Skip to main content
BMC Public Health logoLink to BMC Public Health
. 2019 Nov 5;19:1428. doi: 10.1186/s12889-019-7770-5

Companion dog acquisition and mental well-being: a community-based three-arm controlled study

Lauren Powell 1,, Kate M Edwards 2, Paul McGreevy 3, Adrian Bauman 1, Anthony Podberscek 4, Brendon Neilly 5, Catherine Sherrington 6, Emmanuel Stamatakis 1
PMCID: PMC6829935  PMID: 31684914

Abstract

Background

Dog ownership is suggested to improve mental well-being, although empirical evidence among community dog owners is limited. This study examined changes in human mental well-being following dog acquisition, including four measures: loneliness, positive and negative affect, and psychological distress.

Methods

We conducted an eight-month controlled study involving three groups (n = 71): 17 acquired a dog within 1 month of baseline (dog acquisition); 29 delayed dog acquisition until study completion (lagged control); and 25 had no intentions of acquiring a dog (community control). All participants completed the UCLA Loneliness Scale (possible scores 0–60), Positive and Negative Affect Schedule and Kessler10 at baseline, three-months and eight-months. We used repeated measures ANCOVAs to analyse data with owner age and sex included as covariates. Post-hoc tests were performed for significant effects (p < 0.05).

Results

There was a statistically significant group by time interaction for loneliness (p = 0.03), with an estimated reduction of 8.41 units (95% CI -16.57, − 0.26) from baseline to three-months and 7.12 (95% CI -12.55, − 1.69) from baseline to eight-months in the dog acquisition group. The group by time interaction for positive affect was also significant (p = 0.03), although there was no change in the dog acquisition group.

Conclusions

Companion dog acquisition may reduce loneliness among community dog owners. Our study provides useful direction for future larger trials on the effects of dog ownership on human mental well-being.

Trial registration

This trial was retrospectively registered on 5th July 2017 with the Australian New Zealand Clinical Trials Registry (ACTRN12617000967381).

Keywords: Dog ownership, Companion dogs, Psychological health, Mental well-being, Mental health, Human-animal interactions, Depression, Anxiety, Affect, Loneliness

Introduction

The World Health Organization considers mental well-being as an integral component of health. A positive state of mental well-being allows individuals to recognise their potential, cope with normal stresses, work productively and contribute to society [1]. Many common stressors such as long working hours, poor economic conditions and low physical activity patterns can reduce mental well-being [24]. Further, mental illness is one of the leading contributors to the global burden of disease [5].

Dog ownership is common worldwide. For example, over 50% of households in the United States and 39% in Australia have dogs [6]. It has been suggested that dog ownership can improve human mental well-being through several possible pathways [7]. Dogs may provide their owners with social support and companionship [8, 9] and they may also act as catalysts for increased human social interactions [1012]. Acute human–dog interactions have been shown to elicit positive hormonal effects including reduced cortisol concentrations, a biomarker of stress [1315], and increased oxytocin concentrations [1619]. Dog owners may also be more physically active than non-owners, as a result of dog-walking [2024], with a well-established link between physical activity and positive mental well-being [3, 4].

Most research investigating mental well-being and human–dog interactions has examined the efficacy of animal-assisted therapies to improve psychological outcomes among institutionalised individuals, such as those living in nursing homes, or clinical populations with mental illness or chronic disease [2530]. Among university students, dog-assisted interventions have also demonstrated that acute human–dog interactions have beneficial effects on measures of positive and negative affect [31, 32]. Longitudinal studies of dog ownership and mental well-being among community dwelling dog owners are rare. Only two studies, to date, have analysed the impact of companion animal acquisition on human physical and psychological health [33, 34], one of which reported positive results [33]. Both studies investigated a single indicator of mental well-being prior to and after pet acquisition, with a follow-up period of 6–10 months [33, 34]. A one-year prospective cohort study including n = 955 community-based older adults (≥65 years) has also been used to examine pet ownership and psychological well-being, documenting no association between ownership and overall satisfaction, happiness or perceived mental health [35]. The few cross-sectional correlate studies in the field have produced inconsistent findings [30]. For example, a survey of 1101 individuals residing in Perth, Australia suggested that dog owners are less lonely than non-owners [36], but other research found pet owners and non-owners do not differ in measures of loneliness [37] or psychological distress [37, 38].

The paucity of evidence and the conflicting results are partly attributable to a plethora of methodological challenges that are common in the field of human-animal interaction research [30]. The above cross-sectional studies [3638] are limited as they compare existing dog or pet owners to non-owners and cannot rule-out reverse causation, i.e. the possibility that individuals who are interested in dog ownership experience better health prior to acquiring a dog [39]. Randomised controlled trials, in which human participants are randomly allocated to dog ownership, are not feasible in this field [40, 41]. Randomised assignment of dogs to uninterested members of the community would raise irreconcilable animal welfare concerns, such as the potential for neglect or inadequate care, including veterinary care. Dog ownership necessitates a substantial time and economic commitment which would also introduce human ethical concerns if uninterested individuals were allocated to dog ownership. As randomised controlled trials are not feasible, the strongest possible design for examining the impact of dog ownership on mental well-being may be controlled studies in which non-owners acquire a companion dog [41].

The aim of this controlled study was to examine potential changes in mental well-being among community dog owners following dog acquisition, using four common measures: loneliness, positive and negative affect, and psychological distress.

Methods

Study design

This study formed part of a larger three-arm controlled study in which the primary aim was to investigate the impact of community-based dog ownership on device-based and self-reported human physical activity (to be reported in detail elsewhere). Indicators of mental well-being were considered as secondary outcomes in the larger study but are the primary outcomes of the current manuscript. Upon completion of the baseline measurements, participants self-allocated to one of three treatment groups based on their dog ownership intentions: imminent dog adopters (“dog acquisition”); individuals interested in dog ownership but delayed from acquisition for the study duration (“lagged control”); and individuals who had no interest or plans to acquire a companion dog (“community control”) (Fig. 1). We included two control groups to account for possible differences in sociodemographic characteristics or health behaviours associated with an interest in dog ownership [42].

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Study design and timeline

Recruitment

Between April 2017 and September 2018, participants were recruited using media releases (TV, radio and newspaper); online adoption resources; focused events; University of Sydney communications; researcher attendance at animal welfare shelters; social media advertisements; and flyers distributed in the community.

Participants were eligible if they were aged 18 or over; resided within 60 km of the Sydney city centre, Australia; had an absence of physical limitations that could prevent walking; and did not currently own a dog or other furry pet (e.g. cat, rabbit) or plan to acquire one for the duration of the study. Individuals who had owned a dog in the 12 months prior to recruitment were excluded from the study. Participants in the dog acquisition group had to acquire a dog within 1 month of baseline measurements and be the main/joint carer of the dog. Additionally, the dog had to be free of veterinary conditions that would limit low intensity activities such as walking, and not have entered the last quintile of expected lifespan for their breed.

Participants were reimbursed for the time dedicated to participating in the study upon the completion of all measurements. Dog acquisition and lagged control group participants received a 12-month supply of routine dog medications, including vaccinations, internal and external parasite protection, and one veterinary appointment. Community control participants were offered $150 compensation.

Ethical approval was obtained from the University of Sydney Human Research Ethics Committee (2016/921) and Animal Ethics Committee (2017/1134). The study was registered with the Australian New Zealand Clinical Trials Registry (ACTRN12617000967381). All methods were performed in accordance with the relevant guidelines. All participants provided informed written consent.

Questionnaires

We collected self-reported sociodemographic data including age, gender, level of education, companion animal ownership history, cardiometabolic health and lifestyle health habits, which are described here but will examined in detail elsewhere. Mental well-being questionnaires were administered three times over an eight-month period: at baseline, at three-months and at the end of the study.

Loneliness

The UCLA Loneliness Scale [43, 44] is a valid and reliable tool [45, 46] to measure loneliness and social isolation in community populations [34], including multiple Australian cohorts [4749]. The 20-item questionnaire provides brief descriptions of feelings, such as ‘I am unhappy doing so many things alone.’ Participants reported how often they believed each description was indicative of them. The possible responses were never (0), rarely (1), sometimes (2) and often (3). Individual item scores were then added to provide a total score, with a possible range of 0 to 60 [44].

Positive and negative affect

The Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) [50], and its short form [51] are valid and reliable tools to measure affect [5254] and have been used in similar Australian community cohorts [5557]. The Short PANAS, used in the current study, consists of 10 adjectives describing positive [5] or negative [5] emotions. Participants indicated the intensity of each emotion during the previous week with possible answers ranging from very slightly or not at all [1] to extremely [5]. Total positive and negative affect scores were calculated by adding the scores of each relevant item, with possible scores ranging from five to 25 [53].

Psychological distress

Kessler10 (K10) is a 10-item questionnaire which uses a Likert-type scale to measure psychological distress, specifically anxiety and depression, over the most recent 28-day period [58]. Participants were asked questions such as ‘During the last four weeks, about how often did you feel nervous?’ with 5 possible responses: none of the time (1), a little of the time (2), some of the time (3), most of the time (4) and all of the time (5). K10 has been used extensively across various populations and exhibits good psychometric qualities [5961]. A total K10 score was calculated by summing the individual item scores, with a range of scores from 10 (no distress) to 50 (extreme distress) [62].

At mid- and end-of-study measurements, dog acquisition participants were asked an additional four questions regarding new social interactions they had experienced as a result of their dog (Additional file 1: Supplementary text) [63]. The questionnaire has demonstrated excellent reliability in comparable Australian cohorts [38]. We report these data as ancillary descriptive statistics.

Statistical analysis

We used repeated measures ANCOVAs to examine the change in UCLA loneliness, positive and negative affect, and K10 scores following dog acquisition with owner age and sex included as covariates. In additional analyses, we also included education as a covariate. As the exposure was the same across the lagged control and community control groups (no dog acquisition), we conducted supplementary analyses comparing dog acquisition participants to a pooled group of control participants. To maximise use of available data, we also used repeated measures ANCOVAs to compare differences in questionnaire scores between baseline and three-month mid-study measurements where we included the five participants who did not complete the final eight-month study measurements. Post-hoc tests were performed for significant effects (p < 0.05). Partial Eta Squared (ηp2) was determined as a measure of effect size. SPSS version 24 was used for all statistical analyses.

Results

Ninety-six participants enrolled in the study and completed baseline data collection (26 in the dog acquisition group, 37 in the lagged control group and 33 in the community control group (Additional file 1: Figure S1). Seventy-one participants completed the study. Eight participants were excluded due to ineligibility following baseline measurements, such as moved outside the Sydney area (n = 3 dog acquisition, n = 2 lagged control, n = 3 community control). Six dog acquisition participants dropped out due to failure to acquire a dog (n = 3), unknown reasons (n = 2) or relinquishment (n = 1). Six lagged control participants dropped out for unknown reasons (n = 5) or withdrawing consent (n = 1). Five community control participants dropped out for unknown reasons (n = 3) or withdrawing consent (n = 2). There were no significant differences in baseline characteristics between participants who did not complete the study and the final sample, in terms of age, gender, education, smoking status, alcohol consumption, physical activity, sedentary behaviour patterns, loneliness, positive and negative affect, and psychological distress.

The baseline characteristics of participants who completed the study and were entered in the main analyses (n = 71) are presented in Table 1. There were statistically significant differences between the groups in terms of age (p = 0.01) and education (p = 0.02). Mean age was significantly higher in the community control group. The proportion of individuals who had completed university education was also greater in the lagged control and community control groups compared with the dog acquisition group. At baseline, loneliness (p = 0.66), positive affect (p = 0.39) and psychological distress (p = 0.16) were comparable between the groups. Negative affect was significantly greater in the dog acquisition group (p = 0.02).

Table 1.

Baseline characteristics of the study sample by dog ownership status (n = 71)

Baseline characteristics Dog ownership status
Dog acquisition (n = 17) Lagged control (n = 29) Community control (n = 25)
Age (years) 36.9 (10.6) 38.0 (13.6) 50.7 (18.4)
Gender (female %) 100 75.9 80.0
Physical activity
 Bouts of 10+ mins walking/week a 11.5 (7.6) 8.3 (5.5) 8.9 (7.9)
 Minutes spent walking/week a 303.2 (277.7) 219.8 (192.4) 251.6 (202.7)
 Time spent sedentary (hours/day) a 7.7 (2.7) 7.8 (2.9) 7.4 (3.5)
Smoking status
 Current/Previous (%) 11.8 24.1 32.0
 Never (%) 88.2 75.9 68.0
Alcohol consumption
 1 or more days/week 70.6 55.2 56.0
 Less than once per week 29.4 44.8 44.0
Education b
 Trade certificate/diploma or less (%) 47.1 17.2 12.0
 Bachelor’s or post graduate degree (%) 52.9 82.8 88.0
Previous dog ownership (%) 52.9 65.5 44.0
Dogs as social catalysts c
 Got to know people in their neighbourhood (%) 82.4 N/A N/A
 Considered this person a friend (%) 35.3 N/A N/A
 Could ask for information (%) 76.5 N/A N/A

Data are presented as mean (standard deviation) unless indicated otherwise

aBased on participant’s self-reported physical activity and sedentary behaviour patterns

bHighest level of education completed

cBased on responses to dogs as social catalysts questionnaire at end-of-study measurements

Approximately half of participants in the dog acquisition group (n = 9) had previously owned a dog, most as the primary or co-carer (n = 6). Many dog adopters reported new social interaction following dog acquisition with 82.4% of dog acquisition participants reporting they met people in their neighbourhood because of their dog, and 76.5% considering such people as sources of advice (Table 1). A smaller proportion of dog adopters (35.3%) considered the new social connection as a friend.

Impact of dog acquisition on indicators of mental well-being

Figure 2 presents the estimated marginal mean scores (adjusted for age and sex) for loneliness, positive and negative affect, and psychological distress by study group (n = 71).

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Estimated marginal means and the standard error of the mean for questionnaire scores by dog ownership status, adjusted for age and sex. a Loneliness. Possible UCLA loneliness scores range from 0 to 60. b Positive affect. Possible Positive Affect Schedule (PAS) scores range from 5 to 25. c Negative affect. Possible Negative Affect Schedule (NAS) scores range from 5 to 25. d Psychological distress. Possible Kessler10 (K10) scores range from 10 to 50. *Denotes a statistically significant group by time interaction in repeated measures ANCOVA (p < 0.05)

Loneliness

Repeated measures ANCOVA showed a statistically significant group*time interaction for loneliness (F(4,132) = 2.68, p = 0.03, ηp2 = 0.08). The dog acquisition group displayed an estimated mean reduction of 8.41 units (95% confidence intervals (CI) -16.57, − 0.26, p = 0.04) from baseline to mid-study and 7.12 units (95% CI -12.55, − 1.69, p = 0.01) from baseline to end-of-study.

Positive and negative affect

We observed a significant group*time interaction in repeated measures ANCOVA for positive affect (F(4,132) = 2.75, p = 0.03, ηp2 = 0.08). Among the lagged control group, post-hoc tests estimated a mean reduction of 1.24 units (95% CI -2.33, − 0.15, p = 0.03) in the positive affect scale from baseline to mid-study. There were no significant differences in the dog acquisition (p = 0.15) or control groups (p = 0.43). For negative affect, the group*time interaction was F(4,132) = 2.39, p = 0.05, ηp2 = 0.07.

Psychological distress

There were no statistically significant group by time effects for psychological distress (F(4,132) = 0.61, p = 0.66, ηp2 = 0.02).

Pooled control group analyses

Additional file 1: Figure S2 displays the estimated marginal means (adjusted for age and sex) for loneliness, positive and negative affect, and psychological distress for the dog acquisition and pooled control groups (n = 71). Repeated measures ANCOVA analyses were performed for each outcome (2 group × 3 time points).

The group*time interaction for loneliness was statistically significant (F(2,134) = 4.70, p = 0.01, ηp2 = 0.07). Mirroring the results of the primary analysis, the dog acquisition group displayed a statistically significant reduction of 8.41 units (95% CI -16.57, − 0.26, p = 0.04) from baseline to mid-study and 7.12 units (95% CI -12.55, − 1.69, p = 0.01) from baseline to end-of-study. In contrast to the primary analysis, loneliness scores were also significantly reduced in the combined control group, with a mean reduction of 3.06 units (95% CI -5.25, − 0.86, p = 0.01) between baseline and end-of-study measurements. For positive affect, the group*time interaction was non-significant (F(2,134) = 2.71, p = 0.07, ηp2 = 0.04). For negative affect, there was a statistically significant group*time interaction (F(2,134) = 3.60, p = 0.03, ηp2 = 0.05). Contrary to the primary results, we observed a statistically significant reduction in the dog acquisition group of 3.59 units (95% CI -6.31, − 0.87, p = 0.01) in the negative affect scale from baseline to mid-study and 3.53 units (95% CI -5.51, − 1.55, p = 0.002) from baseline to end-of-study. The combined control group also displayed a significant reduction between baseline and the end-of-study measurements (estimated mean change − 1.3, 95% CI -2.19, − 0.40, p = 0.01). In agreement with the primary analyses, the group*time interaction for psychological distress was not statistically significant (F(2,134) = 1.03, p = 0.36, ηp2 = 0.02).

Baseline to three-month analyses

Supplementary repeated measures ANCOVA analyses including all participants with valid data at baseline and mid-study measures (n = 76) produced similar results to the primary analyses. We observed a significant group*time interaction in loneliness (F (2, 71)=4.66, p = 0.01, ηp2 = 0.12). Among the dog acquisition group, there was a mean reduction of 8.4 (95% CI -15.66, − 1.18, p = 0.03) units in the loneliness scale. There was also a statistically significant group*time interaction in positive affect (F (2, 71)=4.09, p = 0.02, ηp2 = 0.10), with the lagged control group displaying a decrease (− 1.19, 95% CI -2.30, − 0.09, p = 0.04). Among the dog acquisition group, positive affect was not significantly different. The group* time interactions for negative affect and psychological distress were not statistically significant (F (2, 71)=1.86, p = 0.16, ηp2 = 0.05 and F (2, 71)=0.75, p = 0.78, ηp2 = 0.02, respectively).

Additional adjustment for education

Repeated measures ANCOVA analyses with adjustment for owner age, gender and education (n = 71) produced null findings for all four scales. The additional education adjustment nullified the association between dog acquisition and loneliness, presenting a group*time interaction of F(4,130) = 1.85, p = 0.12, ηp2 = 0.05. The group*time interactions for positive affect and negative affect were F(4,130) = 2.41, p = 0.05, ηp2 = 0.07 and F(4,130) = 2.28, p = 0.06, ηp2 = 0.07, respectively. For psychological distress, the group*time interaction was F(4,130) = 0.65, p = 0.63, ηp2 = 0.02.

Discussion

The aim of this study was to examine changes in mental well-being following dog acquisition, including four measures: loneliness, positive and negative affect, and psychological distress. This controlled study provides some of the first longitudinal evidence that dog acquisition may reduce loneliness among community-dwelling dog owners. Following dog acquisition, we observed a moderate reduction [64] in loneliness within 3 months, with the observation persisting until the end of the study. The significant difference in loneliness was also apparent in supplementary analyses including the pooled control group. A possible explanation for our findings is that human–dog interactions elicit acute positive effects on mood [31, 32, 65], and the regular occurrence of these interactions, as seen in dog ownership, produces long-term improvements. Indeed, research investigating the efficacy of canine interactions in reducing psychological distress in university students [31, 32] and preadolescents [65] has found brief human–dog interactions can acutely improve positive affect and reduce negative affect. Considering the association between loneliness and negative mood [66], it is plausible that the potential mood enhancing effects of regular human–dog interactions may reduce loneliness. Similarly, cross-sectional research has shown that support from a companion animal mediates the relationship between loneliness and negative mood in older women [67]. Another possible explanation is that dog ownership increases human social interaction, thereby improving the social well-being of dog owners and reducing their loneliness. Dogs may act as catalysts for social interaction [10, 11, 68, 69]. An ancillary finding in our study to support this explanation was that most dog owners had met people in their neighbourhood because of their dog and some even considered such people as potential sources of advice. Accordingly, a preliminary investigation of the possible mediating role of human social interaction in alleviating loneliness has shown dog walkers who conversed with others during their walks reported lower levels of loneliness compared with dog walkers who did not converse with others [70]. Our results are discordant with a previous quasi-experimental study that investigated companion animal acquisition and loneliness using the UCLA scale [34]. In their sample of 59 adults, 16 of whom had acquired a dog by the end of the study, Gilbey, McNicholas [34] found no significant differences in loneliness following cat or dog acquisition.

In the supplementary analyses including adjustment for education as an indicator of socioeconomic status (SES), the association between dog ownership and loneliness was nullified. Current literature indicates an increased risk of mental illness with low SES [7173]. In the present study, SES may also have influenced the impact of dog acquisition on loneliness. For example, low SES individuals may have experienced reduced social support [74, 75] at baseline and as such, gained greater benefit from the social support and companionship provided by dogs. However, we did not collect data on participants’ social support, which would have aided this interpretation. In the supplementary analyses including the pooled control groups, there was also a reduction in loneliness among this group. The differences in self-reported mental well-being among the pooled control group may be the result of study participation, whereby individuals alter their responses or behaviour due to their awareness of being observed [76, 77].

We did not find evidence that dog acquisition influenced positive affect although, there was a significant difference in the lagged control group, with a moderate reduction [64] in positive affect at 3 months. The difference in positive affect did not persist at 8 months or in supplementary analyses including the pooled control group. Considering negative affect, we found evidence that dog acquisition was associated with a moderate reduction [64] among dog adopters. Although the results did not reach statistical significance in the primary analysis, we observed a significant reduction in the dog acquisition group when we pooled the control groups. Similarly to loneliness, the reduction occurred rapidly within 3 months and persisted until the end of the study. The mood enhancing effects of acute human–dog interactions, detailed above, may improve chronic measures of affect through the occurrence of regular acute human–dog interactions as seen in dog ownership [31, 32, 65]. We also observed a reduction in negative affect in the lagged control group, albeit to a lesser extent than the dog acquisition group, which may be the result of study participation effects [78], as described above.

We did not find evidence that dog acquisition significantly affects psychological distress. Our findings are congruent with prior cross-sectional studies that found companion animal ownership was not associated with symptoms of anxiety or depression [37, 38]. Conversely, the only comparable observational study that has investigated dog acquisition and symptoms of psychological distress reported a significant reduction in General Health Questionnaire scores among a sample of 47 dog adopters (total n = 71) [33]. Other cross-sectional studies have suggested that pet owners report greater depressive symptoms [55]. Overall, the contradictory results highlight the need for further research in dog ownership and mental well-being.

One of the strengths of our controlled study is the longitudinal design. To our knowledge, only two studies to date have used similar designs to investigate dog ownership and human mental well-being [33, 34], one of which was conducted almost three decades ago [33]. Another strength is the use of a broad range of measures to capture mental well-being. There are also several limitations of the study which necessitate cautious interpretation of our findings. Firstly, there is a lack of randomisation of dog ownership, which is not feasible for this exposure. There is also a lack of allocation concealment and blinding. As a result, selection bias may have occurred due to participants’ self-selection to their group. We also found differences between the treatment groups in terms of age and education. To reduce the possible impacts of these imbalances, we investigated changes in mental well-being over time and adjusted for both age and education. However, it must be noted that the adjustment for education nullified the results. There was also a significant difference at baseline between the groups in negative affect. The small sample size of the dog acquisition group suggests this analysis may have been statistically underpowered, which could have contributed to the instability in our results. For example, by altering the grouping of participants, such as pooling the control groups in the supplementary analyses, we found some results were inconsistent with the primary analyses. The pooling of control participants may also have introduced response bias because participants who expressed an interest in dog ownership may be inherently different from those with no ownership intentions. Finally, the absence of males in the dog acquisition group may limit the generalisability of our findings.

Conclusions

In this sample of Australian urban dog owners, acquisition of a dog was associated with a reduction in loneliness within three months, with the observation persisting to the end of the study. Our results are suggestive of a relatively rapid, positive impact of dog acquisition on some indicators of human mental well-being. Our study provides preliminary, albeit unique, insights to inform future larger controlled studies on the relationship between dog ownership and human mental health.

Supplementary information

12889_2019_7770_MOESM1_ESM.docx (410.4KB, docx)

Additional file 1: Supplementary text. Dogs as catalysts for new social interactions. Figure S1. CONSORT flow diagram modified for non-randomized trial design. Figure S2. Estimated marginal means adjusted for age and sex with standard error of the mean for questionnaire scores for dog acquisition and the pooled control group. A) Loneliness. Possible UCLA loneliness scores range from 0 to 60. B) Positive affect. Possible Positive Affect Schedule (PAS) scores range from 5 to 25. C) Negative affect. Possible Negative Affect Schedule (NAS) scores range from 5 to 25. D) Psychological distress. Possible Kessler10 (K10) scores range from 10 to 50. *Denotes a statistically significant group by time interaction in repeated measures ANCOVA (p < 0.05).

Acknowledgements

We thank Animal Medicines Australia for their support with in-kind compensation to participants. We thank Vicki Davy and John Bishop from PetRescue (www.petrescue.com.au) for assisting us in recruiting participants through their website.

Abbreviations

CI

Confidence intervals

K10

Kessler 10

PANAS

Positive and Negative Affect Schedule

Authors’ contributions

ES initiated and designed the study, acquired funding, led the broader research program, and supervised the PAWS pilot study. KE, PM, AB, CS, and AP contributed to the study design. LP led the data collection. BN contributed to the acqusition of data. LP cleaned and analysed the data, drafted the manuscript, and carried out multiple rounds of revisions. ES redrafted parts of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final version of the manuscript before submission.

Funding

This research was supported by a research donation provided by Ms. Lynne Cattell [University of Sydney grant ID: 183100]. The donor had no involvement in the study design; collection, analysis and interpretation of data; writing of the report or the decision to submit the article for publication.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets generated during the current study are not publicly available due to the requirements of the ethical approval but are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Ethical approval was obtained from the University of Sydney Human Research Ethics Committee (2016/921) and Animal Ethics Committee (2017/1134). All participants provided informed written consent.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

Supplementary information accompanies this paper at 10.1186/s12889-019-7770-5.

References

  • 1.Herrman H, Saxena S, Moodie R. Promoting mental health: concepts, emerging evidence, practice: a report of the World Health Organization, Department of Mental Health and Substance Abuse in collaboration with the Victorian Health Promotion Foundation and the University of Melbourne: World Health Organization; 2005.
  • 2.De Moortel D, Thévenon O, De Witte H, Vanroelen C. Working hours mismatch, macroeconomic changes, and mental well-being in Europe. J Health Soc Behav. 2017;58(2):217–231. doi: 10.1177/0022146517706532. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Penedo FJ, Dahn JR. Exercise and well-being: a review of mental and physical health benefits associated with physical activity. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2005;18(2):189–193. doi: 10.1097/00001504-200503000-00013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Paluska SA, Schwenk TL. Physical activity and mental health. Sports Med. 2000;29(3):167–180. doi: 10.2165/00007256-200029030-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.World Health Organization . Mental health action plan 2013–2020. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.GfK . Global GfK survey: pet ownership Germany: GfK. 2016. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Clark Cline KM. Psychological effects of dog ownership: role strain, role enhancement, and depression. J Soc Psychol. 2010;150(2):117–131. doi: 10.1080/00224540903368533. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Knight S, Edwards V. In the company of wolves: the physical, social, and psychological benefits of dog ownership. J Aging Health. 2008;20(4):437–455. doi: 10.1177/0898264308315875. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.McConnell AR, Brown CM, Shoda TM, Stayton LE, Martin CE. Friends with benefits: on the positive consequences of pet ownership. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2011;101(6):1239. doi: 10.1037/a0024506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.McNicholas J, Collis GM. Dogs as catalysts for social interactions: robustness of the effect. Br J Psychol. 2000;91(1):61–70. doi: 10.1348/000712600161673. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Bould E, Bigby C, Bennett PC, Howell T. ‘More people talk to you when you have a dog’–dogs as catalysts for social inclusion of people with intellectual disabilities. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2018;62(10):833–841. doi: 10.1111/jir.12538. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Wood LJ, Giles-Corti B, Bulsara MK, Bosch DA. More than a furry companion: the ripple effect of companion animals on neighborhood interactions and sense of community. Society & Animals. 2007;15(1):43–56. doi: 10.1163/156853007X169333. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Polheber JP, Matchock RL. The presence of a dog attenuates cortisol and heart rate in the Trier social stress test compared to human friends. J Behav Med. 2014;37(5):860–867. doi: 10.1007/s10865-013-9546-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Handlin L, Hydbring-Sandberg E, Nilsson A, Ejdebäck M, Jansson A, Uvnäs-Moberg K. Short-term interaction between dogs and their owners: effects on oxytocin, cortisol, insulin and heart rate—an exploratory study. Anthrozoös. 2011;24(3):301–315. doi: 10.2752/175303711X13045914865385. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Odendaal JS, Meintjes RA. Neurophysiological correlates of affiliative behaviour between humans and dogs. Vet J. 2003;165(3):296–301. doi: 10.1016/S1090-0233(02)00237-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Nagasawa M, Mitsui S, En S, Ohtani N, Ohta M, Sakuma Y, et al. Oxytocin-gaze positive loop and the coevolution of human-dog bonds. Science. 2015;348(6232):333–336. doi: 10.1126/science.1261022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Nagasawa M, Kikusui T, Onaka T, Ohta M. Dog's gaze at its owner increases owner's urinary oxytocin during social interaction. Horm Behav. 2009;55(3):434–441. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Miller SC, Kennedy CC, DeVoe DC, Hickey M, Nelson T, Kogan L. An examination of changes in oxytocin levels in men and women before and after interaction with a bonded dog. Anthrozoös. 2009;22(1):31–42. doi: 10.2752/175303708X390455. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Nagasawa M, Ogawa M, Mogi K, Kikusui T. Intranasal oxytocin treatment increases eye-gaze behavior toward the owner in ancient Japanese dog breeds. Front Psychol. 2017;8:1624. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01624. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Westgarth C, Knuiman M, Christian H. Understanding how dogs encourage and motivate walking: cross-sectional findings from RESIDE. BMC Public Health. 2016;16(1):1019. doi: 10.1186/s12889-016-3660-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Cutt H, Giles-Corti B, Knuiman M, Timperio A, Bull F. Understanding dog Owners' increased levels of physical activity: results from RESIDE. Am J Public Health. 2008;98(1):66–69. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2006.103499. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Rhodes RE, Lim C. Understanding action control of daily walking behavior among dog owners: a community survey. BMC Public Health. 2016;16(1):1165. doi: 10.1186/s12889-016-3814-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Dall PM, Ellis SLH, Ellis BM, Grant PM, Colyer A, Gee NR, et al. The influence of dog ownership on objective measures of free-living physical activity and sedentary behaviour in community-dwelling older adults: a longitudinal case-controlled study. BMC Public Health. 2017;17:496. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 24.Wu Y-T, Luben R, Jones A. Dog ownership supports the maintenance of physical activity during poor weather in older English adults: cross-sectional results from the EPIC Norfolk cohort. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2017;71(9):905. doi: 10.1136/jech-2017-208987. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Wells DL. The effects of animals on human health and well-being. J Soc Issues. 2009;65(3):523–543. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4560.2009.01612.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Le Roux MC, Kemp R. Effect of a companion dog on depression and anxiety levels of elderly residents in a long-term care facility. Psychogeriatrics. 2009;9(1):23–26. doi: 10.1111/j.1479-8301.2009.00268.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Majić T, Gutzmann H, Heinz A, Lang UE, Rapp MA. Animal-assisted therapy and agitation and depression in nursing home residents with dementia: a matched case–control trial. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2013;21(11):1052–1059. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2013.03.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Barker SB, Dawson KS. The effects of animal-assisted therapy on anxiety ratings of hospitalized psychiatric patients. Psychiatr Serv. 1998;49(6):797–801. doi: 10.1176/ps.49.6.797. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Hoffmann AO, Lee AH, Wertenauer F, Ricken R, Jansen JJ, Gallinat J, et al. Dog-assisted intervention significantly reduces anxiety in hospitalized patients with major depression. Eur J Integr Med. 2009;1(3):145–148. doi: 10.1016/j.eujim.2009.08.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Wells DL. The state of research on human–animal relations: implications for human health. Anthrozoös. 2019;32(2):169–181. doi: 10.1080/08927936.2019.1569902. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Grajfoner D, Harte E, Potter L, McGuigan N. The effect of dog-assisted intervention on student well-being, mood, and anxiety. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2017;14(5):483. doi: 10.3390/ijerph14050483. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Crossman MK, Kazdin AE, Knudson K. Brief unstructured interaction with a dog reduces distress. Anthrozoös. 2015;28(4):649–659. doi: 10.1080/08927936.2015.1070008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Serpell J. Beneficial effects of pet ownership on some aspects of human health and behaviour. JRSM. 1991;84(12):717–720. doi: 10.1177/014107689108401208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Gilbey A, McNicholas J, Collis GM. A longitudinal test of the belief that companion animal ownership can help reduce loneliness. Anthrozoös. 2007;20(4):345–353. doi: 10.2752/089279307X245473. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Raina P, Waltner-Toews D, Bonnett B, Woodward C, Abernathy T. Influence of companion animals on the physical and psychological health of older people: an analysis of a one-year longitudinal study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1999;47(3):323–329. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1999.tb02996.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Headey B. Health benefits and health cost savings due to pets: preliminary estimates from an Australian national survey. Soc Indic Res. 1999;47(2):233–243. doi: 10.1023/A:1006892908532. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Antonacopoulos NMD, Pychyl TA. An examination of the potential role of pet ownership, human social support and pet attachment in the psychological health of individuals living alone. Anthrozoös. 2010;23(1):37–54. doi: 10.2752/175303710X12627079939143. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Wood L, Giles-Corti B, Bulsara M. The pet connection: pets as a conduit for social capital? Soc Sci Med. 2005;61(6):1159–1173. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2005.01.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Herzog H. The impact of pets on human health and psychological well-being: fact, fiction, or hypothesis? Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2011;20(4):236–239. doi: 10.1177/0963721411415220. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Chia D, Powell L, Lee V, Haghighi MM, Podberscek A, Ding D, et al. Sociodemographic correlates of prospective dog owners’ intentions to participate in controlled trials of dog ownership and human health. BMC Res Notes. 2018;11(1):169. doi: 10.1186/s13104-018-3277-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Chur-Hansen A, Stern C, Winefield H. Gaps in the evidence about companion animals and human health: some suggestions for progress. Int J Evid Based Healthc. 2010;8(3):140–146. doi: 10.1111/j.1744-1609.2010.00176.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Saunders J, Parast L, Babey SH, Miles JV. Exploring the differences between pet and non-pet owners: implications for human-animal interaction research and policy. PLoS One. 2017;12(6):e0179494. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0179494. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Russell D, Peplau LA, Ferguson ML. Developing a measure of loneliness. J Pers Assess. 1978;42(3):290–294. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa4203_11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Russell DW. UCLA loneliness scale (version 3): reliability, validity, and factor structure. J Pers Assess. 1996;66(1):20–40. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa6601_2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Knight RG, Chisholm BJ, Marsh NV, Godfrey HP. Some normative, reliability, and factor analytic data for the revised UCLA loneliness scale. J Clin Psychol. 1988;44(2):203–206. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(198803)44:2&#x0003c;203::AID-JCLP2270440218&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Russell D, Peplau LA, Cutrona CE. The revised UCLA loneliness scale: concurrent and discriminant validity evidence. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1980;39(3):472. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.39.3.472. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Steed L, Boldy D, Grenade L, Iredell H. The demographics of loneliness among older people in Perth, Western Australia. Australas J Ageing. 2007;26(2):81–86. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-6612.2007.00221.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Botterill E, Gill PR, McLaren S, Gomez R. Marital status and problem gambling among Australian older adults: the mediating role of loneliness. J Gambl Stud. 2016;32(3):1027–1038. doi: 10.1007/s10899-015-9575-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Mellor D, Stokes M, Firth L, Hayashi Y, Cummins R. Need for belonging, relationship satisfaction, loneliness, and life satisfaction. Pers Individ Dif. 2008;45(3):213–218. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2008.03.020. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1988;54(6):1063. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Kercher K. Assessing subjective well-being in the old-old: the PANAS as a measure of orthogonal dimensions of positive and negative affect. Res Aging. 1992;14(2):131–168. doi: 10.1177/0164027592142001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Crawford JR, Henry JD. The positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS): construct validity, measurement properties and normative data in a large non-clinical sample. Br J Clin Psychol. 2004;43(3):245–265. doi: 10.1348/0144665031752934. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Mackinnon A, Jorm AF, Christensen H, Korten AE, Jacomb PA, Rodgers B. A short form of the positive and negative affect schedule: evaluation of factorial validity and invariance across demographic variables in a community sample. Pers Individ Dif. 1999;27(3):405–416. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00251-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Thompson ER. Development and validation of an internationally reliable short-form of the positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS) J Cross-Cult Psychol. 2007;38(2):227–242. doi: 10.1177/0022022106297301. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Parslow RA, Jorm AF, Christensen H, Rodgers B, Jacomb P. Pet ownership and health in older adults: findings from a survey of 2,551 community-based Australians aged 60–64. Gerontology. 2005;51(1):40–47. doi: 10.1159/000081433. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Burns RA, Anstey KJ, Windsor TD. Subjective well-being mediates the effects of resilience and mastery on depression and anxiety in a large community sample of young and middle-aged adults. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2011;45(3):240–248. doi: 10.3109/00048674.2010.529604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Vella-Brodrick DA, Park N, Peterson C. Three ways to be happy: pleasure, engagement, and meaning—findings from Australian and US samples. Soc Indic Res. 2009;90(2):165–179. doi: 10.1007/s11205-008-9251-6. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Kessler RC, Andrews G, Colpe LJ, Hiripi E, Mroczek DK, Normand S-L, et al. Short screening scales to monitor population prevalences and trends in non-specific psychological distress. Psychol Med. 2002;32(6):959–976. doi: 10.1017/S0033291702006074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Fassaert T, De Wit M, Tuinebreijer W, Wouters H, Verhoeff A, Beekman A, et al. Psychometric properties of an interviewer-administered version of the Kessler psychological distress scale (K10) among Dutch, Moroccan and Turkish respondents. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. 2009;18(3):159–168. doi: 10.1002/mpr.288. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Furukawa TA, Kessler RC, Slade T, Andrews G. The performance of the K6 and K10 screening scales for psychological distress in the Australian National Survey of mental health and well-being. Psychol Med. 2003;33(2):357–362. doi: 10.1017/S0033291702006700. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Sampasa-Kanyinga H, Zamorski MA, Colman I. The psychometric properties of the 10-item Kessler psychological distress scale (K10) in Canadian military personnel. PLoS One. 2018;13(4):e0196562. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0196562. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Andrews G, Slade T. Interpreting scores on the Kessler psychological distress scale (K10) Aust N Z J Public Health. 2001;25(6):494–497. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-842X.2001.tb00310.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Wood L, Martin K, Christian H, Nathan A, Lauritsen C, Houghton S, et al. The pet factor-companion animals as a conduit for getting to know people, friendship formation and social support. PLoS One. 2015;10(4):e0122085. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0122085. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences: Routledge. 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Kerns KA, Stuart-Parrigon KL, Coifman KG, van Dulmen MH, Koehn A. Pet dogs: does their presence influence preadolescents' emotional responses to a social stressor? Soc Dev. 2018;27(1):34–44. doi: 10.1111/sode.12246. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Cacioppo JT, Hughes ME, Waite LJ, Hawkley LC, Thisted RA. Loneliness as a specific risk factor for depressive symptoms: cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses. Psychol Aging. 2006;21(1):140. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.21.1.140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Krause-Parello CA. Pet ownership and older women: the relationships among loneliness, pet attachment support, human social support, and depressed mood. Geriatr Nurs. 2012;33(3):194–203. doi: 10.1016/j.gerinurse.2011.12.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Wells DL. The facilitation of social interactions by domestic dogs. Anthrozoös. 2004;17(4):340–352. doi: 10.2752/089279304785643203. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Brooks H, Rushton K, Walker S, Lovell K, Rogers A. Ontological security and connectivity provided by pets: a study in the self-management of the everyday lives of people diagnosed with a long-term mental health condition. BMC Psychiatry. 2016;16(1):409. doi: 10.1186/s12888-016-1111-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Antonacopoulos NMD, Pychyl TA. An examination of the possible benefits for well-being arising from the social interactions that occur while dog walking. Soc Anim. 2014;22(5):459–480. doi: 10.1163/15685306-12341338. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Everson SA, Maty SC, Lynch JW, Kaplan GA. Epidemiologic evidence for the relation between socioeconomic status and depression, obesity, and diabetes. J Psychosom Res. 2002;53(4):891–895. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3999(02)00303-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Hoebel J, Maske UE, Zeeb H, Lampert T. Social inequalities and depressive symptoms in adults: the role of objective and subjective socioeconomic status. PLoS One. 2017;12(1):e0169764. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0169764. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Lorant V, Deliège D, Eaton W, Robert A, Philippot P, Ansseau M. Socioeconomic inequalities in depression: a meta-analysis. Am J Epidemiol. 2003;157(2):98–112. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwf182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Stringhini S, Berkman L, Dugravot A, Ferrie JE, Marmot M, Kivimaki M, et al. Socioeconomic status, structural and functional measures of social support, and mortality: the British Whitehall II cohort study, 1985–2009. Am J Epidemiol. 2012;175(12):1275–1283. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwr461. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Melchiorre MG, Chiatti C, Lamura G, Torres-Gonzales F, Stankunas M, Lindert J, et al. Social support, socio-economic status, health and abuse among older people in seven European countries. PLoS One. 2013;8(1):e54856. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0054856. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.De Amici D, Klersy C, Ramajoli F, Brustia L, Politi P. Impact of the Hawthorne effect in a longitudinal clinical study: the case of anesthesia. Control Clin Trials. 2000;21(2):103–114. doi: 10.1016/S0197-2456(99)00054-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.McCambridge J, Witton J, Elbourne DR. Systematic review of the Hawthorne effect: new concepts are needed to study research participation effects. J Clin Epidemiol. 2014;67(3):267–277. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2013.08.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.McCambridge J, Kypri K, Elbourne D. Research participation effects: a skeleton in the methodological cupboard. J Clin Epidemiol. 2014;67(8):845–849. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2014.03.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

12889_2019_7770_MOESM1_ESM.docx (410.4KB, docx)

Additional file 1: Supplementary text. Dogs as catalysts for new social interactions. Figure S1. CONSORT flow diagram modified for non-randomized trial design. Figure S2. Estimated marginal means adjusted for age and sex with standard error of the mean for questionnaire scores for dog acquisition and the pooled control group. A) Loneliness. Possible UCLA loneliness scores range from 0 to 60. B) Positive affect. Possible Positive Affect Schedule (PAS) scores range from 5 to 25. C) Negative affect. Possible Negative Affect Schedule (NAS) scores range from 5 to 25. D) Psychological distress. Possible Kessler10 (K10) scores range from 10 to 50. *Denotes a statistically significant group by time interaction in repeated measures ANCOVA (p < 0.05).

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated during the current study are not publicly available due to the requirements of the ethical approval but are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.


Articles from BMC Public Health are provided here courtesy of BMC

RESOURCES