4 r = −.324). The statistical association between communion and the general factor was dependent from the sample investigated (Sample 1 r = −.034, Sample 2 r = .142, Sample 3 r = .083, Sample 4 r = −.115). The less restrictive CFA-QC (i.e., allowing for unequal spacing and communalities) produced virtually identical parameter and fit estimates. Therefore, it is not described any further.

Skew-t-CFA converged exclusively in Sample 1. Nonnormal skew-t-distributions were estimated to have 20.85 df and substantial skew for the latent factors, skewGENERAL = 1.39, skewAGENCY = 0.85, skewCOMMUNION = −0.89. Skew-normal-CFA did not converge in Samples 2–4. We estimated t-distributed CFA models without skew parameter in the samples remaining, df = 8.43 (Sample 2), df = 13.78 (Sample 3), df = 6.35 (Sample 4). Standardized residuals between the model-implied and the observed covariance matrices showed no significant localized areas of misfit for Samples 1–3 (i.e., IIP), ranging from −0.27 to 0.22. In Sample 4, standardized residuals ranged from −3.10 to 1.76 indicating local misfit.

Nonconvergence of SP-FA solutions occurred in Sample 2 and Sample 4 (for k ≥ 5). Hybrid solutions were not considered when the smallest latent class comprised less than 5% of the total sample, which was the case in Sample 1 for k ≥ 6, Sample 3 k ≥ 3, Sample 4 k = 4. AICC and BIC univocally selected the most complex hybrid solutions available (Sample 1 k = 5, Sample 2 k = 4, Sample 3 k = 2, Sample 4 k = 3). Further details, including SP-FA model’s entropy statistics and smallest class proportions, are described in the online supplemental materials (Tables S10–S13).

Model Comparison

Direct comparisons between dimensional, hybrid, and categorical candidate models are summarized in Table 2. Smaller values of AICC and BIC indicate favorable fit. Higher values in R2 are preferable, as more variance is explained by model-based factor scores and/or predicted class memberships. Further details into model performance by sample are available in the online supplemental materials (Tables S14–S17).

Table 2. Summary of Model Fit and Variance Explained in External Variables for Dimensional, Categorical, and Hybrid Candidate Models of Interpersonal Problems Across Samples.

Sample κ Factors Classes Statistical model AICC BIC Median R2
Range of ΔR2
General distress Agency Communion Class membership
Note. Most favorable values are highlighted in bold print. κ = number of free parameters; AICC = corrected Akaike’s information criterion; BIC = Bayesian information criterion; Median R2 = average explained variance in external variables by sample; Range of ΔR2 = range of unique variance explained in external variables by sample; CFA-PC = confirmatory factor analysis (perfect circumplex); CFA-QC = confirmatory factor analysis (quasi circumplex); Skew-t-CFA = confirmatory factor analysis with non-normal latent skewed t-distribution; SP-FA = semiparametric factor analysis. LCA = Latent class analysis; VLMR = Vuong-Lo-Mendel-Rubin Test; BLRT = Bootstrapped Likelihood-Ratio Test.
a Optimal number of classes by AICC. b Optimal number of classes by BIC. c Optimal number of classes by VLMR. d Optimal number of classes by BLRT.
Sample 1 (N = 5,400) 20 3 CFA-PC 111,545 111,413 .32 .10–.40 .00–.08 .00–.02
23 3 CFA-QC 111,496 111,344 .32 .10–.40 .00–.08 .00–.02
24 3 Skew-t-CFA 110,337 110,495 .32 .10–.40 .00–.08 .00–.02
36 3 5 SP-FAa,b 110,438 110,674 .32 .04–.14 .00–.04 .00–.01 .00–.00
52 5 LCAc 118,573 118,915 .27 .09–.40
88 9 LCAa,b,d 114,771 115,348 .28 .09–.42
Sample 2 (N = 491) 20 3 CFA-PC 12,545 12,463 .46 .12–.49 .00–.10 .00–.11
23 3 CFA-QC 12,547 12,453 .46 .12–.49 .00–.10 .00–.11
21 3 t-CFA 12,418 12,505 .46 .12–.50 .00–.10 .00–.11
32 3 4 SP-FAa,b 12,305 12,435 .45 .02–.12 .00–.02 .00–.07 .00–.01
34 3 LCAc 12,910 13,047 .33 .03–.45
79 8 LCAa,b,d 12,361 12,662 .39 .12–.51
Sample 3 (N = 656) 20 3 CFA-PC 13,622 13,710 .13 .01–.18 .00–.09 .00–.06
23 3 CFA-QC 13,719 13,617 .13 .01–.18 .00–.09 .00–.06
21 3 t-CFA 13,525 13,617 .13 .01–.18 .00–.09 .00–.06
24 3 2 SP-FAa,b 13,575 13,681 .13 .01–.13 .00–.06 .00–.06 .00–.00
34 3 LCAc 15,024 15,173 .07 .00–.12
61 6 LCAa,b,d 14,446 14,707 .07 .00–.15
Sample 4 (N = 712) 20 3 CFA-PC 30,271 30,181 .25 .01–.23 .00–.29 .00–.15
23 3 CFA-QC 30,262 30,159 .25 .01–.23 .01–.29 .00–.15
21 3 t-CFA 29,831 29,926 .25 .01–.22 .00–.29 .00–.15
28 3 3 SP-FAa,b 29,908 30,034 .25 .00–.12 .00–.17 .00–.12 .00–.02
25 2 LCAc 31,968 32,080 .08 .00–.15
43 4 LCAa,b,d 31,089 31,279 .15 .00–.23

With regard to dimensional models in this study, relaxing the “equal spacing and equal communalities” restriction did not result in consistent improvements in terms of model fit. In contrast, relaxing the restriction of latent normality by means of skew-t-CFA and t-CFA resulted in greatly improved model fit. However, it did not increment the prediction of external variables when compared against the more simplistic CFA model. Visual inspection of the density plots revealed one explanation for this finding: The distribution of estimated factor scores had almost identical shape for all models that included the three dimensions (i.e., CFA-PC, nonnormal CFA, SP-FA; see online supplemental materials, Figures S4–S7). Correlations between factor scores were consistently greater than .95, demonstrating that those scores carried the same information independently of specification.

Model fit favored the dimensional models. The BIC and AICC univocally selected t-distributed CFA models in Samples 1, 3, and 4 (the number of free parameters for those CFA models were κ = 21 and κ = 24). In Sample 2, BIC and AICC selected the hybrid SP-FA model, κ = 32. However, this might have to do with the bimodal distribution observed in Sample 2, because one subset (i.e., outpatients) was selected for extreme values and the other subset was not (i.e., control group).5 Variance explained in external variables favored dimensions, while fully categorical models showed poor performance in predicting external variables. Within fully categorical models, variance explained was greater for more complex solutions. Notably, hybrid models and fully dimensional models performed equally well in predicting external variables. However, critically, estimated class memberships in SP-FA models did not increment the prediction after accounting for the variance explained by factor scores. All things considered (i.e., model fit, variance explained in external variables, consistency across samples), the best performance was achieved by fully dimensional models that permit for nonnormal factor distributions (i.e., skew-t-CFA, t-CFA model).

We report on the associations observed between factor scores from CFA-PC and external variables in Table 3, to evaluate the relevance of interpersonal problems in relation to diverse pathological and personality-related outcomes. Predictive utility of interpersonal dimensions was considered substantial for values of ΔR2 greater than .05. The general factor accounted for the largest share of variance explained in external variables (however, those were mostly markers of symptomatic distress). Agency and communion showed substantial and consistent associations. An agentic style was associated with extraversion, hostility, antagonism, paranoid ideation, disinhibition, low empathy, and low agreeableness. An affiliative style was associated with extraversion, low detachment, empathy, and agreeableness. The predictive utility of interactions was trivial, except for predicting the agreeableness domain. Associations between interpersonal dimensions and HiTOP-related diagnosis counts were less pronounced in the current study (yet, the psychometric properties of diagnosis counts are not clear). Variance explained was largest for the bimodal sample, indicating that the dimensional factor scores performed well in differentiating between outpatients and the healthy control group.

Table 3. Unique Contributions of Factor Scores (3-Factor CFA-PC) in Predicting External Variables.

ΔR2
Sample Measure Predicted external variable General distress Agency Communion Interactions
Note.  Values of ΔR2 > .05 were considered substantial and are highlighted in bold print (the key of the association is noted in parentheses). ΔR² = unique variance explained in external variables; SCL-90-R = Symptom-Checklist-90-Revised; BSI = Brief Symptom Inventory; BIS = Barratt Impulsiveness Scale; EQ = Empathy Quotient; OAS = The Other as Shamer Scale; DERS = Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale; SPQ = Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire; PCL = Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Checklist Scale; SCID-I = Structured Clinical Interview for DSM–IV Axis-I disorders; SIDP-IV = Structured Interview for DSM–IV Personality; BFI-2 = Big-Five-Inventory 2; PID-5-100 = Personality Inventory for DSM–5; N = neuroticism; A = agreeableness; C = conscientiousness; E = extraversion; O = open-mindedness; NEG = negative affectivity; ANT = antagonism; DIS = disinhibition; DET = detachment; PSY = psychoticism.
Sample 1 (N = 5,400) SCL-90-R General Severity Index .39 (+) .00 .00 .01
Somatization .16 (+) .00 .00 .00
Obsessive-compulsiveness .28 (+) .00 .00 .00
Interpersonal sensitivity .40 (+) .00 .00 .01
Depression .29 (+) .00 .00 .00
Anxiety .20 (+) .00 .00 .00
Hostility .27 (+) .08 (+) .00 .00
Paranoid ideation .37 (+) .00 .00 .01
Phobic anxiety .12 (+) .00 .01 .01
Psychoticism .33 (+) .01 .01 .00
Sample 2 (N = 491) BSI General Severity Index .48 (+) .01 .00 .01
Somatization .27 (+) .01 .00 .02
Obsessive-compulsiveness .34 (+) .00 .00 .01
Interpersonal sensitivity .42 (+) .00 .00 .01
Depression .38 (+) .00 .00 .01
Anxiety .36 (+) .01 .00 .01
Hostility .36 (+) .10 (+) .00 .00
Paranoid ideation .34 (+) .05 (+) .00 .01
Phobic anxiety .37 (+) .00 .00 .02
Psychoticism .46 (+) .01 .00 .01
BIS Cognitive impulsiveness .33 (+) .00 .00 .01
EQ Empathy .12 (−) .05 (−) .11 (+) .02
OAS External shame .46 (+) .00 .00 .01
DERS Emotional dysregulation .45 (+) .00 .00 .01
SPQ Schizotypal personality .49 (+) .01 .01 .01
PCL Posttraumatic stress .44 (+) .04 .00 .02
Sample 3 (N = 656) SCID-II/SIDP-IV diagnosis counts Total diagnoses .18 (+) .00 .00 .01
Axis-I .07 (+) .00 .00 .00
Axis-II .14 (+) .02 .00 .01
Internalizing-fear .02 .03 .01 .01
Internalizing-distress .12 (+) .00 .00 .01
Externalizing-antagonism .11 (+) .07 (+) .00 .01
Externalizing-disinhibition .01 .07 (+) .00 .00
Detachment .04 .09 (−) .06 (−) .01
Thought disorder .01 .01 .02 .01
Sample 4 (N = 712) PID-5-100 Domains Negative affect .18 (+) .01 .01 .01
Antagonism .20 (+) .18 (+) .00 .03
Disinhibition .16 (+) .01 .00 .02
Detachment .12 (+) .00 .14 (−) .01
Psychoticism .12 (+) .03 .01 .01
BFI-2 Domains Negative emotionality .13 (+) .02 .00 .01
Agreeableness .17 (−) .17 (−) .09 (+) .06
Conscientiousness .06 (−) .00 .00 .02
Extraversion .02 .27 (+) .15 (+) .02
Open-mindedness .01 .00 .00 .00
BFI-2 Facets N Anxiety .06 (+) .03 .00 .01
Depression .12 (+) .03 .02 .01
Emotional volatility .11 (+) .00 .01 .01
A Compassion .07 (−) .13 (−) .13 (+) .05
Respectfulness .16 (−) .21 (−) .02 .04
Trust .12 (−) .04 .05 (+) .03
C Organization .01 .00 .00 .02
Productiveness .05 (−) .01 .00 .02
Responsibility .08 (−) .01 .00 .01
E Sociability .01 .20 (+) .21 (+) .03
Assertiveness .00 .32 (+) .01 .03
Energy level .04 .04 .13 (+) .02
O Intellectual curiosity .00 .01 .00 .00
Aesthetic sensitivity .00 .01 .00 .00
Creative imagination .01 .02 .00 .02
PID-5–100 Facets NEG Emotional lability .14 (+) .00 .01 .01
Anxiousness .12 (+) .02 .00 .00
Separation insecurity .10 (+) .00 .03 .00
ANT Manipulativeness .13 (+) .19 (+) .00 .02
Deceitfulness .22 (+) .08 (+) .00 .02
Grandiosity .10 (+) .13 (+) .01 .02
DIS Irresponsibility .15 (+) .02 .00 .01
Impulsivity .08 (+) .05 .01 .01
Distractibility .05 (+) .01 .01 .02
DET Withdrawal .13 (+) .00 .14 () .02
Anhedonia .13 (+) .00 .07 () .02
Intimacy avoidance .01 .01 .05 () .01
PSY Unusual beliefs and experiences .05 (+) .04 .00 .01
Eccentricity .11 (+) .03 .02 .01
Perceptual dysregulation .08 (+) .01 .01 .01

Discussion

In the current study, we compared dimensional, categorical, and hybrid models of interpersonal problems considering structural and concurrent validity. Across four samples we found consistent support for the superior validity of a purely dimensional representation (i.e., confirmatory factor analytic models based on the IPC), especially when allowing for nonnormal latent distributions. No evidence was found for the incremental validity of categorical or hybrid approaches.

Dimensions Versus Types

Dimensional models outperformed fully categorical models with regard to fit indices, showing that the covariance between octant scores was more accurately reproduced by three dimensions than by any number of latent classes or hybrid models. Also, the criteria used to explore the optimal number of classes for LCA models did not generate consistent and replicable results. The VLMR selected sparse LCA solutions, whereas the other stopping criteria selected the most complex class solutions available. In most cases the number of suggested classes is difficult to conceptualize or apply in practical work. Furthermore, the dimensional models outperformed purely categorical models in terms of prediction. Most likely, the latent classes are artificial, because fitting categorical models to a truly dimensional latent structure can result in extracting spurious classes (Lubke & Neale, 2006). Such spurious classes would partially cover the dimensional variance, in that the variance explained increases as a function of classes extracted. Our results indicate that this was the case in the present study. With regard to the hybrid approach, the extracted latent classes did not increment the prediction (i.e., after controlling for factor scores). Also, the shapes of the density distributions did not convey the impression that individuals concentrated in distinct clusters. Considering our results, the concordance in shape between the hybrid and fully dimensional approach appears to be more consistent with having nonnormal population distributions. This inherent nonnormality could fully account for the formation of classes in hybrid models. Thus, the additional classes may simply compensate for deviations from the normal assumption (Bauer & Curran, 2003) without providing any substantial information. In light of the evidence presented here, it seems questionable to assume that actual discrete interpersonal subpopulations might exist. Although more evidence will be needed to substantiate this claim, our results seem to be robust with respect to reproducibility and generalizability: They are based on four larger samples of individuals from different countries and cultural backgrounds spanning community recruited participants to patients with severe personality problems using two measures of interpersonal problems.

If the latent structure of interpersonal problems was truly dimensional, categorical scoring of interpersonal problems might result in loss of statistical power (Markon et al., 2011; Morey et al., 2012; Preacher et al., 2005) and impede statistical inferences (Kane, 2013). For illustration, imagine a latent class that was characterized by an octant score profile pattern aligning with high distress and a dominance-related style. It would be indistinguishable whether associations of class membership to other constructs would have resulted from severity or style. For example, Cain et al. (2012) reported six clusters from which the submissive type predicted chronicity of major depression. Closer inspection reveals that chronicity was greatest for the submissive class, but also high for other low agency classes. Based on our findings, we could reframe the authors’ conclusion to say that low agency might have predicted chronicity (instead of class membership). Yet, based on the report we cannot retain a precise point estimate and confidence intervals for the effect, because the categorical approach neglects within-class variance and treats it as random error, artificially shrinking the effect. Another concern is that general distress might have driven the effect, because multidimensional variation was confounded within the classes reported (and because severity appears to be correlated with style). This example shows that inferences based on categorical scoring (i.e., cluster analysis, latent class/profile analysis) might be significantly curtailed, given that the latent structure of interpersonal problems was multidimensional. Still, empirical results from those approaches can be interpreted with reference to IPC-based dimensions, as was demonstrated here.

Relevance of Interpersonal Dimensions

The common variance among general interpersonal distress and clinical measures relates to a general factor of psychopathology (Caspi & Moffitt, 2018), indicating that self-reported interpersonal problems include a generic form of symptomatic distress (Tracey et al., 1996). According to one hypothesis, general interpersonal distress reflects diffuse interpersonal impairments that lead to real-world consequences for the individual. Such impairments have recently been conceptualized as underpinned and maintained by a particular lack of resilience, namely a complex interplay between biological factors, maltreatment, impaired mentalizing, and epistemic mistrust that compromises social learning (Fonagy & Allison, 2014; Nolte, Campbell, & Fonagy, 2019). A second hypothesis is that general interpersonal distress mainly reflects distorted cognition (i.e., dissatisfaction with interpersonal relations). Multimethod approaches could provide further insights into the nature of this factor.

Concerning the IPC-related style dimensions, our results substantiate the notion that high agency aligns robustly with antagonistic personality (Williams & Simms, 2016) and the externalizing symptom spectrum in HiTOP (Kotov et al., 2017). Our study indicates that agentic problems might be less associated with overall distress, mirroring earlier findings (Wright et al., 2012). Unsurprisingly, low communion seems to align with the detachment trait and symptom spectrum (Kotov et al., 2017). Besides, lacking associations between interpersonal styles and many psychopathologies deserve further attention, in that, most prominently, depression and anxiety (i.e., two main areas of pathoplasticity research) could not be related to specific interpersonal styles. Yet, interpersonal styles were indeed strongly associated with normative personality traits at the domain level (i.e., extraversion, agreeableness) and the facet level (e.g., assertiveness, sociability, and compassion), suggesting that interpersonal styles might largely reflect nonpathological dispositions of interacting with others. Prior research has indicated that “cold” problems were better covered by maladaptive traits and “warm” problems were better covered by normative traits (Williams & Simms, 2016; Wright et al., 2012). As a general conclusion, we suggest that variation in interpersonal styles is likely to result from both pathological processes and temperamental differences. Longitudinal investigations could procure further insights into the causal pathways.

Limitations

The current investigation was based on self-report measures of interpersonal functioning. However, correlations between self-reports of IIP and informant-reports of impact messages (Altenstein-Yamanaka et al., 2017; Quilty et al., 2013) or social competences (Leising, Krause, Köhler, Hinsen, & Clifton, 2011) were rather small. This raises the question whether the structure is dependent on the assessment method (self-report, assessment by mental health professionals, or significant others) and also, which method would deliver the most valuable information. Another limitation of our study is that the most adequate model might also be dependent on the populations investigated (e.g., Eaton, Krueger, South, Simms, & Clark, 2011). Lastly, our study did not allow for evaluation of predicting future outcomes (e.g., therapy outcome measures).

Conclusions and Practical Recommendations

A longstanding tradition has modeled interpersonal dispositions in a circumplex (e.g., Alden et al., 1990; Horowitz et al., 2006; Kiesler, 1983; Leary, 1958; Wiggins, 1979). However, the coexistence of dimensional and categorical approaches to score interpersonal problems impeded the conceptual and statistical integration of empirical results. Although the categorical interpretation (i.e., prototype model) might be pragmatic for practitioners and patients, we found little evidence that latent classes can enhance the conception of interpersonal problems. The latent structure of interpersonal problems was best described by IPC-based continuous dimensions, especially when allowing for nonnormal latent distributions. To date, most other constructs in psychopathology research have also shown a dimensional structure (Aslinger, Manuck, Pilkonis, Simms, & Wright, 2018; Carragher et al., 2014; Haslam, Holland, & Kuppens, 2012; Wright et al., 2013b).

For future investigations and meta-analyses, we recommend to use a unitary modeling approach for interpersonal research to advance cumulative science. We believe that our results support the construct validity of dimensional IPC-based scores (see Method section for scoring formulas, see online supplemental material R Codes S1–S3 for latent variable applications to use in statistical software). The IPC-based approach offers a parsimonious model that is easy to interpret and to implement (i.e., standard scoring), useful for prediction purposes, and it does not require model estimation (i.e., no sample size requirements, no risk of overfitting). Another benefit is that the IPC is embedded in a metaframework (Dawood et al., 2018; Pincus, Lukowitsky, & Wright, 2010) that provides a link to motivational (being in control of and being close to others) and behavioral aspects of personality (dominance and nurturance). We highlight the importance of including all three interpersonal dimensions in regression analyses to account for correlations between them and to establish the incremental information of severity (i.e., general distress) and style (i.e., agency and communion). We advise against the use of categorical scoring procedures for heuristic purposes for the stated reasons. One area of work that has most relied on those techniques is pathoplasticity research (e.g., Cain et al., 2010, 2012; Przeworski et al., 2011; Wright et al., 2013), with a consistent finding that certain disorders encompass many diverse interpersonal “types” that provide incremental clinical information above diagnosis. How should this research proceed without enlisting categorical interpersonal models? We argue that intradiagnosis interpersonal heterogeneity will be reflected in high variability (e.g., SDs) of interpersonal style dimensions within diagnostic groups, as well as in low associations between interpersonal style dimensions and the pathology in question. When such high variability and low correlations occur, they can be further investigated with visual plots (as is common in pathoplasticity research) and the incremental validity of the style dimensions can be tested using standard procedures. Notably, depression and anxiety pathologies form main topics in pathoplasticity research, and for those, no substantial associations with interpersonal styles were found in the current study.

We further advise against the use of octant scores, because the IPC-based model yields a parsimonious summary with sufficient approximation to diverse clinical populations. Future structural analyses of psychological constructs should consider nonnormal approaches (when practically feasible for a given sample size) to avoid being misled into retaining hybrid solutions. Furthermore, our results underline that hybrid solutions should be evaluated on the grounds of both structural and criterion-oriented validity.

The interpersonal sphere denotes an important domain of personality functioning. We examined the latent structure of interpersonal problems and illustrated the relevance of interpersonal dimensions for psychopathology research by enlisting associations to clinical symptom markers and personality-related variables. Our study provides guidance and practical recommendations for future investigations to study interpersonal problems and their correlates, including a call for a unitary use of the IPC-based dimensional model. Topics for future research may include the moderating effects of interpersonal style on treatment outcome (i.e., personalized psychotherapy research) or investigating the changes in interpersonal style in the course of mental illness (i.e., pathoplasticity research).

Supplementary Material

DOI: 10.1037/abn0000460.supp

Footnotes

1

Some authors simply used the eight raw octant scores of interpersonal problems (Dammann et al., 2016; Gerhart, Baker, Hoerger, & Ronan, 2014; Quinlan, Deane, Crowe, & Caputi, 2018; Wilson, Stroud, & Durbin, 2017). Others scored the quadrants (König, Onnen, Karl, Rosner, & Butollo, 2016) or cluster analyzed IPC-based scores (Cain et al., 2010).

2

In contrast, regular CFA assumes one single population in which individuals are concentrated in the center of the multivariate normal distribution.

3

However, the coding does not influence how much variance is explained by the polytomous predictor variable.

4

Note that in the current CFA model the general factor is free to correlate with the group factors (unlike in a true bifactor models). Therefore, statistics that are specific to classic bifactor models should not be calculated, because they require unique partitioning of indicator variance.

5

Visual inspection of factor scores revealed that the SP-FA approach was superior in accounting for this inherent bimodality. For reasons of simplicity, we did not account for this bimodality in our candidate models. Yet ideally, one could specify a semiparametric t-distributed confirmatory factor model including three t-distributed factors and two classes.

References

  1. Acton G. S., & Revelle W. (2002). Interpersonal personality measures show circumplex structure based on new psychometric criteria. Journal of Personality Assessment, 79, 446–471. 10.1207/S15327752JPA7903_04 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Alden L. E., Wiggins J. S., & Pincus A. L. (1990). Construction of circumplex scales for the Inventory of Interpersonal Problems. Journal of Personality Assessment, 55, 521–536. 10.1080/00223891.1990.9674088 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Altenstein-Yamanaka D., Zimmermann J., Krieger T., Dörig N., & Grosse Holtforth M. (2017). Self-reported interpersonal problems and impact messages as perceived by significant others are differentially associated with the process and outcome of depression therapy. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 64, 410–423. 10.1037/cou0000221 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. American Psychiatric Association (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed., text rev.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing. [Google Scholar]
  5. American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing. [Google Scholar]
  6. Ansell E. B., Grilo C. M., & White M. A. (2012). Examining the interpersonal model of binge eating and loss of control over eating in women. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 45, 43–50. 10.1002/eat.20897 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Aslinger E. N., Manuck S. B., Pilkonis P. A., Simms L. J., & Wright A. G. C. (2018). Narcissist or narcissistic? Evaluation of the latent structure of narcissistic personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 127, 496–502. 10.1037/abn0000363 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Asparouhov T., & Muthén B. (2016). Structural equation models and mixture models with continuous nonnormal skewed distributions. Structural Equation Modeling, 23, 1–19. 10.1080/10705511.2014.947375 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  9. Badcock J. C., & Dragović M. (2006). Schizotypal personality in mature adults. Personality and Individual Differences, 40, 77–85. 10.1016/j.paid.2005.06.015 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  10. Baron-Cohen S., & Wheelwright S. (2004). The empathy quotient: An investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 163–175. 10.1023/B:JADD.0000022607.19833.00 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Barrett M. S., & Barber J. P. (2007). Interpersonal profiles in major depressive disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 63, 247–266. 10.1002/jclp.20346 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Bauer D. J., & Curran P. J. (2003). Distributional assumptions of growth mixture models: Implications for overextraction of latent trajectory classes. Psychological Methods, 8, 338–363. 10.1037/1082-989X.8.3.338 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Benecke C., Tschiesner R., Boothe B., Frommer J., Huber D., Krause R., & Staats H. (2011). Die DPG-Praxis-Studie [The DPG practice study]. Forum der Psychoanalyse, 27, 203–218. 10.1007/s00451-011-0072-x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  14. Blanchard E. B., Jones-Alexander J., Buckley T. C., & Forneris C. A. (1996). Psychometric properties of the PTSD Checklist (PCL). Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34, 669–673. 10.1016/0005-7967(96)00033-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Blevins C. A., Weathers F. W., Davis M. T., Witte T. K., & Domino J. L. (2015). The posttraumatic stress disorder checklist for DSM–5 (PCL-5): Development and initial psychometric evaluation. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 28, 489–498. 10.1002/jts.22059 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Blomquist K. K., Ansell E. B., White M. A., Masheb R. M., & Grilo C. M. (2012). Interpersonal problems and developmental trajectories of binge eating disorder. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 53, 1088–1095. 10.1016/j.comppsych.2012.05.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Boswell J. F., Cain N. M., Oswald J. M., McAleavey A. A., & Adelman R. (2017). Interpersonal pathoplasticity and trajectories of change in routine adolescent and young adult residential substance abuse treatment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 85, 676–688. 10.1037/ccp0000219 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Boudreaux M. J., Ozer D. J., Oltmanns T. F., & Wright A. G. C. (2018). Development and validation of the circumplex scales of interpersonal problems. Psychological Assessment, 30, 594–609. 10.1037/pas0000505 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Burnham K. P., & Anderson D. R. (2004). Multimodel inference: Understanding AIC and BIC in model selection. Sociological Methods & Research, 33, 261–304. 10.1177/0049124104268644 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  20. Cain N. M., Ansell E. B., Wright A. G., Hopwood C. J., Thomas K. M., Pinto A., et al. Grilo C. M. (2012). Interpersonal pathoplasticity in the course of major depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 80, 78–86. 10.1037/a0026433 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Cain N. M., Pincus A. L., & Grosse Holtforth M. (2010). Interpersonal subtypes in social phobia: Diagnostic and treatment implications. Journal of Personality Assessment, 92, 514–527. 10.1080/00223891.2010.513704 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Carragher N., Krueger R. F., Eaton N. R., Markon K. E., Keyes K. M., Blanco C., et al. Hasin D. S. (2014). ADHD and the externalizing spectrum: Direct comparison of categorical, continuous, and hybrid models of liability in a nationally representative sample. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 49, 1307–1317. 10.1007/s00127-013-0770-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Caspi A., & Moffitt T. E. (2018). All for one and one for all: Mental disorders in one dimension. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 175, 831–844. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2018.17121383 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Constantino M. J., Schwaiger E. M., Smith J. Z., DeGeorge J., McBride C., Ravitz P., & Zuroff D. C. (2010). Patient interpersonal impacts and the early therapeutic alliance in interpersonal therapy for depression. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 47, 418–424. 10.1037/a0021169 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Cooper D., & Anderson T. (2019). Interpersonal subtypes within social anxiety: The identification of distinct social features. Journal of Personality Assessment, 101, 64–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Dammann G., Riemenschneider A., Walter M., Sollberger D., Küchenhoff J., Gündel H., et al. Gremaud-Heitz D. J. (2016). Impact of interpersonal problems in borderline personality disorder inpatients on treatment outcome and psychopathology. Psychopathology, 49, 172–180. 10.1159/000446661 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Dawood S., Dowgwillo E. A., Wu L. Z., & Pincus A. L. (2018). Contemporary Integrative Interpersonal Theory of Personality In Zeigler-Hill V. & Shackleford T. (Eds.), The SAGE handbook of personality and individual differences—Volume 1: The science of personality and individual differences (pp. 171–200). Los Angeles, CA: Sage; 10.4135/9781526451163.n8 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  28. Dawood S., Thomas K. M., Wright A. G., & Hopwood C. J. (2013). Heterogeneity of interpersonal problems among depressed young adults: Associations with substance abuse and pathological personality traits. Journal of Personality Assessment, 95, 513–522. 10.1080/00223891.2013.781031 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Derogatis L. R. (1993). Brief Symptom Inventory: Administration, scoring, and procedures manual. Minneapolis, MN: National Computer Systems. [Google Scholar]
  30. Dinger U., Barrett M. S., Zimmermann J., Schauenburg H., Wright A. G., Renner F., et al. Barber J. P. (2015). Interpersonal problems, dependency, and self-criticism in major depressive disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 71, 93–104. 10.1002/jclp.22120 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Dowgwillo E. A., Roche M. J., & Pincus A. L. (2018). Examining the interpersonal nature of Criterion A of the DSM–5 Section III Alternative Model for Personality Disorders using bootstrapped confidence intervals for the interpersonal circumplex. Journal of Personality Assessment, 100, 581–592. 10.1080/00223891.2018.1464016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Dziak J. J., Coffman D. L., Lanza S. T., & Li R. (2012). Sensitivity and specificity of information criteria (Tech. Rep. No. 12–119). University Park, PA: The Methodology Center, The Pennsylvania State University. [Google Scholar]
  33. Eaton N. R., Krueger R. F., South S. C., Simms L. J., & Clark L. A. (2011). Contrasting prototypes and dimensions in the classification of personality pathology: Evidence that dimensions, but not prototypes, are robust. Psychological Medicine, 41, 1151–1163. 10.1017/S0033291710001650 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Elhai J. D., Biehn T. L., Armour C., Klopper J. J., Frueh B. C., & Palmieri P. A. (2011). Evidence for a unique PTSD construct represented by PTSD’s D1–D3 symptoms. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 25, 340–345. 10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.10.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Erickson T. M., Newman M. G., Siebert E. C., Carlile J. A., Scarsella G. M., & Abelson J. L. (2016). Does worrying mean caring too much? Interpersonal prototypicality of dimensional worry controlling for social anxiety and depressive symptoms. Behavior Therapy, 47, 14–28. 10.1016/j.beth.2015.08.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Euler S., Nolte T., Constantinou M., Griem J., Montague P. R., Fonagy P., & the Personality and Mood Disorders Research Network (2019). Interpersonal problems in borderline personality disorder: Associations with mentalizing, emotion regulation, and impulsiveness. Journal of Personality Disorders. Advance online publication 10.1521/pedi_2019_33_427 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. First M. B., Spitzer R. L., Gibbon M., & Williams J. B. W. (1997). User’s guide for the structured clinical interview for DSM–IV personality disorders (SCID-II, Version 2.0). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. [Google Scholar]
  38. Fonagy P., & Allison E. (2014). The role of mentalizing and epistemic trust in the therapeutic relationship. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 51, 372–380. 10.1037/a0036505 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Gerhart J. I., Baker C. N., Hoerger M., & Ronan G. F. (2014). Experiential avoidance and interpersonal problems: A moderated mediation model. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 3, 291–298. 10.1016/j.jcbs.2014.08.003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  40. Girard J. M., Wright A. G. C., Beeney J. E., Lazarus S. A., Scott L. N., Stepp S. D., & Pilkonis P. A. (2017). Interpersonal problems across levels of the psychopathology hierarchy. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 79, 53–69. 10.1016/j.comppsych.2017.06.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Gomez Penedo J. M., Constantino M. J., Coyne A. E., Bernecker S. L., & Smith-Hansen L. (2018). Patient baseline interpersonal problems as moderators of outcome in two psychotherapies for bulimia nervosa. Psychotherapy Research, 29, 799–811. 10.1080/10503307.2018.1425931 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Gomez Penedo J. M., Constantino M. J., Coyne A. E., Westra H. A., & Antony M. M. (2017). Markers for context-responsiveness: Client baseline interpersonal problems moderate the efficacy of two psychotherapies for generalized anxiety disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 85, 1000–1011. 10.1037/ccp0000233 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Goss K., Gilbert P., & Allan S. (1994). An exploration of shame measures—I: The other as Shamer scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 17, 713–717. 10.1016/0191-8869(94)90149-X [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  44. Gratz K. L., & Roemer L. (2004). Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: Development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 26, 41–54. 10.1023/B:JOBA.0000007455.08539.94 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  45. Grosse Holtforth M., Altenstein D., Krieger T., Flückiger C., Wright A. G., & Caspar F. (2014). Interpersonal differentiation within depression diagnosis: Relating interpersonal subgroups to symptom load and the quality of the early therapeutic alliance. Psychotherapy Research, 24, 429–441. 10.1080/10503307.2013.829253 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Gurtman M. B. (1992). Construct validity of interpersonal personality measures: The interpersonal circumplex as a nomological net. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 105–118. 10.1037/0022-3514.63.1.105 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  47. Gurtman M. B., & Balakrishnan J. D. (1998). Circular measurement redux: The analysis and interpretation of interpersonal circle profiles. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 5, 344–360. 10.1111/j.1468-2850.1998.tb00154.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  48. Gurtman M. B., & Pincus A. L. (2000). Interpersonal Adjective Scales: Confirmation of circumplex structure from multiple perspectives. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 374–384. 10.1177/0146167200265009 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  49. Gurtman M. B., & Pincus A. L. (2003). The circumplex model: Methods and research applications In Schinka J. & Velicer W. (Eds.), Handbook of psychology: Research methods in psychology (2nd ed., pp. 407–428). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
  50. Hallquist M. N., & Wiley J. (2018). MplusAutomation: An R package for facilitating large-scale latent variable analyses in Mplus. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 25, 621–638. 10.1080/10705511.2017.1402334 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Hallquist M. N., & Wright A. G. (2014). Mixture modeling methods for the assessment of normal and abnormal personality, part I: Cross-sectional models. Journal of Personality Assessment, 96, 256–268. 10.1080/00223891.2013.845201 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Haslam N., Holland E., & Kuppens P. (2012). Categories versus dimensions in personality and psychopathology: A quantitative review of taxometric research. Psychological Medicine, 42, 903–920. 10.1017/S0033291711001966 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Henkel M., Zimmermann J., Huber D., Staats H., Wiegand-Grefe S., Taubner S., et al. Benecke C. (2019). Patient characteristics in psychodynamic psychotherapies. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 36, 1–8. 10.1037/pap0000165 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  54. Hopwood C. J., Clarke A. N., & Perez M. (2007). Pathoplasticity of bulimic features and interpersonal problems. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 40, 652–658. 10.1002/eat.20420 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Hopwood C. J., & Good E. W. (2018). Structure and correlates of interpersonal problems and sensitivities. Journal of Personality, 87, 843–855. 10.1111/jopy.12437 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Hopwood C. J., Wright A. G., Ansell E. B., & Pincus A. L. (2013). The interpersonal core of personality pathology. Journal of Personality Disorders, 27, 270–295. 10.1521/pedi.2013.27.3.270 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Horowitz L. M., Alden L. E., Wiggins J. S., & Pincus A. L. (2000). Inventory of Interpersonal Problems manual. Odessa, FL: The Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
  58. Horowitz L. M., Rosenberg S. E., Baer B. A., Ureño G., & Villaseñor V. S. (1988). Inventory of interpersonal problems: Psychometric properties and clinical applications. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 885–892. 10.1037/0022-006X.56.6.885 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Horowitz L. M., Rosenberg S. E., & Bartholomew K. (1993). Interpersonal problems, attachment styles, and outcome in brief dynamic psychotherapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61, 549–560. 10.1037/0022-006X.61.4.549 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Horowitz L. M., Strauss B., & Kordy H. (2000). Inventar zur Erfassung interpersoneller Probleme [The Inventory of Interpersonal Problems] (2nd ed.). Weinheim, Germany: Beltz. [Google Scholar]
  61. Horowitz L. M., Wilson K. R., Turan B., Zolotsev P., Constantino M. J., & Henderson L. (2006). How interpersonal motives clarify the meaning of interpersonal behavior: A revised circumplex model. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 67–86. 10.1207/s15327957pspr1001_4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Huber D., Henrich G., & Klug G. (2007). The Inventory of Interpersonal Problems (IIP): Sensitivity to change. Psychotherapy Research, 17, 474–481. 10.1080/10503300600856160 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  63. Jane J. S., Pagan J. L., Turkheimer E., Fiedler E. R., & Oltmanns T. F. (2006). The interrater reliability of the Structured Interview for DSM–IV Personality. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 47, 368–375. 10.1016/j.comppsych.2006.01.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Kane M. T. (2013). Validating the interpretations and uses of test scores. Journal of Educational Measurement, 50, 1–73. 10.1111/jedm.12000 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  65. Kiesler D. J. (1983). The 1982 interpersonal circle: A taxonomy for complementarity in human transactions. Psychological Review, 90, 185–214. 10.1037/0033-295X.90.3.185 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  66. König J., Onnen M., Karl R., Rosner R., & Butollo W. (2016). Interpersonal subtypes and therapy response in patients treated for posttraumatic stress disorder. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 23, 97–106. 10.1002/cpp.1946 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Kotov R., Krueger R. F., Watson D., Achenbach T. M., Althoff R. R., Bagby R. M., et al. Zimmerman M. (2017). The Hierarchical Taxonomy of Psychopathology (HiTOP): A dimensional alternative to traditional nosologies. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 126, 454–477. 10.1037/abn0000258 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Krieg C. H., & Tracey T. J. (2016). Client interpersonal problems and the initial working alliance. The European Journal of Counselling Psychology, 4, 191–204. 10.5964/ejcop.v4i2.64 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  69. Leary T. (1958). Interpersonal diagnosis of personality. New York, NY: The Ronald Press Company. [Google Scholar]
  70. Leihener F., Wagner A., Haaf B., Schmidt C., Lieb K., Stieglitz R., & Bohus M. (2003). Subtype differentiation of patients with borderline personality disorder using a circumplex model of interpersonal behavior. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 191, 248–254. 10.1097/01.NMD.0000061150.38924.2A [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Leising D., Krause S., Köhler D., Hinsen K., & Clifton A. (2011). Assessing interpersonal functioning: Views from within and without. Journal of Research in Personality, 45, 631–641. 10.1016/j.jrp.2011.08.011 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  72. Lobbestael J., Leurgans M., & Arntz A. (2011). Inter-rater reliability of the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM–IV Axis I Disorders (SCID I) and Axis II Disorders (SCID II). Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 18, 75–79. 10.1002/cpp.693 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Locke K. D. (2010). Circumplex Measures of Interpersonal Constructs In Horowitz L. M. & Strack S. (Eds.), Handbook of interpersonal psychology: Theory, research, assessment, and therapeutic interventions (pp. 313–324). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
  74. Locke K. D., Sayegh L., Penberthy J. K., Weber C., Haentjens K., & Turecki G. (2017). Interpersonal circumplex profiles of persistent depression: Goals, self-efficacy, problems, and effects of group therapy. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 73, 595–611. 10.1002/jclp.22343 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Lo Coco G., Gullo S., Scrima F., & Bruno V. (2012). Obesity and interpersonal problems: An analysis with the interpersonal circumplex. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 19, 390–398. 10.1002/cpp.753 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Lubke G., & Neale M. C. (2006). Distinguishing between latent classes and continuous factors: Resolution by maximum likelihood? Multivariate Behavioral Research, 41, 499–532. 10.1207/s15327906mbr4104_4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Luo X., Nuttall A. K., Locke K. D., & Hopwood C. J. (2018). Dynamic longitudinal relations between binge eating symptoms and severity and style of interpersonal problems. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 127, 30–42. 10.1037/abn0000321 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Maechler M., Rousseeuw P., Croux C., Todorov V., Ruckstuhl A., Salibian-Barrera M., et al. di Palma A. M. (2016). RobustBase: Basic Robust Statistics (R package Version 0.92–7) [Computer software]. http://robustbase.r-forge.r-project.org/
  79. Maples J. L., Carter N. T., Few L. R., Crego C., Gore W. L., Samuel D. B., et al. Miller J. D. (2015). Testing whether the DSM–5 personality disorder trait model can be measured with a reduced set of items: An item response theory investigation of the Personality Inventory for DSM–5. Psychological Assessment, 27, 1195–1210. 10.1037/pas0000120 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Markon K. E., Chmielewski M., & Miller C. J. (2011). The reliability and validity of discrete and continuous measures of psychopathology: A quantitative review. Psychological Bulletin, 137, 856–879. 10.1037/a0023678 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. McEvoy P. M., Burgess M. M., & Nathan P. (2014). The relationship between interpersonal problems, therapeutic alliance, and outcomes following group and individual cognitive behaviour therapy. Journal of Affective Disorders, 157, 25–32. 10.1016/j.jad.2013.12.038 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. McFarquhar T., Luyten P., & Fonagy P. (2018). Changes in interpersonal problems in the psychotherapeutic treatment of depression as measured by the Inventory of Interpersonal Problems: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 226, 108–123. 10.1016/j.jad.2017.09.036 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Miller J. D., Price J., Gentile B., Lynam D. R., & Campbell W. K. (2012). Grandiose and vulnerable narcissism from the perspective of the interpersonal circumplex. Personality and Individual Differences, 53, 507–512. 10.1016/j.paid.2012.04.026 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  84. Monsen J. T., Hagtvet K. A., Havik O. E., & Eilertsen D. E. (2006). Circumplex structure and personality disorder correlates of the Interpersonal Problems model (IIP-C): Construct validity and clinical implications. Psychological Assessment, 18, 165–173. 10.1037/1040-3590.18.2.165 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Morey L. C., Hopwood C. J., Markowitz J. C., Gunderson J. G., Grilo C. M., McGlashan T. H., et al. Skodol A. E. (2012). Comparison of alternative models for personality disorders, II: 6-, 8- and 10-year follow-up. Psychological Medicine, 42, 1705–1713. 10.1017/S0033291711002601 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Muthén L. K., & Muthén B. O. (1998–2017). Mplus user’s guide (8th ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Author. [Google Scholar]
  87. Newman M. G., Jacobson N. C., Erickson T. M., & Fisher A. J. (2017). Interpersonal problems predict differential response to cognitive versus behavioral treatment in a randomized controlled trial. Behavior Therapy, 48, 56–68. 10.1016/j.beth.2016.05.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Nolte T., Campbell C., & Fonagy P. (2019). A mentalization-based and neuroscience-informed model of severe and persistent psychopathology In Pereira J. G., Goncalves J., & Bizzari V. (Eds.), The neurobiology-psychotherapy-pharmacology intervention triangle: The need for common sense in 21st century mental health (p. 161). Wilmington, DE: Vernon Press. [Google Scholar]
  89. Patton J. H., Stanford M. S., & Barratt E. S. (1995). Factor structure of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 51, 768–774. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Pfohl B., Blum N., & Zimmerman M. (1997). Structured interview for DSM–IV personality: SIDP-IV. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing. [Google Scholar]
  91. Pincus A. L. (2018). An interpersonal perspective on Criterion A of the DSM–5 Alternative Model for Personality Disorders. Current Opinion in Psychology, 21, 11–17. 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.08.035 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Pincus A. L., Lukowitsky M. R., & Wright A. G. C. (2010). The interpersonal nexus of personality and psychopathology In Millon T., Krueger R. F., & Simonsen E. (Eds.), Contemporary directions in psychopathology: Scientific foundations of the DSM-V and ICD-11 (pp. 523–552). New York, NY: Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
  93. Preacher K. J., Rucker D. D., MacCallum R. C., & Nicewander W. A. (2005). Use of the extreme groups approach: A critical reexamination and new recommendations. Psychological Methods, 10, 178–192. 10.1037/1082-989X.10.2.178 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Przeworski A., Newman M. G., Pincus A. L., Kasoff M. B., Yamasaki A. S., Castonguay L. G., & Berlin K. S. (2011). Interpersonal pathoplasticity in individuals with generalized anxiety disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 120, 286–298. 10.1037/a0023334 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Puschner B., Kraft S., & Bauer S. (2004). Interpersonal problems and outcome in outpatient psychotherapy: Findings from a long-term longitudinal study in Germany. Journal of Personality Assessment, 83, 223–234. 10.1207/s15327752jpa8303_06 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Quilty L. C., Mainland B. J., McBride C., & Bagby R. M. (2013). Interpersonal problems and impacts: Further evidence for the role of interpersonal functioning in treatment outcome in major depressive disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 150, 393–400. 10.1016/j.jad.2013.04.030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Quinlan E., Deane F. P., Crowe T., & Caputi P. (2018). Do attachment anxiety and hostility mediate the relationship between experiential avoidance and interpersonal problems in mental health careers? Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 7, 63–71. 10.1016/j.jcbs.2018.01.003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  98. Raine A. (1991). The SPQ: A scale for the assessment of schizotypal personality based on DSM–III–R criteria. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 17, 555–564. 10.1093/schbul/17.4.555 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. R Core Team (2017). R: A language and environment for statistical computing [Computer software]. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. [Google Scholar]
  100. Renner F., Jarrett R. B., Vittengl J. R., Barrett M. S., Clark L. A., & Thase M. E. (2012). Interpersonal problems as predictors of therapeutic alliance and symptom improvement in cognitive therapy for depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 138, 458–467. 10.1016/j.jad.2011.12.044 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Rodriguez A., Reise S. P., & Haviland M. G. (2016). Evaluating bifactor models: Calculating and interpreting statistical indices. Psychological Methods, 21, 137–150. 10.1037/met0000045 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Rosseel Y. (2012). lavaan: An R package for structural equation modeling. Journal of Statistical Software, 48, 1–36. 10.18637/jss.v048.i02 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  103. Ruiz M. A., Pincus A. L., Borkovec T. D., Echemendia R. J., Castonguay L. G., & Ragusea S. A. (2004). Validity of the Inventory of Interpersonal Problems for Predicting Treatment Outcome: An investigation with the Pennsylvania Practice Research Network. Journal of Personality Assessment, 83, 213–222. 10.1207/s15327752jpa8303_05 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Salzer S., Pincus A. L., Hoyer J., Kreische R., Leichsenring F., & Leibing E. (2008). Interpersonal subtypes within generalized anxiety disorder. Journal of Personality Assessment, 90, 292–299. 10.1080/00223890701885076 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Salzer S., Streeck U., Jaeger U., Masuhr O., Warwas J., Leichsenring F., & Leibing E. (2010). The relationship between interpersonal subtypes and the modification of interpersonal problems in psychotherapy inpatients. Zeitschrift für Psychosomatische Medizin und Psychotherapie, 56, 191–206. 10.13109/zptm.2010.56.2.191 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Salzer S., Streeck U., Jaeger U., Masuhr O., Warwas J., Leichsenring F., & Leibing E. (2013). Patterns of interpersonal problems in borderline personality disorder. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 201, 94–98. 10.1097/NMD.0b013e3182532b59 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Schmitz N., Hartkamp N., Kiuse J., Franke G. H., Reister G., & Tress W. (2000). The symptom check-list-90-R (SCL-90-R): A German validation study. Quality of Life Research, 9, 185–193. 10.1023/A:1008931926181 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Schwarz G. (1978). Estimating the dimension of a model. Annals of Statistics, 6, 461–464. 10.1214/aos/1176344136 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  109. Shapiro D. A., Barkham M., Rees A., Hardy G. E., Reynolds S., & Startup M. (1994). Effects of treatment duration and severity of depression on the effectiveness of cognitive-behavioral and psychodynamic-interpersonal psychotherapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 522–534. 10.1037/0022-006X.62.3.522 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Simon S., Cain N. M., Wallner Samstag L., Meehan K. B., & Muran J. C. (2015). Assessing interpersonal subtypes in depression. Journal of Personality Assessment, 97, 364–373. 10.1080/00223891.2015.1011330 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Slaney R. B., Pincus A. L., Uliaszek A. A., & Wang K. T. (2006). Conceptions of perfectionism and interpersonal problems: Evaluating groups using the structural summary method for circumplex data. Assessment, 13, 138–153. 10.1177/1073191105284878 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Smart Richman L., & Leary M. R. (2009). Reactions to discrimination, stigmatization, ostracism, and other forms of interpersonal rejection: A multimotive model. Psychological Review, 116, 365–383. 10.1037/a0015250 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Soto C. J., & John O. P. (2017). The next Big Five Inventory (BFI-2): Developing and assessing a hierarchical model with 15 facets to enhance bandwidth, fidelity, and predictive power. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 113, 117–143. 10.1037/pspp0000096 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Tracey T. J., Rounds J., & Gurtman M. (1996). Examination of the general factor with the interpersonal circumplex structure: Application to the Inventory of Interpersonal Problems. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 31, 441–466. 10.1207/s15327906mbr3104_3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Urbán R., Kun B., Farkas J., Paksi B., Kökönyei G., Unoka Z., et al. Demetrovics Z. (2014). Bifactor structural model of symptom checklists: SCL-90-R and Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI) in a non-clinical community sample. Psychiatry Research, 216, 146–154. 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.01.027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Vrieze S. I. (2012). Model selection and psychological theory: A discussion of the differences between the Akaike information criterion (AIC) and the Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC). Psychological Methods, 17, 228–243. 10.1037/a0027127 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Wiggins J. S. (1979). A psychological taxonomy of trait-descriptive terms: The interpersonal domain. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 395–412. 10.1037/0022-3514.37.3.395 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  118. Wiggins J. S. (1991). Agency and communion as conceptual coordinates for the understanding and measurement of interpersonal behavior In Cicchetti D. & Grove W. M. (Eds.), Thinking clearly about psychology: Essays in honor of Paul E. Meehl: Volume 2 Personality and psychopathology (pp. 89–113). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press. [Google Scholar]
  119. Williams T. F., & Simms L. J. (2016). Personality disorder models and their coverage of interpersonal problems. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 7, 15–27. 10.1037/per0000140 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Wilson S., Revelle W., Stroud C. B., & Durbin C. E. (2013). A confirmatory bifactor analysis of the Inventory of Interpersonal Problems—Circumplex and associations of interpersonal traits across multiple relationship contexts and measures. Psychological Assessment, 25, 353–365. 10.1037/a0030989 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Wilson S., Stroud C. B., & Durbin C. E. (2017). Interpersonal dysfunction in personality disorders: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 143, 677–734. 10.1037/bul0000101 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Wright A. G., Hallquist M. N., Morse J. Q., Scott L. N., Stepp S. D., Nolf K. A., & Pilkonis P. A. (2013a). Clarifying interpersonal heterogeneity in borderline personality disorder using latent mixture modeling. Journal of Personality Disorders, 27, 125–143. 10.1521/pedi.2013.27.2.125 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Wright A. G. C., Krueger R. F., Hobbs M. J., Markon K. E., Eaton N. R., & Slade T. (2013b). The structure of psychopathology: Toward an expanded quantitative empirical model. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 122, 281–294. 10.1037/a0030133 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Wright A. G., Pincus A. L., Conroy D. E., & Elliot A. J. (2009). The pathoplastic relationship between interpersonal problems and fear of failure. Journal of Personality, 77, 997–1024. 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00572.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Wright A. G., Pincus A. L., Conroy D. E., & Hilsenroth M. J. (2009). Integrating methods to optimize circumplex description and comparison of groups. Journal of Personality Assessment, 91, 311–322. 10.1080/00223890902935696 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Wright A. G., Pincus A. L., Hopwood C. J., Thomas K. M., Markon K. E., & Krueger R. F. (2012). An interpersonal analysis of pathological personality traits in. DSM–5. Assessment, 19, 263–275. 10.1177/1073191112446657 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Zilcha-Mano S., McCarthy K. S., Dinger U., Chambless D. L., Milrod B. L., Kunik L., & Barber J. P. (2015). Are there subtypes of panic disorder? An interpersonal perspective. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 83, 938–950. 10.1037/a0039373 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Zimmermann J., & Wright A. G. (2017). Beyond description in interpersonal construct validation: Methodological advances in the circumplex structural summary approach. Assessment, 24, 3–23. 10.1177/1073191115621795 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

DOI: 10.1037/abn0000460.supp

Articles from Journal of Abnormal Psychology are provided here courtesy of American Psychological Association

RESOURCES