Abstract
Objective: The excitability of cerebral cortical cells, neural pathway, and neural networks, as well as their plasticity, are key to our exploration of age-related changes in brain structure and function. The combination of transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) with electromyography (EMG) can be applied to the primary motor cortex; it activates the underlying neural group and passes through the corticospinal pathway, which can be quantified using EMG. This meta-analysis aimed to analyze changes in cortical excitability and plasticity in healthy elderly individuals vs. young individuals through TMS-EMG.
Methods: The Cochrane Library, Medline, and EMBASE databases were searched to identify eligible trials published from database inception to June 3, 2019. The Cochrane Risk of Bias Tool and improved Jadad scale were used to assess the methodological quality. A meta-analysis of the comparative effects was conducted using the Review Manager 5.3 software and Stata 14.0 software.
Results: The pooled results revealed that the resting motor threshold values in the elderly group were markedly higher than those reported in the young group (mean difference [MD]: −2.35; 95% confidence interval [CI]: −3.69 to −1.02]; p < (0.00001). The motor evoked potential amplitude significantly reduced in the elderly group vs. the young group (MD: 0.18; 95% CI: 0.09–0.27; p < 0.0001). Moreover, there was significantly longer motor evoked potential latency in the elderly group (MD: −1.07; 95% CI: −1.77 to −0.37]; p =(0.003). There was no significant difference observed in the active motor threshold between the elderly and young groups (MD: −1.52; 95% CI: −3.47 to −0.42]; p =(0.13). Meanwhile, only two studies reported the absence of adverse events.
Conclusion: We found that the excitability of the cerebral cortex declined in elderly individuals vs. young individuals. The findings of the present analysis should be considered with caution owing to the methodological limitations in the included trials. Additional high-quality studies are warranted to validate our findings.
Keywords: plasticity of neural networks, age-related, transcranial magnetic stimulation, resting motor threshold, meta-analysis
Introduction
As the birth rate decreases and life expectancy increases, the aging problem of the global population becomes more prominent (Tatti et al., 2016). One of the most striking features of human motor behavior is the ability to respond rapidly and appropriately to environmental changes. However, this ability gradually declines with age. In particular, the decline in cognitive and motor abilities is associated with advancing age (Hunter et al., 2001). This age-related decline is a precursor of various diseases. An enhanced understanding of the impact of aging on cortical functioning may provide us more insight into understanding of age-related diseases (Seidler et al., 2010).
The human motor cortex is capable of undergoing persistent morphological or functional changes depending on stimuli from the environment; this is termed neuroplasticity (Boroojerdi et al., 2001). Cortical plasticity decreases with extensive changes in neurochemistry and neurophysiology during physiological aging (Brunso-Bechtold et al., 2000; Rossini et al., 2007). Although the regulatory mechanisms of related brain neurons are unclear, an increasing body of evidence suggests that aging is associated with alterations in the neural projections. Normal aging is associated with impairments in dendritic morphology, loss of synaptic contact cellular connectivity, and gene expression, which subsequently cause a relative decrease in the excitability of the cortex (Godde et al., 2002; Sawaki et al., 2003; Burke and Barnes, 2006; Hortobágyi et al., 2006; Oliviero et al., 2006). This eventually leads to a decrease in sensation, motor performance, and cognitive function (Mora et al., 2007; Ward et al., 2008).
Several studies showed that the decline in cognitive, memory, and behavioral abilities in healthy elderly individuals are closely related to vascular damage and amyloid deposition (DeCarli et al., 2012). Further research indicated that ischemia and amyloid deposition are connected through the utilitarian bunch of cerebrovascular cells, supporting glial tissue and neurons. Moreover, as a potential mediator of reactive aging, amyloid deposition may lead to changes in neural networks and circuits (Iadecola, 2010). In this way, the interaction of blood vessels and neurodegenerative injuries may influence the cortical and subcortical neural systems. Importantly, this damage may alter the local field potentials. In other words, we can assess age-related cognitive decline by measuring changes in the local field potentials. Recently, neuroscience research has explored the opportunity to apply Non-Invasive Brain Stimulation technology, which measures changes in the local field potentials of the cerebral cortex, to healthy elderly populations. This approach aims to explore aging-related mechanisms and the changes of cerebral cortex under physiological conditions.
The combination of transcranial magnetic stimulation with electromyography may provide information regarding the local and global potentials of the cerebral cortex, and may be used to measure changes in cortical properties following age-related changes in the plasticity of neural networks. A few studies have investigated age-related changes in the plasticity of neural networks among young and elderly individuals (Godde et al., 2002; Sawaki et al., 2003; Oliviero et al., 2006). Owing to the small sample sizes, device characteristics, parameter settings, and experimental procedures of these studies, the current excitability of the cerebral cortex at different ages remains inconclusive. We conducted a systematic review and meta-analysis of the available data to further examine this trend. Therefore, this meta-analysis aimed to analyze changes in cortical excitability and plasticity in healthy elderly individuals vs. young individuals through TMS-EMG.
Methods
This study was performed according to the guidelines of the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analysis checklist. The checklist and flowchart are shown in Figure 1, Appendix 1 in Supplementary Material.
Search Strategy
We searched the Cochrane Library, Medline, and EMBASE databases for eligible trials published from database inception to June 3, 2019 without language restriction. The search terms are shown in Appendix 2 (Supplementary Material). In addition, we searched the websites of the international clinical trial registry provided by the U.S. National Institutes of Health to avoid missing unpublished studies. Furthermore, the references cited in the searched articles were also carefully reviewed. The search terms were listed in the Appendix 2 (Supplementary Material).
Inclusion Criteria
(1) Patients: healthy individuals aged >18 years. (2) Interventions: all studies that used the TMS to detect brain cortical excitability and plasticity were included. (3) Comparison: healthy elderly (≥50 years) vs. healthy young (<50 years) individuals. (4) Outcomes: primary outcome measures included resting motor threshold (RMT); secondary outcome measures included any of the following: (a) motor evoked potentials (MEPs) amplitude or latency, (b) active motor threshold (AMT), and (c) adverse events. (5) Trial design: cohort or case-control studies.
Exclusion Criteria
(1) Patients with a history of neurological or psychiatric disease, or currently receiving treatment with psychoactive drugs (i.e., sedatives, antipsychotics, antidepressants, etc.). (2) Studies with insufficient data or irrelevant outcomes.
Study Selection and Data Extraction
All potentially eligible studies were independently selected by two reviewers (JL and YL) based on the titles and abstracts. The full text of the selected articles was subsequently obtained and independently reviewed by the reviewers based on the inclusion and exclusion criteria. The following information was independently extracted from the included trials by these two reviewers: title and authors, year of publication, sample size, age and sex of participants, target muscle and hemisphere for TMS, and outcomes (i.e., outcome measures and adverse events). Any disagreements were resolved through discussion with the third reviewer (WY) or further evaluation. The original investigators of studies with missing or incomplete data were contacted to request the data.
Assessment of Risk of Bias (ROB)
The ROB tool of Cochrane (Savović et al., 2014) was used to assess the methodological quality. In addition, the quality of the study was evaluated using the improved Jadad scale. The full score is 7 points, and the threshold for high-quality research is ≥4 points. Two reviewers (JL and YL) independently judged the quality; any disagreements were resolved through discussion with the third reviewer (WY) acting as an arbiter. Moreover, we completed the Standards for Reporting Interventions in Controlled Trials checklist to assess the risk of bias.
Statistical Analysis
The RevMan software (version 5.3; Cochrane Collaboration, Oxford, UK) and Stata software (version 12.0; Stata Corp LP, College Station, TX, USA) were used for statistical analysis. Dichotomous outcomes were expressed with the odds ratio and 95% confidence intervals (CI), while continuous outcomes were expressed with the mean difference (MD, indicators changed from baseline) with 95% CI. Cochran's Q test and I2 statistic were used to assess the homogeneity; the I2 test was performed for further analysis. A random-effects model was used to calculate the pooled effect size for the presence of significant heterogeneity (p ≤ 0.1, I2 ≥ 50%), and a fixed-effects model was applied for the absence of significant heterogeneity (Higgins et al., 2003). We explored the possible sources of heterogeneity through a subgroup analysis, and repeated the sensitivity analysis to study its effect on the overall effect size. A p ≤ 0.05 denoted statistically significant difference.
Results
Study Selection
We initially retrieved 3,445 publications, and 566 duplicate citations were removed. After screening the abstracts and titles, a total of 2,729 publications were excluded. A total of 121 publications from the 150 publications further identified as potentially eligible trials according to the review of the full text were excluded for the following reasons: 14 were conference abstracts, 76 did not report a relevant outcome, and 15 had ineligible study design and comparison. We did not identify any other studies for evaluation after reviewing the bibliographies of the full-text articles collected during the initial search. Finally, 29 studies (Rossini et al., 1992; Kossev et al., 2002; Sale and Semmler, 2005; Hortobágyi et al., 2006; Fujiyama et al., 2009, 2011, 2012, 2014; Pellicciari et al., 2009; Rogasch et al., 2009; Smith et al., 2009, 2011; Cirillo et al., 2010, 2011; Fathi et al., 2010; Todd et al., 2010; Degardin et al., 2011; Levin et al., 2011; Bernard and Seidler, 2012; Young-Bernier et al., 2012, 2014; Cuypers et al., 2013; Bashir et al., 2014; Opie and Semmler, 2014; Dickins et al., 2015, 2017; Opie et al., 2017, 2018; Emonson et al., 2019) fulfilled our inclusion criteria and were selected. Figure 1 depicts the search process and trial selection.
Description of Included Studies
The studies included in the analysis involved a total of 914 participants. The age ranged 18–84 years. The brain cortical excitability and plasticity were examined by TMS-EMG in these studies. The muscles which evoked motor potentials in these studies included the first dorsal interosseous muscle, the abductor pollicis brevis muscle, the extensor carpi radialis muscle, the flexor carpi radialis muscle, the flexor pollicis brevis muscle, and the thenar and plantar muscles. The outcomes used to assess cortical excitability and plasticity included RMT, AMT, MEP amplitude, and MEP latency. The detailed characteristics are shown in Table 1.
Table 1.
References | Region | Participants (Y/O) | Age Y (years) | Age O (years) | Gender T (F/M) | Gender C (F/M) | Muscle | Hemisphere | Outcome indicator | Risk of bias |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Rossini et al., 1992 | Italy | 25/40 | 25.3 ± 5.3 | 66.0 ± 10.4 | 15/10 | 26/14 | TP | All | RMT\MEP Amplitude\MEP Latency | UUUULLU |
Kossev et al., 2002 | Germany | 10/10 | 28.5 ± 5.2 | 56.1 ± 4.9 | 6/4 | 7/3 | ECR | Left | RMT\MEP Amplitude\MEP Latency | UUUULHU |
Sale and Semmler, 2005 | Australia | 10/10 | 26.6 ± 1.3 | 67.6 ± 2.3 | 5/5 | 5/5 | FDI | Left | RMT\MEP Amplitude | UUUULLU |
Hortobágyi et al., 2006 | USA | 6/6 | 27.2 ± 3.7 | 72.7 ± 6.3 | 4/2 | 5/1 | FCR | Left | RMT\MEP Amplitude | UUUULLU |
Rogasch et al., 2009 | Australia | 14/14 | 20.7 ± 1.9 | 68.3 ± 5.6 | 6/8 | 6/8 | APB | Left | RMT\AMT\MEP Amplitude | UUUULLU |
Pellicciari et al., 2009 | Italy | 16/16 | 26.2 ± 0.8 | 62.1 ± 1.5 | 8/8 | 8/8 | APB | Left | RMT\MEP Latency | UUUULLU |
Fujiyama et al., 2009 | Australia | 15/15 | 18–33 | 61–75 | 9/6 | 9/6 | ECR | Left | MEP Amplitude | UUUULLU |
Smith et al., 2009 | Australia | 13/17 | 20.0 ± 2.0 | 63.1 ± 4.2 | 0/13 | 0/17 | FDI | Left | RMT\MEP Amplitude\AMT | UUUULLU |
Fathi et al., 2010 | Japan | 16/16 | 21–39 | 60–79 | 2/14 | 5/11 | APB | Left | RMT\MEP | UULLLLU |
Cirillo et al., 2010 | Australia | 12/14 | 22 ± 2 | 67 ± 4 | 7/5 | 7/7 | APB | Left | RMT\AMT\MEP Amplitude | UUUULLU |
Todd et al., 2010 | Australia | 15/15 | 25 ± 4 | 67 ± 5 | 6/9 | 6/9 | FDI | Left | RMT\MEP\AMT | UULULLU |
Cirillo et al., 2011 | Australia | 16/16 | 23 ± 3 | 67 ± 5 | 9/7 | 9/7 | FDI | Left | RMT\AMT\MEP Amplitude | UUULLLU |
Degardin et al., 2011 | France | 14/14 | 26.4 ± 2.7 | 62.4 ± 7.1 | 6/8 | 8/6 | APB | N/A | RMT\AMT | UUUULLU |
Fujiyama et al., 2011 | Australia | 13/13 | 18–34 | 62–74 | 9/4 | 9/4 | FPB | Left | RMT | UULULLU |
Levin et al., 2011 | Belgium | 6/5 | 23.7 ± 2.3 | 63.8 ± 1.8 | N/A | N/A | APB | Left | RMT\MEP Amplitude | UUUULLU |
Smith et al., 2011 | Australia | 15/15 | 20.1 ± 2.1 | 65.5 ± 3.9 | 0/15 | 0/15 | FDI | Left | RMT\AMT | UUUULLU |
Bernard and Seidler, 2012 | USA | 16/17 | 21 ± 1.83 | 69.53 ± 4.07 | 12/4 | 10/7 | FDI | Left | RMT\MEP Amplitude\MEP Latency | UUUULLU |
Fujiyama et al., 2012 | Australia | 15/15 | 18–29 | 58–84 | 8/7 | 9/6 | ECR | Left | RMT\MEP Amplitude | UUULLLU |
Young-Bernier et al., 2012 | Canada | 25/31 | 22.5 ± 3.5 | 70.3 ± 3.8 | 14/11 | 18/13 | FDI | Left | RMT\MEP Amplitude\MEP Latency | UULULLU |
Cuypers et al., 2013 | Belgium | 14/10 | 22.8 ± 1.7 | 69.3 ± 2.8 | 8/6 | 8/2 | FDI | Left | RMT\MEP Amplitude | UUUULLU |
Bashir et al., 2014 | USA | 10/8 | 23.40 ± 3.50 | 57.38 ± 9.61 | 4/6 | 5/3 | FDI | Left | RMT\MEP Amplitude | UUUULLU |
Fujiyama et al., 2014 | Australia | 20/20 | 22.7 ± 3.3 | 68.3 ± 7.9 | 13/7 | 10/10 | FCR | Left | RMT | UULLLLU |
Opie and Semmler, 2014 | Australia | 22/18 | 22.3 ± 3.1 | 70.8 ± 5.0 | N/A | N/A | FDI | N/A | RMT\AMT\MEP Amplitude | UULLLLU |
Young-Bernier et al., 2014 | Canada | 20/18 | 22.3 ± 3.2 | 70.1 ± 5.6 | 13/7 | 9/9 | FDI | N/A | RMT\MEP Amplitude\MEP Latency | UULULLU |
Dickins et al., 2015 | Australia | 20/20 | 22.95 ± 2.52 | 70.15 ± 3.07 | 10/10 | 10/10 | APB | N/A | RMT\MEP | UULULLU |
Dickins et al., 2017 | Australia | 20/20 | 24.4 ± 3.86 | 69.55 ± 3.99 | 10/10 | 10/10 | APB | N/A | RMT\MEP | UULLLLU |
Opie et al., 2017 | Australia | 15/15 | 22.9 ± 0.5 | 70.8 ± 1.6 | 8/7 | 7/8 | FDI | Left | RMT\MEP | UUULLLU |
Opie et al., 2018 | Australia | 15/18 | 22.5 ± 2.9 | 70.1 ± 6.0 | 8/7 | 12/6 | FDI | Left | RMT\MEP\AMT | UUULLLU |
Emonson et al., 2019 | Australia | 20/20 | 24.5 ± 4.48 | 65.47 ± 5.62 | N/A | N/A | N/A | N/A | RMT | UUULLLU |
Y, young group; O, old group; F, female; M, male; N/A, not applicable; TP, thenar and plantar muscles; ECR, the extensor carpi radialis muscle; APB, the abductor pollicis brevis muscle; FDI, the first dorsal interosseous muscle; FCR, the flexor carpi radialis muscles; FPB, the flexor pollicis brevis muscle.
Methodological Quality of Included Studies
Randomization was not used in the 29 selected studies, and none of the studies described concealment of allocation. Nine studies (Fathi et al., 2010; Todd et al., 2010; Fujiyama et al., 2011, 2014; Young-Bernier et al., 2012, 2014; Opie and Semmler, 2014; Dickins et al., 2015, 2017) used appropriate blinding methods for the participants, and nine studies (Fathi et al., 2010; Cirillo et al., 2011; Fujiyama et al., 2012, 2014; Opie and Semmler, 2014; Dickins et al., 2017; Opie et al., 2017, 2018; Emonson et al., 2019) blinded the outcome assessments. All studies reported complete outcome data. Overall, the methodological quality all the included studies was low. The details are listed in the Appendix 3 (Supplementary Material). The summary of bias in each domain across the included studies is shown in Figures 2, 3.
Primary Outcomes
A total of 28 studies (Rossini et al., 1992; Kossev et al., 2002; Sale and Semmler, 2005; Hortobágyi et al., 2006; Pellicciari et al., 2009; Rogasch et al., 2009; Smith et al., 2009, 2011; Cirillo et al., 2010, 2011; Fathi et al., 2010; Todd et al., 2010; Degardin et al., 2011; Fujiyama et al., 2011, 2012, 2014; Levin et al., 2011; Bernard and Seidler, 2012; Young-Bernier et al., 2012, 2014; Cuypers et al., 2013; Bashir et al., 2014; Opie and Semmler, 2014; Dickins et al., 2015, 2017; Opie et al., 2017, 2018; Emonson et al., 2019) assessed the changes in RMT between the elderly and young groups (Figure 4). Owing to the significant heterogeneity among the studies (I2: 85%; p < 0.00001), we selected the random-effects model. The pooled results revealed that the RMT values in the elderly group were markedly higher than those observed in the young group (MD: −2.65; 95% CI: −3.97 to −1.33; p = 0.008).
Subgroup Analysis of Primary Outcomes
Owing to the high heterogeneity among the pooled results of RMT evaluation (I2 = 85%; p < 0.00001), we divided the studies according to the year of publication and region, and the varied stimulated muscles were examined through a subgroup analysis (Table 2, Figure 4, Appendices 4, 5 in Supplementary Material). The subgroup analysis showed that the muscle, region, or year of publication may be the sources of high heterogeneity.
Table 2.
Group | No. of studies | No. of participants | MD | 95% CI | Z | p (Effect) | I2 (%) | χ2 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Muscle | ||||||||
FDI | 13 | 414 | −4.04 | [−5.73, −2.36] | 4.70 | <0.00001 | 75 | 48.97 |
APB | 8 | 237 | −1.10 | [−4.54, 2.33] | 0.63 | 0.53 | 94 | 109.78 |
ECR | 2 | 50 | −0.49 | [−2.79, 1.80] | 0.42 | 0.67 | 0 | 0.54 |
FCR | 2 | 52 | −0.72 | [−2.24, 0.80] | 0.93 | 0.35 | 0 | 0.01 |
FPB | 1 | 26 | −3.33 | [−4.94, −1.72] | 4.06 | <0.00001 | ||
TP | 1 | 65 | −4.50 | [−6.91, −2.09] | 3.66 | 0.0003 | ||
Publication region | ||||||||
Oceania | 16 | 515 | −1.90 | [−2.95, −0.85] | 3.56 | 0.0004 | 69 | 48.82 |
Europe | 6 | 180 | −5.10 | [−7.95, −2.25] | 3.51 | 0.0005 | 74 | 19.21 |
Asia | 1 | 32 | 6 | [4.23, 7.77] | 6.66 | <0.00001 | ||
America | 5 | 157 | −4.99 | [−7.91, −2.06] | 3.34 | 0.0008 | 0 | 1.51 |
Publication year | ||||||||
≤ 2011 | 15 | 422 | −1.65 | [−3.97, 0.68] | 1.38 | 0.17 | 88 | 118.78 |
>2011 | 13 | 462 | −2.94 | [−4.55, −1.33] | 9.35 | 0.0004 | 84 | 73.13 |
ECR, the extensor carpi radialis muscle; APB, the abductor pollicis brevis muscle; FDI, the first dorsal interosseous muscle; FCR, the flexor carpi radialis muscles; FPB, the flexor pollicis brevis muscle; TP, thenar and plantar muscles.
Subgroup Analysis Based on Different Stimulated Muscles
The pooled results showed that the excitability of the cerebral cortex in the elderly group was significantly reduced compared with that reported in the young group. However, subgroup differences were detected, and when the stimulated muscle was the abductor pollicis brevis muscle (MD: −1.10; 95% CI: −4.54–2.33; p = 0.53), extensor carpi radialis (MD: −0.49; 95% CI: −2.79–1.80; p = 0.67), and flexor carpi radialis (MD: −0.72; 95% CI: −2.24–0.80; p = 0.35), there was no significant difference between two groups (Figure 4). This indicates that the measurement effects of different muscles vary. Therefore, different muscles may be representative of high heterogeneity.
Subgroup Analysis Based on Different Regions
We divided the regions of study into Oceania, Europe, Asia, and America. The results showed that the excitability of the cerebral cortex in the elderly group was significantly reduced compared with that recorded in the young group. However, one study in Asia showed a decrease in RMT in the control group (Appendix 4 in Supplementary Material). It is possible that the statistical power was inadequate due to the small number of studies performed in Asia.
Subgroup Analysis Based on the Year of Publication
We divided the studies according to the year of publication (i.e., in and prior to 2011, and after 2011). The results showed that the excitability of the cerebral cortex in the elderly group was markedly lower than that reported in the young group. However, we did not find significant differences in the two groups among the studies performed in and prior to 2011 (MD: −1.65; 95% CI: −3.97–0.68; p = 0.17) (Appendix 5 in Supplementary Material). This may be related to device characteristics (e.g., type of coil and accuracy of the EMG) or the level of operational proficiency.
Sensitivity Analysis
We excluded one study at a time to determine the effect of each study on the estimated pooled effect size. The results did not show instability (Figure 5).
Secondary Outcomes
MEP Amplitude
A total of 20 studies (Rossini et al., 1992; Sale and Semmler, 2005; Fujiyama et al., 2009, 2012; Rogasch et al., 2009; Smith et al., 2009; Cirillo et al., 2010, 2011; Fathi et al., 2010; Todd et al., 2010; Levin et al., 2011; Bernard and Seidler, 2012; Young-Bernier et al., 2012, 2014; Cuypers et al., 2013; Bashir et al., 2014; Dickins et al., 2015, 2017; Opie et al., 2017, 2018) providing numerical data for the MEP amplitude were included. Owing to the heterogeneity, a random-effects model was applied (I2: 89%; p < 0.00001). The pooled data analysis showed that the MEP amplitude was markedly reduced in the elderly group vs. the young group (MD: 0.18; 95% CI: 0.09–0.27; p < 0.00001) (Figure 6A).
MEP Latency
Seven studies (Rossini et al., 1992; Kossev et al., 2002; Pellicciari et al., 2009; Todd et al., 2010; Bernard and Seidler, 2012; Young-Bernier et al., 2012, 2014) providing numerical data for the MEP latency were included. Owing to the heterogeneity, a random-effects model was used (I2: 79%; p < 0.00001). The pooled results showed that there was a significantly longer MEP latency in the elderly group (MD: −1.07; 95% CI:−1.77 to −0.37; p = 0.003) (Figure 6B).
AMT
Nine studies (Rogasch et al., 2009; Smith et al., 2009, 2011; Cirillo et al., 2010, 2011; Todd et al., 2010; Degardin et al., 2011; Opie and Semmler, 2014; Opie et al., 2018) providing numerical data for the AMT were included. Owing to the heterogeneity, a random-effects model was used (I2: 82%; p < 0.00001). There was no significant difference observed in the AMT between the two groups (MD: −1.52; 95% CI: −3.47 to −0.42; p = 0.13) (Figure 6C).
Adverse Outcomes
Only two (Hortobágyi et al., 2006; Opie et al., 2017) studies did not report adverse events related to TMS. The remaining studies did not report adverse outcomes.
Publication Bias
According to Egger's test, there was no evidence of publication bias among the primary outcome (RMT outcome, Egger's test p = 0.257).
Discussion
Summary of the Main Findings
We conducted a meta-analysis including 29 studies with 914 patients. We used TMS-EMG, including RMT, AMT, MEP amplitude, and MEP latency to evaluate the age-related changes in the plasticity of neural networks among young and elderly individuals. The results indicated that the excitability of the cerebral cortex declined in elderly individuals vs. young individuals. Three important points were noted. Firstly, the RMT and MEP latency in the elderly group was higher than that in the young group. In contrast, the MEP amplitude in the elderly group was lower than that in the young group. Secondly, there was no significant difference in the AMT between the two groups. Third, there were no common adverse reactions reported in the included studies, suggesting that TMS-EMG technology is relatively safe.
Age-Related Changes in the Plasticity of Neural Networks
Driven by a decline in birth rates and an increase in life expectancy, the global demographic structure is rapidly aging. Demographic projections show that the proportion of individuals aged ≥65 years is expected to increase by >150% in 35 years (Tatti et al., 2016). According to this projection, the number of individuals aged >65 years will exceed 1.6 billion by 2050. Age-related diseases profoundly impact the daily activities and quality of life of the elderly (Logsdon et al., 2002; Craik and Bialystok, 2006), posing a great and urgent challenge to society (Ballard, 2010). Therefore, it is essential to fully understand the mechanisms involved in aging.
Aging is associated with functional decline in numerous cognitive areas, such as attention, memory, language, and executive functions (Morrison and Baxter, 2012). The structure and function of many brain regions undergo extensive changes with aging, including reduction in cerebral cortex thickness (Tatti et al., 2016), decreases in neurotransmitter binding potential, and synaptic receptor density and efficacy (Celsis, 2000), change in cortical and cerebellar metabolism (Dukart et al., 2013), gray matter atrophy, white matter loss ventricular enlargement, etc. (Scahill et al., 2003; Bolandzadeh et al., 2012; Bennett and Madden, 2014).
Research on the mechanism of aging and the related physiological and pathological changes is insufficient. However, in recent years, numerous studies investigating age-related changes in the plasticity of neural networks have achieved breakthroughs in this field. A number of studies have shown that elderly individuals with normal cognition mainly showed functional changes in the neural network during memory coding, especially in the posterior medial cortex (Wellman and Sengelaub, 1995; Ahmed et al., 2018). Converging evidence from positron emission tomography studies indicates that nearly one-third of clinically normal elderly individuals harbor fibrillar β-amyloid deposition (Dickerson et al., 2009; Sperling et al., 2009). Animal experiments confirmed that with increasing age, fibrous β-amyloid is gradually deposited, and functional changes in the default network were observed in the posterior medial cortex of rats (Becker et al., 2011; Sousa et al., 2017). These studies suggested that many of the age-related changes in neural network function may be attributed to amyloid deposition (Iadecola, 2010).
Multi-dimensional observations of changes in the neural networks of the brain may deepen our understanding of aging, and its physiological and pathological mechanisms. Non-invasive brain stimulation techniques (e.g., TMS) are used in numerous studies to detect changes in the neural networks. Recently, neuroscience research has explored the possibility of applying this technology to examine the elderly population. Early detection of changes may enable the development of new treatments, thereby increasing the likelihood of effectively preventing or delaying the occurrence of disease.
Diagnostic Applications of TMS for the Plasticity of Neural Networks
Since its introduction by Professor Anthony Barker of the University of Sheffield in 1985 (Barker et al., 1985), TMS has been used to activate the human motor cortex, non-invasively assess the net level of cortical excitability, and determine the integrity of the central motor pathways (Tatti et al., 2016). TMS removes short and powerful magnetic pulses from the skull and generates a secondary electrical current according to the principle of electromagnetic induction of Faraday. The current generated in the cerebral cortex changes the excitability of the underlying neurons and induces neuronal firing. This ultimately results in movement of the corresponding muscles through the corticospinal, corticonuclear, callosal fiber, nerve roots, and peripheral motor pathways (Sanes and Donoghue, 2000; Müller-Dahlhaus et al., 2010). In this manner, TMS may directly reflect the excitability information of different motor cortices and functional integrity of the intracortical neuronal structures. EMG refers to the muscle bioelectrical pattern recorded using an electromyograph. It is often used to determine the functional status of peripheral nerves, neurons, neuromuscular junctions, and muscles. When TMS-EMG is applied to the monitoring of primary motor cortex excitability, it activates the underlying neural group and passes through the corticospinal pathway. This ultimately leads to a motor response to the contralateral muscle MEP, which can be quantified using EMG. The motor thresholds, the MEP amplitude, the MEP latency, and short intracortical inhibition are the most commonly applied techniques for TMS (Edwards et al., 2008). Notably, the results of these techniques are largely influenced by device characteristics, parameter settings, and experimental procedures. In addition, subject-related variables (e.g., eventual pharmacological treatments) affect the outcome indicators of TMS stimulation.
In recent years, investigations regarding the plasticity of neural networks have attracted considerable attention, and researchers use the TMS-EMG technology to evaluate the sensitivity of the motor system to plastic changes. Some studies showed that following the induction of the M1 cortical region of the brain by TMS in the elderly, their ability to stimulate excitability and stimulation-induced adaptations was limited compared with that reported in the young individuals (Dayan et al., 2013; Li et al., 2015). Consistent with the findings of the present meta-analysis, these results indicate that the function and ability of the M1 cortical region in older individuals decrease with age.
Limitations
This meta-analysis had several limitations. Firstly, heterogeneity was detected, which reduced the reliability of the comparisons. Although all the included studies were examined using only TMS-EMG, there was also heterogeneity detected in different characteristics, parameter settings, experimental procedures, and subject-related variables (including state dependency and eventual pharmacological treatments), which may reduce the generalizability of the conclusions. Secondly, only 29 randomized controlled trials were included in this review. Moreover, limitations in the quality of research design and sample sizes affect the extrapolation and strength of this evidence. In addition, due to the limitations of the inclusion of the literature, we can only be included in the cohort and case-control studies. Therefore, it is possible that the statistical power may have been inadequate due to the small number of studies, and it is difficult to precisely evaluate the excitability of neural networks in the different groups. Thirdly, there was no report of adverse events. In the included studies, we only found limited information related to safety owing to poor reporting of adverse events. Hence, the pooled results should be treated with caution. Another limitation is that, in this review, behavioral meta-analysis results are only provided by the included TMS-EMG studies, which may lead to publication bias. This suggests that some studies without significant cortical activation results may not be published.
Conclusions
We found that the excitability of the cerebral cortex declined in elderly individuals vs. young individuals. The findings of this review should be considered with caution owing to the methodological limitation of the included trials. Further high-quality studies are warranted to validate the present findings. It is well established that the normal process of aging is the result of degeneration and compensation of the neural network and its related conduction functions. However, the existing models of age-related changes in the brain do not cover the full spectrum of alterations (Li et al., 2015). Thus, the use of various detection methods is necessary to dynamically observe the age-related changes in neural networks. This approach may assist researchers in developing a new aging model, which reflects the age-related changes in the plasticity of neural networks.
Data Availability Statement
The data supporting the results reported in this article may be obtained from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.
Author Contributions
NX and PH: study concept and design. WY and ZY: data curation. YL and LW: formal analysis. YL and JL: investigation. XT: methodology. YL: resources. LW, MX, and PH: software. XT and PH: supervision and writing-original draft. NX: validation. XT: writing-review and editing. All authors read and approved to publish the final version of the manuscript.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest. The handling editor declared a shared affiliation, though no other collaboration, with the author PH at the time of the review.
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank the staff of South China Research Center for Acupuncture of Guangdong Province for their support of this study.
Glossary
Abbreviations
- AMT
active motor threshold
- EMG
electromyography
- APB
the abductor pollicis brevis muscle
- ECR
the extensor carpi radialis muscle
- FDI
the first dorsal interosseous muscle
- FCR
the flexor carpi radialis muscle
- FPB
the flexor pollicis brevis muscle
- LTP
long-term potentiation
- LTD
long-term depression
- NIBS
Non-Invasive Brain Stimulation
- PRISMA
Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analysis checklist
- RMT
resting motor threshold
- TMS
transcranial magnetic stimulation
- TP
thenar and plantar muscle
- TES
transcranial electrical stimulation
- MEP
motor evoked potential.
Footnotes
Funding. This study was supported by Project Grant National Natural Science Foundation of China (Grants nos. 81774406, 81774394, 81804167, and 81904297), the Qi-Huang Scholar of National Traditional Chinese Medicine Leading Talents Support Program (2018) and 2012A061400017, 2014GKXM031, 2017KYTD03, 2017B030314143, 2017KCXTD006 from the Department of Education of Guangdong Province, China.
Supplementary Material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fncel.2019.00469/full#supplementary-material
References
- Ahmed R. M., Ke Y. D., Vucic S., Ittner L. M., Seeley W., Hodges J. R., et al. (2018). Physiological changes in neurodegeneration - mechanistic insights and clinical utility. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 14, 259–271. 10.1038/nrneurol.2018.23 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ballard J. (2010). Forgetfulness and older adults: concept analysis. J. Adv. Nurs. 66, 1409–1419. 10.1111/j.1365-2648.2010.05279.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barker A. T., Jalinous R., Freeston I. L. (1985). Non-invasive magnetic stimulation of human motor cortex. Lancet 1, 1106–1107. 10.1016/S0140-6736(85)92413-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bashir S., Perez J. M., Horvath J. C., Pena-Gomez C., Vernet M., Capia A., et al. (2014). Differential effects of motor cortical excitability and plasticity in young and old individuals: a Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (TMS) study. Front. Aging Neurosci. 6:111. 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00111 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Becker J. A., Hedden T., Carmasin J., Maye J., Rentz D. M., Putcha D., et al. (2011). Amyloid-beta associated cortical thinning in clinically normal elderly. Ann. Neurol. 69, 1032–1042. 10.1002/ana.22333 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bennett I. J., Madden D. J. (2014). Disconnected aging: cerebral white matter integrity and age-related differences in cognition. Neuroscience 276, 187–205. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.11.026 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bernard J. A., Seidler R. D. (2012). Evidence for motor cortex dedifferentiation in older adults. Neurobiol. Aging 33, 1890–1899. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.06.021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bolandzadeh N., Davis J. C., Tam R., Handy T. C., Liu-Ambrose T. (2012). The association between cognitive function and white matter lesion location in older adults: a systematic review. BMC Neurol. 12:126. 10.1186/1471-2377-12-126 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boroojerdi B., Ziemann U., Chen R., Bütefisch C. M., Cohen L. G. (2001). Mechanisms underlying human motor system plasticity. Muscle Nerve 24, 602–613. 10.1002/mus.1045 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brunso-Bechtold J. K., Linville M. C., Sonntag W. E. (2000). Age-related synaptic changes in sensorimotor cortex of the Brown Norway X fischer 344 rat. Brain Res. 872, 125–133. 10.1016/S0006-8993(00)02515-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burke S. N., Barnes C. A. (2006). Neural plasticity in the ageing brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 30–40. 10.1038/nrn1809 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Celsis P. (2000). Age-related cognitive decline, mild cognitive impairment or preclinical Alzheimer's disease? Ann. Med. 32, 6–14. 10.3109/07853890008995904 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cirillo J., Rogasch N. C., Semmler J. G. (2010). Hemispheric differences in use-dependent corticomotor plasticity in young and old adults. Exp. Brain Res. 205, 57–68. 10.1007/s00221-010-2332-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cirillo J., Todd G., Semmler J. (2011). Corticomotor excitability and plasticity following complex visuomotor training in young and old adults. Eur. J. Neurosci. 34, 1847–1856. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07870.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Craik F. I., Bialystok E. (2006). Planning and task management in older adults: cooking breakfast. Mem. Cognit. 34, 1236–1249. 10.3758/BF03193268 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cuypers K., Thijs H., Duque J., Swinnen S. P., Levin O., Meesen R. L. (2013). Age-related differences in corticospinal excitability during a choice reaction time task. Age 35, 1705–1719. 10.1007/s11357-012-9471-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dayan E., Censor N., Buch E. R., Sandrini M., Cohen L. G. (2013). Noninvasive brain stimulation: from physiology to network dynamics and back. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 838–844. 10.1038/nn.3422 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeCarli C., Kawas C., Morrison J. H., Reuter-Lorenz P. A., Sperling R. A., Wright C. B. (2012). Session II: mechanisms of age-related cognitive change and targets for intervention: neural circuits, networks, and plasticity. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 67, 747–753. 10.1093/gerona/gls111 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Degardin A., Devos D., Cassim F., Bourriez J. L., Defebvre L., Derambure P., et al. (2011). Deficit of sensorimotor integration in normal aging. Neurosci. Lett. 498, 208–212. 10.1016/j.neulet.2011.05.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dickerson B. C., Bakkour A., Salat D. H., Feczko E., Pacheco J., Greve D. N., et al. (2009). The cortical signature of Alzheimer's disease: regionally specific cortical thinning relates to symptom severity in very mild to mild AD dementia and is detectable in asymptomatic amyloid-positive individuals. Cereb. Cortex. 19, 497–510. 10.1093/cercor/bhn113 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dickins D. S. E., Kamke M. R., Sale M. V. (2017). Corticospinal plasticity in bilateral primary motor cortices induced by paired associative stimulation to the dominant hemisphere does not differ between young and older adults. Neural Plast. 2017, 1–14. 10.1155/2017/8319049 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dickins D. S. E., Sale M. V., Kamke M. R. (2015). Plasticity induced by intermittent theta burst stimulation in bilateral motor cortices is not altered in older adults. Neural Plast. 2015, 1–9. 10.1155/2015/323409 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dukart J., Mueller K., Villringer A., Kherif F., Draganski B., Frackowiak R., et al. (2013). Relationship between imaging biomarkers, age, progression and symptom severity in Alzheimer's disease. Neuroimage Clin. 3, 84–94. 10.1016/j.nicl.2013.07.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Edwards M. J., Talelli P., Rothwell J. C. (2008). Clinical applications of transcranial magnetic stimulation in patients with movement disorders. Lancet Neurol. 7, 827–840. 10.1016/S1474-4422(08)70190-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Emonson M. R. L., Fitzgerald P. B., Rogasch N. C., Hoy K. E. (2019). Neurobiological effects of transcranial direct current stimulation in younger adults, older adults and mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychologia 125, 51–61. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.01.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fathi D., Ueki Y., Mima T., Koganemaru S., Nagamine T., Tawfik A., et al. (2010). Effects of aging on the human motor cortical plasticity studied by paired associative stimulation. Clin. Neurophysiol. 121, 90–93. 10.1016/j.clinph.2009.07.048 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fujiyama H., Garry M. I., Levin O., Swinnen S. P., Summers J. J. (2009). Age-related differences in inhibitory processes during interlimb coordination. Brain Res. 1262, 38–47. 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.01.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fujiyama H., Hinder M. R., Schmidt M. W., Garry M. I., Summers J. J. (2012). Age-related differences in corticospinal excitability and inhibition during coordination of upper and lower limbs. Neurobiol. Aging 33, 1484.e1–1484.e14. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.12.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fujiyama H., Hyde J., Hinder M. R., Kim S. J., McCormack G. H., Vickers J. C., et al. (2014). Delayed plastic responses to anodal tDCS in older adults. Front. Aging Neurosci. 6:115. 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00115 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fujiyama H., Tandonnet C., Summers J. J. (2011). Age-related differences in corticospinal excitability during a Go/NoGo task. Psychophysiology. 48, 1448–1455. 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01201.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Godde B., Berkefeld T., David-Jürgens M., Dinse H. R. (2002). Age-related changes in primary somatosensory cortex of rats: evidence for parallel degenerative and plastic-adaptive processes. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 26, 743–752. 10.1016/S0149-7634(02)00061-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Higgins J. P., Thompson S. G., Deeks J. J., Altman D. G. (2003). Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ 327, 557–560. 10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hortobágyi T., del Olmo M. F., Rothwell J. C. (2006). Age reduces cortical reciprocal inhibition in humans. Exp. Brain Res. 171, 322–329. 10.1007/s00221-005-0274-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hunter S. K., Thompson W., Roger D. A. (2001). Reaction time, strength, and physical activity in women aged 20–89 years. J. Aging Phys. Act. 9, 32–42. 10.1123/japa.9.1.32 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Iadecola C. (2010). The overlap between neurodegenerative and vascular factors in the pathogenesis of dementia. Acta Neuropathol. 120, 287–296. 10.1007/s00401-010-0718-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kossev A. R., Schrader C., Däuper J., Dengler R., Rollnik J. D. (2002). Increased intracortical inhibition in middle-aged humans; a study using paired-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neurosci. Lett. 333, 83–86. 10.1016/S0304-3940(02)00986-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levin O., Cuypers K., Netz Y., Thijs H., Nuttin B., Helsen W. F., et al. (2011). Age-related differences in human corticospinal excitability during simple reaction time. Neurosci. Lett. 487, 53–57. 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.09.072 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li H. J., Hou X. H., Liu H. H., Yue C. L., Lu G. M., Zuo X. N. (2015). Putting age-related task activation into large-scale brain networks: a meta-analysis of 114 fMRI studies on healthy aging. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 57, 156–174. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.08.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Logsdon R. G., Gibbons L. E., McCurry S. M., Teri L. (2002). Assessing quality of life in older adults with cognitive impairment. Psychosom. Med. 64, 510–519. 10.1097/00006842-200205000-00016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mora F., Segovia G., del Arco A. (2007). Aging, plasticity and environmental enrichment: structural changes and neurotransmitter dynamics in several areas of the brain. Brain Res. Rev. 55, 78–88. 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2007.03.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morrison J. H., Baxter M. G. (2012). The ageing cortical synapse: hallmarks and implications for cognitive decline. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 240–250. 10.1038/nrn3200 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Müller-Dahlhaus F., Ziemann U., Classen J. (2010). Plasticity resembling spike-timing dependent synaptic plasticity: the evidence in human cortex. Front. Synaptic Neurosci. 2:34. 10.3389/fnsyn.2010.00034 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oliviero A., Profice P., Tonali P. A., Pilato F., Saturno E., Dileone M., et al. (2006). Effects of aging on motor cortex excitability. Neurosci Res. 55, 74–77. 10.1016/j.neures.2006.02.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Opie G. M., Cirillo J., Semmler J. G. (2018). Age-related changes in late I-waves influence motor cortex plasticity induction in older adults. J. Physiol. (Lond). 596, 2597–2609. 10.1113/JP274641 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Opie G. M., Post A. K., Ridding M. C., Ziemann U., Semmler J. G. (2017). Modulating motor cortical neuroplasticity with priming paired associative stimulation in young and old adults. Clin. Neurophysiol. 128, 763–769. 10.1016/j.clinph.2017.02.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Opie G. M., Semmler J. G. (2014). Age-related differences in short- and long-interval intracortical inhibition in a human hand muscle. Brain Stimul. 7, 665–672. 10.1016/j.brs.2014.06.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pellicciari M. C., Miniussi C., Rossini P. M., De Gennaro L. (2009). Increased cortical plasticity in the elderly: changes in the somatosensory cortex after paired associative stimulation. Neuroscience 163, 266–276. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.06.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rogasch N. C., Dartnall T. J., Cirillo J., Nordstrom M. A., Semmler J. G. (2009). Corticomotor plasticity and learning of a ballistic thumb training task are diminished in older adults. J. Appl. Physiol. 107, 1874–1883. 10.1152/japplphysiol.00443.2009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rossini P. M., Desiato M. T., Caramia M. D. (1992). Age-related changes of motor evoked potentials in healthy humans: non-invasive evaluation of central and peripheral motor tracts excitability and conductivity. Brain Res. 593, 14–19. 10.1016/0006-8993(92)91256-E [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rossini P. M., Rossi S., Babiloni C., Polich J. (2007). Clinical neurophysiology of aging brain: from normal aging to neurodegeneration. Prog Neurobiol. 83, 375–400. 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2007.07.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sale M. V., Semmler J. G. (2005). Age-related differences in corticospinal control during functional isometric contractions in left and right hands. J. Appl. Physiol. 99, 1483–1493. 10.1152/japplphysiol.00371.2005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanes J. N., Donoghue J. P. (2000). Plasticity and primary motor cortex. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 23, 393–415. 10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.393 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Savović J., Weeks L., Sterne J. A., Turner L., Altman D. G., Moher D., et al. (2014). Evaluation of the Cochrane Collaboration's tool for assessing the risk of bias in randomized trials: focus groups, online survey, proposed recommendations and their implementation. Syst. Rev. 3:37. 10.1186/2046-4053-3-37 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sawaki L., Yaseen Z., Kopylev L., Cohen L. G. (2003). Age-dependent changes in the ability to encode a novel elementary motor memory. Ann. Neurol. 53, 521–524. 10.1002/ana.10529 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scahill R. I., Frost C., Jenkins R., Whitwell J. L., Rossor M. N., Fox N. C. (2003). A longitudinal study of brain volume changes in normal aging using serial registered magnetic resonance imaging. Arch. Neurol. 60, 989–994. 10.1001/archneur.60.7.989 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seidler R. D., Bernard J. A., Burutolu T. B., Fling B. W., Gordon M. T., Gwin J. T., et al. (2010). Motor control and aging: links to age-related brain structural, functional, and biochemical effects. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 34, 721–733. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.10.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith A. E., Ridding M. C., Higgins R. D., Wittert G. A., Pitcher J. B. (2009). Age-related changes in short-latency motor cortex inhibition. Experimental Brain Research. 198, 489–500. 10.1007/s00221-009-1945-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith A. E., Ridding M. C., Higgins R. D., Wittert G. A., Pitcher J. B. (2011). Cutaneous afferent input does not modulate motor intracortical inhibition in ageing men. Euro. J. Neurosci. 34, 1461–1469. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07869.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sousa A. M. M., Meyer K. A., Santpere G., Gulden F. O., Sestan N. (2017). Evolution of the human nervous system function, structure, and development. Cell 170, 226–247. 10.1016/j.cell.2017.06.036 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sperling R. A., Laviolette P. S., O'Keefe K., O'Brien J., Rentz D. M., Pihlajamaki M., et al. (2009). Amyloid deposition is associated with impaired default network function in older persons without dementia. Neuron 63, 178–188. 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.07.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tatti E., Rossi S., Innocenti I., Rossi A., Santarnecchi E. (2016). Non-invasive brain stimulation of the aging brain: state of the art and future perspectives. Ageing Res. Rev. 29, 66–89. 10.1016/j.arr.2016.05.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Todd G., Kimber T. E., Ridding M. C., Semmler J. G. (2010). Reduced motor cortex plasticity following inhibitory rTMS in older adults. Clin. Neurophysiol. 121, 441–447. 10.1016/j.clinph.2009.11.089 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ward N. S., Swayne O. B., Newton J. M. (2008). Age-dependent changes in the neural correlates of force modulation: an fMRI study. Neurobiol. Aging 29, 1434–1446. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2007.04.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wellman C. L., Sengelaub D. R. (1995). Alterations in dendritic morphology of frontal cortical neurons after basal forebrain lesions in adult and aged rats. Brain Res. 669, 48–58. 10.1016/0006-8993(94)01231-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Young-Bernier M., Davidson P. S., Tremblay F. (2012). Paired-pulse afferent modulation of TMS responses reveals a selective decrease in short latency afferent inhibition with age. Neurobiol Aging 33, 835.e1–835.e11. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.08.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Young-Bernier M., Tanguay A. N., Davidson P. S., Tremblay F. (2014). Short-latency afferent inhibition is a poor predictor of individual susceptibility to rTMS-induced plasticity in the motor cortex of young and older adults. Front. Aging Neurosci. 6:182. 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00182 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The data supporting the results reported in this article may be obtained from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.