Abstract
Rationale
While cannabis-based medicinal products have been shown to be effective for numerous neurological and psychiatric disorders, the evidence base regarding their adverse cognitive effects is poorly understood. The cannabinoid 1 receptor modulates memory performance via intracellular and extracellular mechanisms that alter synaptic transmission and plasticity. While previous literature has consistently shown that chronic cannabis users exhibit marked cognitive impairments, mixed findings have been reported in the context of placebo-controlled experimental trials. It is therefore unclear whether these compounds inherently alter cognitive processes or whether individuals who are genetically predisposed to use cannabis may have underlying cognitive deficits.
Objective
We conducted a meta-analysis to investigate the effects of full and partial cannabinoid 1 receptor (CB1R) agonists, antagonists, and negative allosteric modulators on non-spatial and spatial memory.
Methods
In accordance with the PRISMA guidelines, the EMBASE, MEDLINE, and PsycINFO databases were systematically searched for studies examining the effects of CB1R agonists, antagonists, and negative allosteric modulators on memory performance.
Results
We systematically reviewed 195 studies investigating the effects of cannabinoid compounds on memory. In humans (N = 35 studies, comprising N = 782 subjects), delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) (1.5–5 mg/kg) relative to placebo impaired performance on non-spatial memory tests, whereas only high THC doses (67 mg/kg) impaired spatial memory. Similarly, THC (0.2–4 mg/kg) significantly impaired visuospatial memory in monkeys and non-human primates (N = 8 studies, comprising N = 71 subjects). However, acute THC (0.002–10 mg/kg) had no effect on non-spatial (N = 6 studies, comprising 117 subjects; g = 1.72, 95% confidence interval (CI) − 0.18 to 3.63, p = 0.08) or spatial memory (9 studies, comprising 206 subjects; g = 0.75, 95% confidence interval (CI) − 1.09 to 2.58, p = 0.43). However, acute, full CB1R agonists significantly impaired non-spatial memory (N = 23 studies, 519 subjects; g = − 1.39, 95% CI − 2.72 to − 0.06, p = 0.03). By contrast, the chronic administration of CB1R agonists had no effect on non-spatial memory (N = 5 studies, comprising 146 subjects; g = − 0.05, 95% confidence interval (CI) − 1.32 to 1.22, p = 0.94). Moreover, the acute administration of CB1R antagonists had no effect on non-spatial memory in rodents (N = 9 studies, N = 149 subjects; g = 0.40, 95% CI − 0.11 to 0.92, p = 0.12).
Conclusions
The acute administration of THC, partial CB1R agonist, significantly impaired non-spatial memory in humans, monkeys, and non-human primates but not rodents. However, full CB1R agonists significantly impaired non-spatial memory in a dose-dependent manner but CB1R antagonists had no effect on non-spatial memory in rodents. Moreover, chronic THC administration did not significantly impair spatial or non-spatial memory in rodents, and there is inconclusive evidence on this in humans. Our findings highlight species differences in the effects of cannabinoid compounds on memory.
Electronic supplementary material
The online version of this article (10.1007/s00213-019-05283-3) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
Keywords: Cannabinoid 1 receptor, CB1R agonists, CB1R antagonists, Cognition, Memory
Introduction
Recent changes in legislation in many countries around the world, including the UK, Canada, and 30 states across the USA, have led to the widespread availability of cannabis-based medicinal products. Emerging evidence indicates that cannabis-based medicinal products may have analgesic (De Vita et al. 2018; Fitzcharles et al. 2016), antiemetic (Chang et al. 1979; Orr and McKernan 1981), antidyskinetic (Fox et al. 2002), antispasmodic (Zajicek et al. 2003), antiepileptic (Devinsky et al. 2014), and antipsychotic effects (McGuire et al. 2017; Boggs et al. 2018). However, the evidence base regarding the adverse cognitive effects of these cannabinoid compounds is unclear.
While the marijuana plant contains over 546 chemicals including over 104 cannabinoid compounds (ElSohly et al. 2016), the two most widely studied cannabinoids include delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC), a partial cannabinoid 1 receptor agonist (Huestis et al. 2001), and cannabidiol (CBD), a cannabinoid 1 receptor (CB1R) negative allosteric modulator (Morales et al. 2016). The concentration of THC is higher relative to CBD in street cannabis (THC:CBD ratio, 1:13) (ElSohly et al. 2016) and medical cannabis (THC:CBD ratio, 1:3) (Belendiuk et al. 2015). Since these two compounds have opposite pharmacological effects, the effects of THC are likely to outweigh the effects of CBD in the context of recreational or medicinal cannabis use.
Chronic cannabis users show impairments in memory encoding, storage, and retrieval (Solowij et al. 2011), and these deficits are greater if cannabis use commences prior to the age of 16 (Schuster et al. 2016). While some studies have reported that cannabis-induced memory impairments are no longer shown following abstinence (28 days) (Pope et al. 2001), other studies have shown that prior cannabis users continue to show marked memory impairments despite abstinence (28–60 days) (Thames et al. 2014; Schwartz et al. 1989; Schweinsburg et al. 2008). In line with these findings, cannabis use has also been found to impair memory in first episode psychosis (Núñez et al. 2016) and multiple sclerosis (Patel and Feinstein 2017), whereas cannabidiol has been shown to prevent the adverse effects of THC on memory (Englund et al. 2013a).
However, placebo-controlled experimental trials comparing the effects of THC relative to placebo on memory have reported discrepant findings, reporting no effects on memory (Ganon-Elazar and Akirav 2009; Geresu et al. 2016), memory enhancing (Amal et al. 2010; Bilkei-Gorzo et al. 2017), and memory-impairing effects (Yousefi et al. 2013; Santana et al. 2016; Goodman and Packard 2014). Similarly, while cannabidiol has been postulated to have cognitive enhancing effects (Englund et al. 2013b), the evidence regarding the therapeutic potential of these compounds is largely mixed (McGuire et al. 2017; Boggs et al. 2018; Rosenberg et al. 2017). Moreover, it is unclear if CB1R affinity, dose, treatment duration or treatment paradigms used may influence the effects of these compounds on memory.
Since THC is a partial CB1R agonist (Huestis et al. 2001) that has dose-dependent effects on memory (D’Souza et al. 2005), we aimed to investigate the effects of THC as well as compounds acting as agonists, antagonists, and negative allosteric modulators. We conducted a systematic review and meta-analysis of the acute and chronic effects of all full and partial CB1R agonists, CB1R antagonists, and CB1R negative allosteric modulators on spatial and non-spatial memory in humans, monkeys, non-human primates, rats, and mice. We predicted that the acute and chronic administration of full and partial CB1R agonists would induce spatial and non-spatial memory impairments, whereas CB1R antagonists and negative allosteric modulators would improve spatial and non-spatial memory performance.
Method
Search strategy
A single search was conducted for both animal and human studies. In accordance with the PRISMA guidelines, EMBASE, MEDLINE, PsycINFO, and PsycARTICLES databases were searched from 1950 to 1 September 2018 using the following search terms: (1) “cannabinoid 1 receptor agonist” OR “cannabinoid agonist” OR “cannabinoid receptor 1 antagonist” OR “cannabinoid antagonist” OR “CB1R negative allosteric modulator” OR “cannabis” OR “tetrahydrocannabinol” OR “anandamide” OR “WIN,55,212-2” OR “ACPA” OR “CP55940” OR “AM251” OR “SR161716A” OR “rimonabant” OR “cannabidiol” AND (2) “memory” OR “encoding” OR “recall” OR “retrieval.” The search criteria were registered on the international prospective register for systematic reviews.
Selection criteria
The same criteria were applied for animal and human studies. General inclusion criteria for the systematic review and meta-analysis were (1) original research articles; (2) in vivo experimental methods; (3) comparison of drug relative to control (either placebo or vehicle); and (4) use of a memory paradigm (see supplementary materials 1 for full descriptions of memory paradigms). General exclusion criteria for the systematic review and meta-analysis were (1) review articles; (2) in vitro experimental methods; (3) failure to use a memory paradigm; (4) use of receptor knockout paradigms; (5) use of disease models; and (6) use of concurrent environmental manipulations (e.g. stress or food deprivation models). Studies were meta-analyzed together if they met the following criteria: use of the same (1) species; (2) pharmacological compound (e.g., CB1R agonist vs. antagonist); (3) intraperitoneal administration.
Data extraction
In accordance with the PRISMA guidelines, the following variables were extracted from all animal and human studies: (1) authors; (2) year of publication; (3) sample characteristics (species, strain, sex, age, weight, and sample size); (4) drug characteristics (name, dose, route of drug administration, time point of drug administration); (5) memory paradigm; (6) results (mean and variance of memory performance in drug-treated group and control-treated group) (see supplementary materials). In cases where studies reported multiple drug doses, the highest available dose was selected for the primary analyses, but secondary dose-response relationships were examined where sufficient data were available. In this context, acute administration was defined as the administration of 1 dose, sub-chronic was defined as the administration of 2–4 doses, and chronic administration was defined as the administration of 5 or more doses on consecutive days. Memory performance data were extracted from tables or graphs using the plot digitizer where necessary (WebPlotDigitizer 2018). In cases where it was unclear if datasets were independent, the manuscript reporting the largest dataset was selected. All datasets included in the meta-analysis were independent.
Study sample and methodological characteristics
The study flow chart is shown in supplementary figure 1. The literature search identified 2679 records which were manually screened by two independent researchers. After removing duplicates (N = 185), conference abstracts (N = 183), review articles (N = 90), irrelevant records (N = 1784), and books (N = 1), 436 full-text records were screened for eligibility. An additional 241 records were excluded due to the simultaneous administration of more than one drug (N = 13); failure to use CB1R agonist, antagonist, or negative allosteric modulator (N = 153); failure to use a memory paradigm (N = 8); and use of a disease model (N = 67). In total, 195 articles were deemed suitable for inclusion in the systematic review including 38 human studies, 8 primate studies, and 149 rodent studies, and 60 of these studies were suitable for inclusion in the meta-analyses.
Statistical analysis
The main outcome measure in our analysis was the summary effect size (Hedge’s g (Hedges and Olkin 1985)) for the difference in memory performance between control-treated and drug-treated groups. We performed separate meta-analyses for CB1R agonists, antagonists, and negative allosteric modulators. In each meta-analysis, we compared the active treatment against control and analyzed acute and chronic studies separately. CB1R agonists with different formulations including WIN,55,212-2; ACPA; CP55,940; and delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) were grouped into a single analysis. However, we also investigated the effects of full agonists (WIN,55,212-2; ACPA; CP55,940; anandamide) and partial agonist THC relative to vehicle. All comparisons were conducted with the statistical programming language R Studio (version 3.3.2) using the “metafor” package. Standardized effect sizes (Hedges’ g using a 95% confidence interval and a significance level of p < 0.05 (two-tailed)) for individual studies were first estimated. An overall summary effect size was then calculated by entering these individual study effect sizes into a random effects meta-analytic model using restricted maximum likelihood estimation.
Meta-analysis was conducted if there were at least 3 studies that used the same species, drug, administration route, and memory paradigm. Consistent with previous research, the highest drug dose was selected from each study (Kokkinou et al. 2018). All datasets included in the meta-analysis were independent. If at least 5 studies were included in a meta-analysis, dose-response relationships were also investigated.
Between-study inconsistency was estimated using the I2 value (I2 < 50% indicates low to moderate inconsistency, whereas I2 > 50% indicates moderate to high inconsistency). Publication bias was assessed in cases where there were at least 5 available studies by visual inspection of a funnel plot and the use of the Egger’s test. In cases where publication bias was suspected, a trim-fill analysis was conducted. If at least 5 studies were included in a meta-analysis, leave-one-out sensitivity analyses were conducted to ensure that the results were not driven by a single study.
Since previous literature has shown age-dependent (Solowij et al. 2011) and dose-dependent effects (D’Souza et al. 2005), meta-regressions were conducted to examine the effect of age and dose of the pharmacological compound on memory performance. We then compared subgroups by fitting a meta-regression model where the subgroup category acted as the moderating variable of interest. If this showed statistically significant differences between subgroups, a random-effects meta-analysis was conducted for each subgroup. The subgroups that we investigated were species, age, sex, compound (full, partial agonists), dose, paradigm, and drug administration timing (drug given before vs. after paradigm training), separately for non-spatial and spatial memory.
Results
All datasets included in the meta-analyses were independent.
We report meta-analytic findings investigating the effects of cannabinoid compounds on spatial and non-spatial memory in rodents, followed by separate analyses studies using mice (see supplementary tables 1–3) and rats (see supplementary tables 4–6). Non-human primate or monkey studies are summarized in supplementary tables 7–9, and human studies are summarized in supplementary tables 10–12. See supplementary materials for descriptions of memory paradigms.
Acute effects of CB1R agonists on non-spatial memory
In a meta-analysis of 29 studies, CB1R agonists (N = 261) relative to vehicle (N = 258) significantly impaired memory performance on non-spatial memory paradigms (g = − 1.79, 95% confidence interval (CI) − 3.13 to − 0.45, p = 0.009) (see Fig. 1 and supplementary Figure 1 for funnel plot). There were high levels of between-study inconsistency (I2 = 97.50, p < 0.001). Egger’s test indicated that there was evidence of publication bias (z = 4.84, p < 0.0001), and a trim-fill analysis indicated that there were no missing studies. The results remained significant in all cases of the leave-one-out sensitivity analysis.
Fig. 1.
Forest plot from a meta-analysis of the acute effects of CB1R agonists relative to vehicle on non-spatial memory performance in rodents (g = − 1.79, 95% confidence interval (CI) − 3.13 to − 0.45, p = 0.009)
Effect of species
The magnitude of the effect of CB1R agonists vs. vehicle on non-spatial memory did not significantly vary with rodent species (z = 1.13, p = 0.26).
Effect of sex
The magnitude of the effect of CB1R agonists vs. vehicle on non-spatial memory did not significantly vary with sex (z = 0.79, p = 0.43).
Effect of paradigm
The magnitude of the effect of paradigm on CB1R agonists vs. vehicle on non-spatial memory did not significantly vary with the use of the inhibitory avoidance paradigm or the novel object paradigm (z = 0.86, p = 0.40).
Effect of drug administration timing
The magnitude of the effect of CB1R agonists vs. vehicle on non-spatial memory did not vary with drug administration timing (z = − 0.06, p = 0.95).
Effect of dose
The magnitude of the effect of CB1R agonists vs. vehicle on non-spatial memory inversely varied with dose (z = − 2.10, p = 0.04).
Effect of age
The magnitude of the effect of CB1R agonists vs. vehicle on non-spatial memory did not vary with age (z = 0.63, p = 0.53) or if rodents were adolescents (postnatal day (PND) < 65) or adults (PND > 65) (z = 1.19, p = 0.24).
Effect of drug
A meta-analysis of 23 studies indicated that full CB1R agonists relative to vehicle significantly impaired non-spatial memory (g = − 1.39, 95% confidence interval (CI) − 2.72 to − 0.06, p = 0.03 (see supplementary figures 2–3 for forest and funnel plots, respectively). The magnitude of the effect of CB1R agonists vs. vehicle on non-spatial memory did not vary with the compound used (z = − 0.21, p = 0.84). By contrast, a meta-analysis of 6 studies showed that partial CB1R agonist, delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol, had no effects on memory performance (6 studies, g = − 4.04, 95% confidence interval (CI) − 9.30 to 1.17, p = 0.13) (see supplementary figures 4–5 for forest and funnel plots, respectively).
Acute effects of CB1R antagonists on non-spatial memory
In a meta-analysis of 9 studies, CB1R antagonists (N = 73) relative to vehicle (N = 76) had no effects on non-spatial memory performance (g = 0.40, 95% confidence interval (CI) − 0.11 to 0.92, p = 0.12) (see Fig. 2 and supplementary figure 6). There were moderate levels of between-study inconsistency (I2 = 56.78%, p = 0.02). Egger’s test indicated that there was no evidence of publication bias (z = − 0.20, p = 0.84), and a trim-fill analysis indicated that there were no missing studies. Findings remained unchanged in a leave-one-out sensitivity analysis.
Fig. 2.
Forest plot from a meta-analysis of the effects of CB1R antagonists relative to vehicle on non-spatial memory performance in rodents (g = 0.40, 95% confidence interval (CI) − 0.11 to 0.92, p = 0.12)
Effect of species
The magnitude of the effect of CB1R antagonists vs. vehicle on non-spatial memory did not vary with rodent species (z = − 1.10, p = 0.29).
Effect of sex
The magnitude of the effect of CB1R antagonists vs. vehicle on non-spatial memory did not vary with sex (z = 1.54, p = 0.12).
Effect of paradigm
The magnitude of the effect of CB1R antagonists vs. vehicle on non-spatial memory did not vary with paradigm (z = − 1.73, p = 0.08).
Effect of drug administration timing
The magnitude of the effect of CB1R antagonists vs. vehicle on non-spatial memory was greater following pre-training relative to post-training drug administration (z = − 2.30, p = 0.02).
Effects of dose
The magnitude of the effect of CB1R antagonists vs. vehicle on non-spatial memory did not vary with dose (z = 0.34, p = 0.73).
Effect of age
The magnitude of the effect of CB1R antagonists vs. vehicle on non-spatial memory did not vary with age (z = − 0.62, p = 0.53).
Effect of drug
The magnitude of the effect of CB1R antagonists vs. vehicle on non-spatial memory did not vary with the compound used (z = 0.11, p = 0.91).
Acute effects of CB1R negative allosteric modulators on non-spatial memory
There were insufficient studies to investigate the effects of negative allosteric modulators on non-spatial memory.
Acute effects of CB1R agonists on spatial memory
In a meta-analysis of 11 studies, CB1R agonists (N = 113) relative to vehicle (N = 113) did not significantly impair memory performance on spatial memory paradigms (g = 0.75, 95% confidence interval (CI) − 1.68 to 3.18, p = 0.55) (see supplementary figures 7–8 for forest and funnel plot, respectively). There were moderate-high levels of between-study inconsistency (I2 = 97.50, p < 0.001). Egger’s test indicated that there was evidence of publication bias (z = − 1.61, p = 0.11), and a trim-fill analysis indicated that there were no missing studies. The results remained unchanged in all cases of the leave-one-out sensitivity analysis.
Effect of species
Moreover, the magnitude of the effect of CB1R agonists vs. vehicle on spatial memory did not vary with rodent species (z = − 0.74, p = 0.50).
Effect of sex
The magnitude of the effect of CB1R agonists vs. vehicle on spatial memory was greater in females relative to males (z = − 2.20, p = 0.03). However, there were insufficient studies to conduct further subgroup analyses on this.
Effect of paradigm
The magnitude of the effect of CB1 agonists vs. vehicle on spatial memory did not vary with the paradigm used (z = 1.41, p = 0.16).
Effect of drug administration timing
The magnitude of the effect of CB1R agonists vs. vehicle on spatial memory did not vary with drug administration time point (z = 0.89, p = 0.38).
Effect of dose
The magnitude of the effect of CB1R agonists vs. vehicle on spatial memory did not vary with dose (z = 0.4, p = 0.69).
Effect of age
The magnitude of the effect of CB1R agonists vs. vehicle did not vary with age (z = 0.92, p = 0.36).
Effect of drug
While there were insufficient studies to investigate the effects of full CB1R on spatial memory. Findings remained unchanged when restricting the analysis to partial CB1R agonist, THC relative to vehicle (9 studies, g = 0.75, 95% confidence interval (CI) − 1.09 to 2.58, p = 0.43) (see supplementary figures 9–10 for forest and funnel plots, respectively).
Acute effects of CB1R antagonists and negative allosteric modulators on spatial memory
There were insufficient studies to investigate the effects of cannabinoid 1 receptor antagonists and negative allosteric modulators on spatial memory.
Moderator analyses
The magnitude of the acute effect of CB1R agonists vs. vehicle did not significantly vary depending on the type of memory investigated (non-spatial versus spatial) (z = 1.89, p = 0.06). However, the magnitude of the chronic effects of CB1R agonists vs. vehicle was greater for non-spatial memory relative to spatial memory (z = 2.40, p = 0.02). The magnitude of the acute effect of CB1R agonists vs. vehicle on non-spatial and spatial memory did not significantly vary depending on whether rodents were adolescents or adults (z = − 0.64, p = 0.52).
Chronic effects of CB1R agonists on non-spatial memory
Our search identified 5 studies investigating the chronic effects of CB1R agonists (N = 53) relative to vehicle (N = 52) on non-spatial memory. Relative to vehicle, chronic administration of CB1R agonists did not significantly impair memory performance (g = − 0.05, 95% confidence interval (CI) − 1.32 to 1.22, p = 0.94) (see Fig. 3 and supplementary figure 11). There was no evidence of between-study inconsistency (I2 = 91.85%, p < 0.001). Egger’s test indicated that there was no evidence of publication bias (z = 0.42, p = 0.67), and a trim-fill analysis indicated that there were no missing studies.
Fig. 3.
Forest plot from a meta-analysis of the chronic effects of CB1R agonists relative to vehicle on non-spatial memory performance in rodents (g = − 0.05, 95% confidence interval (CI), − 1.32 to 1.22, p = 0.94)
Effect of species
Since all 5 studies were conducted in rats, no further sensitivity analyses were conducted.
Effect of sex
The magnitude of the effect of chronic CB1R agonists vs. vehicle on non-spatial memory did not vary with sex (z = − 0.97, p = 0.33).
Effect of paradigm
Since all 5 studies used the novel object paradigm, no further sensitivity analyses were conducted.
Effect of drug administration timing
The magnitude of the effect of chronic CB1R agonists vs. vehicle on non-spatial memory did not significantly vary with drug administration timing (z = − 1.92, p = 0.05).
Effect of dose
The magnitude of the effect of chronic CB1R agonists vs. vehicle on non-spatial memory did not significantly vary with dose (z = 0.20, p = 0.88).
Effect of age
The magnitude of the effect of chronic CB1R agonists relative to vehicle on non-spatial memory did not vary with age (z = 0.30, p = 0.78).
Effect of drug
In a meta-analysis of 5 studies, chronic CB1R agonists relative to vehicle did not significantly impair non-spatial memory (N = 5 studies, N = 146 subjects, g = − 0.05, 95% confidence interval (CI), − 1.32 to 1.22, p = 0.94). There were insufficient studies to investigate the effects of THC vs. vehicle on non-spatial memory.
Chronic effects of CB1R agonists on spatial memory
Our search identified 5 studies investigating the chronic effects of CB1R agonists (N = 76) relative to vehicle (N = 70) on spatial memory, as determined by the Morris water maze (see supplementary figures 12–13 for forest and funnel plots, respectively). Relative to vehicle, chronic administration of CB1R agonists did not significantly impair memory performance (g = − 0.23, 95% confidence interval (CI) − 0.62 to 0.16, p = 0.24) (see supplementary figures 14–15 for forest and funnel plots, respectively). There was no evidence of between-study inconsistency (I2 = 0.0%, p < 0.0001). Egger’s test indicated that there was no evidence of publication bias (z = 0.91, p = 0.36), and a trim-fill analysis indicated that there were no missing studies.
Effect of species
The magnitude of the effect of chronic CB1R agonists vs. vehicle on spatial memory did not vary with species (z = − 0.50, p = 0.63).
Effect of sex
The magnitude of the effect of chronic CB1R agonists vs. vehicle on spatial memory did not vary with sex (z = − 1.20, p = 0.24).
Effect of paradigm
Since all 5 studies used the Morris water maze, no further sensitivity analyses were conducted.
Effect of drug administration timing
The magnitude of the effect of chronic CB1R agonists vs. vehicle on spatial memory did not vary with drug administration timing (z = − 0.97, p = 0.33).
Effect of dose
The magnitude of the effect of chronic CB1R agonists vs. vehicle on spatial memory did not vary with dose (z = 0.55, p = 0.58).
Effect of age
The magnitude of the effect of chronic CB1R agonists vs. vehicle on spatial memory did not vary with age (z = − 0.79, p = 0.43).
Effect of drug
There were insufficient studies to investigate the effects of full vs. partial CB1R agonists.
Chronic effects of cannabinoid 1 receptor antagonists/negative allosteric modulators on spatial memory
There were no studies investigating the chronic effects of CB1R antagonists or negative allosteric modulators on spatial memory.
The effects of cannabinoid compounds on memory in monkeys and non-human primates
See supplementary material tables 7–9 for a review of monkey and non-human primate studies. In monkeys and non-human primates, THC (0.2–4 mg/kg) significantly impaired memory performance on the delayed matching-to-sample task in 4 out of 5 studies. THC (0.2–0.3 mg/kg) also significantly impaired memory performance on the visual-spatial paired associates test in a single study.
The effects of cannabinoid compounds on memory in humans
See supplementary material tables 10–12 for a review of human studies. THC (1.5–5 mg/kg) decreased immediate and delayed recall performance on the Hopkins verbal learning test in 4 out of 5 studies in the context of acute and sub-chronic administration. However, THC given chronically for 7 days (10–30 mg) had no effect on immediate or delayed recall on the Hopkins verbal learning test. Acute and sub-chronic THC (7.5–15 mg/kg) decreased emotional memory in 2 out of 2 studies. In 2 studies, acute THC (7.5 mg/kg) and acute CBD (32 mg) had no effect on memory extinction. Acute THC (0.018–0.036 mg/kg) decreased memory performance on the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test in 2 out of 2 studies. Acute rimonabant (20 mg/kg) had no effect on verbal learning on the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test in 2 out of 2 studies. Chronic (10 weeks) CBD (200 mg) improved word recall on the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test. In 3 studies, acute THC decreased spatial memory at 66.67 mg/kg, acute THC improved memory at 5 mg but had no effect on spatial memory at 2 or 3 mg/kg.
Discussion
In line with our predictions, the acute administration of THC significantly impaired immediate and delayed recall, emotional memory, and verbal learning in humans, and visuospatial memory in non-human primates and monkeys. In contrast to our predictions, the acute and chronic administration of delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol had no effect on spatial or non-spatial memory in rodents. Moreover, full CB1R agonists had no effect on spatial memory but CB1R agonists significantly impaired non-spatial memory in rodents.
In contrast to our predictions, CB1R agonists did not impair spatial or non-spatial memory when administered chronically, irrespective of age, sex, rodent species or drug administration timing relative to task training. This finding was not influenced by rodent species, sex, paradigm, drug administration timing, dose, age or the compound used. While further studies are needed to investigate the chronic effects of THC in humans, a single study found that THC (10–20 mg) had no effect on verbal memory performance in humans when administered chronically for 7 days (Mathai et al. 2018).
Interestingly, the detrimental effects of acute, full CB1R agonists on non-spatial memory were not influenced by rodent species, sex, age, paradigm, drug administration timing but impairments were inversely associated with dose. However, in contrast to our predictions, CB1R antagonists had no effect on non-spatial memory and this finding was not influenced by rodent species, sex, paradigm, dose, compound or age. However, the acute effects of CB1R antagonists on non-spatial memory were greater if the drug was administered prior to task training than after training.
Strengths and limitations
A strength of the study was that we examined the effects of CB1R agonists, antagonists, and negative allosteric modulators on memory performance across a wide range of species using an array of spatial and non-spatial memory paradigms. Although this allowed us to quantitatively examine the consistency of the acute and chronic effects of CB1R agonists on memory in rodents, insufficient data was available to quantitatively investigate the acute or chronic effects of these compounds in humans, monkeys or non-human primates.
A limitation of the study was that we combined different drug doses which may potentially include effective and ineffective doses. However, in order to address this limitation, we investigated the dose-dependent effects of CB1R agonists and antagonists on memory performance. Moreover, although adolescents and adults were combined, we did not observe any age-dependent effects of CB1R agonists and antagonists on memory, and thus this is also unlikely to be a significant confound.
The high levels of between-study inconsistency observed may be linked to the combination of various pharmacological compounds with different binding profiles. However, we addressed this limitation by conducting sensitivity analyses of full and partial CB1R agonists. Another limitation of the study was that we combined mice and rat studies into a single meta-analysis. However, we aimed to overcome this limitation by conducting moderator analyses investigating species effects.
Another limitation of the study was that some meta-analyses included between 4 and 10 studies. As such, we cannot exclude the possibility of type II errors since these analyses may have been statistically underpowered to detect small group differences. While we found evidence of slight publication bias for meta-analyses of the acute effects of agonists on spatial and non-spatial memory, there was no evidence of publication bias for meta-analysis of acute antagonists or chronic agonists on spatial or non-spatial memory.
Previous literature has shown that THC briefly increases locomotor activity which is followed by a decrease in locomotor activity relative to baseline conditions (Bruijnzeel et al. 2016). As such, we cannot exclude the possibility that the locomotor effects of THC influenced memory performance in rodents. However, since we did not show evidence of impairments on spatial memory paradigms which involve locomotor function, locomotor effects of cannabinoids are unlikely to be a significant confound.
Implications
Our finding that CB1R compounds selectively altered non-spatial memory but had no effects on spatial memory in rodents is consistent with previous literature indicating that spatial and non-spatial memory have distinct, dissociable neurobiological correlates (Park et al. 2016) and that brain lesions can selectively impair non-spatial memory in the absence of spatial memory deficits (Ravizza et al. 2006; De Renzi and Nichelli 1975) and vice versa (Rhodes et al. 2008; Backer Cave and Squire 1992; Hanley et al. 1991). However, our findings extend the existing literature by showing that the CB1R contributes to the mechanisms underlying non-spatial memory. However, future studies are needed to investigate this quantitatively in human, monkey or non-human primates.
Our finding that partial agonist THC significantly impaired non-spatial memory performance in humans on tasks involving verbal recall indicates that the endocannabinoid system may play a key role in the mechanisms underlying verbal memory. Previous literature has shown that cannabis users show non-spatial, verbal memory impairments with effect sizes ranging between d = 0.1–0.7 (Schoeler et al. 2016). However, in contrast to our predictions, THC did not significantly impair non-spatial memory in rodents. This discrepancy may be explained by species differences as well as methodological differences in the studies used across species.
In particular, if THC specifically impairs verbal memory in humans, this may not be seen in rodents due to differences in language capabilities. Species differences may also be linked to underlying differences in the endocannabinoid system, particularly the cannabinoid 1 receptor, that mediates the effects of these compounds (Huestis et al. 2001). Alternatively, these discrepant findings may relate to the use of intraperitoneal administration in rodents and intravenous administration in humans. However, since comparable THC peak plasma levels are reached following intravenous administration in humans (5 mg/kg) (Ohlsson et al. 1980) and following intraperitoneal administration in rats (3–10 mg/kg) (Nguyen et al. 2016), it is unlikely that species differences in drug bioavailability may explain this discrepant finding but further work is needed to investigate this.
It should also be recognized that there have been surprisingly few studies of THC effects on memory in rodents and, given the wide confidence interval in our meta-analysis, our analyses highlight the need for further studies of effects of both acute and chronic THC on memory. While we did not observe sex differences in the effects of CB1R agonists on non-spatial memory, we observed sex differences in the effects of CB1R agonists on spatial memory despite the fact that there were no significant effects of CB1R agonists on spatial memory.
This finding is consistent with previous work showing that humans show sex differences in CB1R availability (Park et al. 2016) as well as sex differences in the effects of THC on spatial memory (Ravizza et al. 2006).
Surprisingly, there were no differences in the effects of CB1R agonists vs. vehicle on memory between adults and adolescents. This finding is at odds with previous suggestions that adolescents may be more vulnerable to the effects of cannabinoids (Bossong and Niesink 2010). While no placebo-controlled studies have been conducted in human adolescents, THC vs. placebo has been found to significantly impair memory in adolescent monkeys (Backer Cave and Squire 1992). Moreover, further work is needed to investigate if low doses of THC may improve memory in old age in humans, as shown in rodents (Amal et al. 2010). In contrast to our predictions, CB1R antagonists failed to significantly enhance non-spatial memory and the evidence is largely mixed. Since we found that the effects of CB1R antagonists on non-spatial memory were greater when administered prior to task training, discrepant findings may be due to methodological differences in drug administration methods.
Despite differences in the acute vs. chronic effects of cannabinoid compounds on memory, these findings were not influenced by differences in age, sex, species or drug administration methods. Since few chronic studies have been conducted, further work is needed to investigate factors that may account for the different effects of acute vs. chronic drug administration. Our finding that the acute administration of CB1R agonists impairs memory, but chronic administration does not, may reflect underlying changes in the cannabinoid 1 receptor. Since chronic exposure to cannabinoids decreases the expression of extracellular cannabinoid receptors (Hsieh et al. 2002), the chronic administration of cannabinoids may have less dramatic downstream effects on memory due to the presence of cannabinoid 1 receptors. In line with this, chronic cannabis users exhibit fewer cannabinoids 1 receptors that normalize following abstinence (D’Souza et al. 2016).
While these findings are consistent with literature showing that cannabis-induced memory impairments are not shown following abstinence (28 days) (Pope et al. 2001), other studies have shown that prior cannabis users continue to show marked memory impairments despite abstinence (28–60 days) (Thames et al. 2014; Schwartz et al. 1989; Schweinsburg et al. 2008). Therefore, underlying genetic differences may predispose individuals to THC-induced cognitive impairments. In line with this, converging lines of evidence indicate that functional polymorphisms in the cannabinoid 1 receptor (CNR1) gene are associated with behavioral and functional measures of working memory performance in healthy volunteers. In particular, healthy volunteers exhibiting functional polymorphisms in the CNR1 gene (C carriers of rs1406977) show reduced BOLD responses in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex during working memory performance in the absence of behavioral impairments in working memory (Taurisano et al. 2016), whereas healthy volunteers showing functional polymorphisms in the CNR1 gene (GG carriers of rs2180619) show reduced working memory performance and lower performance under higher working memory load demands (Ruiz-Contreras et al. 2014).
Future studies are also needed to investigate the effects of full CB1R agonists and antagonists on both spatial and non-spatial memory in humans. Future studies are needed to identify if the endocannabinoid system may modulate other aspects of memory such as episodic, semantic, and procedural memory. Our finding that chronic THC had no effect on spatial or non-spatial memory performance is consistent with findings from human literature showing that THC has no effect on memory performance when administered chronically (Mathai et al. 2018). However, future studies are needed to investigate the chronic effects of THC in placebo-controlled trials in order for this to be quantitatively investigated. Moreover, while there is preliminary evidence to suggest that negative allosteric modulators can enhance memory performance in humans when administered chronically, further studies are needed.
Our finding that the acute administration of full CB1R agonists impairs non-spatial memory performance in rodents is consistent with literature showing that CB1R activation inhibits LTP, a key physiological mechanism underlying memory formation (Navakkode and Korte 2014a). CB1R activation has been specifically shown to impair LTP by dysregulating the protein synthesis involved in the formation and degradation of synaptic connections (Navakkode and Korte 2014b). Our finding that CB1R activation impairs memory performance is also consistent with evidence that CB1R activation impairs memory by directly altering mitochondrial energy metabolism required for cellular activity (Huestis et al. 2001). The deleterious effects of CB1R activation on memory performance are mediated via CB1R-dependent modulation of soluble adenylyl cyclase (sAC), an enzyme that catalyzes the conversion of adenosine triphosphate (ATP), a vital source of energy, into cyclic adenosine monophosphate (cAMP), a second messenger involved in intracellular signaling cascades that activates protein kinase A (Morales et al. 2016).
Conclusions
Our findings collectively show that the THC has species-specific effects on memory, impairing non-spatial memory in humans, monkeys, non-human primates but not rodents. Moreover, the chronic administration of CB1R agonists failed to impair non-spatial or spatial memory in rodents. In contrast to our predictions, acute CB1R agonists selectively impaired non-spatial, but not spatial memory, and the acute administration of CB1R antagonists had no effects on non-spatial memory in rodents. Future placebo-controlled studies in humans are needed to investigate the cognitive effects of chronic THC administration as well as the cognitive effects of low dose THC for cognitive impairment in old age.
Electronic supplementary material
(DOCX 784 kb)
Funding information
F.B. and O.H. were supported by the METSY EU FP7 grant. K.B. was supported by the Rosetrees Trust, the Stoney Gate Trust, and the Royal College of Psychiatrists. R.M. was supported by the Wellcome Trust (no. 200102/Z/15/Z). A.V. was supported by the Medical Research Council (MR/N025377/1). MV was supported by the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Biomedical Research Centre at South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and King’s College London.
Compliance with ethical standards
Conflict of interest
F.B., K.B., E.B., R.M., and M.V. reported no conflicts of interest. Dr. Vernon reported receiving investigator-initiated research funding from F. Hoffman La Roche and UCB Biopharma SPRL. Dr. Howes reported receiving investigator-initiated research funding from or participating in advisory or speaker meetings organized by AstraZeneca, Autifony, Bristol-Myers Squibb, Eli Lilly, Heptares, Janssen, Lundbeck, Lyden-Delta, Otsuka, Servier, Sunovion, RAND, and Roche.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- Amal H, Fridman-Rozevich L, Senn R, Strelnikov A, Gafni M, Keren O et al (2010) Long-term consequences of a single treatment of mice with an ultra-low dose of 9 -tetrahydrocannabinol (THC). Behav Brain Res. [cited 2017 Dec 21];206:245–53. Available from: https://ac.els-cdn.com/S0166432809005361/1-s2.0-S0166432809005361-main.pdf?_tid=f7da5b6e-e6a2-11e7-aae2-00000aab0f27&acdnat=1513897507_fa5011d103954ceea9fac3426e510320 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Cave Carolyn Backer, Squire Larry R. Intact verbal and nonverbal short-term memory following damage to the human hippocampus. Hippocampus. 1992;2(2):151–163. doi: 10.1002/hipo.450020207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Belendiuk Katherine A., Babson Kimberly A., Vandrey Ryan, Bonn-Miller Marcel O. Cannabis species and cannabinoid concentration preference among sleep-disturbed medicinal cannabis users. Addictive Behaviors. 2015;50:178–181. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2015.06.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bilkei-Gorzo Andras, Albayram Onder, Draffehn Astrid, Michel Kerstin, Piyanova Anastasia, Oppenheimer Hannah, Dvir-Ginzberg Mona, Rácz Ildiko, Ulas Thomas, Imbeault Sophie, Bab Itai, Schultze Joachim L, Zimmer Andreas. A chronic low dose of Δ9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) restores cognitive function in old mice. Nature Medicine. 2017;23(6):782–787. doi: 10.1038/nm.4311. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boggs Douglas L., Surti Toral, Gupta Aarti, Gupta Swapnil, Niciu Mark, Pittman Brian, Schnakenberg Martin Ashley M., Thurnauer Halle, Davies Andrew, D’Souza Deepak C., Ranganathan Mohini. The effects of cannabidiol (CBD) on cognition and symptoms in outpatients with chronic schizophrenia a randomized placebo controlled trial. Psychopharmacology. 2018;235(7):1923–1932. doi: 10.1007/s00213-018-4885-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bossong Matthijs G., Niesink Raymond J.M. Adolescent brain maturation, the endogenous cannabinoid system and the neurobiology of cannabis-induced schizophrenia. Progress in Neurobiology. 2010;92(3):370–385. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2010.06.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bruijnzeel AW, Qi X, Guzhva L V., Wall S, Deng J V., Gold MS et al (2016) Behavioral characterization of the effects of cannabis smoke and anandamide in rats. Dominguez JM, editor. PLoS One. [cited 2018 Nov 29];11(4):e0153327. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/27065006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Chang A, Shiling D, … Rosenberg SA (1979) Delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol as an antiemetic in cancer patients receiving high-dose methotrexate: a prospective, randomized evaluation. Am Coll Physicians. [cited 2018 Nov 29]; Available from: http://annals.org/data/journals/aim/19553/aime197912010-00002.pdf [DOI] [PubMed]
- D’Souza Deepak Cyril, Abi-Saab Walid Michel, Madonick Steven, Forselius-Bielen Kimberlee, Doersch Anne, Braley Gabriel, Gueorguieva Ralitza, Cooper Thomas B., Krystal John Harrison. Delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol effects in schizophrenia: Implications for cognition, psychosis, and addiction. Biological Psychiatry. 2005;57(6):594–608. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.12.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- D’Souza DC, Cortes-Briones JA, Ranganathan M, Thurnauer H, Creatura G, Surti T et al (2016) Rapid changes in CB1 receptor availability in cannabis dependent males after abstinence from cannabis. Biol psychiatry Cogn Neurosci neuroimaging. [cited 2016 May 31];1(1):60–7. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26858993 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- De Renzi Ennio, Nichelli Paolo. Verbal and Non-Verbal Short-Term Memory Impairment Following Hemispheric Damage. Cortex. 1975;11(4):341–354. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(75)80026-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Vita Martin J., Moskal Dezarie, Maisto Stephen A., Ansell Emily B. Association of Cannabinoid Administration With Experimental Pain in Healthy Adults. JAMA Psychiatry. 2018;75(11):1118. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2018.2503. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Devinsky O, Roberta Cilio M, Cross H, Fernandez-Ruiz J, French J, Hill C et al (2014) Cannabidiol: pharmacology and potential therapeutic role in epilepsy and other neuropsychiatric disorders CRITICAL REVIEW AND INVITED COMMENTARY. Epilepsia. [cited 2018 Nov 29];55(6):791–802. Available from: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/pdf/10.1111/epi.12631 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- ElSohly Mahmoud A., Mehmedic Zlatko, Foster Susan, Gon Chandrani, Chandra Suman, Church James C. Changes in Cannabis Potency Over the Last 2 Decades (1995–2014): Analysis of Current Data in the United States. Biological Psychiatry. 2016;79(7):613–619. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.01.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Englund Amir, Morrison Paul D, Nottage Judith, Hague Dominic, Kane Fergus, Bonaccorso Stefania, Stone James M, Reichenberg Avi, Brenneisen Rudolf, Holt David, Feilding Amanda, Walker Lucy, Murray Robin M, Kapur Shitij. Cannabidiol inhibits THC-elicited paranoid symptoms and hippocampal-dependent memory impairment. Journal of Psychopharmacology. 2012;27(1):19–27. doi: 10.1177/0269881112460109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Englund Amir, Morrison Paul D, Nottage Judith, Hague Dominic, Kane Fergus, Bonaccorso Stefania, Stone James M, Reichenberg Avi, Brenneisen Rudolf, Holt David, Feilding Amanda, Walker Lucy, Murray Robin M, Kapur Shitij. Cannabidiol inhibits THC-elicited paranoid symptoms and hippocampal-dependent memory impairment. Journal of Psychopharmacology. 2012;27(1):19–27. doi: 10.1177/0269881112460109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fitzcharles M-A, Ste-Marie PA, Auser WH, Clauw DJ, Jamal S, Karsh J, et al. Efficacy, tolerability, and safety of cannabinoid treatments in the rheumatic diseases: a systematic review of randomized controlled trials. Arthritis Care Res (Hoboken) 2016;68(5):681–688. doi: 10.1002/acr.22727. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox SH, Henry B, Hill M, Crossman A, Brotchie J (2002) Stimulation of cannabinoid receptors reduces levodopa-induced dyskinesia in the MPTP-lesioned nonhuman primate model of Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord. [cited 2018 Nov 13];17(6):1180–7. Available from: http://doi.wiley.com/10.1002/mds.10289 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Ganon-Elazar E., Akirav I. Cannabinoid Receptor Activation in the Basolateral Amygdala Blocks the Effects of Stress on the Conditioning and Extinction of Inhibitory Avoidance. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29(36):11078–11088. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1223-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geresu B, Onaivi E, Engidawork E (2016) Behavioral evidence for the interaction between cannabinoids and Catha edulis F. (Khat) in mice. [cited 2017 Dec 21]; Available from: https://ac.els-cdn.com/S0006899316305431/1-s2.0-S0006899316305431-main.pdf?_tid=3d4e1f8e-e65b-11e7-8bb8-00000aab0f01&acdnat=1513866700_f79652d904bee3cfe5c7bca5f613f8fb [DOI] [PubMed]
- Goodman J., Packard M.G. Peripheral and intra-dorsolateral striatum injections of the cannabinoid receptor agonist WIN 55,212-2 impair consolidation of stimulus–response memory. Neuroscience. 2014;274:128–137. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.05.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hanley JR, Young AW, Pearson NA (1991) Impairment of the visuo-spatial sketch pad. Q J Exp Psychol. [cited 2018 Jun 9];43–101. Available from: http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/pdf/10.1080/14640749108401001 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Hedges LV, Olkin I (1985) Statistical methods for meta-analysis. Academic Press. 369 p
- Hsieh C., Brown S., Derleth C., Mackie K. Internalization and Recycling of the CB1 Cannabinoid Receptor. Journal of Neurochemistry. 2002;73(2):493–501. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.1999.0730493.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huestis Marilyn A., Gorelick David A., Heishman Stephen J., Preston Kenzie L., Nelson Richard A., Moolchan Eric T., Frank Richard A. Blockade of Effects of Smoked Marijuana by the CB1-Selective Cannabinoid Receptor Antagonist SR141716. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2001;58(4):322. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.4.322. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kokkinou M, Ashok A H, Howes O D. The effects of ketamine on dopaminergic function: meta-analysis and review of the implications for neuropsychiatric disorders. Molecular Psychiatry. 2017;23(1):59–69. doi: 10.1038/mp.2017.190. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mathai David S., Holst Manuela, Rodgman Christopher, Haile Colin N., Keller Jake, Hussain Mariyah Z., Kosten Thomas R., Newton Thomas F., Verrico Christopher D. Guanfacine Attenuates Adverse Effects of Dronabinol (THC) on Working Memory in Adolescent-Onset Heavy Cannabis Users: A Pilot Study. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. 2018;30(1):66–76. doi: 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.16120328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGuire P, Robson P, Cubala WJ, Vasile D, Morrison PD, Barron R, Taylor A, Wright S (2017) Cannabidiol (CBD) as an adjunctive therapy in schizophrenia: a multicenter randomized controlled trial. Am J Psychiatry. [cited 2018 Jan 17];appi.ajp.2017.1. Available from: http://ajp.psychiatryonline.org/doi/10.1176/appi.ajp.2017.17030325 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Morales Paula, Goya Pilar, Jagerovic Nadine, Hernandez-Folgado Laura. Allosteric Modulators of the CB1 Cannabinoid Receptor: A Structural Update Review. Cannabis and Cannabinoid Research. 2016;1(1):22–30. doi: 10.1089/can.2015.0005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Navakkode Sheeja, Korte Martin. Pharmacological activation of CB1 receptor modulates long term potentiation by interfering with protein synthesis. Neuropharmacology. 2014;79:525–533. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.11.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Navakkode Sheeja, Korte Martin. Pharmacological activation of CB1 receptor modulates long term potentiation by interfering with protein synthesis. Neuropharmacology. 2014;79:525–533. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.11.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nguyen Jacques D., Aarde Shawn M., Vandewater Sophia A., Grant Yanabel, Stouffer David G., Parsons Loren H., Cole Maury, Taffe Michael A. Inhaled delivery of Δ9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) to rats by e-cigarette vapor technology. Neuropharmacology. 2016;109:112–120. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2016.05.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Núñez C, Ochoa S, Huerta-Ramos E, Baños I, Barajas A, Dolz M, et al (2016) Cannabis use and cognitive function in first episode psychosis: differential effect of heavy use. Psychopharmacology (Berl). [cited 2018 Nov 29];233(5):809–21. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26621349 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Ohlsson A, Lindgren J, Wahlen A, Agurell S, Hollister LE, Gillespie HK et al (1980) Plasma delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol concentrations and clinical effects after oral and intravenous administration and smoking. [cited 2019 May 16]. Available from: https://ascpt.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/pdf/10.1038/clpt.1980.181 [DOI] [PubMed]
- ORR LEO E., McKERNAN JOSEPH F. Antiemetic Effect of Δ9-Tetrahydrocannabinol in Chemotherapy-Associated Nausea and Emesis As Compared to Placebo and Compazine. The Journal of Clinical Pharmacology. 1981;21(S1):76S–80S. doi: 10.1002/j.1552-4604.1981.tb02578.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Park Ga Young, Kim Taekyung, Park Jinsick, Lee Eun Mi, Ryu Han Uk, Kim Sun I., Kim In Young, Kang Joong Koo, Jang Dong Pyo, Husain Masud. Neural correlates of spatial and nonspatial attention determined using intracranial electroencephalographic signals in humans. Human Brain Mapping. 2016;37(8):3041–3054. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23225. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patel VP, Feinstein A (2017) Cannabis and cognitive functioning in multiple sclerosis: the role of gender. Mult Scler J - Exp Transl Clin. [cited 2018 Nov 29];3(2):2055217317713027. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28634543 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Pope Harrison G., Gruber Amanda J., Hudson James I., Huestis Marilyn A., Yurgelun-Todd Deborah. Neuropsychological Performance in Long-term Cannabis Users. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2001;58(10):909. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.10.909. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ravizza SM, Mccormick CA, Schlerf JE, Justus T, Ivry RB, Fiez JA et al (2006) Cerebellar damage produces selective deficits in verbal working memory. Brain. [cited 2018 Jun 9];129:306–20. Available from: https://watermark.silverchair.com/awh685.pdf?token=AQECAHi208BE49Ooan9kkhW_Ercy7Dm3ZL_9Cf3qfKAc485ysgAAAaQwggGgBgkqhkiG9w0BBwagggGRMIIBjQIBADCCAYYGCSqGSIb3DQEHATAeBglghkgBZQMEAS4wEQQMtBhwE0z_PRY4C2qlAgEQgIIBV5hIfQ-AwSm-IM5QeW1858jDDSW6ZTS327HyUVawyF9n6bjciQFeob7BHxAWtOZPhJ485ZHCrZgTcnaMkxuRmtnQd97hb6PeI2XlMXnuQdnpnJt-SnShtz4lZTRZnhRy65zq9wR-KT5LCKHKlNpyavrx9v2csGk1Tm-XLPDvlLte-pv3dwtPMrxH5MKxnWgXt1yRjnBIvHTkcQY0KDj7UwdZbb3AU0bFGOex6Gu0jt0ors5UMd9YuKvq3OW0NhL0W1SkIP3SnP_Yop-GewWYeo30Dix57LEvppAzurrxFJ0cgHJHuSh4ju7g9NdbZoTDm1EwIpfATlsmhQsG6xIAue6a1huojvwPDwBth62mmSWOIkeiNEdMzX5zoThA5Jj5sbI8dtVuMeQO_I-H_3aVuXKwvTei3NLL1vFmsUAcovGH8C4lr_mZImUfoKxPsL6tN6tH_q40LsY [DOI] [PubMed]
- Rhodes, S., Seth, P., Coghill D (2008) Neuropsychological evidence for intact verbal but impaired spatial working memory in ADHD. Front hum Neurosci. [cited 2018 Jun 9];2. Available from: http://www.frontiersin.org/10.3389/conf.neuro.09.2009.01.300/event_abstract
- Rosenberg Evan C., Louik Jay, Conway Erin, Devinsky Orrin, Friedman Daniel. Quality of Life in Childhood Epilepsy in pediatric patients enrolled in a prospective, open-label clinical study with cannabidiol. Epilepsia. 2017;58(8):e96–e100. doi: 10.1111/epi.13815. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruiz-Contreras AE, Carrillo-Sánchez K, Ortega-Mora I, Barrera-Tlapa MA, Román-López T V., Rosas-Escobar CB et al (2014) Performance in working memory and attentional control is associated with the rs2180619 SNP in the CNR1 gene. Genes, Brain Behav. [cited 2019 May 7];13(2):173–8. Available from: http://doi.wiley.com/10.1111/gbb.12097 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Santana Fabiana, Sierra Rodrigo O., Haubrich Josué, Crestani Ana Paula, Duran Johanna Marcela, de Freitas Cassini Lindsey, de Oliveira Alvares Lucas, Quillfeldt Jorge A. Involvement of the infralimbic cortex and CA1 hippocampal area in reconsolidation of a contextual fear memory through CB1 receptors: Effects of CP55,940. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 2016;127:42–47. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2015.11.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schoeler T., Kambeitz J., Behlke I., Murray R., Bhattacharyya S. The effects of cannabis on memory function in users with and without a psychotic disorder: findings from a combined meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine. 2015;46(1):177–188. doi: 10.1017/S0033291715001646. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schuster Randi Melissa, Hoeppner Susanne S., Evins A. Eden, Gilman Jodi M. Early onset marijuana use is associated with learning inefficiencies. Neuropsychology. 2016;30(4):405–415. doi: 10.1037/neu0000281. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schwartz Richard H. Short-term Memory Impairment in Cannabis-Dependent Adolescents. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine. 1989;143(10):1214. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.1989.02150220110030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schweinsburg AD, Brown SA, Tapert SF (2008) The influence of marijuana use on neurocognitive functioning in adolescents. Curr Drug Abuse Rev. [cited 2018 Nov 29];1(1):99–111. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19630709 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Solowij N, Jones KA, Rozman ME, Davis SM, Ciarrochi J et al (2011) Verbal learning and memory in adolescent cannabis users, alcohol users and non-users Publication Details. [cited 2018 Nov 29]. Available from: http://josephciarrochi.com/wpcontent/uploads/2011/08/SolowijAdolescRAVLTPsychopharmacol.pdf [DOI] [PubMed]
- Taurisano P, Antonucci LA, Fazio L, Rampino A, Romano R, Porcelli A, Masellis R, Colizzi M, Quarto T, Torretta S, di Giorgio A, Pergola G, Bertolino A, Blasi G. Prefrontal activity during working memory is modulated by the interaction of variation in CB1 and COX2 coding genes and correlates with frequency of cannabis use. Cortex. 2016;81:231–238. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.05.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thames April D., Arbid Natalie, Sayegh Philip. Cannabis use and neurocognitive functioning in a non-clinical sample of users. Addictive Behaviors. 2014;39(5):994–999. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.01.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- WebPlotDigitizer (2018) Extract data from plots, images, and maps. [cited 2018 Mar 23]. Available from: https://automeris.io/WebPlotDigitizer/
- Yousefi B, Farjad M, Nasehi M, Zarrindast MR (2013) Involvement of the CA1 GABA A receptors in ACPA-induced impairment of spatial and non-spatial novelty detection in mice. Neurobiol Learn Mem. [cited 2017 Dec 23];100:32–40. Available from: https://ac.els-cdn.com/S1074742712001785/1-s2.0-S1074742712001785-main.pdf?_tid=3de3ce84-e823-11e7-b32b-00000aab0f26&acdnat=1514062552_e02ea18b54c5522865866b7988587b29 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Zajicek John, Fox Patrick, Sanders Hilary, Wright David, Vickery Jane, Nunn Andrew, Thompson Alan. Cannabinoids for treatment of spasticity and other symptoms related to multiple sclerosis (CAMS study): multicentre randomised placebo-controlled trial. The Lancet. 2003;362(9395):1517–1526. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(03)14738-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
(DOCX 784 kb)