Abstract
This article examined the differences in causes and health consequences between synthetic drug and heroin abuse in urban China. Two-group comparisons were conducted to quantify differences in individual characteristics, causes of drug use, and HIV/STI risky sexual behavior between synthetic drug and heroin users; logistic regressions were employed to assess the net effect of synthetic drug use on risky sexual behavior. Results revealed that causes of synthetic drug use differed from those of heroin use; a combination of the knowledge gap concerning the harmful impact of synthetic drugs and the lesser punishment for their use appeared a main reason behind the shift from heroin to synthetic drugs; and synthetic drug use was a significant and powerful risk factor for HIV/STI risky sexual behavior. Educational and behavioral interventions are urgently needed to prevent the initiation of synthetic drug use among users to reduce their HIV/STI risky sexual behavior.
Keywords: causes and consequences of drug abuse, synthetic drug, heroin, HIV/STI risk behavior, China
Drug abuse as a widespread social and health problem in China dated back to the late 18th century (Lu, Fang, & Wang, 2008; Tang, Zhao, Zhao, & Cubells, 2006). By the early 20th century, China was home to more than 85% of opium addicts worldwide and the world’s largest opium producer (United Nations Office on Drugs and Crime [UNODC], 2008). Widespread opium abuse continued in China until the establishment of the People’s Republic in 1949. The government launched a series of nationwide campaigns in the early 1950s to fight against the demon of opium (Lowinger, 1977), which rid China free of illicit drugs for the next three decades.
Since the 1980s, however, the government’s ability to control illicit drugs in China has been compromised amid profound social and economic changes unleashed by the open-door policy and the ensuing market transition (Lu et al., 2008; Yang, 2006). A combination of increasing urbanization and residential mobility, growing respect for individual rights and privacy, and widespread corruption has greatly undermined the traditional values and the effectiveness of social control over individual behavior (Wang, 2004; Yang, 2000; Yang & Luo, 2009). Not surprisingly, China has seen a resurgence of drug abuse. The resurface of illicit drugs, mainly opium and heroin, started in the remote rural areas and among ethnic minority populations but quickly evolved into a nationwide epidemic; smoking of drugs was soon replaced by injection drug use (Lai et al., 2000; Lu et al., 2008; Qian, Schumacher, Chen, & Ruan, 2006; Zheng et al., 1994). More recently, there has been a dramatic increase in the abuse of various synthetic drugs (Fang, Wang, Shi, Liu, & Lu, 2006), which are broadly defined as chemical compounds produced in a laboratory using chemicals rather than natural ingredients (Xia & Yang, 2017). While the list of synthetic drugs considered illicit (if used for nonmedical reasons) in China is long, the most common includes methamphetamine (Bingdu), ketamine (K fen), and ecstasy/MDMA (Yaotou Wan), which are quickly replacing heroin to become the most prevalent drugs abused in urban China (Fang et al., 2006; Han, 2007; Li, Zhang, & Liu, 2008; National Narcotics Control Commission [NNCC], 2012; Wang & Liu, 2007; Xia & Yang, 2017). In this article, synthetic drugs are defined as any illicit drugs on the list of banned drugs other than heroin.
Unlike that of heroin, the abuse of synthetic drugs is believed to stimulate sexual activity, contribute to unsafe sex, and increase the risk of sexually acquiring and/or transmitting HIV and other sexually transmitted infections (STIs; Bao et al., 2015; Colfax & Guzman, 2006; Liao et al., 2014; Xu et al., 2014). This may help explain that unsafe sex has taken over injection drug use to become the dominant route of HIV transmission in China. Sexual transmission of HIV is projected to be the decisive factor in the future course of the HIV epidemic in China (Merli, Hertog, Wang, & Li, 2006).
Despite the emerging synthetic drug epidemic and the often-assumed increased sexual risk of HIV/STIs from the abuse of synthetic drugs, we know very little of what may have caused the shift in the main drugs of abuse in China, and little empirical research has directly compared sexual risk behavior between heroin and synthetic drug users while controlling for potentially confounding factors. This article attempts to address this research gap. Using survey data from samples of synthetic drug and heroin users in Shanghai, we will examine differences in causes and sexual risk behavior between synthetic drug and heroin users. To the extent possible, qualitative data from the formative research phase of an on-going survey research will be used to supplement the survey data.
BACKGROUND
THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES ON DRUG USE
Research to understand why some people abuse drugs while others do not has generated several theoretical perspectives on the social and behavioral causes of drug abuse (Payne & Gainey, 2005). However, the literature is generally silent on any potential differences between synthetic drug and heroin use. We will compare causes of synthetic drug and heroin use on three broader perspectives.
Social Control and Drug Use.
The basic premise of social control theory (Black, 1984; Gibbs, 1982; Hirschi, 1969) is that we are born with the tendency to pursue pleasure, which is tempered by formal and normative structures that shape behavior (Coser, 1982; Nagasawa, Qian, & Wong, 2000). As use of drugs could be pleasurable, individuals may be inclined towards drug use, but avoid it for fear of social sanctions. Ultimately, individuals’ perception of the likelihood/severity of social sanctions (felt social control) and their attitudes about possible social sanctions will determine the likelihood of drug use. Subjective evaluation of the likelihood and severity of social sanctions is influenced by social bonds, which are embedded in people’s attachment to others, commitment to institutions, involvement in normal activities, and beliefs in social norms/rules (Hirschi, 1969). The stronger such social bonds are, the stronger the felt social control, and consequently the less likely that individuals will indulge in drugs.
Social Learning and Drug Use.
Social learning theory emphasizes social influences of behavior and argues that drug use is not inborn but socially learned (Akers, 1985, 1998). Such social learning occurs through differential association, imitation, and differential reinforcement. An important risk factor for drug use is the presence of drug users in one’s social networks (Bahr, Hoffmann, & Yang, 2005; Buchana & Latkin, 2008). Other things being equal, the more drug users there are in the network, the more likely that the individual will learn the same behavior.
Sensation Seeking and Drug Use.
While social control and social learning focus on interpersonal factors as moderators or facilitators of individual behavior, sensation seeking perspective emphasizes intrapersonal traits as contributing factors of drug abuse (Newcomb & Earleywine, 1996). Instead of looking to external forces, it emphasizes factors within individuals. Sensation seeking is a set of personality traits characterized by a drive for novel and complex thrills and willingness to take risks to seek sensations/thrills (Roberti, 2004; Yanovitzky, 2006; Zuckerman, 1994). As drug use could be pleasurable and socially risky, and generates a sense of thrill (Wood, Cochran, Pfefferbaum, & Arneklev, 1995), it is linked to individuals’ level of sensation seeking. The higher the level of sensation seeking, the more likely individuals will seek the thrill of drugs.
SYNTHETIC DRUG USE AND RISKY SEXUAL BEHAVIOR
There is compelling evidence that methamphetamine (MA) use stimulates sexual activity (Corsi & Booth, 2008; Green, 2003; Kurtz, 2005; Zule, Costenbader, Meyer, & Wechsberg, 2007). In fact, a major reason to use MA may be to enhance sexual ability (Diaz, Heckert, & Sánchez, 2005). Use of MA is associated with the release of neurotransmitters in the brain, which increases sexual desire and reduces sexual inhibitions (Anglin, Burke, Perrochet, Stamper, & Dawud-Noursi, 2000; Halkitis, Fischgrund, & Parsons, 2005), leading to an increased number of partners and amount/duration of sex (Drumright, Patterson, & Strathdee, 2006). It also may lead to deficits in impulse control and impaired decision-making, causing users to prefer immediate reward at the expense of severe future consequences (Hanson, Luciana, & Sullwold, 2008; Homer, Solomon, Moeller, Mascia, DeRaleau, & Halkitis, 2008; Verdejo-Garcia, Pérez-Garcia, & Bechara, 2006). Increased sexual impulsivity can in turn lead to unsafe sex (Semple, Zians, Grant, & Patterson, 2005, 2006).
Likewise, ecstasy is believed to increase the release of serotonin and block serotonin re-uptake (Freese, Miotto, & Reback, 2002; Uys & Niesink, 2005). Its use is associated with feelings of sensuality, emotional closeness, sexual arousal, and reduced sexual inhibitions. Use of ecstasy is also linked to high level of impulsivity and impaired decision-making and in turn risky sex (Butler & Montgomery, 2004; Morgan, Impallomeni, Pirona, & Rogers, 2006; Schilder, Lampinen, Miller, & Hogg, 2005; Stephens, Holliday, & Jarboe, 2015).
The use of ketamine is associated with mental distortion, loss of memory, and muscle control (Drumright et al., 2006). Ketamine acts at the N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor to affect the neurochemistry of behavior and sensory information (Colfax & Guzman, 2006; Freese et al., 2002). Its use produces strong hallucinogenic and euphoric effects; users may experience mood elevation, amnesia, cognitive impairment, perception distortion, and a state of sedation. Because of its mind/body-altering effects, ketamine has been dubbed a rape drug. Its use has been associated with unprotected sex (Drumright et al., 2006; Smith, Larive, & Romanelli, 2002). Decreased sensation of pain also may lead to more traumatic sex that increases tissue damage and blood/semen contact (Drumright et al., 2006).
Polydrug use, highly prevalent among synthetic drug users (Halkitis & Palamar, 2008; Halkitis, Palamar, & Mukherjee, 2007; O’Grady, Arria, Fitzelle, & Wish, 2008), can further intensify the effects of drugs and increase risky sexual behavior and other adverse health consequences (Hirshfield, Schrimshaw, Stall, Margolis, Downing, & Chiasson, 2015; Parsons, Grov, & Golub, 2012; Stall et al., 2003; Yu, Wall, Chiasson, & Hirshfield, 2015). Studies indicate that polydrug use is preferred by users to achieve a desired high (Uys & Niesink, 2005). The interaction of the stimulant and depressive effects of different drugs may help achieve a high that is more intense and enjoyable and lasts longer than that of any single drug. Polydrug use also is associated with high levels of impulsivity and in turn unsafe sex (Butler & Montgomery, 2004; Parsons et al., 2012; Semple et al., 2005).
In addition to the pharmacological impact of synthetic drugs, the group or venue settings, which are more likely to be associated with synthetic drug use, are also believed to be conducive to risky sexual behaviors (Laidler, 2005; Maxwell, 2005). Evidence suggests that synthetic drug use is more prevalent in entertainment venues, such as dance clubs and discos, or social group gatherings in China (Xia & Yang, 2008) and worldwide (Calafat, Gómez, Juan, & Becoña, 2007; Mattison, Ross, Wolfson, Franklin, & HNRC Group, 2001; Parks & Kennedy, 2004), which is found to foster frequent polydrug use (Fendrich, Wislar, Johnson, & Hubbell, 2003; Klitzman, 2006; Lankenau & Clatts, 2005; Operario et al., 2006) and an increase in sexual partners and the likelihood of group sex (Fendrich et al., 2003; Zule et al., 2007). The environment of many entertainment venues, such as hot temperatures, loud music, and overcrowding, can further intensify the stimulating effect of synthetic drugs (Calafat, Juan, Becoña, Mantecón, & Ramón, 2009; Parrott, 2004).
DATA AND METHODS
This article is a secondary data analysis of two pre-existing companion surveys of synthetic drug and heroin users. The surveys were conducted in 2006 and 2007 in Shanghai using one-to-one private and confidential questionnaires and in-depth interviews by trained interviewers. The main purpose of the surveys was to collect empirical data to better understand the patterns and trends of drug abuse in Shanghai (Xia & Yang, 2017). Research protocols were reviewed and approved by the Research and Academic Integrity Committee (equivalent to an institutional review board in the United States) at the Shanghai Academy of Social Sciences for the protection of human subjects in research with a focus on protection of participants’ drug use status and their otherwise entitled benefits in community and particularly in an institutional setting (detention or treatment/rehab facility).
Because synthetic drug and heroin users were subject to different legal punishments, they were sampled separately from different sources. Details of the surveys have been described elsewhere (Yang & Xia, 2010; Xia & Yang, 2017). Briefly, synthetic drug users were sampled from those who were administratively detained (Zian Juliu or detained for violating government ordinances) or receiving compulsive drug treatment and rehabilitation. The interviewers consisted of researchers, research assistants, and graduate students from the Shanghai Academy of Social Sciences and/or the Shanghai Center for Drug Reaction Surveillance (Shanghai Yaopin Buliang Fanying Jiance Zhongxin). All the interviewers had sociology, pharmacology, or public health education background; they were further trained in protection of human subjects in research, emphasizing voluntary participation and no harm to their well-being in an institutional environment. During the field work, the trained interviewers randomly visited the police stations and compulsive drug treatment centers around the city when new detainees were brought in. Heroin users were randomly sampled from the rosters of the city’s compulsory rehab facilities.
For both samples, the trained interviewers approached each detainee and invited him/her for a 30-minute private and face-to-face interview in a private room without the presence of any staff members from the detention/rehab center. The interviewer first checked for eligibility. To be eligible, the potential participant must be a synthetic drug or heroin user, aged 18 and over, and able to give informed consent to participate in the study. The interviewers then explained to the eligible participants the purpose of the study and informed them that the interview would be one-to-one in a private room between him/her and the interviewer only. They were also informed of potential risks, including discomfort when asked personal questions about drugs and sexual behavior by a stranger and potential breach of confidentiality by the interviewer, and their rights to refuse to participate or to answer any specific questions. Potential participants were assured of confidentiality and shown that the questionnaire contained no name or any other identifying information; they were further assured that the data collected would not be shared with the detention/rehab center and would not affect their otherwise entitled benefits and rights in the center. Verbal consent was obtained from 730 synthetic drug users and 705 heroin users, who subsequently completed the private face-to-face interview.
In addition to the quantitative survey data, whenever possible, qualitative information from the key informant in-depth interviews and focus groups in the formative phase of an ongoing research project in China (hereafter “qualitative data”) will be used in the analysis to supplement the survey data in discussion and interpretation of the findings.
The two samples will be merged for use in data analysis; answers to the survey question of “what drugs you often use,” which allowed the respondents to report all drugs they used, will be used to classify respondents as synthetic drug or heroin users. A synthetic drug user is a respondent who answered to have used any drugs other than heroin. Few heroin users were polydrug users, but many synthetic drug users reported to have used more than one synthetic drug. Differences between single and polydrug users have been examined elsewhere (Yang & Xia, 2010). This paper will focus on comparison between synthetic drug and heroin users.
Data analysis will use STATA version 12 (StataCorp, 2011) and proceed in two parts. The first part will focus on bivariate comparisons between synthetic drug and heroin users in sociodemographic characteristics, drug use settings, post-use physiological and psychological characteristics, self-reported reasons for using the drug, and risky sexual behaviors. For categorical variables, Pearson’s chi-squared test of independence in crosstabulation will be used; for ratio variables, Wald’s F test of equality of means between two-groups after mean estimation will be employed (StataCorp, 2011). The results will provide bivariate tests if synthetic drug users differ significantly from heroin users in both individual characteristics and causes and consequences of their drug use. In the second part of data analysis, we will use logistic regression to further assess the net effect of type of drug used and drug use setting on risky sexual behaviors.
MEASURES
All measures were based on self-reports; most are self-descriptive and need no further explanation. For causes of drug use, three composite index variables were created to represent the three broader theoretical perspectives. Whenever feasible, additional variables were used to supplement the index variables. For perceived social control, we used as a proxy personal experience or involvement in events/behaviors indicating disrespect for social norms and rules; respondents reported (1 for yes and 2 for no) on nine events/behaviors (e.g., fighting in public; riding bus without a ticket). Answers were summed across the nine events to form a social control composite index. Ranging in value from 9 to 18, the higher the index value, the lesser the likelihood that the respondent had personal experience/involvement in any of the nine events, indicating stronger perceived social control. Cronbach’s alpha is 0.71 for the index variable. The index variable will be supplemented with self-report of relationship to parents and agree/disagree with statements reflecting commitment to institutions (going to school is useless and work to earn a living is boring), beliefs in social norms/rules (life does not have to be serious and one cannot always follow rules), and the extent of involvement in normal activities (feelings of aimless and empty/boredom in life and the need to find things to do to release extra energy).
For social influence, the index variable was based on respondents’ answers on a five-point scale (1 for strongly disagree to 5 for strongly agree) on nine statements (e.g., I always try to copy my friends; I often spend my leisure time with friends). The answers were summed across the nine statements to form the composite index. Ranging in value from 9 to 45, the higher the index, the more the respondent was potentially amenable to peer influences. Cronbach’s alpha is 0.71 for the social influence index variable. The index variable will be supplemented with two dichotomous variables: having drug-using friends before own use and reporting peer, family, or environmental influences as reasons for drug use.
For sensation-seeking, the index variable was formed using respondents’ answers (1 for disagree and 2 for agree) on eight statements (e.g., my life is OK but lacks excitement, and I like to play something new and thrill). The eight 1/2 answers were summed to create the sensation-seeking index. Ranging in value from 8 to 16, the higher the index, the stronger the sensation-seeking tendency the respondent had. Cronbach’s alpha is 0.73 for the index variable. In addition, self-reporting satisfying curiosity and seeking pleasure/excitement as reasons for drug use will be used to supplement the index variable and to capture directly sensation seeking as the cause of drug use.
For consequences of drug use, three dichotomous variables were used to measure percentage of self-reporting post-use physiological dependence, psychological dependence, and destructive behavior. Risky sexual behavior was measured by (1) ever having unprotected sex with a non-stable partner, which includes anyone other than spouse; (2) having unprotected sex with a non-stable partner in the past 30 days; (3) having unprotected sex with a non-stable partner in the past three sexual intercourses, and (4) ever having group sex following use of the drug. In addition, incidence of STIs after starting drug use will be used as a proxy for post-drug use during unprotected sex to supplement the four direct measures of unprotected sex.
RESULTS
Of the list of 107 government-banned drugs at the time of the surveys, methamphetamine, ketamine, and ecstasy stood out as the main drugs used in Shanghai; a majority (95.9%) of synthetic drug users in our sample used one or more of the three main synthetic drugs. On average, synthetic drug users were younger (32.6 vs. 35.3 years old), better educated (43.3% vs. 36.9% having high school or more education), and less likely to be migrants (23.0% vs. 31.8%) than heroin users; they did not significantly differ from heroin users in gender composition, marital status, or employment status (Table 1). Compared to heroin users, synthetic drug users started using drugs at older ages (30.1 vs. 27.4 years old) and averaged much shorter duration (2.5 vs. 7.9 years) since first use with the drugs. Synthetic drug users were significantly more likely to be polydrug users (44.5% vs. 5.3%) and use drugs in entertainment venues than heroin users (65.1% vs. 11.6%); they were significantly more likely to describe feelings of agitation, including sexual arousal, but less likely to report feelings of calm, comfort, and sleepiness following use of drug.
TABLE 1.
Synthetic Drug Users | Heroin Users | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
% or Mean | n | % or Mean | n | |
Sociodemographic characteristics: | ||||
Male (%) | 73.7 | 538 | 71.2 | 502 |
Age (mean in years) | 32.6** | 729 | 35.3 | 705 |
Married (%) | 40.6 | 296 | 42.8 | 302 |
High school or more education (%) | 43.3* | 316 | 36.9 | 260 |
Employed (%) | 54.5 | 398 | 55.1 | 386 |
Temporary migrant (%) | 23.0** | 168 | 31.8 | 224 |
Drug use characteristics: | ||||
Age first use (mean) | 30.1** | 726 | 27.4 | 705 |
Years since first use (mean) | 2.5** | 726 | 7.9 | 705 |
Polydrug use (%) | 44.5** | 319 | 5.3 | 37 |
Use drugs in entertainment venue (%) | 65.1** | 475 | 11.6 | 82 |
Post-use feeling agitated (%)a | 66.2** | 483 | 24.4 | 172 |
Post-use feeling relaxed (%)b | 38.5** | 281 | 80.1 | 565 |
Note.
Includes self-reported post-use feelings of excited/madness, sexual arousal, wanted to vent, and sleepless/talkative;
includes self-reported post-use feelings of carefree/worry-free, very comfortable, and sleepy.
p < .05.
p < .01.
In terms of likely causes for their drug use (Table 2), synthetic drug users scored higher on the social control index measure (16.0 vs. 15.6) than heroin users, indicating that synthetic drug users had experienced fewer events of disrespect of social norms and rules than heroin users. However, none of the other measures of social control failed to separate synthetic drug users from heroin users. While synthetic drug users were less likely than heroin users to self-report peer, family, or environmental influences as reasons for using drugs, they appeared to have been subjected to greater social influence than heroin users. Not only were synthetic drug users more likely to report having drug-using friends before their own use, among those who had drug-using friends synthetic drug users had more drug-using friends than heroin users (2.6 vs. 0.8). With respect to tendency of sensation seeking, neither measure showed a statistically significant difference between synthetic drug and heroin users (Table 2).
TABLE 2.
Synthetic Drug Users |
Heroin Users |
|||
---|---|---|---|---|
% or Mean | n | % or Mean | n | |
Social control: | ||||
Composite social control index variable (mean) | 16.0** | 729 | 15.6 | 705 |
Having good/very good relationship to parents (%) | 72.6 | 530 | 74.2 | 523 |
Felt going to school is useless (%) | 44.8 | 323 | 43.2 | 304 |
Felt working to earn a living is pointless/boring (%) | 43.1 | 311 | 39.3 | 276 |
Agree that life does not need to be serious (%) | 60.7 | 438 | 63.9 | 449 |
Agree that one cannot always follow rules (%) | 66.5 | 480 | 64.5 | 454 |
Felt life aimless and empty (%) | 45.6 | 329 | 41.6 | 293 |
Felt the need to release extra energy (%) | 51.7 | 374 | 50.1 | 353 |
Social influence: | ||||
Composite social influence index variable (mean) | 27.1** | 725 | 26.1 | 705 |
Had drug using friends before own use (%) | 55.0** | 377 | 44.7 | 304 |
Number of drug using friends (mean) | 2.6** | 686 | 0.8 | 680 |
Peer/family/setting influence as reasons to use (%)a | 67.7** | 494 | 70.6 | 498 |
Sensation seeking: | ||||
Composite sensation seeking index variable (mean) | 12.9 | 724 | 13.0 | 705 |
Sensation seeking as reason to use (%)b | 77.5 | 566 | 78.9 | 556 |
Note.
Includes self-reported peer, family, or environmental influence as reasons for drug use;
includes self-reported curiosity and sensation seeking as reason for drug use.
p < .01.
As Table 3 reveals, synthetic drug users self-reported significantly less post drug-use feelings of dependence than heroin users (11.8% vs. 75.9% felt physiological dependence and 35.2% vs. 85.7% psychological dependence). However, they did not differ significantly from heroin users in any destructive behavior following the use of drugs. In terms of risky sexual behavior, all five measures indicated significant differences between synthetic drug users and heroin users. Compared to heroin users, synthetic drug users were more likely to have unprotected sex with a non-stable partner in their lifetime (54.1% vs. 34.6%), in the 30 days prior to the interview (43.0% vs. 23.3%), and in the past three sexual intercourses (22.6% vs. 13.6%). Synthetic drug users were also significantly more likely to ever have group sex following drug use than heroin users (11.0% vs. 3.5%). In contrast to the four direct measures, all of which point to a riskier sexual life among synthetic drug users, the indirect measure of unprotected sex (self-report of any STIs since started using drugs) revealed that fewer synthetic drug users than heroin users (3.8% vs. 7.2%) reported STIs, suggesting indirectly a safer sexual life among synthetic drug users than heroin users.
TABLE 3.
Synthetic Drug Users |
Heroin Users |
|||
---|---|---|---|---|
% | n | % | n | |
Post-use dependence and behavior: | ||||
Physiological dependence (%) | 11.8** | 86 | 75.9 | 535 |
Psychological dependence (%) | 35.2** | 257 | 85.7 | 604 |
Destructive behavior (%) | 10.6 | 77 | 10.1 | 71 |
Risky sexual behavior: | ||||
Ever had unprotected sex (U.S.) with a non-stable partner (%) | 54.1** | 395 | 34.6 | 244 |
Had U.S. with a non-stable partner past 30 days (%) | 43.0** | 314 | 23.3 | 164 |
Had U.S. with a non-stable partner past 3 sexual experiences (%) | 22.6** | 165 | 13.6 | 96 |
Ever had group sex following drug use (%) | 11.0** | 76 | 3.5 | 24 |
STDs since starting drug use (%) | 3.8** | 28 | 7.2 | 51 |
p < .01.
Table 4 shows the logistic regression analysis of synthetic drug use as a risk factor of unprotected sex with a non-stable partner. For the lifetime measure and before controlling for drug-use related characteristics (Model 1), synthetic drug use was a significant risk factor. With sociodemographic characteristics and causes of drug use controlled for in the model, synthetic drug use more than doubled the odds ratio (OR = 2.39) of ever having unprotected sex with a non-stable partner (USWNP). Being male and young were associated with significantly higher and lower odds, respectively, of lifetime USWNP. All three composite measures of causes of drug use were also significantly associated with lifetime USWNP. Other things being equal, individuals with greater felt social control were associated with lower odds (OR = 0.82) while those who were more susceptible to social influence and who reported a greater sensation seeking trait were associated with significantly higher odds of lifetime USWNP.
TABLE 4.
Unprotected Sex With a Non-stable Partner |
||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Lifetime | Past 30 Days | Past 3 Sexual Experiences | ||||
Independent Variables | 1 | 2 | 1 | 2 | 1 | 2 |
Sociodemographic characteristics | ||||||
Malea | 1.47** | 1.49** | 1.19 | 1.12 | 3.74** | 3.51** |
Age | 0.97** | 0.99 | 0.97** | 0.96** | 1.00 | 1.02 |
Currently marrieda | 0.84 | 0.78* | 0.74* | 0.69** | 0.87 | 0.84 |
High school or more educationa | 0.91 | 0.87 | 0.76* | 0.73* | 0.97 | 0.90 |
Employeda | 0.94 | 0.91 | 1.12 | 1.09 | 0.84 | 0.78 |
Temporary migranta | 0.75b | 0.78 | 0.75b | 0.74b | 0.92 | 0.93 |
Causes of drug use | ||||||
Social control index variable | 0.82** | 0.84** | 0.83** | 0.85** | 0.78** | 0.80** |
Social influence index variable | 1.04** | 1.03** | 1.02 | 1.02 | 1.00 | 1.00 |
Sensation seeking index variable | 1.25** | 1.20** | 1.26** | 1.21** | 1.26** | 1.17** |
Drugs used and related factors | ||||||
Synthetic drug usera | 2.39** | 2.09** | 2.70** | 2.14** | 1.93** | 1.60b |
Age of first use of the drug | — | 0.98 | — | 1.01 | — | 0.98 |
Polydrug usera | — | 1.53* | — | 1.49* | — | 1.43b |
Ever had group sex after using drug | — | 2.16** | — | 2.35** | — | 4.23** |
Used drug in entertainment venues | — | 1.24 | — | 1.00 | — | 1.04 |
Sample size | 1423 | 1352 | 1423 | 1352 | 1423 | 1352 |
Model pseudo R2 | 0.14** | 0.15** | 0.13** | 0.15** | 0.15** | 0.19** |
Note. Results presented in the table are odds ratios. A non-stable partner is any heterosexual partner other than spouse, girl/boyfriend, or lover.
Entered as a dummy variable; the reference categories are female, currently not married, less than high school education, unemployed, non-migrant, heroin user single drug user for the corresponding dummy variables;
Significant at the 10% (p < .10) level.
p < .05.
p < .01.
When the four drug-use related characteristics were controlled for in Model 2, synthetic drug use remained a significant risk factor for lifetime USWNP, although the strength of the association was somewhat reduced (OR down from 2.39 to 2.09). Of the drug-use related characteristics, polydrug use and group sex after using drugs were significantly associated with increased odds of lifetime USWNP. The association between group sex and lifetime USWNP was particularly strong, more than doubling the odds ratios (2.16). Using drugs in a venue setting was, however, not associated with ever having USWNP. Sociodemographically, being male remained a risk factor while age ceased to be a risk factor. In addition, being currently married now became a significant protective factor, associated with a decreased odds of life-time USWNP. All three measures of causes of drug use remained significant factors in understanding lifetime USWNP in Model 2.
For the 30-day measure of risky sex, synthetic drug use was again associated with more than doubling the odds of unprotected sex with a non-stable partner, with (Model 2) or without (Model 1) controlling for the drug-use related characteristics. Sociodemographically, being male ceased to be a significant risk factor of USWNP in the 30 days prior to the interview while being currently married and having high school or more education became significantly associated with lower odds of USWNP in both models. Of the three composite measures of causes of drug use, social influence ceased to be associated with the odds of USWNP in the 30 days prior to the interview. Like the patterns for the lifetime risky sex measure, felt social control was associated with significantly lower odds while sensation seeking trait was associated with significantly higher odds of USWNP.
Finally, when the analysis was limited to the past three sexual intercourses, synthetic drug use was again associated with significantly higher odds of USWNP when the drug-use related characteristics were not controlled for (OR = 1.93, Model 1). When they were controlled for in Model 2, synthetic drug-use was associated with increased odds of USWNP only at the 10% significance level. Polydrug use was, too, associated with USWNP only at the 10% level. Having group sex after using drugs turned out to be a strong risk factor, more than quadrupling the odds (OR = 4.23) of any USWNP in the past three sexual intercourses. Sociodemographically, being male was the only significant risk factor, more than tripling the odds of USWNP in both Model 1 (OR = 3.74) and Model 2 (OR = 3.51).
CONCLUSIONS AND DISCUSSION
Despite the rapidly evolving synthetic drug epidemic and its potential association with increased sexual transmission of HIV and other STIs in China, we know no empirical research into what may have caused the shift in the main drugs of abuse and if synthetic drug use is indeed associated with increased HIV/STI risky sexual behavior. Using data from two identical surveys of synthetic drug and heroin users in Shanghai, we first quantified individual differences in sociodemographic characteristics, causes of drug abuse, and post drug-use unprotected sex with a non-stable partner between synthetic drug and heroin users. We then focused on synthetic drug use as a potential contributing factor of HIV/STI risky sexual behavior while controlling for differences in sociodemographic characteristics, causes of drug abuse, and drug-use related characteristics.
Results revealed that synthetic drug users differed significantly from heroin users in that they were on average younger, better educated, and less likely to be migrants. Probably reflective of the more recently emerging synthetic drug epidemic, synthetic drug users had been using the drugs for a much shorter time than heroin users. Qualitative data from in-depth interviews and focus group discussion also indicated that compared to heroin synthetic drugs were more expensive and their use considered trending and high status, therefore appealing more to the young and those with higher income. Consistent with the literature (Halkitis & Palamar, 2008; Halkitis, Palamar, & Mukherjee, 2007; O’Grady, Arria, Fitzelle, & Wish, 2008), synthetic drug users were more likely to be polydrug users and to use drugs in entertainment venues, both of which were considered risk factors of unprotected sex with casual partner(s).
However, the multiple regression analyses (Table 4) did not find any significant association between using drugs in entertainment venues and risky sexual behavior. It may be that the association was mediated by the other drug-use related factors controlled for in the analyses. Such mediation was indirectly confirmed by rerunning Model 2 (Table 4) without the three statistically significant drug-use related variables, of which the results (not presented but available upon request) indicated that “used drug in entertainment venue” was a significant and powerful risk factor for all three measures of unprotected sex with casual partner(s). It appears that use drugs in a venue or group setting is not of itself a risk factor; it is rather the type of drugs used (synthetic drug or polydrug vs. heroin) and post-use group sex in a venue/group setting that are associated with elevated likelihood of unprotected sex with casual partner(s).
Synthetic drug users were significantly more likely to report post-use feelings of excited/madness, sexual arousal, and talkative but much less likely to report feelings of relaxed/calm, care-free/worry-free, and sleepy. Qualitative data corroborated the survey findings. When asked about post-use feelings, in-depth interview participants replied “… heroin made you forget everything, good or bad, and was sometime used to numb the mind and obtain temporary relief from life experiences… ” and like sleeping pills “all you did was to fall asleep after using heroin.” By contrast, participants described synthetic drugs as “energizers” and use of them made you feel “full of energy” and look for things to do to “vent.”
While the literature on causes of drug abuse generally does not make distinctions between synthetic drug and heroin use, we found some significant differences between the two drug using groups in our study sample. Synthetic drug users reported slightly fewer personal experiences of events disrespect of laws and social norms (Table 2), indicating indirectly that they generally felt stronger social control than heroin users. In other words, felt social control had less of a deterrent effect on synthetic drug than heroin use. Qualitative data further suggested that even with the recent scaled-up measures against synthetic drug use, punishment remained lighter for synthetic drug use than for heroin use. For example, people caught using heroin would be immediately sent to years of compulsory drug rehab while those caught using synthetic drugs would only be fined and subject to a few days of administrative detention. This lighter punishment, along with the perception that use of synthetic drugs could not be detected after a day or less, was cited by focus group participants as the reason for many users to choose or switch to synthetic drugs.
Compared to their heroin counterparts, synthetic drug users appeared to be more amenable to social influences and were more likely to self-report having more drug-using friends before their own use of the drugs. However, it is interesting to note that synthetic drug users were slightly less likely than heroin users to self-report peer, family, or environmental influence as reasons to use the drugs (Table 2). A further separation of the three sources of social influence (results not presented but available upon request) revealed that significantly more synthetic drug users than heroin users (36.4% vs. 18.7%) reported setting or environmental influence as the reason to use but fewer reported family influence (2.1% vs. 5.3%) as the reason. Further, peer influence was cited most frequently as a cause of drug use for both groups (57.4% for synthetic drug and 61.7% for heroin users, respectively), but the two groups did not differ significantly in reporting peer influence as the reason. Qualitative data, however, suggested greater peer influences in understanding synthetic drug use, particularly among the young. Participants in in-depth interviews and focus groups frequently pointed to the importance of social network of friends and peer pressure for synthetic drug use but less so for heroin use. It appeared that synthetic drug use was influenced more by peers or surrounding environments while heroin use was more by family.
With respect to drug dependence, synthetic drug users self-reported significantly less physiological and psychological dependence. It is not clear if the dichotomous self-report of drug dependence (yes or no) could accurately capture respondents’ status of drug dependence. But there appeared a general lack of awareness of the harmful effect of synthetic drugs. Qualitative data frequently pointed to this knowledge gap. Unlike heroin, of which the harmful impact of abuse has been well publicized through years of continuous public anti-drug education and more recently through its link to HIV/AIDS, synthetic drugs are still considered by many harmless and non-addictive. Participants in focus groups all seemed to agree that synthetic drugs were not as addictive as heroin, a major reason for heroin users to switch to synthetic drugs. At the same time, many believed in the stimulant effect of synthetic drugs, such as help to stay alert, keep concentrated, lose weight, and socialize, which was also cited as the reason people choose/switch to synthetic drugs.
Compared to heroin users, synthetic drug users were significantly more likely to engage in risky sex after using drugs and consequently at greater risks of acquiring or transmitting HIV/STIs. Even so, synthetic drug users appeared to have significantly fewer STIs since starting drug use. As synthetic drugs are more recently available, it may be that their users, on average, have been subject to the harmful behavioral impact of synthetic drugs and in turn risk of STIs for shorter durations than heroin users, which was clearly suggested by the average years since first use of the drug (Table 1). However, the control of years since first use (results not presented but available upon request) did not make any difference; synthetic drug users remained to have fewer STIs than heroin users. Future research is needed to examine as why synthetic drug users’ riskier sexual life did not lead to higher incidence of STIs.
Consistent with the bivariate comparisons, the logistic regression results confirmed that after controlling for individual sociodemographic characteristics, causes of drug use, and drug use related factors synthetic drug use remained a significant and powerful risk factor for having unprotected sex with a casual partner. In addition, polydrug use and having group sex following drug use, the two characteristics that were highly correlated with synthetic drug use (Fendrich et al., 2003; Xia & Yang, 2008; Zule et al., 2007), were both independent and significant risk factors on their own for having unprotected sex with a casual partner.
One study limitation is that the data were more than 10 years old and were based on samples of drug users in compulsive drug rehab facilities (heroin users) or detention centers (synthetic drug users). They might not be representative of community drug users and given the fast-evolving drug epidemics in China they might no longer reflect the current patterns of drug abuse. However, the qualitative data from an on-going research, which we used to supplement the survey findings in the discussion, appeared to have corroborated the broader patterns and findings from the survey data. Future research should try every effort to recruit representative samples of community drug users. Another limitation is that the survey data were based entirely on self-reports. Given the sensitivity of the data, self-report of drug use/sexual behavior and their related characteristics may be subject to biases, the extent and direction of which could not be assessed. Future research should try to incorporate biomarker testing of drug use and STIs to verify/supplement the self-reported data. Future research could also benefit from using a computer assisted interview technique in collecting sensitive behavioral data, such as computer assisted personal interview, audio computer assisted self-interview, or some combination of computer assisted self-interview and face-to-face personal interview.
With the study limitations in mind, the results helped to shed light on the two questions the study tried to answer. First, the causes of drug use likely differ between synthetic drugs and heroin. There remains a general knowledge gap concerning the harmful impact of the various synthetic drugs, which, along with the lesser formal control over and punishment for synthetic drug use, appears one important reason people try or switch to synthetic drugs. Educational intervention is urgently needed. To be effective, education campaigns need to pay special attention to developing messages that emphasize the long-term harmful impact while recognizing the short-term “good” stimulating effects, such as keeping users alert and concentrated, as well as the fact that synthetic drug use may take longer to develop dependence, but it is addictive with serious health consequences. An educational intervention needs also to pay more attention to peer influences on the initiation of synthetic drug use, imploring parents and schools to be vigilant on youth’s network of friends.
Second, synthetic drug users are indeed riskier in their sexual life than heroin users, putting them and their partner(s) at elevated risks of acquiring/transmitting HIV/STIs. The riskier sexual life likely results from both the stimulant effect of synthetic drugs and from polydrug use and group sex following drug use, which are more characteristic of synthetic drug users than heroin users. Although the survey data did not show higher incidence of STIs among synthetic drug users than heroin users, it may just be the limitation of self-reporting or the issue of time. There is no question that unprotected sex with casual partner(s) increases the risk of acquiring and/or transmitting HIV/STIs. Behavioral interventions are urgently needed to reduce the unprotected sex with casual partner(s) among synthetic drug users. Again, to be effective, the interventions need to address both the knowledge gaps, including the stimulant effect of synthetic drugs and the association between unprotected sex and HIV/STIs, and the influences of peers and surrounding environments.
Acknowledgments
This research was partly supported through National Institute on Drug Abuse Grant 1R01DA039904-01A1.
Contributor Information
Xiushi Yang, Department of Sociology and Criminal Justice, Old Dominion University, Norfolk, Virginia.
Guomei Xia, Shanghai Academy of Social Sciences, Shanghai, China.
REFERENCES
- Akers RL (1985). Deviant behavior: A social learning approach. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. [Google Scholar]
- Akers RL (1998). Social learning and social structure: A general theory of crime and deviance. Boston, MA: Northeastern University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Anglin MD, Burke C, Perrochet B, Stamper E, & Dawud-Noursi S (2000). History of the methamphetamine problem. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 32, 137–141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bahr SJ, Hoffmann JP, & Yang X (2005). Parental and peer influences on the risk of adolescent drug use. Journal of Primary Prevention, 26, 529–551. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bao YP, Liu ZM, Li JH, Zhang RM, Hao W, Zhao M, … Lu L (2015). Club drug use and associated high-risk sexual behaviour in six provinces in China. Addiction, 110(Suppl 1), 11–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Black D (Ed.). (1984). Toward a general theory of social control. New York, NY: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- Buchanan AS, & Latkin CA (2008). Drug use in the social networks of heroin and cocaine users before and after drug cessation. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 96, 286–289. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Butler GKL, & Montgomery AMJ (2004). Impulsivity, risk taking and recreational ‘ecstasy’ (MDMA) use. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 76, 55–62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calafat A, Gómez CF, Juan M, & Becoña E (2007). Weekend nightlife recreational habits: Prominent intrapersonal “risk factors” for drug use? Substance Use & Misuse, 42, 1443–1454. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calafat A, Juan M, Becoña E, Mantecón A, & Ramón A (2009). Risky sexual behaviour and drug use in recreational nightlife context. A gender perspective. Psicothema, 21,227–233. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Colfax G, & Guzman R (2006). Club drugs and HIV infection: A review. Clinical Infectious Diseases, 42, 1463–1469. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corsi KF, & Booth RE (2008). HIV risk behaviors among heterosexual methamphet-amine users: Literature review from 2000 to present. Current Drug Abuse Reviews, 1, 292–296. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coser LA (1982). The notion of control in sociological theory In Gibbs JP (ed.), Social control: Views from the social sciences (pp. 13–22). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications. [Google Scholar]
- Diaz RM, Heckert AL, & Sánchez J (2005). Reasons for stimulant use among Latino gay men in San Francisco: A comparison between methamphetamine and cocaine users. Journal of Urban Health, 82(1s1), i71–i78. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Drumright LN, Patterson TL, & Strathdee SA (2006). Club drugs as causal risk factors for HIV acquisition among men who have sex with men: A review. Substance Use and Misuse, 41, 1551–1601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fang YX, Wang YB, Shi J, Liu ZM, & Lu L (2006). Recent trends in drug abuse in China. Acta Pharmcologica Sinica, 27, 140–144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fendrich M, Wislar JS, Johnson TP, & Hubbell A (2003). A contextual profile of club drug use among adults in Chicago. Addiction, 98,1693–1703. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freese TE, Miotto K, & Reback CJ (2002). The effects and consequences of selected club drugs. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 23, 151–156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gibbs JP (Ed.). (1982). Social control: Views from the social sciences. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications. [Google Scholar]
- Green AI (2003). “Chem friendly”: The institutional basis of “club drug” use in a sample of urban gay men. Deviant Behavior, 24, 427–447. [Google Scholar]
- Halkitis PN, Fischgrund BN, & Parsons JT (2005). Explanations for methamphet-amine use among gay and bisexual men in New York City. Substance Use and Misuse, 40, 1331–1345. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Halkitis PN, & Palamar JJ (2008). Multivariate modeling of club drug use initiation among gay and bisexual men. Substance Use and Misuse, 43, 871–879. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Halkitis PN, Palamar JJ, & Mukherjee PP (2007). Poly-club-drug use among gay and bisexual men: A longitudinal analysis. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 89, 153–160. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Han D (2007). Face to face with drug abusers. Nanjing, China: Jiangsu People’s Publisher. [Google Scholar]
- Hanson KL, Luciana M, & Sullwold K (2008). Reward-related decision-making deficits and elevated impulsivity among MDMA and other drug users. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 96, 99–110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hirschi T (1969). Causes of delinquency. Berkeley: University of California Press. [Google Scholar]
- Hirshfield S, Schrimshaw EW, Stall RD, Margolis AD, Downing MJ, & Chiasson MA (2015). Drug use, sexual risk, and syndemic production among men who have sex with men who engage in group sexual Encounters. American Journal of Public Health, 105, 1849–1858. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Homer BD, Solomon TM, Moeller RW, Mascia A, DeRaleau L, & Halkitis PN (2008). Methamphetamine abuse and impairment of social functioning: A review of the underlying neurophysiological causes and behavioral implications. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 301–310. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klitzman R (2006). From “male bonding rituals” to “suicide Tuesday”: A qualitative study of issues faced by gay male ecstasy (MDMA) users. Journal of Homosexuality, 51, 7–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kurtz SP (2005). Post-circuit blues: Motivations and consequences of crystal meth use among gay men in Miami. AIDS and Behavior, 9, 63–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lai S, Chen J, Celentano D, Page JB, Lai H, Yang J, … Yu X (2000). Adoption of injection practices in heroin users in Guangxi province, China. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 32, 285–292. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laidler KAJ (2005). The rise of club drugs in a heroin society: The case of Hong Kong. Substance Use and Misuse, 40, 1257–1278. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lankenau SE, & Clatts MC (2005). Patterns of polydrug use among ketamine injectors in New York City. Substance Use and Misuse, 40, 1381–1397. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li J, Zhang R, & Liu H (2008). A preliminary study of methamphetamine use in Yunnan, China. AIDS Patient Care & STDs, 22, 543–544. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liao M, Kang D, Tao X, Cox C, Qian Y, Wang G, … Jia Y (2014). Syndemics of syphilis, HCV infection, and methamphetamine use along the east coast of China. BMC Public Health, 14, 172. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lowinger P (1977). The solution to narcotic addition in the People’s Republic of China. American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 4, 165–178. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lu L, Fang Y, & Wang X (2008). Drug abuse in China: Past, present and future. Cellular and Molecular Neurobiology, 28, 479–490. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mattison AM, Ross MW, Wolfson T, Franklin D, & HNRC Group. (2001). Circuit party attendance, club drug use, and unsafe sex in gay men. Journal of Substance Abuse, 13, 119–126. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maxwell JC (2005). Party drugs: Properties, prevalence, patterns, and problems. Substance Use & Misuse, 40, 1203–1240. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Merli MG, Hertog S, Wang B, & Li J (2006). Modelling the spread of HIV/AIDS in China: The role of sexual transmission. Population Studies, 60, 1–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morgan MJ, Impallomeni LC, Pirona A, & Rogers RD (2006). Elevated impulsivity and impaired decision-making in abstinent ecstasy (MDMA) users compared to polydrug and drug-naive controls. Neuropsychopharmacology, 31, 1562–1573. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nagasawa R, Qian Z, & Wong P (2000). Social control theory as a theory of conformity: The case of Asian/Pacific drug and alcohol nonuse. Sociological Perspectives, 43,581–603. [Google Scholar]
- Newcomb M, & Earleywine M (1996). Intrapersonal contributors to drug use: The willing host. American Behavioral Scientist, 39, 823–837. [Google Scholar]
- National Narcotics Control Commission. (2012). Annual report on drug control in China (2012). Beijing, China: Ministry of Public Security of People’s Republic of China. [Google Scholar]
- O’Grady KE, Arria AM, Fitzelle DMB, & Wish ED (2008). Heavy drinking and polydrug use among college students. Journal of Drug Issues, 38, 445–466. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Operario D, Choi KH, Chu PL, McFarland W, Secura GM, Behel S, … Valleroy L (2006). Prevalence and correlates of substance use among young Asian Pacific Islander men who have sex with men. Prevention Science, 7, 19–29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parks KA, & Kennedy CL (2004). Club drugs: Reasons for and consequences of use. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 36, 295–302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parrott AC (2004). MDMA (3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine) or ecstasy: The neuropsychobiological implications of taking it at dances and raves. Neuropsychobiology, 50, 329–335. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parsons JT, Grov C, & Golub SA (2012). Sexual compulsivity, co-occurring psychosocial health problems, and HIV risk among gay and bisexual men: Further evidence of a syndemic. American Journal of Public Health, 102, 156–162. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Payne BK, & Gainey RR (2005). Drugs and policing: A scientific perspective. Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas Publisher, Ltd. [Google Scholar]
- Qian HZ, Schumacher JE, Chen HT, & Ruan YH (2006). Injection drug use and HIV/AIDS in China: Review of current situation, prevention and policy implications. Harm Reduction Journal, 3, 4–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roberti JW (2004). A review of behavioral and biological correlates of sensation seeking. Journal of Research in Personality, 38, 256–279. [Google Scholar]
- Schilder AJ, Lampinen TM, Miller ML, & Hogg RS (2005). Crystal methamphetamine and ecstasy differ in relation to un safe sex among young gay men. Canadian Journal of Public Health, 96, 340–343. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Semple SJ, Zians J, Grant I, & Patterson TL (2005). Impulsivity and methamphetamine use. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 29, 85–93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Semple SJ, Zians J, Grant I, & Patterson TL (2006). Sexual compulsivity in a sample of HIV-positive methamphetamine-using gay and bisexual men. AIDS and Behavior, 10, 587–598. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith KM, Larive LL, & Romanelli F (2002). Club drugs: Methylenedioxymeth-amphetamine, flunitrazepam, ketamine hydrochloride, and gamma-hydroxybutyrate. American Journal of Health-System Pharmacy, 59, 1067–1076. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stall R, Mills TC, Williamson J, Hart T, Greenwood G, Paul J, … Catanla JA (2003). Association of co-occurring psychosocial health problems and increased vulnerability to HIV/AIDS among urban men who have sex with men. American Journal of Public Health, 93, 939–942. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- StataCorp. (2011). Stata: Release 12 [Computer software.] College Station, TX: StataCorp. [Google Scholar]
- Stephens T, Holliday RC, & Jarboe J (2015). Self-reported ecstasy (MDMA) use and past occurrence of sexually transmitted infections (STIs) in a cohort juvenile detainees in the USA. Journal of Community Health, 40, 308–313. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tang Y, Zhao D, Zhao C, & Cubells JF (2006). Opiate addiction in China: Current situation and treatment, Addiction, 101, 657–665. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- United Nations Office on Drugs and Crime. (2008). 2008 World drug report. New York, NY: United Nations. [Google Scholar]
- Uys JDK, & Niesink RJM (2005). Pharmacological aspects of the combined use of 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA, ecstasy) and gamma-hydroxybutyric acid (GHB): A review of the literature. Drug and Alcohol Review, 24, 359–368. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Verdejo-Garcia A, Pérez-Garcia M, & Bechara A (2006). Emotion, decision-making and substance dependence: A somatic-marker model of addition. Current Neuropharmacology, 4, 17–31. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang FL (2004). Reformed migration control and new targeted people: China’s hukou system in the 2000s. China Quarterly, 177, 115–132. [Google Scholar]
- Wang Y, & Liu Z (2007). Non-traditional drug abuse in China. Chinese Journal of Drug Abuse Prevention and Treatment, 13, 63–66. [Google Scholar]
- Wood PB, Cochran JK, Pfefferbaum B, & Arneklev BJ (1995). Sensation-seeking and delinquent substance use: An extension of learning theory. Journal of Drug Issues, 25, 173–193. [Google Scholar]
- Xia G, & Yang X (2008). Cultural perspectives on changes in drugs of abuse. Shehui Kexue, 2, 99–106. [Google Scholar]
- Xia G, & Yang X (2017). Shehuixue shiye xia de xinxing dupin [Sociological study of new drugs]. Shanghai, China: Shanghai Academy of Social Sciences Publishing. [Google Scholar]
- Xu JJ, Qian HZ, Chu ZQ, Zhang J, Hu QH, Jiang YJ, … Shang H (2014). Recreational drug use among Chinese men who have sex with men: A risky combination with unprotected sex for acquiring HIV Infection. BioMed Research International, 2014, Article ID 725361. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang X (2000). The fertility impact of temporary migration in China: A detachment hypothesis. European Journal of Population, 16, 163–183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang X (2006). Temporary migration and HIV risk behaviors in China. Environment and Planning A, 38, 1527–1543. [Google Scholar]
- Yang X, & Luo H (2009). Migration, urbanization, and drug use and casual sex in China: A multilevel analysis. Environment and Planning A, 41 , 581–597. [Google Scholar]
- Yang X, & Xia G (2010). Cause and consequences of increasing club drug use in China: A descriptive assessment. Substance Use and Misuse, 45, 224–239. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yanovitzky I (2006). Sensation seeking and alcohol use by college students: Examining multiple pathways of effects. Journal of Health Communication, 11, 269–280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yu G, Wall MM, Chiasson MA, & Hirshfield S (2015). Complex drug use patterns and associated HIV transmission risk behaviors in an internet sample of U.S. men who have sex with men. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 44, 421–428. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zheng X, Tian C, Choi K, Zhang J, Cheng H, Yang X, … Hearst N (1994). Injecting drug use and HIV infection in southwest China. AIDS, 8, 1141–1147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zuckerman M (1994). Behavioral expressions and biosocial bases of sensation seeking. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Zule WA, Costenbader EC, Meyer WJ Jr., & Wechsberg WM (2007). Methamphetamine use and risky sexual behaviors during heterosexual encounters. Sexually Transmitted Diseases, 34, 689–694. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zule WA, Costenbader E, Coomes CM, Meyer WJ, Riehman K, Poehlman J, & Wechsberg WM (2007). Stimulant use and sexual risk behaviors for HIV in rural North Carolina. Journal of Rural Health, 23,74–78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]