Abstract
Focal application of a strong static magnetic field over the human scalp induces measurable local changes in brain function. Whether it also induces distant effects across the brain and how these local and distant effects collectively affect motor behavior remains unclear. Here we applied transcranial static magnetic field stimulation (tSMS) over the supplementary motor area (SMA) in healthy subjects. At a behavioral level, tSMS increased the time to initiate movement while decreasing errors in choice reaction-time tasks. At a functional level, tSMS increased SMA resting-state fMRI activity and bilateral functional connectivity between the SMA and both the paracentral lobule and the lateral frontotemporal cortex, including the inferior frontal gyrus. These results suggest that tSMS over the SMA can induce behavioral aftereffects associated with modulation of both local and distant functionally-connected cortical circuits involved in the control of speed-accuracy tradeoffs, thus offering a promising protocol for cognitive and clinical research.
Subject terms: Decision, Transcranial magnetic stimulation, Magnetic resonance imaging, Motor cortex
Pineda-Pardo et al. show that focal application of a strong permanent magnet over the supplementary motor area shifts the speed-accuracy tradeoff towards accuracy by modulating both cortical circuits and distant cortical networks. This study suggests the utility of transcranial static magnetic field stimulation for cognitive research.
Introduction
The supplementary motor area (SMA), located in the medial frontal cortex, delimits the motor from the prefrontal cortical areas and is critical for linking cognition to action in normal behaviors1. The most posterior section of the SMA, the SMA proper2, is directly connected to the primary motor cortex (M1) with scarce prefrontal projections3 and has been traditionally associated to programming and anticipation of motor plans4–7. In contrast, the most anterior section of the SMA—the pre-SMA2 has wide connections to the prefrontal cortex3,8 and is predominantly involved in cognitive operations underlying behavioral change, such as action switching9,10, movement stopping11–13, and setting speed-accuracy tradeoffs in decision making14,15. This unique role played by the SMA as a crossroads between cognition and action makes it an attractive non-invasive brain stimulation (NIBS) target for treating neurological and psychiatric disorders in which the control of movements or behaviors is altered, including Tourette syndrome16–18, obsessive compulsive disorder19, and Parkinson’s disease20,21.
Transcranial static magnetic field stimulation (tSMS) is a recent NIBS technique that consists of focally applying a relatively strong neodymium magnet over the scalp22. tSMS induces a reduction of cortical excitability that outlasts the duration of the stimulation, as measured by decreased motor-evoked potentials, somatosensory-evoked potentials, and intra-epidermal electrical stimulation-evoked potentials when applied over the motor cortex22–32, and by locally increased electroencephalography α-activity when applied over the visual cortex33 or the somatosensory cortex34. Despite one study reporting negative findings35, possibly due to methodological differences36, converging evidence supports the ability of tSMS to induce local effects. Conversely, little is known about the ability of tSMS to induce neuroanatomically distant effects across the brain37 and to affect motor behavior38–40.
Here we applied tSMS over the SMA (i.e. both SMA proper and pre-SMA) in healthy subjects (Fig. 1a), running two independent experiments in order to establish the behavioral relevance and uncover the network effects of the intervention. Experiment 1 was a randomized double-blind sham-controlled parallel study in which we measured the behavioral after-effects of tSMS (Fig. 1b; Experiment 1a in Results and Methods section). We used three different choice-reaction time (CRT) tasks to assess the impact of SMA tSMS on withholding predicted actions (fully-cued), motor planning (uncued-compatible) and stimulus–response compatibility (uncued-incompatible) during motor choice behavior, and we fitted drift-diffusion models to the data in order to gain insight about the underlying cognitive processes41. A subgroup of subjects also repeated the experiment in a crossover design (Experiment 1b) and a new group performed the same experiment but with tSMS of M1 (Experiment 1c). Experiment 2 was a randomized double-blind sham-controlled crossover study in which we measured the functional after-effects of SMA tSMS using resting-state functional magnetic resonance image (fMRI) (Fig. 1c). We investigated the local and distant tSMS after-effects by quantifying the amplitude of low-frequency fluctuations (ALFF), the regional homogeneity (ReHo), and the whole-brain seed-based functional connectivity using left and right SMA regions of interest (ROIs).
Results
Experiment 1a: Behavioral findings, parallel design
We tested the behavioral after-effects of 30-min tSMS of the SMA in a double-blind sham-controlled parallel study on 42 healthy subjects who performed three CRT tasks (Fig. 2a): 20 subjects performed the tasks after receiving real tSMS (mean ± SD; 31.9 ± 9.3 years old, 14 females) and 22 after receiving sham tSMS (31.2 ± 8.4, 14 females). Real and sham groups were matched for age (unpaired t test: p = 0.82) and gender (two proportion z test: p = 0.66). One subject who received sham tSMS did not complete the fully-cued task and another subject who received sham tSMS did not complete the uncued-compatible task, because they did not follow correctly the instructions.
Reaction times of correct trials were progressively longer, as expected, from the fully-cued (575.5 ± 102.9 ms), to the uncued-compatible (688.1 ± 118.2 ms), to the uncued-incompatible tasks (817.7 ± 152.7 ms; three-way independent-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA), Task: F(2,472) = 144.4, p < 0.0001). Importantly, reaction times were longer in subjects that received tSMS (709.4 ± 166.1 ms) compared to sham (681.0 ± 150.8 ms; three-way independent-measures ANOVA, Stimulation: F(1,472) = 6.8, p = 0.0096), independently of the task (Stimulation × Task: F(2,472) = 0.1, p = 0.87) or the target location (Stimulation × Location: F(3,472) < 0.1, p > 0.99; Stimulation × Task × Location: F(6,472) < 0.1, p > 0.99). The longer reaction times after tSMS were mostly due to longer initiation times (Intervention: F(1,472) = 8.3, p = 0.0041), with little difference of movement times (Stimulation: F(1,472) = 2.6, p = 0.11; Fig. 2b).
The overall error rate was low: 3.1% of trials in the fully-cued task, due to anticipation errors, 0.7% in the uncued-compatible task, and 1.9% in the uncued-incompatible task (three-way independent-measures ANOVA, Task: F(2,472) = 19.3, p < 0.0001). Nevertheless, the error rate was lower in subjects that received tSMS compared to sham (Stimulation: F(1,472) = 9.8, p = 0.0018), depending on the task (Stimulation × Task: F(2,472) = 4.6, p = 0.0107). Specifically, in the fully-cued task the error rate was lower after tSMS compared to sham (follow-up two-way ANOVA, Stimulation: F(1,156) = 9.7, p = 0.0021), no differences were observed in the uncued-compatible task (F(1,156) = 0.7, p = 0.39), and in the uncued-incompatible task the error rate was again lower after tSMS compared to sham (F(1,160) = 2.7, p = 0.0451) (Fig. 2c).
At the end of the experiment, there was a tendency for subjects to correctly guess whether they had received real tSMS (14 of 20 correct guesses) or sham (13 of 22 correct guesses; χ2 = 3.58, p = 0.0585). Interestingly, correctly guessing real tSMS—but not sham—was associated with slower initiation times (four-way independent-measures ANOVA, Stimulation × Guessing: F(1,448) = 19.44, p < 0.0001).
Experiment 1a: Drift-diffusion models
To further characterize the processes modulated by tSMS, we fitted drift-diffusion models to our behavioral data (Fig. 2e). These models assume that the decision process in each trial of the CRT task occurs as a noisy diffusion process that accumulates evidence until either a positive or a negative threshold is reached, respectively, leading to a correct or incorrect response. Error rates and response time distributions for correct and incorrect responses are thus translated into three main parameters of the modeled diffusion process: the drift rate of evidence accumulation v, the separation a between decision thresholds, and the non-decision time Ter that precedes (and follows) the decision process.
Interestingly, the general increase of initiation times and reduction of errors induced by tSMS compared to sham in the three tasks reflected specific differences in the model parameters in each task (Fig. 2e, f). In the fully-cued task, the threshold separation was larger after tSMS (a = 1.05, 95% confidence interval (c.i.) [1.00–1.12]) compared to sham (a = 0.92 [0.88–0.97]; p = 0.0014). In the uncued-compatible task, the drift rate was significantly slower after tSMS (v = 3.74 [3.57–3.94]) compared to sham (v = 4.25 [4.05–4.51]; p = 0.0023). Last, in the uncued-incompatible task, the drift rate was significantly faster after tSMS (v = 3.43 [3.28–3.60]) compared to sham (v = 3.14 [3.01–3.29]; p = 0.0269), and the non-decision time significantly longer after tSMS (Ter = 359.9 [349.8–368.6] ms) compared to sham (Ter = 339.2 [331.0–346.8] ms; p = 0.0025).
Experiment 1b: Behavioral findings, crossover design
Sixteen of the subjects who participated in Experiment 1 (8 real, 8 sham) also repeated the experiment at least 1 week later in a double-blind crossover design (33.1 ± 8.1 years old, 11 females). Again, tSMS increased reaction times compared to sham (three-way mixed ANOVA, Stimulation: F(1,180) = 15.8, p = 0.0001), independently of the task (Stimulation × Task: F(2,180) = 0.1, p = 0.87) or target location (Stimulation × Location: F(3,180) < 0.1, p > 0.99; Stimulation × Task × Location: F(6,180) < 0.1, p > 0.99). The effect was mostly due to increased initiation times (Stimulation: F(1,180) = 17.6, p < 0.0001), with a smaller but significant increase of movement times (Stimulation: F(1,180) = 5.8, p = 0.0169). Error rates were particularly low in this subgroup, possibly due to a learning effect in the second session: 2.0% of trials in the fully-cued task, 0.8% in the uncued-compatible task, and 1.4% in the uncued-incompatible task. Consequently, the error rate decrease induced by tSMS did not reach significance (F(1,180) = 0.16, p = 0.69).
The ability of subjects to correctly guess whether they received real tSMS or sham was not significant after the first session (9 of 16 correct guesses; χ2 = 0.41, p = 0.52), but became significant after second session (13 of 16 correct guesses; χ2 = 6.35, p = 0.0117), with some subjects reporting feeling slower after the real compared to the sham tSMS session. Due to lower error rates and the possible confound of learning effects, drift-diffusion models were not applied to these data.
Experiment 1c: Behavioral findings, tSMS of M1
In order to gain insight into possible contribution of SMA-M1 vs. SMA-frontal projections in our behavioral findings, we performed an additional single-blind behavioral experiment in 17 subjects (29.4 ± 7.4 years old, 12 females) who received tSMS over the right M1. The new data were compared against the sham group of Experiment 1a, with no differences in age (p = 0.47) or gender (p = 0.65).
We found that the ability of M1 tSMS to modulate behavioral performance, as measured by initiation time, was highly dependent on the task (three-way ANOVA, Stimulation × Task, F(2,436) = 7.4, p = 0.0007). Specifically, M1 tSMS increased the initiation times compared to sham in the fully-cued task (sham: 331.9 ± 51.4 ms; real: 395.3 ± 41.7 ms; Tukey, p < 0.0001), but not in the uncued-compatible nor in the uncued-incompatible tasks (p > 0.91). In the fully-cued task, M1 tSMS also significantly decreases the error rate (sham: 4.9 ± 3.8%; real: 2.5 ± 1.7%; unpaired t test, p = 0.0168).
At the end of the experiment, 10 of 17 subjects correctly guessed that they had received real tSMS.
Experiment 2: fMRI findings, local effects
We tested the functional after-effects of 30-min tSMS of the SMA in a randomized double-blind sham-controlled crossover experiment in 20 subjects (28.5 ± 5.2 years old, 9 females), in which resting-state fMRI was acquired at baseline and immediately after 30 min of tSMS (or sham) applied over the SMA (Fig. 1b). One subject was discarded due to excessive motion (>35% noisy samples) in at least one acquisition.
We first quantified the local effects induced by tSMS in the cortical area below the magnet, as assessed by changes in ALFF and ReHo in the left and right SMA ROIs defined from the AAL2 atlas. The ALFF was overall higher in the left compared to the right SMA (three-way repeated-measures ANOVA, Side: F(1,18) = 23.8, p = 0.0001). Importantly, tSMS induced a significant effect compared to sham (Time × Stimulation: F(1,18) = 5.9, p = 0.0260), which depended on the side of the brain (Time × Stimulation × Side: F(1,18) = 5.9, p = 0.0260). Namely, tSMS did not induce any detectable change in the left SMA (two-way follow-up ANOVA, Time × Stimulation: F(1,18) = 1.0, p = 0.32), but its effect was significant in the right SMA (Time × Stimulation: F(1,18) = 12.2, p = 0.0026), where the ALFF increased after real tSMS (Tukey: p = 0.0283) but not after sham (p = 0.29). When the time factor was collapsed to analyze ALFF differences (i.e. post–pre stimulation values), in the right SMA the effect of tSMS was significantly greater than sham (two-way repeated-measures ANOVA, Stimulation × Side: F(1,18) = 5.9, p = 0.0260; Tukey = 0.0005; Fig. 3a).
Comparable results were obtained when analyzing ReHo: (1) higher ReHo in the left compared to the right SMA (three-way repeated-measures ANOVA, Side: F(1,18) = 13.3, p = 0.0018); (2) significant side-specific effect of tSMS (Time × Stimulation × Side: F(1,18) = 7.0, p = 0.0168), with no detectable change in the left SMA (two-way follow-up ANOVA, Time × Stimulation: F(1,18) = 0.0, p = 0.89), and increase in ReHo after tSMS (Time × Stimulation: F(1,18) = 9.8, p = 0.0058; Tukey: p = 0.0262) but not after sham (p = 0.59) in the right SMA; (3) greater post–pre effect of tSMS compared to sham in the right SMA (two-way repeated-measures ANOVA, Stimulation × Side: F(1,18) = 6.9, p = 0.0168; Tukey = 0.0134; Fig. 3b). In fact, ALFF and ReHo measures were correlated both in their baseline values (r = 0.73, p < 0.001; Fig. 3c) and post–pre differences (r = 0.55, p < 0.001; Fig. 3d).
At the single-subject level, greater post–pre effect of tSMS compared to sham was observed (i) in 13 of 19 subjects with ALFF, (ii) in 13 of 19 subjects with ReHo, (iii) in 11 subjects with both ALFF and ReHo, and (iv) in 4 subjects with neither ALFF nor ReHo (Fig. 3d).
To control for whether the overall higher local activity we observed in the left compared to the right SMA could be a baseline bias of our dataset, we analyzed resting-state fMRI data from 49 subjects selected at random from the Human Connectome Project (HCP) (age [22–35] years old, 26 females). We found, again, that the ALFF of the left SMA (0.65 ± 0.10) was significantly higher compared to the right SMA (0.62 ± 0.11; paired t test: p = 0.0004). This physiological asymmetry might explain the right laterality of tSMS effects on local SMA activity.
Experiment 2: fMRI findings, distant effects
We then quantified the distant effects induced by tSMS, as assessed by functional connectivity between the left and right SMA ROIs and the whole brain. Baseline functional connectivity maps of the left and right SMA showed positive connectivity with the expected regions in the motor, salience, and attention networks (Fig. 3e). These positive connectivity maps were used as inclusion masks for the comparison between stimulation conditions (real vs. sham).
Consistent with the right laterality of local effects, distant effects induced by tSMS were significant only when the seed was placed in the right SMA. Specifically, a significant effect of tSMS compared to sham (Time × Stimulation interaction) was observed for a cluster that covered the anterior portion of the left superior temporal gyrus and the lateral portion of the left orbitofrontal gyrus, extending to the left anterior insula, the left precentral gyrus in its more ventral–lateral portion, and the inferior frontal gyrus (left frontotemporal cluster; t-peak = 5.29; puncorr < 0.001, family-wise error (FWE)-corrected pcluster < 0.05). Relaxing the significance threshold (puncorr < 0.01, FWE-corrected pcluster < 0.05) uncovered bilateral effects with two additional clusters (Fig. 3f): a cluster mirroring the first cluster on the right side of the brain (right frontotemporal cluster; t-peak = 3.88), and another cluster bilaterally located in the dorsal portion of the precentral and postcentral gyri reaching the SMA proper (paracentral cluster; t-peak = 5.29). See Table 1 for local maxima of the three clusters. The net effect of tSMS was an increased functional connectivity compared to sham for all clusters (Fig. 3g).
Table 1.
MNI coordinates (mm) | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Cluster | X | Y | Z | t-peak | Cluster extent (voxels) | Atlas-based peak location |
Left frontotemporal | −40 | 18 | −20 | 5.29 | 2176 | Left OFG/left STG |
−38 | −8 | −16 | 5.04 | Left anterior insula | ||
−56 | 6 | 0 | 4.46 | Left precentral gyrus | ||
Right frontotemporal | 54 | 26 | −4 | 3.88 | 903 | Right IFG |
62 | −12 | 20 | 3.82 | Right postcentral gyrus | ||
66 | 6 | −2 | 3.69 | Right STG | ||
Paracentral | 4 | −46 | 78 | 5.29 | 1103 | Postcentral gyrus |
−20 | −48 | 64 | 5.07 | Superior parietal lobe | ||
−2 | −46 | 72 | 4.85 | Postcentral gyrus |
At the single-subject level, greater post–pre effect of tSMS compared to sham was observed in 17 of 19 subjects for each of the three clusters, with 15 subjects showing a net positive effect for all three clusters (Fig. 3h).
In order to investigate whether the increased functional connectivity between the SMA and the distant clusters genuinely reflected increased co-activity of the underlying networks rather than simply being an artifact of the local changes42, we analyzed the activity in the distant clusters, and we found a trend toward significant change in ALFF (three-way repeated-measures ANOVA, Time × Stimulation: F(1,18) = 3.7, p = 0.069) and a significant change in ReHo (F(1,18) = 5.9, p = 0.0255). Namely, with both measures the activity in the distant clusters increased after real tSMS (Fisher: ALFF, p = 0.0455; ReHo, p = 0.0069) but not after sham (ALFF, p = 0.56; ReHo, p = 0.70). Furthermore, no positive correlation was observed between local effects and distant effects (Fig. 3h). These results suggest that the distant effects induced by tSMS of the SMA were not simply an indirect consequence of the local effects.
At the end of the experiment, subjects were not able to correctly guess whether they had received real tSMS or sham, neither after the first session (9 of 20 correct guesses; χ2 = 0.20, p = 0.65) nor after the second session (10 of 20 correct guesses; χ2 = 0.00, p = 1.00).
Discussion
We show that tSMS of the SMA, compared to sham, induces both behavioral and functional after-effects. First, tSMS increases the time to initiate movement while decreasing errors in choice-reaction time tasks, by either elevating decision threshold when withholding predicted actions (fully-cued task), slowing evidence accumulation (i.e. decreasing the drift rate) during motor planning (uncued-compatible task), or increasing non-decision time when solving stimulus–response conflicts (uncued-incompatible task). Second, tSMS increases the local resting-state fMRI activity of the SMA as well as the bilateral functional connectivity between the SMA and both the paracentral lobule and the frontotemporal cortex, including the inferior frontal gyrus. tSMS over the SMA can thus shift the speed-accuracy tradeoff in favor of accuracy by means of different processes depending of the cognitive demands before overt action, and can modulate both local cortical circuits below the magnet and distant functionally connected cortical networks that may account for specific changes in cognitive processes associated to motor behavior.
Transcranial static magnetic field stimulation
tSMS has been recently added to the family of inhibitory NIBS techniques22. Previous studies had focused on relatively short applications of tSMS and short-lived after-effects22–30,32–35,37,39,40,43. However, we recently showed that tSMS applied for 30 min over M1 induces physiological after-effects that last at least 30 min after the end of the stimulation31. Here we thus adopted this new 30-min tSMS protocol, which provided us with sufficient post-stimulation time to measure the after-effects of tSMS using behavioral tasks and resting-state fMRI.
The exact focality of tSMS is unknown, but the magnetic field is not distorted by soft tissue or bone, so its spatial extent only depends on the geometry and physical properties of the magnet44,45. By applying tSMS to the SMA, we intended to neuromodulate the entire supplementary motor complex, including both SMA proper and pre-SMA, as confirmed by neuronavigation. We used ROIs from the AAL2 atlas46 that are consistent with this broad anatomical SMA localization. The rationale was to modulate both more caudal motor-related and more rostral associative-related circuits, thus maximizing the possibility to induce behavioral changes and long-distant fMRI effects, as a first step toward future clinical applications.
The mechanisms of action of tSMS are not completely clear. At the cellular level, tSMS likely alters the function of membrane ion channels due to the diamagnetic anisotropic properties of phospholipids47–51. At systems level, 30-min tSMS applied over M1 decreases corticospinal excitability while increasing intracortical excitability31. It is not known whether these changes are induced by the sustained exposure to the intensity or to the spatial gradient52,53 of the magnetic field (e.g. from 2 to 3 cm from the surface along the cylinder axis of the MAG45r there is a magnetic field difference of 74 mT, from 166 to 92 mT, which corresponds to a spatial gradient of 7400 mT/m). Interestingly, the intensity of the static magnetic field inside the scanner is one order of magnitude higher (3 T), but its spatial gradient is virtually zero. Even the fast-switching magnetic gradients applied during MRI acquisitions are two orders of magnitude lower than tSMS (45 mT/m in our scanner). The fact that we could actually see effects induced by tSMS compared to sham with resting-state fMRI thus suggests that the spatial gradient of the magnetic field may play a mechanistic role in tSMS52,53. Importantly, our subjects did not perform the behavioral tasks in the scanner, so any inference between our behavioral and functional findings should be made with caution. Nevertheless, our results suggest that the effects of 30-min tSMS over the SMA reach behavioral relevance and are measureable with resting-state fMRI using a sham-controlled paradigm.
Behavioral findings
tSMS of the SMA significantly affected the speed-accuracy tradeoff: initiation times increased in favor of decreasing the error rate. This is consistent with previous studies investigating the SMA role in adjusting behavior. For example, cathodal tDCS of the SMA induces a slowing of reaction times associated with a decrease in the incidence of movement releases by startling acoustic stimuli54 and results in the prevention of impulsive reactions, reducing the number of errors55. Conversely, anodal tDCS of the SMA decreases the probability of withholding anticipated actions56. The change in speed-accuracy tradeoff in favor of accuracy is also consistent with the increased efficiency of response suppression observed after pre-SMA stimulation with rTMS inhibitory protocols57–59. However, other studies reported the opposite effect60,61, possibly due to differences among NIBS techniques, tasks, and exact target locations within the SMA. Interestingly, we observed that tSMS of M1 induced a similar increase of initiation times in favor of decreasing errors, but only for the fully-cued task. This suggest that the modulation of the speed-accuracy tradeoff by SMA tSMS in the fully-cued task might be more specifically mediated by SMA-M1 connections, whereas the modulations induced by SMA tSMS in the uncued-compatible and uncued-incompatible tasks might be more specifically mediated by SMA-frontal connections.
Drift-diffusion models allowed us to further characterize the involvement of SMA in choosing adequate behavioral options (the what) and selecting the right moment (the when) and location (the where) of action initiation and execution in behavioral adaptation62–64. The increased decision threshold induced by tSMS in the fully-cued task suggests a more cautious, conservative action initiation in deciding when to move while withholding predicted actions. Our result thus extends similar findings of increased caution in deciding where to move, observed with random dot motion tasks after cTBS of the right pre-SMA65,66 (but see ref. 67). Interestingly, with the same dot motion tasks cTBS of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) was shown to slow down the accumulation of evidence67, similarly to what we observed with tSMS in the uncued-compatible task, which only involved motor planning. This suggests that both DLPFC and the SMA may be involved in the accumulation of evidence about where to move, depending on the cognitive demands68. In addition, the longer non-decision time—and faster accumulation of evidence—induced by tSMS in the uncued-incompatible task offers insight into the cognitive processes underlying the role of the SMA in what to execute in situations of stimulus–response conflict69–74. It is noteworthy that non-decision time was markedly longer—and the drift rate slower—in the uncued-incompatible compared to the uncued-compatible tasks, resembling the switch cost in task-switching paradigms75–77. Longer non-decision time most likely reflected a delayed decision onset78, which humans can use as a strategy to trade accuracy over speed as previously shown with motion-interference tasks79. The increase of non-decision time induced by tSMS in the uncued-incompatible task may thus reflect this strategy of delaying decision onset to degrade consolidated/automatic associations for resolving the stimulus–response conflict, thereby allowing evidence to be more efficiently accumulated for launching a well-prepared and more accurate action plan. Overall, the modulations induced by tSMS on the cognitive dynamics that preceded overt behavior in our tasks support a causal role of SMA in determining both when to move in situations of withholding predicted actions, where to initiate and execute the action, and what to execute in situations of stimulus–response conflict.
Drift-diffusion models likely have direct neural correlates. In fact, choice behaviors can be driven by populations of cortical integrator neurons tuned to specific choices, which accumulate information by increasing their firing rate, determining the corresponding behavioral response if a certain threshold is reached14,80,81. Considering the decision threshold as the distance between the baseline firing and the firing that determines the behavioral response, the activity of these cortical integration neurons closely corresponds to the parameters of the drift-diffusion model. Therefore, our result that the same perturbation (i.e. tSMS of SMA) modulates different parameters of the drift-diffusion model in different tasks suggests that different subpopulations of neurons may drive when, where, and what decisions in the SMA. This is in agreement with fMRI results suggesting that the decision of what to move is more specifically located in the pre-SMA, whereas the decision of when to move is more confined to the SMA proper82.
Intriguingly, correctly guessing real tSMS was associated with slower initiation times, and the ability of our subjects to correctly guess the tSMS experimental condition (i.e. real vs. sham) increased after the behavioral experiments (from a tendency after the first session, to a significant guessing ability after the second session), while no guessing ability whatsoever was seen after the fMRI sessions (the experimenter handling tSMS was the same in all cases). Note that no guessing ability was observed in previous studies applying tSMS over other points of the scalp. Two complementary possibilities might explain the present finding. The first one is that during task performance a richer sensorimotor and cognitive feedback (compared to the fMRI session) was perceived via changes in speed and/or errors, a prevailing error awareness function of pre-SMA83. The second one is that our stimulation protocol combined with behavioral activation of the local or distant brain circuits modulated the sense of agency of our subjects, that is, the experience of initiating and controlling an action84, which was previously attributed to SMA activity85,86. Even though this finding will require further investigation, it may open new avenues of research in consciousness and action87.
fMRI findings
tSMS of the SMA induced a small but significant increase in the local resting-state fMRI activity, as measured by ALFF and ReHo, particularly in the right SMA. These measures correlate with regional brain metabolism88–91, suggesting that the overall local effect of tSMS was to increase the metabolism of the SMA. At first glance, this increased metabolism might seem at odds with our behavioral findings, since a higher decision threshold—shifting the speed-accuracy tradeoff towards accuracy—would be expected to reduce the baseline firing of cortical integrator neurons tuned to perceptual choices14, leading to a decrease rather than increase of pre-SMA activity92–96. However, increased regional metabolism is a common observation after the application of inhibitory NIBS techniques, for example, after 1-Hz rTMS97,98 or cathodal tDCS99,100, possibly reflecting increased local levels of inhibitory synaptic activity in specific populations of intracortical neurons. We recently showed that when the same tSMS protocol used here is applied to M1, it does decrease corticospinal excitability, but it also increases intracortical facilitation while reducing intracortical inhibition31. The increase in local resting-state fMRI activity observed here is thus consistent with tSMS-induced increase of intracortical excitability of the SMA, presumably concomitant with reduced baseline activity of specific populations of cortical integrator neurons14.
Intriguingly, the overall higher local activity we observed in the left compared to the right SMA—both in our dataset and in an independent sample from the HCP—implies that even though the placement of the magnet was symmetric, the baseline functional state of the SMA below the magnet was not. This baseline functional asymmetry is not totally unexpected, since stronger resting-state functional connectivity was previously observed with left compared to right seeds in both pre-SMA101 and SMA proper102 (but see ref. 103). Its causes and implications go beyond the scope of the present work and deserve further investigation, but this asymmetry might explain why the right SMA was more responsive to tSMS than the left SMA in our subjects. The preferential tSMS boost to the right hemisphere, involved in inhibitory control and action switching13,104–106, might contribute to the more cautious action initiation observed in our behavioral experiments.
tSMS of the SMA also increased the functional connectivity between the right SMA and both the paracentral lobule and the frontotemporal cortex bilaterally, including the inferior frontal gyrus. Increased resting-state functional connectivity is not an unusual finding after the application of inhibitory NIBS techniques107–109. The increased connectivity with paracentral lobule is in agreement with recent results obtained with 1-Hz rTMS or cTBS of the left SMA109. Somewhat surprisingly, however, cTBS of the left SMA seemed to decrease rather than increasing the functional connectivity between the SMA and the inferior frontal gyrus109. This difference between the effect of tSMS compared to 1-Hz rTMS or cTBS might be due to mechanistic differences between NIBS techniques, differences in the exact location of stimulation, or to an underlying sham effect (not controlled in ref. 109). Furthermore, we cannot exclude possible interactions, in terms of metaplasticity110, between NIBS techniques and the magnetic field of the scanner25. These issues will require further investigation for the correct interpretation of resting-state fMRI results in the translation of NIBS techniques to clinical applications.
The increased connectivity between the SMA and the paracentral lobule may explain the motor components of the behavioral modulations induced by tSMS in our tasks, likely subserved by excitatory projections from the SMA proper to M13,111,112. In addition, we cannot exclude contributions from cortico-cortical and cortico-subcortical pathways between the pre-SMA and M172,113,114. Conversely, the increased connectivity with the frontotemporal cortex may contribute to the observed shift in speed-accuracy tradeoff, directly mediated by the frontal aslant tract, which is a recently described pathway that connects the SMA with the ventral premotor cortex, including the inferior frontal gyrus (Brodmann areas 44 and 45) and the precentral Brodmann area 6115–118. Brodmann areas 44 and 45 are involved in the execution of complex hand movements and sensorimotor integration119, as part of the movement initiation network82. Similarly, the frontal aslant tract also plays a critical role in the execution of visually guided hand movements120, and the inferior frontal gyrus—particularly on the right side—is a key hub in the network that controls motor inhibition, attentional control and response switching106,121,122. The inferior frontal gyrus also plays a critical role in the accumulation of evidence during action programming123–126. Enhanced connectivity between SMA and the frontotemporal clusters could thus contribute to the specific processes used to trade accuracy over speed depending on the cognitive demands in our behavioral tasks. Overall, tSMS of the SMA seems to modulate both more posterior functional networks related to the motor functions of the SMA proper and more frontal networks related the cognitive functions of the pre-SMA.
Implications for future clinical applications
tSMS is a portable, easy to apply, inexpensive NIBS technique. The present findings might thus be helpful for informing the use of SMA tSMS for future clinical applications. NIBS of the SMA have provided promising results for treating several brain disorders, such as Tourette syndrome16–18,127, obsessive compulsive disorder19, and Parkinson’s disease20,21. Intriguingly, 1-Hz rTMS of the SMA seems to improve both the cardinal features of Parkinson’s disease21 and levodopa-induced dyskinesias20. This dual effect might appear paradoxical from a univocal perspective of local SMA changes, but could be explained by the duality of the main distant pathways modulated by SMA stimulation. On the one hand, the pathway from SMA to M1 might be critical for controlling the cardinal motor features128. On the other hand, the pathway from the SMA to the inferior frontal gyrus through the frontal aslant tract might be critical for controlling levodopa-induced dyskinesias129. Beyond Parkinson’s disease, the involvement of the frontal aslant tract in both motor function and language, together with the extension of our frontotemporal clusters to the lateral precentral gyrus (i.e. face motor cortex), renders the SMA an appealing target for possible treatment of speech alterations, such as vocal tics, stuttering130, and aphasia131,132. Importantly, the possible differences between our fMRI findings with tSMS of SMA and previous findings with 1-Hz rTMS and cTBS suggests that possible clinical effects might differ—in either positive or negative ways—compared to other inhibitory techniques. The possible relationship between the neurophysiological response to a single tSMS session and the clinical response to repeated sessions (see e.g. ref. 133) will require further investigation. Furthermore, the effects of multiple-sessions of tSMS on cortical excitability, connectivity, and clinical symptoms remain to be established.
Overall, our results show that tSMS of the SMA can induce behavioral after-effects associated with the functional modulation of both local cortical circuits below the magnet and distant functionally connected cortical networks. tSMS of SMA may thus be a promising protocol for cognitive research and future clinical applications.
Methods
Subjects
A total of 65 healthy subjects (mean ± SD 30.5 ± 8.2 years old, 43 females) participated in this study. Forty-two subjects participated in Experiment 1 (31.5 ± 8.8 years old, 28 females), 20 subjects in Experiment 2 (28.5 ± 5.2 years old, 9 females), and 10 subjects participating in both. Seventeen subjects (13 had not participated in the previous experiments) participated in the follow-up Experiment 1c. Sample sizes were conservatively determined based on common practice in the relevant literature. Subjects were recruited from our institution and among students from our university. All subjects gave their informed consent. The study was performed according to the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the local Ethics Committee (Comité Ético de Investigación de HM Hospitales). All experiments were performed at CINAC, Hospital Universitario HM Puerta del Sur, Móstoles, Madrid, Spain.
tSMS protocol
In all experiments, a cylindrical nickel-plated NdFeB magnet of 45 mm diameter, 30 mm thickness, and 360 g weight (MAG45r; Neurek SL, Toledo, Spain; the Big Magnet in ref. 22) was used for tSMS, while a non-magnetic steel cylinder, with the same size, weight, and appearance of the magnet, was used for sham stimulation (MAG45s; Neurek SL, Toledo, Spain). tSMS (or sham) was applied for 30 min with south polarity over the SMA, centered 3 cm anterior to Cz (except in the control Experiment 1c, in which tSMS was applied over the right M1, centered at C4). Subjects were seated comfortably in a semi-darkened room, and were instructed to refrain from speaking and to remain awake while in a calm, relaxed state. At the end of each session (i.e. after the end of the tasks in Experiments 1a–c, and after the post-tSMS fMRI recordings in Experiment 2), subjects were asked to guess whether they received real or sham tSMS. The duration of tSMS application was chosen based on our recent findings showing that 30-min tSMS over M1 reduces corticospinal excitability for at least 30 min after the end of the application31. The application of tSMS for 30 min is a safe procedure134. In a sample of 10 subjects, we used MRI-guided neuronavigation (Brainsight) to confirm that the target corresponded to the SMA (Fig. 1a). The variability of SMA tSMS targeting was negligible on the mediolateral axis (SD: 1.3 mm) compared to the anteroposterior axis (10.7 mm).
Experiment 1: Behavioral protocol
Experiment 1a was a randomized double-blind, sham-controlled study with a parallel design (Fig. 1b). Two independent groups of subjects received either tSMS (n = 20) or sham (n = 22) and, immediately after, performed three CRT tasks (based on ref. 135). A subgroup of subjects (n = 16) also repeated the experiment in a randomized double-blind, sham-controlled crossover design, with at least 1 week between the two experimental sessions (Experiment 1b). Experiment 1c was an additional follow-up experiment in a group of subjects (n = 17) who received tSMS over the right M1 instead of SMA in a single-blind design (i.e. subjects were told that they would receive either tSMS or sham, but they all received tSMS).
The CRT tasks consisted of a fully-cued task to assess withholding of predicted actions, an uncued-compatible task to assess motor planning, and an uncued-incompatible task to assess stimulus–response compatibility during motor choice behavior (Fig. 2a). The tasks were performed only after tSMS (or sham) to avoid learning and cumulative effects. In all tasks, two movement parameters were manipulated: hand (right vs. left) and direction (up vs. down), corresponding to four target buttons. Two additional central buttons represented the rest position at the beginning of each trial. For each task, a fixation screen (empty circles) initiated the trial. Subsequently a cue was shown for different inter-stimulus intervals (ISI: 0–200–400–800–1600–3200 ms) with different information quantity on each task. In the fully-cued task, the cue presented one empty circle positioned on top/bottom or right/left position. Subjects were instructed to wait before responding to the corresponding position until the circle filled (go signal). In the uncued-compatible task, the cue showed four empty circles, and therefore showing no prior information of where the movement should be executed. Subjects waited until one filled circle appeared to initiate a movement to the corresponding button. In the uncued-incompatible task, the cue also presented four empty circles, whereby subjects had to wait until one circle filled. However, in this condition, the correct movement was to the button diagonally opposite to that indicated by the filled circle. We calculated four behavioral measures per task and movement position: initiation time (time from the go signal to the release of the home button); movement time (time from the release of the home button till reaching the target button); reaction time (initiation time + movement time); and error rate (percent of incorrectly performed trials). Initiation times, movement times, and reaction times were calculated as the average over correct trials. Each task consisted of a block of 75 trials, with 15 trials at each of the five ISI intervals randomly mixed. An approximately equal right- and left-hand responses were incorporated (counterbalanced). Within each group, the order of the CRT conditions was counterbalanced. The execution of the three tasks lasted 21.5 ± 5.3 min.
Experiment 1: Drift-diffusion model
Initiation times from Experiment 1a were fitted to drift-diffusion models41. When applying drift-diffusion models to our data, we are reducing our CRT tasks to two-choice decisions, with the following assumptions: in the fully-cued task we are modeling the binary go/no-go decision of when to respond (the where is given by the cue); in the uncued-compatible and uncued-incompatible tasks we are modeling a binary choice of correct vs. incorrect movement (e.g. left vs. right hand), respectively without and with stimulus-response conflict. Importantly, although these assumptions of binary decisions limit the interpretation of the model parameters in absolute terms, our interest is not on the absolute values of the parameters but on their differences between tSMS and sham sessions.
Due to the low number of errors, for each task and stimulation group we pooled the data from all subjects into a single population distribution—similarly to previous studies136–138 after eliminating initiation times >1000 ms and counting initiation times <150 ms as errors (pooled n = 1400–1530 trials). Pooling across subjects is justified by the fact that we are not interested in the cognitive mechanisms operating at the level of individual minds, but on the overall effects of tSMS compared to sham at the population level. The resulting population distributions (Fig. 2f) were modeled with the EZ-diffusion model, which analytically estimates decision threshold separation a, drift rate of evidence accumulation v and non-decision time Ter from the probability of correct responses Pc, and the mean and variance of the reaction times (MRT and VRT, respectively), according to the following equations139:
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
where MDT is the mean decision time and s is a scaling parameter set at an arbitrary value that represents the standard deviation of the change in the accumulated evidence (s = 1 here). Intuitively, the accuracy Pc and the variance of reaction times VRT determine the drift rate v, the decision threshold a, and the mean decision time MDT. Consequently, the mean reaction time MRT solely contributes to the non-decision time Ter. Importantly, even though the EZ-diffusion model makes the simplistic assumption of no across-trials variability in the parameters139, it was suggested to provide unbiased estimates of relative parameter changes between conditions140 and to be more powerful than the full diffusion model for detecting experimental effects, even on data generated from the full diffusion model141.
Experiment 2: fMRI protocol
Experiment 2 was a randomized double-blind, sham-controlled study with a crossover design (Fig. 1c). One group of subjects (n = 20) underwent MRI scans before and immediately after tSMS or sham. Each subject underwent two experimental sessions, at least 1 week apart. MRI scans were performed in a 3.0 T scanner (mMR Biograph, Siemens, Erlangen, Germany), with the following acquisition protocol: three-dimensional (3D) T1-weighted magnetization prepared-rapid gradient-echo image with parameters TR/TE/TI 2300/3.34/900 ms, flip angle 8°, and isotropic spatial resolution 1 mm3 (FoV: 256 mm, matrix: 256 × 256, slice thickness: 1 mm); resting-state fMRI using a single-shot gradient-echo planar imaging (EPI) 2D pulse sequence, with acquisition parameters TR/TE 2400/30 ms, optimum flip angle using the Ernst equation (i.e. 79°), isotropic spatial resolution of 3 mm3 (field of view: 192 mm; matrix: 64 × 64; slice thickness: 3 mm), and acceleration factor through parallel imaging × 2 (IPAT2) − the acquisition of this sequence lasted 10 min giving rise to 250 fMRI volumes; and a fieldmap generated from two 2D gradient-echo images, with acquisition parameters TR/TE1/TE2 455/4.92/7.38 ms, flip angle 60°, and same spatial resolution as the fMRI acquisition. During fMRI acquisition, subjects were instructed to remain calm, motionless, without engaging in any cognitive task, and to remain eyes open to avoid falling asleep.
Experiment 2: Resting-state fMRI pre-processing
T1-weighted MRI images were corrected for intensity bias using N4 bias-correction algorithm142, denoized using an optimized non-local means filter143, skull stripped using BET-FSL144, and segmented in white matter, gray matter, and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) tissues using FAST-FSL145. Skull-stripped images were non-linearly registered to the MNI152 (1 mm3) space using an affine transformation followed by diffeomorphic symmetric normalization in ANTs (Advanced Normalization Tools)146. To avoid partial volume contamination from gray matter into the white matter and CSF masks, white matter and CSF masks were eroded. Eroded CSF mask was confined to the ventricles using the mask provided in FSL.
The first five volumes of the resting-state fMRI scan were discarded to allow signal stabilization. Then, the fMRI series were de-spiked, corrected for slice timing, realigned to the first volume, skull stripped, and corrected for EPI distortion using the fieldmap. A scrubbing process was carried out147, taking three data-quality metrics: frame-wise displacement, the derivative of the root mean square function across voxels (DVARS), and the standard deviation. Both DVARS and standard deviation were computed for motion corrected images using a gray matter mask. Resting-state fMRI volumes overpassing a conservative value of 0.3 for frame-wise displacement, or a cut-off of 75% percentile + 1.5 × inter-quartile range for DVARS and standard deviation were tagged as noisy volumes. Although the noisy samples were not removed from the series during pre-processing, they were not considered for the estimation of functional connectivity147,148.
The mean of the resting-state fMRI series was subtracted, and linear trends were removed. A linear affine transformation was obtained between the average resting-state fMRI volume and the T1-weighted image using ANTS. The inverse of this transformation was applied to the white matter and ventricle CSF masks in order to extract white matter and ventricle CSF average resting-state fMRI activity. The nuisance regressors matrix contained the white matter and ventricle CSF average time-series, the six motion-correction residuals, and their derivatives. All 16 regressors were Z-scored, not considering the noisy volumes, and were regressed out from the dataset. The resulting residuals were band-pass filtered (0.009–0.08 Hz) using a second-order Butterworth filter. Pre-processed resting-state fMRI was normalized to MNI152 (3 mm3) and smoothed using a 8-mm full-width at half-maximum Gaussian kernel.
Experiment 2: Resting-state fMRI analysis
Left and right SMA ROIs were extracted from the AAL2 atlas46. For each ROI, we estimated the following measures: the power of the resting-state fMRI time-series in the selected frequency band, also known as ALFFs, computed as the variance of the time-series without considering the noisy samples; the ReHo, which quantifies the short-range temporal similarity by computing the Kendall’s coefficient of concordance149; and the functional connectivity maps, computed as the Z-transformed Pearson’s linear correlation between the resting fMRI time-series of each ROI and all voxels in the whole brain.
HCP data
We also analyzed resting-state fMRI from 49 subjects from the 2014 HCP S1200 data release, which is publicly available at http://humanconnectome.org (see ref. 150 for a detailed explanation of the entire acquisition protocol). Briefly, HCP MRI data were acquired on a 3 T Siemens Skyra-Connectome scanner. For resting-state fMRI acquisitions, the participants were with their eyes open and fixated on a cross hair on the screen. A simultaneous multi-slice pulse sequence with an acceleration factor of eight was used to acquire two resting-state fMRI runs with opposite phase encoding direction (right-left (RL) and left-right (LR)). Each run consisted of 1200 volumes with parameters TR/TE (repetition time/echo time) 720/33.1 ms, flip angle 52°, and isotropic spatial resolution 2-mm isotropic spatial resolution. Only the acquisition with LR phase encoding direction was used in here. We used the pre-processed and artifact-removed resting-state fMRI series as provided by the HCP S1200 data release151. Pre-processed series were temporally demeaned, linearly detrended, and band-pass filtered (0.009–0.08 Hz) using a second-order Butterworth filter. The ALFF was computed as the variance of the time-series in the left and right SMA ROI, as in Experiment 2, but without tagging and discarding noisy samples.
Statistics and reproducibility
In Experiment 1a (parallel design), reaction times, initiation times, movement times, and error rates were entered separately into three-way independent-measures ANOVA, with the following factors: Task (fully-cued, uncued-compatible, and uncued-incompatible), Location (upper left, lower left, upper right, lower right), and Stimulation (real, sham). Two-way follow-up ANOVAs were applied for individual tasks in case of significant interaction. The analyses were also repeated by adding a fourth factor—Guessing (correct, incorrect)—representing the ability of subjects to guess whether they had received real tSMS or sham, when asked at the end of the experiment. The same ANOVA strategy was used in Experiment 1b (crossover design, the Stimulation factor was compared within subjects) and Experiment 1c (using the sham data of Experiment 1a). The Pearson’s χ2 test was used to evaluate the a posteriori ability of subjects to discriminate whether they had received real or sham stimulation. Results were considered significant at p < 0.05.
For the drift-diffusion model analysis, we employed bootstrapping techniques for constructing 95% confidence intervals on the estimated parameters and for performing hypothesis testing on the effects of tSMS compared to sham. Namely, for each task and stimulation condition, to construct 95% confidence we obtained 100,000 bootstrapped datasets by random resampling with replacement from the measured initiation times within stimulation conditions, then we applied the EZ-diffusion model to each bootstrapped dataset, and we extracted the 2.5% and 97.5% percentile from the distribution of the estimated parameters. For performing hypothesis testing, we obtained 100,000 bootstrapped pairs of datasets by random resampling with replacement from the initiation times between stimulation conditions (i.e. from the entire dataset of both tSMS and sham groups together), we applied the EZ-diffusion model separately to each pair of bootstrapped datasets, and for each parameter of the model (a, v, and Ter), we estimated the p value as the probability that the absolute difference between the bootstrapped pair of datasets was greater than the absolute difference between the actual tSMS-sham values. The estimated p value represents the probability that the model parameters estimated from the tSMS and sham groups are drawn from the same statistical distribution of initiation times (null hypothesis), or from different distributions (alternative hypothesis). Since we had three tasks, we applied Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons (multiplying p values by 3). Results were considered significant at p < 0.05.
In Experiment 2, ALFF (log-transformed) and ReHo were separately entered into three-way repeated-measures ANOVA, with Time (pre, post stimulation), Stimulation (real, sham), and Side (left, right ROI) as main factors, followed by follow-up ANOVAs for each Stimulation condition in case of significant interaction and Tukey’s post hoc tests. Post–pre stimulation values were also separately entered into two-way repeated-measures ANOVA, followed by Tukey’s post hoc tests. Results were considered significant at p < 0.05. Group functional connectivity maps for left and right SMA were separately obtained by running a random-effect second-level analysis in SPM12, using a one-sample t test. Statistical maps showing group connectivity were thresholded at the voxel level (p < 0.05, FWE-corrected for multiple comparisons) to define inclusion masks of positive baseline functional connectivity prior to the statistical comparison between real and sham tSMS. The effect of tSMS was separately tested for left and right SMA functional connectivity by considering the Time × Stimulation interaction significant at puncorr < 0.001, with FWE correction at the cluster level (pcluster < 0.05). ALFF (log-transformed) and ReHo were also calculated in the distant clusters of significant functional connectivity and separately entered into three-way repeated-measures ANOVA, with Time (pre, post stimulation), Stimulation (real, sham), and Cluster as main factors, followed by Fisher’s least significant difference post hoc tests. Results were considered significant at p < 0.05. Again, the Pearson’s χ2 test was used to evaluate the a posteriori ability of subjects to discriminate whether they had received real or sham stimulation.
In the Human Connectome Project Data, left vs. right ALFF values were compared with a two-sided paired t test.
Reporting summary
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Research Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by MINECO/AEI/FEDER-UE (SAF2016-80647-R and SAF2017-86246-R) and The Michael J. Fox Foundation (Grant 9205). J.A.P.-P. was supported by the Spanish Ministry of Education through the National Program Juan de la Cierva (FJCI-2015-25095). I.O. was supported by the Carlos III Health Institute with a post-doctoral grant (Sara Borrell, CD15/00092). Data were provided in part by the Human Connectome Project, WU-Minn Consortium (Principal Investigators: David Van Essen and Kamil Ugurbil; 1U54MH091657) funded by the 16 NIH Institutes and Centers that support the NIH Blueprint for Neuroscience Research, and by the McDonnell Center for Systems Neuroscience at Washington University.
Author contributions
J.A.P.-P., I.O., A.O., and G.F. designed the study. J.A.P.-P., I.O., and G.F. performed the behavioral and imaging experiments. P.G. contributed to behavioral experiments and analyses. J.A.P.-P. performed imaging analysis. G.F. performed statistical analysis. J.A.P.-P and G.F. prepared the figures. J.A.P.-P., I.O., and G.F. drafted the first version of the manuscript. J.A.P.-P., I.O., P.G., M.D., B.A.S., J.A.O., A.O., and G.F. provided critical review and edited the manuscript.
Data availability
The datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are available in the OSF repository, https://osf.io/n3au4/.
Competing interests
A.O. and G.F. declare the following competing interests: are cofounders of the company Neurek SL, which is a spinoff of the Foundation of the Hospital Nacional de Parapléjicos, and inventors listed on the following patents: P201030610 and PCT/ES2011/070290 (patent abandoned). J.A.P.-P., I.O., P.G., M.D., B.A.S., and J.A.O. declare no competing financial or non-financial interests.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
These authors contributed equally: José A. Pineda-Pardo, Ignacio Obeso.
Supplementary information
Supplementary information is available for this paper at 10.1038/s42003-019-0643-8.
References
- 1.Nachev P, Kennard C, Husain M. Functional role of the supplementary and pre-supplementary motor areas. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2008;9:856–869. doi: 10.1038/nrn2478. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Matsuzaka Y, Aizawa H, Tanji J. A motor area rostral to the supplementary motor area (presupplementary motor area) in the monkey: neuronal activity during a learned motor task. J. Neurophysiol. 1992;68:653–662. doi: 10.1152/jn.1992.68.3.653. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Luppino G, Matelli M, Camarda R, Rizzolatti G. Corticocortical connections of area F3 (SMA-proper) and area F6 (pre-SMA) in the macaque monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 1993;338:114–140. doi: 10.1002/cne.903380109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Mushiake H, Inase M, Tanji J. Neuronal activity in the primate premotor, supplementary, and precentral motor cortex during visually guided and internally determined sequential movements. J. Neurophysiol. 1991;66:705–718. doi: 10.1152/jn.1991.66.3.705. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Tanji J, Shima K. Role for supplementary motor area cells in planning several movements ahead. Nature. 1994;371:413–416. doi: 10.1038/371413a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Gerloff C, Corwell B, Chen R, Hallett M, Cohen LG. Stimulation over the human supplementary motor area interferes with the organization of future elements in complex motor sequences. Brain. 1997;120:1587–1602. doi: 10.1093/brain/120.9.1587. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Hanakawa T, Dimyan MA, Hallett M. Motor planning, imagery, and execution in the distributed motor network: a time-course study with functional MRI. Cereb. Cortex. 2008;18:2775–2788. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Wang Y, Isoda M, Matsuzaka Y, Shima K, Tanji J. Prefrontal cortical cells projecting to the supplementary eye field and presupplementary motor area in the monkey. Neurosci. Res. 2005;53:1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2005.05.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Matsuzaka Y, Tanji J. Changing directions of forthcoming arm movements: neuronal activity in the presupplementary and supplementary motor area of monkey cerebral cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 1996;76:2327–2342. doi: 10.1152/jn.1996.76.4.2327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Kennerley SW, Sakai K, Rushworth MF. Organization of action sequences and the role of the pre-SMA. J. Neurophysiol. 2004;91:978–993. doi: 10.1152/jn.00651.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Floden D, Stuss DT. Inhibitory control is slowed in patients with right superior medial frontal damage. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2006;18:1843–1849. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.11.1843. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Swick D, Ashley V, Turken U. Are the neural correlates of stopping and not going identical? Quantitative meta-analysis of two response inhibition tasks. Neuroimage. 2011;56:1655–1665. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Obeso I, Robles N, Marron EM, Redolar-Ripoll D. Dissociating the role of the pre-SMA in response inhibition and switching: a combined online and offline TMS approach. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013;7:150. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00150. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Bogacz R, Wagenmakers EJ, Forstmann BU, Nieuwenhuis S. The neural basis of the speed-accuracy tradeoff. Trends Neurosci. 2010;33:10–16. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2009.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Weigard, A., Beltz, A., Reddy, S. N. & Wilson, S. J. Characterizing the role of the pre-SMA in the control of speed/accuracy trade-off with directed functional connectivity mapping and multiple solution reduction. Hum. Brain Mapp. 40, 1829–1843 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 16.Le K, Liu L, Sun M, Hu L, Xiao N. Transcranial magnetic stimulation at 1 Hertz improves clinical symptoms in children with Tourette syndrome for at least 6 months. J. Clin. Neurosci. 2013;20:257–262. doi: 10.1016/j.jocn.2012.01.049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Wu SW, et al. Functional MRI-navigated repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation over supplementary motor area in chronic tic disorders. Brain Stimul. 2014;7:212–218. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2013.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Landeros-Weisenberger A, et al. Randomized sham controlled double-blind trial of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation for adults with severe Tourette syndrome. Brain Stimul. 2015;8:574–581. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2014.11.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.D’Urso G, et al. Transcranial direct current stimulation for obsessive-compulsive disorder: a randomized, controlled, partial crossover trial. Depress. Anxiety. 2016;33:1132–1140. doi: 10.1002/da.22578. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Koch G, et al. rTMS of supplementary motor area modulates therapy-induced dyskinesias in Parkinson disease. Neurology. 2005;65:623–625. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000172861.36430.95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Shirota Y, et al. Supplementary motor area stimulation for Parkinson disease: a randomized controlled study. Neurology. 2013;80:1400–1405. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31828c2f66. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Oliviero A, et al. Transcranial static magnetic field stimulation of the human motor cortex. J. Physiol. 2011;589:4949–4958. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2011.211953. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Silbert BI, Pevcic DD, Patterson HI, Windnagel KA, Thickbroom GW. Inverse correlation between resting motor threshold and corticomotor excitability after static magnetic stimulation of human motor cortex. Brain Stimul. 2013;6:817–820. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2013.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Kirimoto H, et al. Effect of transcranial static magnetic field stimulation over the sensorimotor cortex on somatosensory evoked potentials in humans. Brain Stimul. 2014;7:836–840. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2014.09.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Nojima I, Koganemaru S, Fukuyama H, Mima T. Static magnetic field can transiently alter the human intracortical inhibitory system. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2015;126:2314–2319. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2015.01.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Kirimoto H, Asao A, Tamaki H, Onishi H. Non-invasive modulation of somatosensory evoked potentials by the application of static magnetic fields over the primary and supplementary motor cortices. Sci. Rep. 2016;6:34509. doi: 10.1038/srep34509. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Nojima I, Koganemaru S, Mima T. Combination of static magnetic fields and peripheral nerve stimulation can alter focal cortical excitability. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2016;10:598. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00598. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Arias P, Adan-Arcay L, Puerta-Catoira B, Madrid A, Cudeiro J. Transcranial static magnetic field stimulation of M1 reduces corticospinal excitability without distorting sensorimotor integration in humans. Brain Stimul. 2017;10:340–342. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2017.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Dileone M, et al. Dopamine-dependent changes of cortical excitability induced by transcranial static magnetic field stimulation in Parkinson’s disease. Sci. Rep. 2017;7:4329. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-04254-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Kirimoto H, et al. Transcranial static magnetic field stimulation over the primary motor cortex induces plastic changes in cortical nociceptive processing. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2018;12:63. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Dileone M, Mordillo-Mateos L, Oliviero A, Foffani G. Long-lasting effects of transcranial static magnetic field stimulation on motor cortex excitability. Brain Stimul. 2018;11:676–688. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2018.02.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Davila-Perez P, Pascual-Leone A, Cudeiro J. Effects of transcranial static magnetic stimulation on motor cortex evaluated by different TMS waveforms and current directions. Neuroscience. 2019;413:22–30. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2019.05.065. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Gonzalez-Rosa JJ, et al. Static magnetic field stimulation over the visual cortex increases alpha oscillations and slows visual search in humans. J. Neurosci. 2015;35:9182–9193. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4232-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Carrasco-Lopez C, et al. Static magnetic field stimulation over parietal cortex enhances somatosensory detection in humans. J. Neurosci. 2017;37:3840–3847. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2123-16.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Kufner M, Bruckner S, Kammer T. No modulatory effects by transcranial static magnetic field stimulation of human motor and somatosensory cortex. Brain Stimul. 2017;10:703–710. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2017.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Foffani G, Dileone M. No modulatory effects by tSMS when delivered during a cognitive task. Brain Stimul. 2017;10:867. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2017.04.121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Sheffield, A., Ahn, S., Alagapan, S. & Frohlich, F. Modulating neural oscillations by transcranial static magnetic field stimulation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: a crossover, double-blind, sham-controlled pilot study. Eur. J. Neurosci. 49, 250–262 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 38.Nojima I, et al. Transcranial static magnetic stimulation over the primary motor cortex alters sequential implicit motor learning. Neurosci. Lett. 2018;696:33–37. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2018.12.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Nakagawa, K., Sasaki, A. & Nakazawa, K. Accuracy in pinch force control can be altered by static magnetic field stimulation over the primary motor cortex. Neuromodulation10.1111/ner.12912 (2019). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 40.Lacroix A, et al. Static magnetic stimulation of the primary motor cortex impairs online but not offline motor sequence learning. Sci. Rep. 2019;9:9886. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-46379-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Ratcliff R, Smith PL, Brown SD, McKoon G. Diffusion Decision Model: current issues and history. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2016;20:260–281. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2016.01.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Duff EP, Makin T, Cottaar M, Smith SM, Woolrich MW. Disambiguating brain functional connectivity. Neuroimage. 2018;173:540–550. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.01.053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Lozano-Soto, E. et al. Transcranial static magnetic field stimulation (tSMS) of the visual cortex decreases experimental photophobia. Cephalalgia10.1177/0333102417736899 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 44.Rivadulla C, Foffani G, Oliviero A. Magnetic field strength and reproducibility of neodymium magnets useful for transcranial static magnetic field stimulation of the human cortex. Neuromodulation. 2014;17:438–441. doi: 10.1111/ner.12125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Tharayil JJ, Goetz SM, Bernabei JM, Peterchev AV. Field distribution of transcranial static magnetic stimulation in realistic human head model. Neuromodulation. 2018;21:340–347. doi: 10.1111/ner.12699. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Tzourio-Mazoyer N, et al. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage. 2002;15:273–289. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0978. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Maret G, Dransfeld K. Macromolecules and membranes in high magnetic fields. Phys. B+C. 1977;86:1077–1083. doi: 10.1016/0378-4363(77)90800-2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Tenforde TS, Liburdy RP. Magnetic deformation of phospholipid bilayers: effects on liposome shape and solute permeability at prephase transition temperatures. J. Theor. Biol. 1988;133:385–396. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5193(88)80329-1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Azanza MJ, del Moral A. Cell membrane biochemistry and neurobiological approach to biomagnetism. Prog. Neurobiol. 1994;44:517–601. doi: 10.1016/0301-0082(94)90004-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Rosen AD. Mechanism of action of moderate-intensity static magnetic fields on biological systems. Cell Biochem. Biophys. 2003;39:163–173. doi: 10.1385/CBB:39:2:163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Hughes S, El Haj AJ, Dobson J, Martinac B. The influence of static magnetic fields on mechanosensitive ion channel activity in artificial liposomes. Eur. Biophys. J. 2005;34:461–468. doi: 10.1007/s00249-005-0484-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.McLean MJ, Holcomb RR, Wamil AW, Pickett JD, Cavopol AV. Blockade of sensory neuron action potentials by a static magnetic field in the 10 mT range. Bioelectromagnetics. 1995;16:20–32. doi: 10.1002/bem.2250160108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Cavopol AV, Wamil AW, Holcomb RR, McLean MJ. Measurement and analysis of static magnetic fields that block action potentials in cultured neurons. Bioelectromagnetics. 1995;16:197–206. doi: 10.1002/bem.2250160308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Carlsen AN, Eagles JS, MacKinnon CD. Transcranial direct current stimulation over the supplementary motor area modulates the preparatory activation level in the human motor system. Behav. Brain Res. 2015;279:68–75. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.11.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Spieser L, van den Wildenberg W, Hasbroucq T, Ridderinkhof KR, Burle B. Controlling your impulses: electrical stimulation of the human supplementary motor complex prevents impulsive errors. J. Neurosci. 2015;35:3010–3015. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1642-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Hayduk-Costa G, Drummond NM, Carlsen AN. Anodal tDCS over SMA decreases the probability of withholding an anticipated action. Behav. Brain Res. 2013;257:208–214. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.09.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Zandbelt BB, Bloemendaal M, Hoogendam JM, Kahn RS, Vink M. Transcranial magnetic stimulation and functional MRI reveal cortical and subcortical interactions during stop-signal response inhibition. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2013;25:157–174. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Obeso I, et al. Stimulation of the pre-SMA influences cerebral blood flow in frontal areas involved with inhibitory control of action. Brain Stimul. 2013;6:769–776. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2013.02.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Obeso I, et al. Theta burst magnetic stimulation over the pre-supplementary motor area improves motor inhibition. Brain Stimul. 2017;10:944–951. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2017.05.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Hsu TY, et al. Modulating inhibitory control with direct current stimulation of the superior medial frontal cortex. Neuroimage. 2011;56:2249–2257. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.03.059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Watanabe T, et al. Effects of rTMS of pre-supplementary motor area on fronto basal ganglia network activity during stop-signal task. J. Neurosci. 2015;35:4813–4823. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3761-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Hyland B, Chen DF, Maier V, Palmeri A, Wiesendanger M. What is the role of the supplementary motor area in movement initiation? Prog. Brain Res. 1989;80:431–436. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(08)62240-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Rushworth MF, Walton ME, Kennerley SW, Bannerman DM. Action sets and decisions in the medial frontal cortex. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2004;8:410–417. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.07.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Ridderinkhof KR, Ullsperger M, Crone EA, Nieuwenhuis S. The role of the medial frontal cortex in cognitive control. Science. 2004;306:443–447. doi: 10.1126/science.1100301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Tosun T, Berkay D, Sack AT, Cakmak YO, Balci F. Inhibition of pre-supplementary motor area by continuous theta burst stimulation leads to more cautious decision-making and more efficient sensory evidence integration. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2017;29:1433–1444. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Berkay D, Eser HY, Sack AT, Cakmak YO, Balci F. The modulatory role of pre-SMA in speed-accuracy tradeoff: a bi-directional TMS study. Neuropsychologia. 2018;109:255–261. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.12.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Georgiev D, et al. Continuous theta burst stimulation over the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and the pre-sma alter drift rate and response thresholds respectively during perceptual decision-making. Brain Stimul. 2016;9:601–608. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2016.04.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Rowe JB, Hughes L, Nimmo-Smith I. Action selection: a race model for selected and non-selected actions distinguishes the contribution of premotor and prefrontal areas. Neuroimage. 2010;51:888–896. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.02.045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Rushworth MF, Hadland KA, Paus T, Sipila PK. Role of the human medial frontal cortex in task switching: a combined fMRI and TMS study. J. Neurophysiol. 2002;87:2577–2592. doi: 10.1152/jn.2002.87.5.2577. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Wittfoth M, Buck D, Fahle M, Herrmann M. Comparison of two Simon tasks: neuronal correlates of conflict resolution based on coherent motion perception. Neuroimage. 2006;32:921–929. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.03.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Chen Z, Lei X, Ding C, Li H, Chen A. The neural mechanisms of semantic and response conflicts: an fMRI study of practice-related effects in the Stroop task. Neuroimage. 2013;66:577–584. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.10.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Duque J, Olivier E, Rushworth M. Top-down inhibitory control exerted by the medial frontal cortex during action selection under conflict. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2013;25:1634–1648. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00421. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Mückschel M, Stock AK, Dippel G, Chmielewski W, Beste C. Interacting sources of interference during sensorimotor integration processes. Neuroimage. 2016;125:342–349. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.09.075. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Li Q, et al. Conflict detection and resolution rely on a combination of common and distinct cognitive control networks. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2017;83:123–131. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.09.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Karayanidis F, et al. Anticipatory reconfiguration elicited by fully and partially informative cues that validly predict a switch in task. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2009;9:202–215. doi: 10.3758/CABN.9.2.202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Madden DJ, et al. Cerebral white matter integrity mediates adult age differences in cognitive performance. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2009;21:289–302. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Schmitz F, Voss A. Components of task switching: a closer look at task switching and cue switching. Acta Psychol. (Amst.) 2014;151:184–196. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.06.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Teichert T, Grinband J, Ferrera V. The importance of decision onset. J. Neurophysiol. 2016;115:643–661. doi: 10.1152/jn.00274.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Teichert T, Ferrera VP, Grinband J. Humans optimize decision-making by delaying decision onset. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e89638. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0089638. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Roitman JD, Shadlen MN. Response of neurons in the lateral intraparietal area during a combined visual discrimination reaction time task. J. Neurosci. 2002;22:9475–9489. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-21-09475.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Mazurek ME, Roitman JD, Ditterich J, Shadlen MN. A role for neural integrators in perceptual decision making. Cereb. Cortex. 2003;13:1257–1269. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhg097. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Hoffstaedter F, Grefkes C, Zilles K, Eickhoff SB. The “what” and “when” of self-initiated movements. Cereb. Cortex. 2013;23:520–530. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr391. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Klein TA, et al. Neural correlates of error awareness. Neuroimage. 2007;34:1774–1781. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.11.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Braun N, et al. The senses of agency and ownership: a review. Front. Psychol. 2018;9:535. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00535. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Moore JW, Ruge D, Wenke D, Rothwell J, Haggard P. Disrupting the experience of control in the human brain: pre-supplementary motor area contributes to the sense of agency. Proc. Biol. Sci. 2010;277:2503–2509. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.0404. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Kuhn S, Brass M, Haggard P. Feeling in control: neural correlates of experience of agency. Cortex. 2013;49:1935–1942. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Crivelli D, Balconi M. The agent brain: a review of non-invasive brain stimulation studies on sensing agency. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2017;11:229. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00229. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Zou Q, Wu CW, Stein EA, Zang Y, Yang Y. Static and dynamic characteristics of cerebral blood flow during the resting state. Neuroimage. 2009;48:515–524. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.07.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Li Z, Zhu Y, Childress AR, Detre JA, Wang Z. Relations between BOLD fMRI-derived resting brain activity and cerebral blood flow. PLoS ONE. 2012;7:e44556. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0044556. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Jiang L, Zuo XN. Regional homogeneity: a multimodal, multiscale neuroimaging marker of the human connectome. Neuroscientist. 2016;22:486–505. doi: 10.1177/1073858415595004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Bernier M, Croteau E, Castellano CA, Cunnane SC, Whittingstall K. Spatial distribution of resting-state BOLD regional homogeneity as a predictor of brain glucose uptake: a study in healthy aging. Neuroimage. 2017;150:14–22. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.01.055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Ivanoff J, Branning P, Marois R. fMRI evidence for a dual process account of the speed-accuracy tradeoff in decision-making. PLoS ONE. 2008;3:e2635. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0002635. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Forstmann BU, et al. Striatum and pre-SMA facilitate decision-making under time pressure. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2008;105:17538–17542. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0805903105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.van Veen V, Krug MK, Carter CS. The neural and computational basis of controlled speed-accuracy tradeoff during task performance. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2008;20:1952–1965. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Mansfield EL, Karayanidis F, Jamadar S, Heathcote A, Forstmann BU. Adjustments of response threshold during task switching: a model-based functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:14688–14692. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2390-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Vallesi A, McIntosh AR, Crescentini C, Stuss DT. fMRI investigation of speed-accuracy strategy switching. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2012;33:1677–1688. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21312. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Lee L, et al. Acute remapping within the motor system induced by low-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. J. Neurosci. 2003;23:5308–5318. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-12-05308.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Rounis E, et al. Frequency specific changes in regional cerebral blood flow and motor system connectivity following rTMS to the primary motor cortex. Neuroimage. 2005;26:164–176. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.01.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Lang N, et al. How does transcranial DC stimulation of the primary motor cortex alter regional neuronal activity in the human brain? Eur. J. Neurosci. 2005;22:495–504. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04233.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Merzagora AC, et al. Prefrontal hemodynamic changes produced by anodal direct current stimulation. Neuroimage. 2010;49:2304–2310. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Lou W, Peck KK, Brennan N, Mallela A, Holodny A. Left-lateralization of resting state functional connectivity between the presupplementary motor area and primary language areas. NeuroReport. 2017;28:545–550. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0000000000000783. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Yan LR, et al. Network asymmetry of motor areas revealed by resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging. Behav. Brain Res. 2012;227:125–133. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.11.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Dinomais M, et al. Hemispheric asymmetry of supplementary motor area proper: a functional connectivity study of the motor network. Mot. Control. 2016;20:33–49. doi: 10.1123/mc.2014-0076. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Rubia K, et al. Mapping motor inhibition: conjunctive brain activations across different versions of go/no-go and stop tasks. Neuroimage. 2001;13:250–261. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0685. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Aron AR, et al. Converging evidence for a fronto-basal-ganglia network for inhibitory control of action and cognition. J. Neurosci. 2007;27:11860–11864. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3644-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Kenner NM, et al. Inhibitory motor control in response stopping and response switching. J. Neurosci. 2010;30:8512–8518. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1096-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Eldaief MC, Halko MA, Buckner RL, Pascual-Leone A. Transcranial magnetic stimulation modulates the brain’s intrinsic activity in a frequency-dependent manner. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2011;108:21229–21234. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1113103109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Gratton C, Lee TG, Nomura EM, D’Esposito M. The effect of theta-burst TMS on cognitive control networks measured with resting state fMRI. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2013;7:124. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Ji GJ, Yu F, Liao W, Wang K. Dynamic aftereffects in supplementary motor network following inhibitory transcranial magnetic stimulation protocols. Neuroimage. 2017;149:285–294. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.01.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Muller-Dahlhaus F, Ziemann U. Metaplasticity in human cortex. Neuroscientist. 2015;21:185–202. doi: 10.1177/1073858414526645. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Arai N, Lu MK, Ugawa Y, Ziemann U. Effective connectivity between human supplementary motor area and primary motor cortex: a paired-coil TMS study. Exp. Brain Res. 2012;220:79–87. doi: 10.1007/s00221-012-3117-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Shirota Y, et al. Increased primary motor cortical excitability by a single-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation over the supplementary motor area. Exp. Brain Res. 2012;219:339–349. doi: 10.1007/s00221-012-3095-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Mars RB, et al. Short-latency influence of medial frontal cortex on primary motor cortex during action selection under conflict. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:6926–6931. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1396-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Neubert FX, Mars RB, Buch ER, Olivier E, Rushworth MF. Cortical and subcortical interactions during action reprogramming and their related white matter pathways. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2010;107:13240–13245. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1000674107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Ford A, McGregor KM, Case K, Crosson B, White KD. Structural connectivity of Broca’s area and medial frontal cortex. Neuroimage. 2010;52:1230–1237. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Catani M, et al. Short frontal lobe connections of the human brain. Cortex. 2012;48:273–291. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.12.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Thiebaut de Schotten M, Dell’Acqua F, Valabregue R, Catani M. Monkey to human comparative anatomy of the frontal lobe association tracts. Cortex. 2012;48:82–96. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.10.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Vergani F, et al. White matter connections of the supplementary motor area in humans. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry. 2014;85:1377–1385. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2013-307492. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Binkofski F, Buccino G. Motor functions of the Broca’s region. Brain Lang. 2004;89:362–369. doi: 10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00358-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Budisavljevic S, et al. The role of the frontal aslant tract and premotor connections in visually guided hand movements. Neuroimage. 2017;146:419–428. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.10.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Hampshire A, Chamberlain SR, Monti MM, Duncan J, Owen AM. The role of the right inferior frontal gyrus: inhibition and attentional control. Neuroimage. 2010;50:1313–1319. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Aron AR, Robbins TW, Poldrack RA. Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex: one decade on. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2014;18:177–185. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123.Philiastides MG, Sajda P. EEG-informed fMRI reveals spatiotemporal characteristics of perceptual decision making. J. Neurosci. 2007;27:13082–13091. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3540-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124.Noppeney U, Ostwald D, Werner S. Perceptual decisions formed by accumulation of audiovisual evidence in prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 2010;30:7434–7446. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0455-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125.White CN, et al. Decomposing decision components in the stop-signal task: a model-based approach to individual differences in inhibitory control. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2014;26:1601–1614. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00567. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126.Mulder MJ, van Maanen L, Forstmann BU. Perceptual decision neurosciences—a model-based review. Neuroscience. 2014;277:872–884. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.07.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127.Bloch Y, Arad S, Levkovitz Y. Deep TMS add-on treatment for intractable Tourette syndrome: a feasibility study. World J. Biol. Psychiatry. 2016;17:557–561. doi: 10.3109/15622975.2014.964767. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 128.Pollok B, et al. Increased SMA-M1 coherence in Parkinson’s disease—pathophysiology or compensation? Exp. Neurol. 2013;247:178–181. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2013.04.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129.Cerasa A, et al. A network centred on the inferior frontal cortex is critically involved in levodopa-induced dyskinesias. Brain. 2015;138:414–427. doi: 10.1093/brain/awu329. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 130.Kronfeld-Duenias V, Amir O, Ezrati-Vinacour R, Civier O, Ben-Shachar M. The frontal aslant tract underlies speech fluency in persistent developmental stuttering. Brain Struct. Funct. 2016;221:365–381. doi: 10.1007/s00429-014-0912-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 131.Catani M, et al. A novel frontal pathway underlies verbal fluency in primary progressive aphasia. Brain. 2013;136:2619–2628. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 132.Mandelli ML, et al. Frontal white matter tracts sustaining speech production in primary progressive aphasia. J. Neurosci. 2014;34:9754–9767. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3464-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 133.Oliveira-Maia AJ, Press D, Pascual-Leone A. Modulation of motor cortex excitability predicts antidepressant response to prefrontal cortex repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. Brain Stimul. 2017;10:787–794. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2017.03.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 134.Oliviero A, et al. Safety study of transcranial static magnetic field stimulation (tSMS) of the human cortex. Brain Stimul. 2015;8:481–485. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2014.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 135.Jahanshahi M, Brown RG, Marsden CD. A comparative study of simple and choice reaction time in Parkinson’s, Huntington’s and cerebellar disease. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry. 1993;56:1169–1177. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.56.11.1169. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 136.Krajbich I, Armel C, Rangel A. Visual fixations and the computation and comparison of value in simple choice. Nat. Neurosci. 2010;13:1292–1298. doi: 10.1038/nn.2635. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 137.Bitzer S, Park H, Blankenburg F, Kiebel SJ. Perceptual decision making: drift-diffusion model is equivalent to a Bayesian model. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2014;8:102. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 138.Tavares G, Perona P, Rangel A. The attentional drift diffusion model of simple perceptual decision-making. Front. Neurosci. 2017;11:468. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2017.00468. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 139.Wagenmakers EJ, van der Maas HL, Grasman RP. An EZ-diffusion model for response time and accuracy. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2007;14:3–22. doi: 10.3758/BF03194023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 140.van Ravenzwaaij D, Oberauer K. How to use the diffusion model: parameter recovery of three methods: EZ, fast-dm, and DMAT. J. Math. Psychol. 2009;53:463–473. doi: 10.1016/j.jmp.2009.09.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 141.van Ravenzwaaij D, Donkin C, Vandekerckhove J. The EZ diffusion model provides a powerful test of simple empirical effects. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2017;24:547–556. doi: 10.3758/s13423-016-1081-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 142.Tustison NJ, et al. N4ITK: improved N3 bias correction. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging. 2010;29:1310–1320. doi: 10.1109/TMI.2010.2046908. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 143.Wiest-Daessle N, Prima S, Coupe P, Morrissey SP, Barillot C. Rician noise removal by non-local means filtering for low signal-to-noise ratio MRI: applications to DT-MRI. Med. Image Comput. Comput. Assist. Inter. 2008;11:171–179. doi: 10.1007/978-3-540-85990-1_21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 144.Smith SM. Fast robust automated brain extraction. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2002;17:143–155. doi: 10.1002/hbm.10062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 145.Zhang Y, Brady M, Smith S. Segmentation of brain MR images through a hidden Markov random field model and the expectation-maximization algorithm. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging. 2001;20:45–57. doi: 10.1109/42.906424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 146.Avants BB, Epstein CL, Grossman M, Gee JC. Symmetric diffeomorphic image registration with cross-correlation: evaluating automated labeling of elderly and neurodegenerative brain. Med. Image Anal. 2008;12:26–41. doi: 10.1016/j.media.2007.06.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 147.Power JD, Barnes KA, Snyder AZ, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. Neuroimage. 2012;59:2142–2154. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 148.Power JD, et al. Methods to detect, characterize, and remove motion artifact in resting state fMRI. Neuroimage. 2014;84:320–341. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 149.Zang Y, Jiang T, Lu Y, He Y, Tian L. Regional homogeneity approach to fMRI data analysis. Neuroimage. 2004;22:394–400. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.12.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 150.Van Essen DC, et al. The WU-Minn Human Connectome Project: an overview. Neuroimage. 2013;80:62–79. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 151.Glasser MF, et al. The minimal preprocessing pipelines for the Human Connectome Project. Neuroimage. 2013;80:105–124. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 152.Murata T, Hamada T, Shimokawa T, Tanifuji M, Yanagida T. Stochastic process underlying emergent recognition of visual objects hidden in degraded images. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e115658. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0115658. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are available in the OSF repository, https://osf.io/n3au4/.