Skip to main content
BMC Pediatrics logoLink to BMC Pediatrics
. 2019 Oct 29;19:390. doi: 10.1186/s12887-019-1719-9

Prenatal pet keeping and caregiver-reported attention deficit hyperactivity disorder through preadolescence in a United States birth cohort

Andrea E Cassidy-Bushrow 1,2,, Alexandra R Sitarik 1, Tisa M Johnson-Hooper 3,4, Jannel M Phillips 4,5, Kyra Jones 1, Christine Cole Johnson 1,2, Jennifer K Straughen 1,2
PMCID: PMC6819335  PMID: 31660906

Abstract

Background

While the keeping of pets has been shown to protect against childhood allergic disease and obesity, less is known regarding potential associations of prenatal pet keeping and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). We sought to examine the associations between prenatal dog or cat keeping with caregiver-reported ADHD in preadolescents in the Wayne County Health, Environment, Allergy and Asthma Longitudinal Study (WHEALS) birth cohort (N = 1258).

Methods

At an interview with the caregiver at child age 10–12 years, caregivers reported if the WHEALS child had ever been diagnosed with ADHD. Similarly, during an interview with the mother prenatally, pet keeping (defined as dog or cat kept inside ≥1 h/day) was ascertained. Logistic regression models were fit to examine the association of prenatal pet keeping (dog keeping and cat keeping, separately) with ADHD.

Results

A subset of 627 children were included in the analyses: 93 who had ADHD and 534 with neurotypical development. After accounting for confounders and loss to follow-up, maternal prenatal dog exposure was associated with 2.23 times (95% CI: 1.15, 4.31; p = 0.017) greater odds of ADHD among boys. Prenatal dog keeping was not statistically significantly associated with ADHD in girls (odds ratio = 0.27, 95% CI: 0.06, 1.12; p = 0.070). Prenatal cat keeping was not associated with ADHD.

Conclusions

In boys, but not girls, maternal prenatal dog keeping was positively associated with ADHD. Further study to confirm these findings and to identify potential mechanisms of this association (e.g., modification of the gut microbiome, exposure to environmental toxicants or pet-related medications) is needed.

Keywords: Pet keeping, Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, Prenatal, Birth cohort

Background

The keeping of pets has been shown to have a variety of both positive and negative health effects [18]. Children exposed to pets during early life are less likely to develop allergic diseases as well as obesity [913]. However, little is known regarding the association between pet keeping and neurodevelopmental disorders, including attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), with limited published studies showing positive and null associations between pet keeping and neurodevelopmental disorders or measures of related symptoms [1416].

Pets may influence neurodevelopment via several pathways. Pets are associated with changes in the gut microbiome of members of their household [17, 18]. The gut microbiome may influence risk of ADHD via the gut-brain axis [1921]. Additionally, pets may introduce environmental toxicants (e.g., pesticides) into the home [22], and pesticide exposure may be a risk factor for ADHD [23]. Finally, attachment to a pet may impact emotional development [24]; emotional dysregulation is a feature of ADHD [25].

ADHD is a complex disorder hallmarked by hyperactivity, inattention and impulsivity at a developmentally inappropriate level [26]. In the United States, ~ 9.0% of children are affected by ADHD [27]. While there is a large genetic component to ADHD (estimated heritability > 70%) [28], environmental factors also impact the risk of ADHD [29]. Given that ADHD results in a sizeable economic burden [30] and negative impact on quality of life [31], there is a need for further study of potential environmental risk factors, particularly those in early life [32].

The goal of this study was to examine if prenatal pet keeping was associated with ADHD in preadolescence (ages ~ 10–12 years). To achieve this goal, we leveraged data from the racially and socioeconomically diverse Wayne County Health, Environment, Allergy and Asthma Longitudinal Study (WHEALS) birth cohort (Detroit, Michigan) [11, 33, 34].

Methods

Study population

WHEALS recruited pregnant women with due dates from September 2003 through December 2007, and who were seeing a practitioner at 1 of 5 clinics in the Henry Ford Health System to establish a birth cohort [11, 34]. All women were in their second trimester or later, were aged 21–49 years, and were living in a predefined geographic area in western Wayne County that included the western portion of the city of Detroit as well as the suburban areas immediately surrounding the city. Mothers were interviewed in the clinic prenatally and postpartum interviewer-administered questionnaires were completed at child age 1, 6, 12, and 24 months. Children and their parent/guardian were invited to return for a clinic visit at child age 2 years and again at child age 10–12 years for assessment of child health. All participants provided written, informed consent and study protocols were approved by the Henry Ford Health System Institutional Review Board; at the age 10–12 year visit, children provided written, informed assent.

Definition of ADHD, other neurodevelopmental disorders, and Neurotypical development

At the age 10–12 year visit, the caregiver (95% the mother) reported if the child had ever been diagnosed with ADHD. The caregiver also reported if the child had ever been diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder (ASD), Asperger’s syndrome, or sensory processing disorder (SPD); Asperger’s syndrome was asked separately since children may have been diagnosed prior to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5). A report of “suspect” diagnosis was also considered positive. Children were considered neurotypical (NT) if they did not have a caregiver-reported ASD, Asperger’s, ADHD, or SPD diagnosis. Children without an ADHD diagnosis but who had a caregiver-reported ASD, Asperger’s or SPD diagnosis were excluded from the analysis, given the small sample size with these conditions.

A subset of 325 WHEALS children had their medical record abstracted for additional health information, including ADHD diagnosis. Caregiver report of ADHD diagnosis was validated within this subset using the kappa (κ) statistic to evaluate agreement. Landis and Koch [35] criteria were used to evaluate strength of agreement. There was near perfect agreement between caregiver-reported ADHD and the medical record (κ = 0.84, 95% CI 0.78, 0.91). Given that only a subset of children had medical record review and the high level of agreement between caregiver report and the medical record, we utilized caregiver-reported ADHD in statistical analysis.

Prenatal pet keeping

During the prenatal maternal interview, the mother was asked about presence of pets in the home, the number of hours they spent indoors, and the type of pet (e.g., dog or cat). Some studies have attributed health benefits primarily to dog ownership [2, 36, 37]; thus, we defined prenatal dog keeping and prenatal cat keeping seperately, as report of having a dog or cat, respectively, indoors at least 1 h per day.

Covariates

The mother self-reported race, date of birth, marital status, household income, education, parity, smoking history and exposure to environmental tobacco smoke at the prenatal interview. Maternal prenatal care records were abstracted to obtain antibiotic and antifungal use [38]. Height and weight were also abstracted and body mass index (BMI) at first prenatal care visit (mean gestational age at measure 9.1 ± 4.9 weeks; 83% taken during the first trimester) was calculated. Although first measured BMI in pregnancy and self-reported prepregnancy BMI have been shown to be highly correlated in other studies [39], the first measured BMI during pregnancy represents both prepregnancy body size and early pregnancy-related weight gain. Delivery records for WHEALS women were abstracted to obtain delivery mode, birth weight, and gestational age at delivery. Low birth weight was defined as a birthweight < 2500 g and preterm delivery as gestational age at delivery < 37 weeks.

Statistical analysis

Differences in basic characteristics by ADHD vs. NT development were first compared using standard parametric statistical methods, including chi-square tests and analysis of variance. Prenatal pet exposure (dogs and cats, separately) was then associated with ADHD using logistic regression to obtain odds ratios (OR) and 95% CIs. Given the growing and consistent evidence that male fetuses are more vulnerable than female fetuses to exposures during gestation [40], sex-specific effects of pets on ADHD was a priori hypothesized and formally tested using interaction terms in logistic regression models. When the interaction term was statistically significant, sex-specific models were fit to aid in interpretation.

Not all WHEALS children/caregiver pairs completed a 10–12 year questionnaire (a comparison of those with and without 10–12 year questionnaire data is presented in Table 1). Inverse probability weights (IPW) [4143] were used to account for loss to follow-up by age 10–12 years, defined by completion of the 10–12-year questionnaire. Weights were calculated as the inverse of the “treatment” received. In other words, if p = probability of follow-up, then w = 1/p for children with follow-up, and w = 1/(1-p) for children without follow-up. Briefly, IPWs up weight the importance of children who were followed through age 10–12 years but were not very likely to have been followed based on baseline characteristics (e.g., children of mothers who smoked during pregnancy). Similarly, it down weights the importance of children who were followed through age 10–12 years but were very likely to have been followed (e.g., children of mothers with a high level of education). The following factors were included in the calculation of the IPWs: maternal race, insurance, household income, maternal education, firstborn child, maternal smoking during pregnancy, prenatal environmental tobacco smoke exposure, urban residence, marital status, mode of delivery, prenatal alcohol use, child sex, prenatal dogs, prenatal cats, maternal history of allergies and asthma, maternal age at delivery, gestational age at delivery, birthweight, and completion of the 2-year clinic visit. Balance in these covariates was assessed using standardized differences before and after weighting, with imbalance defined as absolute value > 0.20. In addition to weighting the models to account for loss to follow-up, models were additionally adjusted for potentially confounding covariates. Confounders were selected based on having a significant association with either prenatal pet keeping or ADHD. These adjustment covariates included maternal race, household income, maternal smoking during pregnancy, prenatal antifungal use, child sex, maternal BMI first measured in pregnancy, gestational age at delivery, and birthweight.

Table 1.

Differences in maternal and child characteristics in Wayne County Health, Environment, Allergy and Asthma Longitudinal Study participants with and without the 10–12 year questionnaire data, before and after inverse probability weighting (IPW)

Covariate Completed the 10–12-Year Questionnaire Before IPW After IPW
No
N = 613
Yes
N = 645
Pa Db Pa Db
Maternal Characteristic Mean ± SD or N (%)
 Age at delivery (years) 28.9 ± 5.1 30.2 ± 5.3 < 0.001 0.25 0.642 0.02
 Race White 133 (21.7%) 157 (24.3%) 0.570 0.10 0.962 0.03
Black 387 (63.1%) 391 (60.6%)
Hispanic 40 (6.5%) 38 (5.9%)
Arabic 31 (5.1%) 28 (4.3%)
Mixed/Other 22 (3.6%) 31 (4.8%)
 Marital status Unmarried 273 (44.5%) 212 (32.9%) < 0.001 0.24 0.335 0.04
Married 340 (55.5%) 433 (67.1%)
 Insurance coverage HAP 169 (27.6%) 333 (51.6%) < 0.001 0.71 0.815 0.04
Other insurance 198 (32.3%) 233 (36.1%)
No insurance 9 (1.5%) 6 (0.9%)
Refused/do not know/missing 237 (38.7%) 73 (11.3%)
 Household income <$20,000 112 (18.3%) 70 (10.9%) < 0.001 0.32 0.938 0.05
$20,000- < $40,000 150 (24.5%) 145 (22.5%)
$40,000- < $80,000 173 (28.2%) 174 (27.0%)
$80,000- < $100,000 49 (8.0%) 86 (13.3%)
≥$100,000 52 (8.5%) 96 (14.9%)
Refused to answer 77 (12.6%) 74 (11.5%)
 Education <HS diploma 50 (8.2%) 24 (3.7%) < 0.001 0.41 0.967 0.02
HS diploma 131 (21.4%) 97 (15.0%)
Some college 313 (51.1%) 292 (45.3%)
≥Bachelor’s degree 119 (19.4%) 232 (36.0%)
 Mother smoked during pregnancy No 517 (84.3%) 591 (91.6%) < 0.001 − 0.23 0.420 − 0.03
Yes 96 (15.7%) 54 (8.4%)
 ETS during pregnancy No 421 (68.7%) 490 (76%) 0.004 −0.16 0.666 −0.02
Yes 192 (31.3%) 155 (24%)
 Prenatal alcohol use No 590 (96.6%) 616 (95.7%) 0.405 0.05 0.330 −0.04
Yes 21 (3.4%) 28 (4.3%)
 Location of residence Suburban 250 (40.8%) 305 (47.3%) 0.020 −0.13 0.224 −0.05
Urban 363 (59.2%) 340 (52.7%)
 Doctor-diagnosed hay fever or allergic rhinitis No 516 (85.0%) 538 (84.9%) 0.941 0.001 0.613 0.02
Yes 91 (15.0%) 96 (15.1%)
 Doctor-diagnosed asthma No 492 (80.3%) 513 (79.7%) 0.790 0.01 0.584 0.02
Yes 121 (19.7%) 131 (20.3%)
 Prenatal indoor dog(s) No 476 (77.7%) 479 (74.3%) 0.160 0.08 0.797 −0.01
Yes 137 (22.3%) 166 (25.7%)
 Prenatal indoor cat(s) No 520 (84.8%) 535 (82.9%) 0.364 0.05 0.538 0.02
Yes 93 (15.2%) 110 (17.1%)
Clinical Characteristics
 Mode of delivery Vaginal 385 (63.4%) 399 (62.1%) 0.616 0.03 0.513 −0.03
C-section 222 (36.6%) 244 (37.9%)
 Firstborn No 401 (65.4%) 397 (61.6%) 0.155 0.08 0.986 −0.001
Yes 212 (34.6%) 248 (38.4%)
 Child sex Male 308 (50.3%) 314 (48.7%) 0.560 0.03 0.557 0.02
Female 304 (49.7%) 331 (51.3%)
 Gestational age at delivery (weeks)c

38.7 ± 1.8

(N = 595)

38.8 ± 1.7

(N = 637)

0.307 0.05 0.902 0.004
 Birthweight (grams)c

3258 ± 548

(N = 567)

3346 ± 594

(N = 615)

0.008 0.15 0.880 −0.006
 Completed 2-year clinic visit No 425 (69.3%) 137 (21.2%) < 0.001 1.10 0.596 0.02
Yes 188 (30.7%) 508 (78.8%)

ETS environmental tobacco smoke, HAP Health Alliance Plan, HS high school, SD standard deviation

aCalculated using chi-square test for categorical covariates and analysis of variance for continuous covariates. Bold values indicate statistically significant P<0.05

bStandardized difference (difference in means or proportions divided by standard error); imbalance defined as absolute value> 0.20

cContinuous covariates with missing data, sample size is presented

If complete case analysis were used in the adjusted models, approximately 25% of the data would be excluded. To better handle this missingness, multiple imputation was used; all adjustment covariates as well as exposures (prenatal dogs/cats) and outcome (ADHD) were used to calculate 25 total imputed datasets. Each imputed dataset was modeled using logistic regression (with IPWs normalized to sum to the actual sample size as well as confounders included); final effect estimates were obtained by pooling results. All analyses were performed using SAS version 9.4 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC) and R version 3.4.1.

Results

Of the original 1258 WHEALS children, 645 caregivers completed a 10–12 year questionnaire. Of these, 2 did not complete the neurodevelopmental disorder section of the questionnaire. Children with ASD/Asperger’s (n = 6), SPD (n = 7), or both (n = 3) without an ADHD diagnosis were excluded. Therefore, our final analytic sample consisted of 627 children: 93 with an ADHD diagnosis and 534 NT children. Differences in those who did and did not complete the 10–12 year questionnaire data are presented in Table 1. Briefly, mothers who completed the questionnaire were older and were more likely to be married, have higher household incomes, have higher levels of education, and were more likely to live in the suburbs (all p < 0.05). Additionally, they were less likely to smoke prenatally and be exposed to environmental tobacco smoke prenatally; the children of these mothers were also heavier at birth and were more likely to complete the 2-year clinic visit (all p < 0.05). However, though the standardized differences (D) of these effects were often large, inverse probability weighting adequately removed these imbalances (Table 1; absolute value of all D < 0.05 after IPW).

Table 2 presents descriptive characteristics by ADHD vs NT. As expected, boys were more likely to have ADHD than girls (p < 0.001). Children with ADHD were more likely to have mothers that used antifungals prenatally (p = 0.004) and who had a higher BMI recorded at the first prenatal visit (p = 0.002). Additionally, children with ADHD had an earlier mean gestational age at delivery (38.3 ± 2.2 weeks compared to 38.8 ± 1.6 weeks; p = 0.007) and were more likely to have been born preterm than NT children (p = 0.018).

Table 2.

Descriptive characteristics of Wayne County Health, Environment, Allergy and Asthma Longitudinal Study children with caregiver-reported attention deficit hyperactive disorder (ADHD) or neurotypical (NT) development

Covariate NT
(N = 534)
ADHD
(N = 93)
P a
Maternal Characteristic Mean ± SD or N (%)
 Age at delivery (years) 30.1 ± 5.2 30.0 ± 5.3 0.814
 Race 0.980
  White 127 (23.8%) 23 (24.7%)
  Black 325 (60.9%) 56 (60.2%)
  Mixed/Other 82 (15.4%) 14 (15.1%)
 Marital status 0.634
  Unmarried 176 (33.0%) 33 (35.5%)
  Married 358 (67.0%) 60 (64.5%)
 Household income 0.208
  < $20,000 53 (9.9%) 14 (15.1%)
  $20,000- < $40,000 118 (22.1%) 25 (26.9%)
  $40,000- < $80,000 142 (26.6%) 27 (29.0%)
  $80,000- < $100,000 75 (14.0%) 10 (10.8%)
  ≥ $100,000 83 (15.5%) 7 (7.5%)
  Refused to answer 63 (11.8%) 10 (10.8%)
 Education 0.097
  < High school diploma 20 (3.7%) 2 (2.2%)
  High school diploma 81 (15.2%) 14 (15.1%)
  Some college 235 (44.0%) 53 (57.0%)
  ≥ Bachelor’s degree 198 (37.1%) 24 (25.8%)
 Smoked during pregnancy 0.600
  No 491 (91.9%) 84 (90.3%)
  Yes 43 (8.1%) 9 (9.7%)
 Prenatal ETS exposure 0.303
  No 411 (77.0%) 67 (72.0%)
  Yes 123 (23.0%) 26 (28.0%)
 Prenatal antibiotic use 0.774
  No 192 (45.2%) 38 (46.9%)
  Yes 233 (54.8%) 43 (53.1%)
 Prenatal antifungal use 0.004
  No 356 (83.8%) 57 (70.4%)
  Yes 69 (16.2%) 24 (29.6%)
 BMI from first prenatal visit (kg/m2)b

30.3 ± 7.7

(N = 495)

33.1 ± 9.6

(N = 87)

0.002
 Prenatal indoor dog(s) 0.308
  No 400 (74.9%) 65 (69.9%)
  Yes 134 (25.1%) 28 (30.1%)
 Prenatal indoor cat(s) 0.863
  No 444 (83.1%) 78 (83.9%)
  Yes 90 (16.9%) 15 (16.1%)
Child Characteristic
 Mode of delivery 0.513
  Vaginal 332 (62.3%) 54 (58.7%)
  C-section 201 (37.7%) 38 (41.3%)
 Firstborn 0.343
  No 332 (62.2%) 53 (57.0%)
  Yes 202 (37.8%) 40 (43.0%)
 Gestational age at delivery (weeks)b

38.8 ± 1.6

(N = 529)

38.3 ± 2.2

(N = 91)

0.007
 Preterm birth 0.018
  No 492 (93.0%) 78 (85.7%)
  Yes 37 (7.0%) 13 (14.3%)
 Birthweight (g)b

3352 ± 573

(N = 514)

3300 ± 680

(N = 84)

0.453
 Low birth weight 0.229
  No 483 (94.0%) 76 (90.5%)
  Yes 31 (6.0%) 8 (9.5%)
 Child sex < 0.001
  Male 237 (44.4%) 69 (74.2%)
  Female 297 (55.6%) 24 (25.8%)
 Child age at 10–12-year visit (years) 10.3 ± 0.9 10.3 ± 1.0 0.646

BMI body mass index, ETS environmental tobacco smoke, SD standard deviation

aCalculated using chi-square test for categorical covariates and analysis of variance for continuous covariates. Bold values indicate statistically significant P<0.05

bContinuous covariates with missing data, sample size is presented

In unadjusted and unweighted analyses, children with ADHD were more likely to have had a mother exposed to dogs prenatally (30.1%) compared to children with NT development (25.1%), although this was not statistically significant (p = 0.308). There was no statistically significant difference in prenatal exposure to cats in children with ADHD compared to NT children (16.1% vs. 16.9%, respectively; p = 0.863).

After adjustment for loss to follow-up using IPW, the association between prenatal exposure to dogs and ADHD remained nonsignificant (p = 0.124), though the direction of association was the same (OR = 1.47, 95% CI: 0.90, 2.41; Table 3, Model 1). Results were similar after additional adjustment for potential confounding factors (Table 3, Model 2). Further, there was no statistically significant association between prenatal cat exposure and ADHD (Table 3; all p > 0.543).

Table 3.

Association between prenatal indoor pet keeping and caregiver-reported attention deficit hyperactivity disorder

Model 1a Model 2b
OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI)
P P
Prenatal indoor dog(s) 1.47 (0.90, 2.41) 1.35 (0.77, 2.36)
0.124 0.300
Prenatal indoor cat(s) 1.13 (0.64, 2.01) 1.23 (0.63, 2.39)
0.666 0.543

OR odds ratio

aModel 1: inverse probability weights (IPW)

bModel 2: IPW + confounders, pooled estimate from multiple imputation model

Confounders: maternal race, household income, maternal smoking during pregnancy, prenatal antifungal use, child sex, maternal body mass index first measured in pregnancy, gestational age at birth, and birthweight

When examining potential sex-specific effects of prenatal exposure to dogs on ADHD (Fig. 1), a significant interaction was found (Model 2; p = 0.007). Specifically, maternal prenatal exposure to dogs was statistically significantly associated with increased odds of ADHD among males (OR = 2.23, 95% CI: 1.15, 4.31, p = 0.017), but not females (OR = 0.27, 95% CI: 0.06, 1.12, p = 0.070). Conversely, the effect of cats on ADHD was not significantly modified by sex (Model 2; interaction p = 0.909; Fig. 1).

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Sex-specific association between prenatal indoor pets and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Interaction P-value is for the child sex by pet keeping interaction term; sex-specific P-value is from models stratified by sex. Model 1 accounts for inverse probability weights (IPW); Model 2 accounts for IPW and confounders (pooled estimate from multiple imputation model). Confounders: maternal race, household income, maternal smoking during pregnancy, prenatal antifungal use, maternal body mass index first measured in pregnancy, gestational age at birth, and birthweight. Abbreviations: OR, odds ratio

Discussion

In the current study, prenatal exposure to dogs, but not cats, was associated with development of ADHD in male offspring. Nearly 3 times as many males as females are diagnosed with ADHD [44, 45]. Male fetuses appear to be more susceptible to negative effects of prenatal environmental exposures [40] and environmental exposures in early life not only influence the gut microbiome but also have sex-specific effects on the immune system [46]. Our findings that the association between prenatal dog keeping and ADHD is sex-specific suggest that future studies examining pets and ADHD should a priori consider evaluation of potential sex-specific effects.

Little is known about the keeping of pets during the prenatal period on risk of ADHD, although studies looking at childhood pet exposure with ADHD symptoms and/or diagnosis have found similar results. In the GINIPlus and LISAPlus German birth cohorts, ever owning a pet between birth and age 10 years was associated with higher scores for emotional symptoms and hyperactivity/impulsivity on the strengths and difficulties questionnaire at child age 10 years [14]. Similarly, in the 2003 California Health Interview Survey, currently allowing either a dog or cat into the house were positively associated with ADHD; however, this was slightly attenuated and no longer statistically significant after doubly robust adjustment for confounders (models adjusted for propensity score variables while also weighing the regression model for the propensity score weights) [15]. Our approach to simultaneously account for nonresponse bias and potential confounding factors, while a strength of the current study, does not preclude the possibility that unmeasured confounding factors contribute to our findings. Neither the Casas, Tiesler [14] or the Miles, Parast [15] study, however, examined potential sex-specific effects of pet keeping on the strengths and difficulties questionnaire or ADHD, making us unable to compare our sex-specific findings to previous studies.

We and others have shown that pets alter the microbiome of house dust [4749]. We have also shown that prenatal exposure to pets alters the gut microbiome of neonates at approximately 1 month of age [17]. Initial colonizers of the gut play an essential role in establishment and maturation of the gut microbiome, which reaches adult stage at approximately 3–5 years of age [50]; differences in early colonizers could alter the trajectory of the gut microbiome and influence both current and future health. A small but growing body of literature suggests that the gut microbiome may influence the risk of ADHD [19, 20]. In a cross-sectional study of treatment naïve children in China, children with ADHD had lower levels of Faecalibacterium, Dialister and Sutterella than healthy controls, and lower levels of Faecalibacterium were associated with increased ADHD symptom severity [20]. Consistent with these findings, data from the WHEALS cohort showed that the keeping of indoor pets was associated with lower abundance of Faecalibacterium in neonatal stool at age ~ 1 month [17]. Mechanistically, the human gut microbiome is capable of producing a vast range of bioactive metabolites that could influence neurodevelopment and ADHD, potentially via upregulation of proinflammatory cytokines, altered mitochondrial function and blood-brain barrier or gut permeability, or through stimulating afferent endings of the vagus nerve [5154]. Future studies evaluating potential mediating effects of the gut microbiome on the association of prenatal pet keeping and ADHD could further illuminate the pathway.

Prenatal pet keeping may also influence child neurodevelopment via mechanisms other than the gut microbiome. Pets could be a conduit for other environmental exposures, either through the application of pesticides to the pet itself (e.g., flea or tick medications) or by introduction of outdoor pesticides or toxicants into the home. For example, in the Childhood Autism Risks from Genetics and the Environment (CHARGE) study, a population-based study of children in California with ASD, developmental delays, and children from the general population, children with ASD had higher odds of having prenatal exposure to the flea and tick medication imidacloprid [16]. Pet dogs have been shown to act as a vehicle between outdoor pesticide application (diazinon) and introduction of these residues in the home [22]. A recent systematic review showed consistent evidence that prenatal exposure to organophosphate pesticides, which includes diazinon, is associated with adverse neurodevelopment across childhood; proposed mechanisms linking organophosphate pesticide exposure with negative impacts on neurodevelopment include impacting synaptic formation in utero or development of brain anomalies (e.g. damage to neurons or alterations in size of specific brain regions) [55]. Alternatively, Endenburg and van Lith [24], in a review on the influence of animals on child development, describe that it may not be the ownership of a pet, but rather the attachment to a pet that may influence emotional development. We do not have information on flea or tick medications used in the prenatal period, nor on the maternal or WHEALS child attachment to the pets in the home. Future studies that evaluate pet medications and attachment to and type of interaction with pets is needed.

Our study has a number of strengths and limitations. The prevalence of ADHD in WHEALS is comparable to the prevalence of ADHD reported in Michigan in 2011 (14.5% versus 12.8%) [56]. It is unlikely our results are due to reverse causality, as the keeping of pets prenatally would not be influenced by offspring behavior in childhood. WHEALS is a longitudinal birth cohort established in the prenatal period. Data on prenatal pet keeping was asked during the WHEALS prenatal interview, thus we do not have issues of recall bias, which has been an issue in other studies assessing prenatal risk factors for ADHD [32]. We do not have data on family history of ADHD. Given that ADHD is highly heritable [28], it is possible our parameter estimates for pet keeping with ADHD are biased by not accounting for family history or genetic risk. We did not have information on ADHD subtype; future analyses that obtain additional phenotypic information are needed. Our outcomes were caregiver-reported, which places us at risk for self-report bias. However, because most schools require a physician’s diagnosis (in writing) for a child to be considered potentially eligible for special education [57], it is likely in our sample that most caregiver reports of ADHD were based on medical diagnosis. Consistent with this, in our subsample of WHEALS children who had medical record abstraction, there was near perfect agreement between caregiver-reported and medical record documented ADHD.

Conclusions

In conclusion, we found evidence that maternal exposure to dogs prenatally is positively associated with caregiver-reported ADHD in boys at ages 10–12 years. Future studies that replicate these findings are needed, as are studies that evaluate potential mechanisms linking these factors (e.g., the microbiome).

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Abbreviations

ADHD

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder

ASD

Autism spectrum disorder

BMI

Body mass index

CI

Confidence interval

IPW

Inverse probability weights

NT

Neurotypical

OR

Odds Ratio

SPD

Sensory processing disorder

WHEALS

Wayne County Health, Environment, Allergy and Asthma Longitudinal Study

Authors’ contributions

AECB and JKS conceptualized the study question and were major contributors in writing the manuscript. ARS conducted the statistical analysis. CCJ designed the original WHEALS study. TMJH and JMP provided clinical and neuropsychological expertise. KJ oversaw data collection at the 10–12 year visit. All authors read, provided critical feedback, and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This study was funded by the National Institutes of Health (R01 AI050681, R01 HL113010, R01 HD082147, and P01 AI089473) and the Fund for Henry Ford Hospital. Study funders did not play a role in the design of the study, in the collection, analysis, and interpretation of data, or in writing the manuscript.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets analysed for this study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request and with necessary permissions.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Parents and/or legal guardians provided written, informed consent for the study. At the age 10–12 year visit, children provided written, informed assent. Study protocols were approved by the Henry Ford Health System Institutional Review Board (Federal Wide Assurance number 00005846; IRB# 00000253 and 00008660).

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare they have no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  • 1.Friedmann E, Son H. The human-companion animal bond: how humans benefit. Vet Clin North Am Small Anim Pract. 2009;39(2):293–326. doi: 10.1016/j.cvsm.2008.10.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Levine GN, Allen K, Braun LT, Christian HE, Friedmann E, Taubert KA, et al. Pet ownership and cardiovascular risk: a scientific statement from the American Heart Association. Circulation. 2013;127(23):2353–2363. doi: 10.1161/CIR.0b013e31829201e1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Arhant-Sudhir K, Arhant-Sudhir R, Sudhir K. Pet ownership and cardiovascular risk reduction: supporting evidence, conflicting data and underlying mechanisms. Clin Exp Pharmacol Physiol. 2011;38(11):734–738. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1681.2011.05583.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Cline KM. Psychological effects of dog ownership: role strain, role enhancement, and depression. J Soc Psychol. 2010;150(2):117–131. doi: 10.1080/00224540903368533. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Cutt H, Giles-Corti B, Knuiman M, Burke V. Dog ownership, health and physical activity: a critical review of the literature. Health & place. 2007;13(1):261–272. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2006.01.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Hodgson K, Barton L, Darling M, Antao V, Kim FA, Monavvari A. Pets' impact on your patients' health: leveraging benefits and mitigating risk. J Am Board Fam Med. 2015;28(4):526–534. doi: 10.3122/jabfm.2015.04.140254. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Podberscek AL. Positive and negative aspects of our relationship with companion animals. Vet Res Commun. 2006;30(1):21–27. doi: 10.1007/s11259-006-0005-0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.O'Neil J. Zoonotic infections from common household pets. J Nurse Pract. 2018;14(5):363–372. doi: 10.1016/j.nurpra.2017.12.025. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Wegienka G, Johnson CC, Havstad S, Ownby DR, Nicholas C, Zoratti EM. Lifetime dog and cat exposure and dog- and cat-specific sensitization at age 18 years. Clin Exp Allergy. 2011;41(7):979–986. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2222.2011.03747.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Timperio A, Salmon J, Chu B, Andrianopoulos N. Is dog ownership or dog walking associated with weight status in children and their parents? Health Promot J Austr. 2008;19(1):60–63. doi: 10.1071/HE08060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Havstad S, Wegienka G, Zoratti EM, Lynch SV, Boushey HA, Nicholas C, et al. Effect of prenatal indoor pet exposure on the trajectory of total IgE levels in early childhood. J Allergy Clin Immunol. 2011;128(4):880–5 e4. doi: 10.1016/j.jaci.2011.06.039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Ownby DR, Johnson CC, Peterson EL. Exposure to dogs and cats in the first year of life and risk of allergic sensitization at 6 to 7 years of age. JAMA. 2002;288(8):963–972. doi: 10.1001/jama.288.8.963. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Cassidy-Bushrow AE, Wegienka G, Havstad S, Levin AM, Lynch SV, Ownby DR, et al. Does pet-keeping modify the Association of Delivery Mode with offspring body size? Matern Child Health J. 2015;19(6):1426–1433. doi: 10.1007/s10995-014-1649-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Casas L, Tiesler C, Thiering E, Bruske I, Koletzko S, Bauer CP, et al. Indoor factors and behavioural problems in children: the GINIplus and LISAplus birth cohort studies. Int J Hyg Environ Health. 2013;216(2):146–154. doi: 10.1016/j.ijheh.2012.03.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Miles JNV, Parast L, Babey SH, Griffin BA, Saunders JM. A propensity-score-weighted population-based study of the health benefits of dogs and cats for children. Anthrozoos. 2017;30(3):429–440. doi: 10.1080/08927936.2017.1335103. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Keil AP, Daniels JL, Hertz-Picciotto I. Autism spectrum disorder, flea and tick medication, and adjustments for exposure misclassification: the CHARGE (CHildhood autism risks from genetics and environment) case-control study. Environ Health. 2014;13(1):3. doi: 10.1186/1476-069X-13-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Levin AM, Sitarik AR, Havstad SL, Fujimura KE, Wegienka G, Cassidy-Bushrow AE, et al. Joint effects of pregnancy, sociocultural, and environmental factors on early life gut microbiome structure and diversity. Sci Rep. 2016;6:31775. doi: 10.1038/srep31775. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Tun HM, Konya T, Takaro TK, Brook JR, Chari R, Field CJ, et al. Exposure to household furry pets influences the gut microbiota of infant at 3-4 months following various birth scenarios. Microbiome. 2017;5(1):40. doi: 10.1186/s40168-017-0254-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Aarts E, Ederveen THA, Naaijen J, Zwiers MP, Boekhorst J, Timmerman HM, et al. Gut microbiome in ADHD and its relation to neural reward anticipation. PLoS One. 2017;12(9):e0183509. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0183509. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Jiang HY, Zhou YY, Zhou GL, Li YC, Yuan J, Li XH, et al. Gut microbiota profiles in treatment-naive children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Behav Brain Res. 2018;347:408–413. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2018.03.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Sandgren AM, Brummer RJM. ADHD-originating in the gut? The emergence of a new explanatory model. Med Hypotheses. 2018;120:135–145. doi: 10.1016/j.mehy.2018.08.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Morgan MK, Stout DM, Jones PA, Barr DB. An observational study of the potential for human exposures to pet-borne diazinon residues following lawn applications. Environ Res. 2008;107(3):336–342. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2008.03.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Polanska K, Jurewicz J, Hanke W. Review of current evidence on the impact of pesticides, polychlorinated biphenyls and selected metals on attention deficit / hyperactivity disorder in children. Int J Occup Med Environ Health. 2013;26(1):16–38. doi: 10.2478/s13382-013-0073-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Endenburg N, van Lith HA. The influence of animals on the development of children. Vet J (London, England : 1997) 2011;190(2):208–214. doi: 10.1016/j.tvjl.2010.11.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.van Stralen J. Emotional dysregulation in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Atten Defic Hyperact Disord. 2016;8(4):175–187. doi: 10.1007/s12402-016-0199-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Tarver J, Daley D, Sayal K. Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): an updated review of the essential facts. Child Care Health Dev. 2014;40(6):762–774. doi: 10.1111/cch.12139. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Akinbami LJ, Liu X, Pastor PN, Reuben CA. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder among children aged 5-17 years in the United States. NCHS Data Brief. 1998;2011(70):1–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Biederman J, Faraone SV. Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Lancet. 2005;366(9481):237–248. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(05)66915-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Froehlich TE, Anixt JS, Loe IM, Chirdkiatgumchai V, Kuan L, Gilman RC. Update on environmental risk factors for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2011;13(5):333–344. doi: 10.1007/s11920-011-0221-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Gupte-Singh K, Singh RR, Lawson KA. Economic burden of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder among pediatric patients in the United States. Value Health. 2017;20(4):602–609. doi: 10.1016/j.jval.2017.01.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Klassen AF, Miller A, Fine S. Health-related quality of life in children and adolescents who have a diagnosis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics. 2004;114(5):e541–e547. doi: 10.1542/peds.2004-0844. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Sciberras E, Mulraney M, Silva D, Coghill D. Prenatal risk factors and the etiology of ADHD-review of existing evidence. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2017;19((1)):1. doi: 10.1007/s11920-017-0753-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Cassidy-Bushrow AE, Wegienka G, Barone CJ, 2nd, Valentini RP, Yee J, Havstad S, et al. Race-specific relationship of birth weight and renal function among healthy young children. Pediatr Nephrol. 2012;27(8):1317–1323. doi: 10.1007/s00467-012-2136-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Wegienka G, Havstad S, Joseph CL, Zoratti E, Ownby D, Woodcroft K, et al. Racial disparities in allergic outcomes in African Americans emerge as early as age 2 years. Clin Exp Allergy. 2012;42(6):909–917. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2222.2011.03946.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Landis JR, Koch GG. The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics. 1977;33(1):159–174. doi: 10.2307/2529310. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Rabinowitz P, Conti L. Links among human health, animal health, and ecosystem health. Annu Rev Public Health. 2013;34:189–204. doi: 10.1146/annurev-publhealth-031912-114426. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Wells Deborah L. Domestic dogs and human health: An overview. British Journal of Health Psychology. 2007;12(1):145–156. doi: 10.1348/135910706X103284. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Wegienka G, Havstad S, Zoratti EM, Kim H, Ownby DR, Johnson CC. Combined effects of prenatal medication use and delivery type are associated with eczema at age 2 years. Clin Exp Allergy. 2015;45(3):660–668. doi: 10.1111/cea.12467. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Shin D, Chung H, Weatherspoon L, Song WO. Validity of Prepregnancy weight status estimated from self-reported height and weight. Matern Child Health J. 2014;18(7):1667–1674. doi: 10.1007/s10995-013-1407-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.DiPietro JA, Voegtline KM. The gestational foundation of sex differences in development and vulnerability. Neuroscience. 2017;342:4–20. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.07.068. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Hernan MA, Hernandez-Diaz S, Robins JM. A structural approach to selection bias. Epidemiology. 2004;15(5):615–625. doi: 10.1097/01.ede.0000135174.63482.43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Robins JM, Hernan MA, Brumback B. Marginal structural models and causal inference in epidemiology. Epidemiology. 2000;11(5):550–560. doi: 10.1097/00001648-200009000-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Curtis LH, Hammill BG, Eisenstein EL, Kramer JM, Anstrom KJ. Using inverse probability-weighted estimators in comparative effectiveness analyses with observational databases. Med Care. 2007;45(10 Supl 2):S103–S107. doi: 10.1097/MLR.0b013e31806518ac. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Boyle CA, Boulet S, Schieve LA, Cohen RA, Blumberg SJ, Yeargin-Allsopp M, et al. Trends in the prevalence of developmental disabilities in US children, 1997-2008. Pediatrics. 2011;127(6):1034–1042. doi: 10.1542/peds.2010-2989. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Ramtekkar UP, Reiersen AM, Todorov AA, Todd RD. Sex and age differences in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms and diagnoses: implications for DSM-V and ICD-11. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;49(3):217–28.e1-3. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Klein SL, Flanagan KL. Sex differences in immune responses. Nat Rev Immunol. 2016;16(10):626–638. doi: 10.1038/nri.2016.90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Sitarik AR, Havstad S, Levin AM, Lynch SV, Fujimura KE, Ownby DR, et al. Dog introduction alters the home dust microbiota. Indoor Air. 2018;28(4):539–547. doi: 10.1111/ina.12456. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Fujimura KE, Johnson CC, Ownby DR, Cox MJ, Brodie EL, Havstad SL, et al. Man's best friend? The effect of pet ownership on house dust microbial communities. J Allergy Clin Immunol. 2010;126(2):410–2, 2 e1-3. doi: 10.1016/j.jaci.2010.05.042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Barberan A, Dunn RR, Reich BJ, Pacifici K, Laber EB, Menninger HL, et al. The ecology of microscopic life in household dust. Proc Biol Sci. 2015;282(1814):20151139. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2015.1139. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Rodriguez JM, Murphy K, Stanton C, Ross RP, Kober OI, Juge N, et al. The composition of the gut microbiota throughout life, with an emphasis on early life. Microb Ecol Health Dis. 2015;26:26050. doi: 10.3402/mehd.v26.26050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Principi N, Esposito S. Gut microbiota and central nervous system development. J Infect. 2016;73(6):536–546. doi: 10.1016/j.jinf.2016.09.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Gilbert JA, Blaser MJ, Caporaso JG, Jansson JK, Lynch SV, Knight R. Current understanding of the human microbiome. Nat Med. 2018;24(4):392–400. doi: 10.1038/nm.4517. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Ly V, Bottelier M, Hoekstra PJ, Arias Vasquez A, Buitelaar JK, Rommelse NN. Elimination diets' efficacy and mechanisms in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and autism spectrum disorder. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2017;26(9):1067–1079. doi: 10.1007/s00787-017-0959-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Evans JM, Morris LS, Marchesi JR. The gut microbiome: the role of a virtual organ in the endocrinology of the host. J Endocrinol. 2013;218(3):R37–R47. doi: 10.1530/JOE-13-0131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Sapbamrer R, Hongsibsong S. Effects of prenatal and postnatal exposure to organophosphate pesticides on child neurodevelopment in different age groups: a systematic review. Environ Sci Pollut Res Int. 2019;26(18):18267–18290. doi: 10.1007/s11356-019-05126-w. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Division of Human Development and Disability, National Center on Birth Defects and Developmental Disabilities, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. State-based Prevalence Data of Parent Reported ADHD Diagnosis by a Health Care Provider. Available at: https://www.cdc.gov/ncbddd/adhd/prevalence.html, Accessed 06/28/2018.
  • 57.Grice K. Eligibility under IDEA for other health impaired children. School Law Bull. 2002;Summer:7–12.

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets analysed for this study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request and with necessary permissions.


Articles from BMC Pediatrics are provided here courtesy of BMC

RESOURCES