Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2020 Nov 1.
Published in final edited form as: Chronobiol Int. 2019 Aug 27;36(11):1504–1514. doi: 10.1080/07420528.2019.1658113

Chronotype and postmenopausal breast cancer risk among women in the California Teachers Study

S Hurley 1,*, D Goldberg 1, J Von Behren 1, J Clague DeHart 2, S Wang 3, P Reynolds 1
PMCID: PMC6818501  NIHMSID: NIHMS1540107  PMID: 31452403

Abstract

Chronotype is the behavioral manifestation of an individual’s underlying circadian rhythm, generally characterized by one’s propensity to sleep at a particular time during the 24-hour cycle. Evening chronotypes (“night owls”) generally suffer from worse physical and mental health compared to morning chronotypes (“morning larks”) – for reasons that have yet to be explained. One hypothesis is that evening chronotypes may be more susceptible to circadian disruption, a condition where the coordinated timing of biologic processes breaks down. The role of chronotype as an independent or modifying risk factor for cancer has not been widely explored. The objective of the current study was to evaluate the risk of breast cancer associated with chronotype in a case-control study nested within the California Teachers Study (CTS) cohort. The study population consisted of 39686 post-menopausal CTS participants who provided information on chronotype by completing a questionnaire in 2012-2013. 2719 cases of primary invasive breast cancer diagnosed from 1995/1996 through completion of the chronotype questionnaire were identified by linkage of the CTS to the California Cancer Registry. 36967 CTS participants who had remained cancer-free during this same time period served as controls. Chronotype was ascertained by responses to an abbreviated version of the Horne-Ostberg Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire (MEQ) and was characterized into five categories: definite morning, more morning than evening, neither morning or evening, more evening than morning, definite evening. Multivariable unconditional logistic regression analyses were performed to estimate the odds ratios (ORs) and 95% confidence intervals (95% CIs) for each of the chronotypes, adjusted for established breast cancer risk factors. Compared to definite morning types, definite evening types had an increased risk of breast cancer with elevated ORs that were statistically significant in both the crude (OR = 1.24, 95% CI: 1.10 – 1.40) and fully-adjusted models (OR = 1.20, 95% CI: 1.06-1.35). The risk estimates in the fully-adjusted model for all other chronotypes did not significantly differ from one. These results suggest that evening chronotype may be an independent risk factor for breast cancer among a population of women who are not known to have engaged in any substantial night shift work. Further research in other populations of non-shift workers is warranted.

Keywords: chronotype, breast cancer risk, circadian disruption, case-control, circadian rhythm

INTRODUCTION

Nearly all human physiologic processes are governed to some degree by endogenous daily cellular oscillations, referred to as circadian rhythms. These rhythms not only regulate sleep/wake cycles, they modulate metabolism, hormonal secretions, immune function, and cell cycle control that are critical to maintaining normal behavior and physiology (1). Circadian rhythms are orchestrated by the circadian clock, a collection of specialized neurons in the suprachiasmatic nuclei (SCN) located in the hypothalamus – the proper functioning of which is contingent on external environmental cues (2). The daily oscillation of daytime light and nighttime darkness serves as the primary environmental cue that entrains this system to maintain a regular periodicity of approximately 24 hours. When environmental cues of entrainment are absent or experienced at times out-of-sync with the 24-hour light-dark cycle, the coordinated timing of biologic processes breaks down, resulting in circadian disruption (2).

Chronotype is the behavioral manifestation of an individual’s underlying circadian rhythms, primarily characterized by one’s propensity to sleep at a particular time during the 24-hour cycle. Evening chronotypes or “night owls” are those individuals who tend to be more active in the evening and go to sleep later than their “morning lark” counterparts. For reasons that have yet to be explained, evening chronotypes generally suffer from worse physical and mental health compared to morning chronotype (37). One hypothesis that has been set forth is that evening chronotypes may be more susceptible to circadian disruption (8).

The recognition that a number of biologic processes critical to carcinogenesis, including cellular proliferation, apoptosis, and DNA repair mechanisms operate under strong circadian rhythms, has led to the hypothesis that circadian disruption may be an important risk factor for the development and progression of cancer (3, 917). This hypothesis gained prominence with the classification in 2011 of “shiftwork that involves circadian disruption” as a probable human carcinogen by the International Agency for Research on Cancer (IARC) (18). In addition to strong laboratory evidence for carcinogenic effects of light-at-night exposures in rodents, the IARC classification was largely based on epidemiologic evidence for increased breast cancer risks among female night shift workers – primarily nurses and flight attendants. The degree to which these epidemiologic findings have been confounded by evening chronotypes self-selecting into these professions is a matter of debate and the role that chronotype plays as either an independent risk factor or modifying factor in the development of breast cancer remains unclear.

To date there have been only a handful of epidemiologic studies conducted to examine the association between chronotype and breast cancer risk, yielding conflicting results. A hospital-based case-control study conducted in India (in which less than 3% of the study population had reported ever working night shifts), found that both morning and evening types had increased risks of breast cancer compared to ‘neither morning nor evening’ types (19). Findings from two other studies conducted among occupations with large proportions of night shift workers (but controlling for night-shift work), suggested that compared to morning types, both evening and ‘neither morning nor evening’ types may have elevated risks of breast cancer compared to morning types (20, 21).

The objective of the current analysis was to examine the association between chronotype and breast cancer risk among approximately 39,000 post-menopausal participants in the California Teachers Study –a population of women who are not known to have engaged in any substantial night shift work.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

The California Teachers Study (CTS) is an ongoing prospective cohort study comprised of over 133,000 female California professional employees who responded to a 1995-1996 mailing that was sent to 329,000 active and retired female enrollees in California’s State Teachers Retirement System. Upon entry into the cohort, CTS members completed a baseline questionnaire that included questions on reproductive history, personal and family medical history, health behaviors and other lifestyle factors. Six subsequent mailed questionnaires have been administered to update the baseline data and collect new information on exposures, risk factors, and health outcomes of emerging interest. Chronotype was assessed on the fifth CTS Questionnaire (Q5), administered in 2012-2015.

The CTS cohort has been actively followed since its inception in 1995/1996. Probabilistic data record linkages of the CTS are conducted annually to ascertain cancer diagnosis, death and change of address. State and national mortality files, as well as reports from relatives, are used to ascertain date and cause of death. Address changes for continued follow-up are obtained by several methods including annual mailings, notifications of moves received from participants, and linkages to nationwide consumer reporting companies and the U.S. Postal Service National Change of Address database. Cancer outcomes are identified from annual linkages with the California Cancer Registry (CCR), a legally mandated statewide population-based cancer reporting system. Case ascertainment for the CCR is estimated to be 99% complete (22). Details of the creation and characteristics of the CTS cohort appear elsewhere (23). Upon entry into the study, all participants provided their informed consent to use their data for research purposes such as this study. Subsequent to enrollment, two CTS participants requested to be withdrawn from the study and are not included in the present analysis. The use of human subjects in the CTS has been approved by the Institutional Review Boards at all participating institutions and by the California Committee for the Protection of Human Subjects, California Health and Human Services Agency.

Eligible Population for Analysis

The study population eligible for the current analysis was drawn from the population of 45724 post-menopausal CTS participants who provided chronotype information on the CTS Q5 prior to January 1, 2014, were under the age of 90 years, and had resided in California continuously since entry into the CTS cohort. Ninety-six percent of Q5 respondents provided information on chronotype. From these, we excluded 2401 women who had a diagnosis of any cancer prior to baseline entry into the cohort (identified through both self-report and via the CCR-CTS data linkages) to identify a pool of 43323 eligible women from which our cases and controls were drawn.

Identification of Breast Cancer Cases and Controls

For the purpose of the current analysis, a breast cancer case was defined as having a diagnosis of primary invasive carcinoma of the breast (SEER site = 26000) identified by the annual linkages of the CTS to the CCR during the time interval between joining the cohort in 1995/1996 and December 31, 2013. Participants diagnosed with in situ cancer of the breast were excluded. The remainder of our eligible population (as described above) served as our controls, excluding those who had any cancer diagnosis at any site prior to the time of Q5 completion, identified by the routine CTS-CCR linkages. This process resulted in the identification of 2719 cases of primary invasive breast cancer and 36967 cancer-free controls included in our analysis.

Assessment of Chronotype

Chronotype was ascertained by information collected on the CTS Q5 survey. Overall, approximately 60% of CTS members who were alive and eligible to participate responded to the CTS Q5 survey, 96% of whom provided valid data on chronotype. Information on chronotype was collected using an abbreviated version of the Horne-Ostberg Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire (MEQ). The MEQ is a widely-used, standardized and validated survey instrument (24) that has been shown to correlate with physiologic measures of chronotype (25, 26). Although the original MEQ consists of 19 questions, it has been demonstrated that one question (question 19) predicts chronotype nearly as well as the full 19-item MEQ (14, 27). This question asks: “One hears about ‘morning’ and ‘evening’ types of people. Which one of these types do you consider yourself to be?.” The response categories include “definitely a morning type,” “more a morning than an evening type,” “neither a morning or an evening type,” “more an evening than a morning type,” and “definitely an evening type.” In order to capture potential changes in chronotype throughout life, we adapted this question to ask about which chronotypes respondents considered themselves to be “at different times in your life” including three time frames: “now,” “in your 30-40’s”, and “in your teens/in college.”

Statistical Analysis

Unconditional logistic regression models were run using PROC LOGISTIC in the SAS/STAT software version 9.4 of the SAS system (28) to estimate odds ratios (ORs) and 95% confidence intervals (CIs) for each of the four chronotypes (“more morning than evening,” “neither morning or evening,” “more evening than morning,” “definite evening”) with “definite morning” serving as the referent category. Both crude and multivariable-adjusted ORs were estimated adjusting for age and the following breast cancer risk factors: race/ethnicity; family history of breast cancer, age at menarche, age at first full-term pregnancy, breast feeding history, age at menopause, menopausal hormone therapy (HT) use, body mass index (BMI), strenuous and moderate physical activity, alcohol consumption, and smoking pack-years (see Table 3a footnotes for details of covariate specification).

Table 3a.

Risk of breast cancer associated with chronotype: results from logistic regression analysis among 39686 California Teacher Study participants

Chronotype # cases Crude Odds Ratio a 95% CI Multivariate Adjusted Odds Ratiob 95% CI
Definite morning 955 1.00 1.00
More morning than evening 563 1.10 0.99 – 1.23 1.09 0.98 – 1.22
Neither morning/evening 385 1.13 1.00 – 1.28 1.11 0.98 – 1.25
More evening than morning 424 1.14 1.01 – 1.29 1.11 0.99 – 1.26
Definite evening type 392 1.24 1.10 – 1.40 1.20 1.06 – 1.35
a

Adjusted for age

b

Adjusted for age, race, family history of breast cancer, age at menarche, smoking pack-years, BMI, alcohol consumption, physical activity, age at first full-term pregnancy, breast feeding history, age at menopause, ever use of hormone therapy.

Initially, models were run for chronotype assessed at each of the three different time periods (e.g., “now”, in “30-40’s”, “in teens/in college”). However, most participants (61%) reported stable chronotype throughout their lives and the risk estimates for earlier periods in life were generally similar to those reported for “now” (i.e. concurrent to Q5 completion) (data not shown). Therefore, we present our results only for the “now” chronotype.

We also conducted a number of subset analyses. To address the possibility that a case’s perception of her chronotype might be influenced by her disease or treatment, we repeated our analyses, excluding the 352 breast cancer cases that were diagnosed within two years prior to completing Q5. We also repeated our analyses restricted to the subset of participants who reported stable chronotype throughout their lives. To identify stable chronotypes, we first collapsed the “definite mornings” and “more mornings” into one “morning” category and the “definite evening” and the “more evening” into one “evening category.” We then classified respondents as “Stable Chronotypes” if they reported the same chronotype for “now” as they did for their “teens/in college” (i.e. they were either “morning”, “evening”, or “neither” at both points in time). This resulted in the identification of 24111 individuals with stable chronotypes. We also stratified the data by BMI (< 25 kg/m2, ≥25kg/m2) and tumor hormone receptor status (ER− and PR−, ER+ or PR+); likelihood-ratio tests were used to formally test for interactions effects for these two factors. All p-values are based on two-sided tests with a p-value of < 0.05 considered to be statistically significant.

RESULTS

Morning chronotypes were the most common chronotype in this population of 39686 post-menopausal women. At the time of the CTS Q5 survey, 38% reported their current chronotype as ‘definite morning’, 21% reported ‘more morning than evening’, 15% reported ‘more evening than morning’, 13% reported ‘definite evening’, and 13% reported ‘neither morning nor evening’ (Table 1). The prevalence of definite morning and definite evening types was similar to what was reported in the NHS II while the prevalence of neither type was considerably higher in our population (Supplemental Table 1). In comparison to women enrolled in the Indian hospital-based study, the distribution of chronotype was similar. However, the Indian women, while having a roughly similar distribution of morning types, skewed much more heavily towards ‘definite’ rather than ‘more’ morning types than did the women in our study (Supplemental Table 1).

Table 1.

Distribution of self-reported chronotype at three different times of life among 39686 California Teacher Study (CTS) participants.

Chronotype Now (at Q5) (Age 40-80s) Age 30-40’s Teen/College Years
n % n % n %
Definite morning 15175 38 13312 34 12076 30
More morning than evening 8241 21 8593 22 5693 14
Neither morning or evening 5308 13 5798 15 6156 16
More evening than morning 5935 15 6172 16 8142 20
Definite evening 5027 13 4247 11 6121 15
Unknown - - 1564 2 1498 5

The proportion of women in our study who described themselves as morning chronotypes was slightly lower for earlier periods of life (Table 1). Overall 60% were identified as having a stable chronotype by virtue of reporting the same chronotype in their teen/college years as at the time of Q5 completion. Twenty-percent of participants reported shifting towards a morning chronotype later in life; 8% reported shifting towards an evening chronotype and 8% towards a neither chronotype (data not shown).

The distributions of breast cancer risk factors and chronotype by case status are presented in Table 2. The distributions of breast cancer risk factors were consistent with the established literature on breast cancer risk factors and consistent with those reported in previous analyses of the full CTS cohort (23). Overall the distribution of chronotype did not appear to substantially differ between cases and controls, but cases were significantly less likely to report definite morning chronotype (38% vs. 35%, p=0.002).

Table 2.

Distribution of chronotype and breast cancer risk factor characteristics, by case-control status among 39686 California Teacher Study (CTS) participants.

Characteristica Case control status All
Non-case Case
N % N % N % p value
All 36967 100 2719 100 39686 100
Chronotype 0.002
 Definite morning type 14220 38 955 35 15175 38
 More morning than evening type 7678 21 563 21 8241 21
 Neither morning/evening type 4923 13 385 14 5308 13
 More evening than morning type 5511 15 424 16 5935 15
 Definite Evening type 4635 13 392 14 5027 13
Age <0.001
 40-49 years 686 2 33 1 719 2
 50-59 years 6423 17 283 10 6706 17
 60-69 years 15562 42 969 36 16531 42
 70-79 years 9605 26 963 35 10568 27
 80-89 years 4691 13 471 17 5162 13
Race/Ethnicityb 0.049
 White 32316 87 2422 89 34738 88
 Black 810 2 55 2 865 2
 Hispanic 1525 4 82 3 1607 4
 Asian/Pacific Islander 1486 4 105 4 1591 4
 Other 830 2 55 2 885 2
Smoking pack-yearsb <0.001
 Never smokers 24731 67 1667 61 26398 67
 ≤10 6517 18 500 18 7017 18
 11-20 2156 6 189 7 2345 6
 21-30 1172 3 103 4 1275 3
 ≥31 1229 3 152 6 1381 3
 Unknown 1162 3 108 4 1270 3
BMI (kg/m2) 0.011
 15-24 17780 48 1236 45 19016 48
 25-29 10585 29 841 31 11426 29
 30-54.8 6826 18 492 18 7318 18
 Unknown 1776 5 150 6 1926 5
Alcohol consumption (g/day) b <0.001
 None 10992 30 755 28 11747 30
 <20 21744 59 1580 58 23324 59
 ≥20 2938 8 282 10 3220 8
 Unknown 1293 3 102 4 1395 4
Physical activity (hours per week) d <0.001
 0 to < 2.25 11226 30 951 35 12177 31
 2.25 to < 5.50 12430 34 915 34 13345 34
 5.50 to 24.00 13216 36 846 31 14062 35
 Unknown 95 0 7 0 102 0
Family history of breast cancerc <0.001
 No 29867 81 2009 74 31876 80
 Yes 6113 17 626 23 6739 17
 Unknown 987 3 84 3 1071 3
Age at menarche (years)b 0.307
 ≤11 8546 23 669 25 9215 23
 12-13 20978 57 1499 55 22477 57
 ≥14 7017 19 519 19 7536 19
 Unknown/Never 426 1 32 1 458 1
Age at 1st full-term pregnancy (years) b 0.650
 No full-term pregnancy 8420 23 625 23 9045 23
 ≤24 9967 27 710 26 10677 27
 25-29 11301 31 864 32 12165 31
 ≥30 6758 18 485 18 7243 18
 Unknown 521 1 35 1 556 1
Breast feeding history (months) b 0.031
 Never pregnant 6328 17 468 17 6796 17
 Pregnancy, but no live birth 2055 6 155 6 2210 6
 0 5311 14 452 17 5763 15
 >0 and <6 6148 17 461 17 6609 17
 6-11 5445 15 373 14 5818 15
 ≥12 11028 30 769 27 11797 30
 Unknown 652 2 41 2 693 2
Age at menopause (years) 0.063
 < 40 2919 8 188 7 3107 8
 40-49 9810 27 776 29 10586 27
 50-54 11903 32 831 31 12734 32
 55-59 4818 13 346 13 5164 13
 60-70 623 2 45 2 668 2
 Unknown 6894 19 533 20 7427 19
Hormone therapy (HT) usee <0.001
 Never 13010 35 825 30 13835 35
 Ever 23920 65 1891 70 25811 65
 Unknown 37 0 3 0 40 0
a

Unless otherwise noted, covariate assessment was based on the CTS Q5 survey

b

Assessment based on the CTS baseline survey

c

Assessment based on reporting family history (in a first degree relative) on any of the CTS surveys from baseline through the Q4 survey

e

Assessment based on ever reporting use of HT on any of the CTS surveys from baseline through the Q5 survey

d

Average strenuous and moderate physical activity during past three years as reported on Q5 survey

The estimated risks of breast cancer associated with chronotype are shown in Table 3a. Point estimates of risk were similar from the crude and fully-adjusted multivariable models. Compared to definite morning types, definite evening types had an increased risk of breast cancer with elevated ORs that were statistically significant in both the crude (OR = 1.24, 95% CI: 1.10 – 1.40) and fully-adjusted models (OR = 1.20, 95% CI: 1.06-1.35). None of the other chronotypes was significantly related to breast cancer risk in fully adjusted models. To address the concern that the perception of chronotype might be influenced by a woman’s diagnosis or treatment for breast cancer, we excluded all breast cancer cases who responded less than 2 years after completing the questionnaire -- as was done in the NHS II analysis (21). While modestly attenuated, results from these analyses (Table 3b) were similar to those observed among the full set of breast cancer cases.

Table 3b.

Risk of breast cancer associated with chronotype: results from logistic regression analysis among 39334 California Teacher Study participants, excluding cases who were diagnosed within two years of completing the Q5 questionnaire (n=352).

Chronotype # cases Crude Odds Ratio a 95% CI Multivariate Adjusted Odds Ratiob 95% CI
Definite morning 844 1.00 1.00
More morning than evening 492 1.09 0.97 – 1.22 1.08 0.96 – 1.21
Neither morning/evening 336 1.11 0.98 – 1.27 1.09 0.96 – 1.25
More evening than morning 359 1.09 0.96 – 1.24 1.07 0.94 – 1.22
Definite evening type 336 1.20 1.05 – 1.37 1.16 1.02 – 1.33
a

Adjusted for age

b

Adjusted for age, race, family history of breast cancer, age at menarche, smoking pack-years, BMI, alcohol consumption, physical activity, age at first full-term pregnancy, breast feeding history, age at menopause, ever use of hormone therapy.

When the data were stratified by stable versus non-stable chronotypes, estimated breast cancer risks were somewhat more robust among women who reported stable chronotypes (adjusted ORs for definite evening types=1.20, 95% CI=1.04-1.40 among stable chronotypes and 1.14, 95% CI = 0.88 – 1.47 among non-stable chronotypes) but the log-likelihood ratio test for interaction was not significant (p > 0.05). Stratification by BMI (< 25 kg/m2 vs ≥ 25 kg/m2) resulted in similar, albeit slightly higher, estimated adjusted ORs among women with high BMI than those with lower BMI (ORadj = 1.23, 95% CI: 1.04-1.46 vs ORadj = 1.18, 95% CI: 0.97-1.43) but the log-likelihood test for interaction was not significant (p > 0.05). Among the small subset of cases (n=331) whose tumors were estrogen and progesterone receptor negative (ER−/PR−), the point estimate of risk associated with evening chronotype appeared marginally higher than that for the 2162 cases with hormonally responsive tumors (ER+ or PR+) but confidence intervals were wide and overlapping (ORadj =1.36, 95% CI=0.98-1.88 for ER−/PR− cases versus ORadj=1.18, 95% CI=1.02 – 1.35 for ER+/PR+ cases).

DISCUSSION

This study found a modest, but statistically significant, elevated risk of post-menopausal breast cancer for women with a definite evening chronotype compared to those with a definite morning chronotype, after adjustment for established breast cancer risk factors. These findings add to the small body of epidemiologic literature on chronotype and breast cancer risk. Our results are consistent with those from two other studies that suggested higher breast cancer risks associated with evening chronotypes. In a nested case-control study of Danish military night shift workers, an adjusted OR of 1.8 (95% CI: 1.2 – 2.9) for breast cancer was reported for evening compared to morning chronotypes (20). Likewise, although of only borderline statistical significance, a nested-control study among the Nurses Health Study (NHS) II cohort, also reported a similar magnitude of elevated risks for definite evening compared to definite morning chronotypes (ORadjusted=1.15, 95% CI: 0.98-1.34)(21). Both of these analyses that were conducted among shift workers were adjusted for night-shift work, suggesting that evening chronotypes are intrinsically more prone to breast cancer, independent of circadian disruption. A population-based study in India (the only study other than our own to be conducted primarily among non-shift workers) reported elevated risks for both evening and morning chronotypes compared to those with a preference for neither morning nor evening (19). Unfortunately, the choice of a different referent category in the India study makes it difficult to directly compare their results to our own. Both the Danish military study and the NHS II study also reported elevated breast cancer risks for women who reported a preference for neither morning nor evening compared to morning chronotypes, with ORs of 1.6 (95% CI: 1.0 – 2.7) and 1.27 (95% CI: 1.04 – 1.56), respectively. In our study, the point estimate for the neither chronotype category was marginally elevated but was not statistically significant.

Our results, in the context of these other epidemiologic findings, suggest that evening chronotype may be a risk factor for breast cancer. These findings are further supported by a substantial body of evidence from laboratory studies that suggest potentially relevant etiologic mechanisms. Although the concept of chronotype dates back centuries (3, 14), it is only recently that we have begun to understand its genetic and molecular underpinnings (2, 3, 15). Today, chronotype is thought to be primarily a reflection of one’s genes (3, 14, 29). Circadian rhythms are regulated by a complex feedback system modulated by the transcriptional expression of at least a dozen core circadian genes, many of which act as tumor suppressors, including the circadian locomotor output cycles kaput (CLOCK), period (Per1, Per2, Per3), and cytochrome (Cry1, Cry2) genes (2, 30, 31). As described in a number of recent review articles certain polymorphisms in these core circadian genes have been shown to be both correlated with cellular and physiologic processes considered to be hallmarks of cancer (2, 3, 11, 13, 15, 32, 33) and associated with chronotype (34, 35). The etiologic relevance of chronotype to breast cancer, in particular, is further underscored by in vivo evidence that the circadian clock interacts with estrogen and androgen signaling pathways and the observation that Per1 and Per2 gene expression is suppressed more commonly in breast cancer tumors than in normal breast tissue (2, 3, 7, 15, 32).

Beyond its independent role in carcinogenesis, it has been hypothesized that chronotype may be an important susceptibility factor to consider when evaluating circadian disruption in studies of shift work (8, 36, 37). Few epidemiologic studies of shift workers have had information on chronotype and findings have been somewhat inconsistent (20, 3840). Results from the aforementioned Danish study of military shift workers, however, lends credence to this hypothesis. Specifically, the Danish study reported breast cancer risks associated with long-term night shift work that was considerably stronger among morning than among evening chronotypes (20). Presumably, this is a reflection of a greater propensity for circadian disruption among morning chronotypes working at night – a time that is out-of-sync with their intrinsic circadian rhythms. While in our study we did not have information about shift work, as a study of active and retired teachers, the participants in our study are unlikely to have engaged in night shift work. However, among our study population who typically need to report to their school jobs early in the morning, it may be the evening chronotypes who are especially vulnerable to circadian disruption.

This study has a number of strengths worth noting. Other than the small hospital-based case-control study conducted in India, our study is the only study to date conducted among women not employed in night shift work. The large size of our study population provided sufficient numbers of subjects to evaluate five gradations of chronotype to capture the extremes and reduce the potential for misclassification. While reliance on a single question to characterize chronotype may have introduced some misclassification, this single question has been shown to robustly correlate (r=0.89) with chronotype based on the full MEQ (27). Furthermore, the nesting of our study within a well-defined cohort with excellent follow-up provides reassurance that cases and controls were drawn from a common base population with equal opportunity to be identified as a case.

As a retrospective case-control study, however, our study has a few limitations worth noting. Chronotype was ascertained by self-report after case diagnosis. If the onset of breast cancer altered chronotype, or the perception of chronotype, our results could be biased by differential recall between cases and controls. However, chronotype is not widely recognized as a risk factor for breast cancer so it seems unlikely that cases would differentially recall their chronotype based on perceived risks associated with their disease. The similarity of our results based on reported chronotype during earlier periods of life, and risks that persisted in both those who reported stable and non-stable chronotypes during the life course, offer support to the validity of our findings. Additionally, when we excluded from our analyses cases that were diagnosed within two years of completing the Q5 survey, results remained essentially unchanged.

Another concern inherent to the case-control study design is selection bias. The overall Q5 survey participation rate was reasonable (approximately 60%) and nearly all responders (96%) provided valid chronotype data. Although Q5 responders were slightly less likely to be in the very youngest age group at baseline and slightly more likely to be non-Hispanic white than non-responders, the sociodemographic profiles of Q5 responders and non-responders were generally similar (data not shown). Thus, our results are likely to be generalizable to the full CTS cohort.

Survival bias, however, must also be considered when interpreting results from retrospective case-control cancer studies such as this one. It is difficult to ascertain the degree to which survival bias may have impacted our results. A modestly higher proportion of cases (43%) than controls (31%) were excluded from our analyses because they were deceased at the time of the Q5 survey. Among the breast cancer cases diagnosed since baseline, an evaluation of the distribution of summary stage at diagnosis indicated that compared to those not included in our study, the cases that were included in our study were less likely to be diagnosed at a remote late stage (0.8% versus 3.4%). These observations suggest that the cases in our study may disproportionately represent cases diagnosed at an earlier stage and with better survival and thus may not be fully representative of all breast cancers that occurred in the CTS since baseline. If chronotype is related to survival among cases, this could lead to biased estimates of risk. The degree to which chronotype might influence survival of breast cancer patients is not known. To our knowledge, the only breast cancer study that has addressed this issue reported slower breast cancer progression among morning chronotypes and no difference in progression for evening chronotypes when compared to neither chronotypes (12). Without knowing if chronotype similarly affects survival among controls, it is not possible to ascertain the degree and direction of any possible survival bias. There is however, emerging evidence that among the general population morning chronotypes may also have lower rates of overall mortality compared to evening chronotypes (41).

Overall, our results suggest that chronotype may be an independent risk factor for post-menopausal breast cancer. Our findings are notable in that they were observed among women who are unlikely to have engaged in night shift work. The degree to which our findings can be extended to other women, including premenopausal women, is not known. Studies in other populations of non-shift workers are warranted. While beyond the scope of the current analysis, a greater understanding of the mechanisms by which chronotype affects breast cancer risk could ultimately yield important etiologic clues leading to new strategies for prevention and treatment.

Supplementary Material

Supplementary Table 1

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

The authors would like to thank the California Teachers Study Steering Committee that is responsible for the formation and maintenance of the Study within which this research was conducted. A full list of California Teachers Study team members is available at https://www.calteachersstudy.org/team. We also express our appreciation to all the participants in the California Teachers Study and to the phlebotomists, the researchers, analysts and staff who have contributed to the success of this research, including Christine Duffy and Jane Sullivan-Halley.

DECLARATION OF INTEREST STATEMENT

None of the authors has any financial conflicts of interest. The California Teachers Study and the research reported in this publication were supported by the National Cancer Institute of the National Institutes of Health under award number U01-CA199277; P30-CA033572; P30-CA023100; UM1-CA164917; R01-CA077398; and R01 CA207020. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Cancer Institute or the National Institutes of Health.

The collection of cancer incidence data used in the California Teachers Study was supported by the California Department of Public Health pursuant to California Health and Safety Code Section 103885; Centers for Disease Control and Prevention’s National Program of Cancer Registries, under cooperative agreement 5NU58DP006344; the National Cancer Institute’s Surveillance, Epidemiology and End Results Program under contract HHSN261201800032I awarded to the University of California, San Francisco, contract HHSN261201800015I awarded to the University of Southern California, and contract HHSN261201800009I awarded to the Public Health Institute. The opinions, findings, and conclusions expressed herein are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official views of the State of California, Department of Public Health, the National Cancer Institute, the National Institutes of Health, the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention or their Contractors and Subcontractors, or the Regents of the University of California, or any of its programs.

Footnotes

DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT

All of the data associated with this publication and in the California Teachers Study are available for research use. The California Teachers Study welcomes all such inquiries and encourages individuals to visit https://www.calteachersstudy.org/for-researchers.

REFERENCES

  • 1.Haus EL, Smolensky MH. Shift work and cancer risk: potential mechanistic roles of circadian disruption, light at night, and sleep deprivation. Sleep Med Rev. 2013;17(4):273–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Fu L, Lee CC. The circadian clock: pacemaker and tumour suppressor. Nat Rev Cancer. 2003;3(5):350–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Gery S, Koeffler HP. Circadian rhythms and cancer. Cell Cycle. 2010;9(6):1097–103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Kanerva N, Kronholm E, Partonen T, Ovaskainen ML, Kaartinen NE, Konttinen H, Broms U, Mannisto S. Tendency toward eveningness is associated with unhealthy dietary habits. Chronobiol Int. 2012;29(7):920–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Kantermann T, Theadom A, Roenneberg T, Cropley M. Fibromyalgia syndrome and chronotype: late chronotypes are more affected. J Biol Rhythms. 2012;27(2):176–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Kitamura S, Hida A, Watanabe M, Enomoto M, Aritake-Okada S, Moriguchi Y, Kamei Y, Mishima K. Evening preference is related to the incidence of depressive states independent of sleep-wake conditions. Chronobiol Int. 2010;27(9-10):1797–812. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Paine SJ, Gander PH, Travier N. The epidemiology of morningness/eveningness: influence of age, gender, ethnicity, and socioeconomic factors in adults (30-49 years). J Biol Rhythms. 2006;21(1):68–76. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Erren TC. Shift work and cancer research: can chronotype predict susceptibility in night-shift and rotating-shift workers? Occup Environ Med. 2013;70(4):283–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Blask DE. Melatonin, sleep disturbance and cancer risk. Sleep Med Rev. 2009;13(4):257–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Blask DE, Dauchy RT, Sauer LA. Putting cancer to sleep at night: the neuroendocrine/circadian melatonin signal. Endocrine. 2005;27(2):179–88. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Greene MW. Circadian rhythms and tumor growth. Cancer Lett. 2012;318(2):115–23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Hahm BJ, Jo B, Dhabhar FS, Palesh O, Aldridge-Gerry A, Bajestan SN, Neri E, Nouriani B, Spiegel D, Zeitzer JM. Bedtime misalignment and progression of breast cancer. Chronobiol Int. 2014;31(2):214–21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Lahti T, Merikanto I, Partonen T. Circadian clock disruptions and the risk of cancer. Ann Med. 2012;44(8):847–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Roenneberg T, Kuehnle T, Juda M, Kantermann T, Allebrandt K, Gordijn M, Merrow M. Epidemiology of the human circadian clock. Sleep Med Rev. 2007;11(6):429–38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Savvidis C, Koutsilieris M. Circadian rhythm disruption in cancer biology. Mol Med. 2012;18:1249–60. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Stevens RG. Circadian disruption and breast cancer: from melatonin to clock genes. Epidemiology. 2005;16(2):254–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Stevens RG. Working against our endogenous circadian clock: Breast cancer and electric lighting in the modern world. Mutat Res. 2009;679(1-2):6–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.International Agency for Research on Cancer. IARC Monographs on the Evaluation of Carcinogenic Risks to Humans Painting, Fire-fighting, and Shiftwork. Lyon: International Agency for Research on Cancer; 2010. p. 563–764. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Wirth MD, Burch JB, Hebert JR, Kowtal P, Mehrotra-Kapoor A, Steck SE, Hurley TG, Gupta PC, Pednekar MS, Youngstedt SD, et al. Case-control study of breast cancer in India: Role of PERIOD3 clock gene length polymorphism and chronotype. Cancer Invest. 2014;32(7):321–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Hansen J, Lassen CF. Nested case-control study of night shift work and breast cancer risk among women in the Danish military. Occup Environ Med. 2012;69(8):551–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Ramin C, Devore EE, Pierre-Paul J, Duffy JF, Hankinson SE, Schernhammer ES. Chronotype and breast cancer risk in a cohort of US nurses. Chronobiol Int. 2013;30(9):1181–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.California Cancer Registry. [Available from: http://www.ccrcal.org/abouttheccr.html.
  • 23.Bernstein L, Allen M, Anton-Culver H, Deapen D, Horn-Ross PL, Peel D, Pinder R, Reynolds P, Sullivan-Halley J, West D, et al. High breast cancer incidence rates among California teachers: results from the California Teachers Study (United States). Cancer Causes Control. 2002;13(7):625–35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Horne JA, Ostberg O. A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. Int J Chronobiol. 1976;4(2):97–110. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Chelminski I, Petros TV, Plaud JJ, Ferraro R. Psychometric properties of the reduced Horne and Ostberg questionniare. Personality and Individual Differences. 2000;29:469–78. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Duffy JF, Rimmer DW, Czeisler CA. Association of intrinsic circadian period with morningness-eveningness, usual wake time, and circadian phase. Behav Neurosci. 2001;115(4):895–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Adan A, Almirall H. Horne and Ostberg morningness eveningness questionnaire - a reduced scale. Personality and Individual Differences. 1991;12:241–53. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.SAS Version 9.4. Cary, NC, USA: SAS Institute Inc. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Kelleher FC, Rao A, Maguire A. Circadian molecular clocks and cancer. Cancer Lett. 2014;342(1):9–18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Chen ST, Choo KB, Hou MF, Yeh KT, Kuo SJ, Chang JG. Deregulated expression of the PER1, PER2 and PER3 genes in breast cancers. Carcinogenesis. 2005;26(7):1241–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Dai H, Zhang L, Cao M, Song F, Zheng H, Zhu X, Wei Q, Zhang W, Chen K. The role of polymorphisms in circadian pathway genes in breast tumorigenesis. Breast Cancer Res Treat. 2011;127(2):531–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Samuelsson LB, Bovbjerg DH, Roecklein KA, Hall MH. Sleep and circadian disruption and incident breast cancer risk: An evidence-based and theoretical review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2018;84:35–48. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Wood PA, Yang X, Hrushesky WJ. Clock genes and cancer. Integr Cancer Ther. 2009;8(4):303–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Archer SN, Robilliard DL, Skene DJ, Smits M, Williams A, Arendt J, von Schantz M. A length polymorphism in the circadian clock gene Per3 is linked to delayed sleep phase syndrome and extreme diurnal preference. Sleep. 2003;26(4):413–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Lazar AS, Slak A, Lo JC, Santhi N, von Schantz M, Archer SN, Groeger JA, Dijk DJ. Sleep, diurnal preference, health, and psychological well-being: a prospective single-allelic-variation study. Chronobiol Int. 2012;29(2):131–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Erren TC, Morfeld P, Stork J, Knauth P, von Mulmann MJ, Breitstadt R, Muller U, Emmerich M, Piekarski C. Shift work, chronodisruption and cancer?--The IARC 2007 challenge for research and prevention and 10 theses from the Cologne Colloquium 2008. Scand J Work Environ Health. 2009;35(1):74–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Saksvik IB, Bjorvatn B, Hetland H, Sandal GM, Pallesen S. Individual differences in tolerance to shift work--a systematic review. Sleep Med Rev. 2011;15(4):221–35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Fritschi L, Erren TC, Glass DC, Girschik J, Thomson AK, Saunders C, Boyle T, El-Zaemey S, Rogers P, Peters S, et al. The association between different night shiftwork factors and breast cancer: a case-control study. Br J Cancer. 2013;109(9):2472–80. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Fritschi L, Valerie Gross J, Wild U, Heyworth JS, Glass DC, Erren TC. Shift work that involves circadian disruption and breast cancer: a first application of chronobiological theory and the consequent challenges. Occup Environ Med. 2018;75(3):231–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Papantoniou K, Castano-Vinyals G, Espinosa A, Aragones N, Perez-Gomez B, Ardanaz E, Altzibar JM, Sanchez VM, Gomez-Acebo I, Llorca J, et al. Breast cancer risk and night shift work in a case-control study in a Spanish population. Eur J Epidemiol. 2016;31(9):867–78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Knutson KL, von Schantz M. Associations between chronotype, morbidity and mortality in the UK Biobank cohort. Chronobiol Int. 2018;35(8):1045–53. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Table 1

RESOURCES