Skip to main content
The Journal of Neuroscience logoLink to The Journal of Neuroscience
. 2019 Nov 6;39(45):8949–8968. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2858-18.2019

Posterior Hippocampal Spindle Ripples Co-occur with Neocortical Theta Bursts and Downstates-Upstates, and Phase-Lock with Parietal Spindles during NREM Sleep in Humans

Xi Jiang 1,, Jorge Gonzalez-Martinez 3, Eric Halgren 1,2,
PMCID: PMC6832672  PMID: 31530646

Abstract

Human anterior and posterior hippocampus (aHC, pHC) differ in connectivity and behavioral correlates. Here we report physiological differences in humans of both sexes. During NREM sleep, the human hippocampus generates sharpwave ripples (SWRs) similar to those which in rodents mark memory replay. We show that while pHC generates SWRs, it also generates approximately as many spindle ripples (SSR: ripples phase-locked to local spindles). In contrast, SSRs are rare in aHC. Like SWRs, SSRs often co-occur with neocortical theta bursts (TBs), downstates (DSs), sleep spindles (SSs), and upstates (USs), which coordinate cortico-hippocampal interactions and facilitate consolidation in rodents. SWRs co-occur with these waves in widespread cortical areas, especially frontocentral. These waves typically occur in the sequence TB-DS-SS-US, with SWRs usually occurring before SS-US. In contrast, SSRs occur ∼350 ms later, with a strong preference for co-occurrence with posterior-parietal SSs. pHC-SSs were strongly phase-locked with parietal-SSs, and pHC-SSRs were phase-coupled with pHC-SSs and parietal-SSs. Human SWRs (and associated replay events, if any) are separated by ∼5 s on average, whereas ripples on successive SSR peaks are separated by only ∼80 ms. These distinctive physiological properties of pHC-SSR enable an alternative mechanism for hippocampal engagement with neocortex.

SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Rodent hippocampal neurons replay waking events during sharpwave ripples (SWRs) in NREM sleep, facilitating memory transfer to a permanent cortical store. We show that human anterior hippocampus also produces SWRs, but spindle ripples predominate in posterior. Whereas SWRs typically occur as cortex emerges from inactivity, spindle ripples typically occur at peak cortical activity. Furthermore, posterior hippocampal spindle ripples are tightly coupled to posterior parietal locations activated by conscious recollection. Finally, multiple spindle ripples can recur within a second, whereas SWRs are separated by ∼5 s. The human posterior hippocampus is considered homologous to rodent dorsal hippocampus, which is thought to be specialized for consolidation of specific memory details. We speculate that these distinct physiological characteristics of posterior hippocampal spindle ripples may support a related function in humans.

Keywords: electrophysiology, hippocampus, human, memory, neocortex, sleep

Introduction

Hippocampal sharpwave ripples (HC-SWRs) are striking complexes of local field potentials (LFPs) recorded during NREM sleep and rest periods (Buzsáki, 2015). Pyramidal cell firing during HC-SWR tends to reproduce the spatiotemporal patterns established during the preceding active periods; this “replay” is hypothesized to guide cortical activity as it consolidates memories (Wilson and McNaughton, 1994; Skaggs and McNaughton, 1996; Nádasdy et al., 1999). Disrupting HC-SWR impairs memory consolidation (Girardeau et al., 2009; Maingret et al., 2016). The coordination of HC ripples with cortical consolidation is thought to be orchestrated by neocortical LFP graphoelements (NC-GEs) such as sleep spindles (SSs), downstates (DSs), and upstates (USs), and possibly theta bursts (TBs) (Gonzalez et al., 2018). Specifically, in rodents, evidence suggests that coordination between HC-SWRs and NC-SSs (Siapas and Wilson, 1998; Sirota et al., 2003; Peyrache et al., 2009) and DSs/USs (Battaglia et al., 2004; Mölle et al., 2006; Headley et al., 2017) is central to replay (Diekelmann and Born, 2010; O'Neill et al., 2010; Girardeau and Zugaro, 2011), and recent human data also point to NC-TB, NC-SS, and NC-US coordination being involved in targeted memory reactivation (Göldi et al., 2019). The NC-GEs themselves have systematic temporal relations, typically occurring in the order TBs→DSs→SSs→USs (Mak-McCully et al., 2017; Gonzalez et al., 2018).

HC-SWRs have been extensively studied in rodents. However, two macaque studies have confirmed the presence of HC-SWRs during NREM sleep with the typical biphasic negative-positive waves in radiatum, and ∼100 Hz ripples in pyramidale (Skaggs et al., 2007; Ramirez-Villegas et al., 2015). HC-SWRs have also been observed in recordings from hippocampal electrodes implanted in humans to identify epileptogenic tissue. In a series of studies, Bragin et al. (1997, 1999) described HC-SWRs but mainly focused on the use of ripples to identify epileptogenic tissue. Axmacher et al. (2008) described HC ripples that occurred mainly in waking but were phase-locked to δ. Previously, we described typical SWRs in human HC (Jiang et al., 2019).

Recently, Staresina et al. (2015) reported that HC ripples in humans during NREM are tightly phase-coupled to hippocampal SSs (HC-SSs) while presenting no clear evidence for sharp-wave ripples. In this study, we aimed to confirm the presence of ripples associated with HC-SSs (henceforth referred to as HC-SSRs), and to compare their characteristics and NC-GE associations (if any) with those of HC-SWRs, which we reported previously (Jiang et al., 2019). Furthermore, since Staresina et al. (2015) recorded HC-SSRs mainly in posterior HC (pHC), whereas previous human HC recordings were mainly in anterior HC (aHC) where most of the human hippocampal volume lies (Destrieux et al., 2013), we examined whether HC-SSRs selectively occurred in pHC, with HC-SWRs dominant in aHC. The possibility that aHC versus pHC have distinct LFP contexts is also suggested by their distinct anatomy and functional correlates (Ranganath and Ritchey, 2012; Poppenk et al., 2013; Strange et al., 2014). Furthermore, since cortical SSs preferentially occur in NREM Stage N2 compared with N3 (Mak-McCully et al., 2017), we contrasted these stages when performing our NC-GE to HC-GE association analyses. Finally, provided that HC-SSRs can be identified, we sought to determine their temporal relationship to HC-SWRs, as well as NC-GEs.

We report here that, indeed, both SWRs and SSRs exist in the HC, but with distinct anatomy, cortical relations, and timing. Whereas HC-SSRs and HC-SSs are largely confined to pHC, HC-SWRs predominate in aHC. pHC-SSs and pHC-SSRs tend to co-occur with NC-SSs, especially in parietal sites, whereas aHC-SWRs co-occur with more extensive cortical locations, especially frontal. While aHC-SWRs usually occur before SSs/USs, pHC-SSs/SSRs tend to occur during NC-SSs/USs, following NC-TBs/DSs. Hippocampal and parietal spindles become highly synchronous in ∼2 cycles, and both modulate HC ripples. Thus, pHC-SSRs interact in close synchrony with parietal NC-SSs within the context of more widespread cortical rhythms during NREM. The contrasting anatomy and physiology of pHC-SSRs and aHC-SWRs provide another possible mechanism for hippocampocortical coordination during NREM sleep.

Materials and Methods

Patient selection.

Twenty patients with long-standing drug-resistant partial seizures underwent stereoelectroencephalography (SEEG) depth electrode implantation to localize seizure onset and thus direct surgical treatment (for demographic and clinical information, see Table 1). Patients were selected from a group of 54 for minimal pathology. The 20 remaining patients included 7 males, 29.8 ± 11.9 years of age (range 16–58 years). Electrode targets and implantation durations were chosen entirely on clinical grounds (Gonzalez-Martinez et al., 2013). All patients gave fully informed consent for data usage as monitored by the local Institutional Review Board, in accordance with clinical guidelines and regulations at Cleveland Clinic.

Table 1.

List of patients, age, sex, handedness, language dominance, NC channel counts, and the lengths sleep period recordings useda

Patient no. Age (yr) Sex Handedness No. of cortical channels HC seizure-free? HC interictal-free? No. of sleep periods Mean duration of NREM (h) SD NREM (h) Total N2 duration (h) Total N3 duration (h)
1 20 M R 19 Y N 3 2.2 0.78 2.8 3.3
2 51 F R 12 Y N 4 7.5 1.87 27.3 0
3 58 F R 24 Y N 4 7.1 2.44 23.1 0.30
4 42 M L 17 Y N 4 3.1 0.33 1.8 9.0
5 18 F L 21 Y Y 1 3.7 1.9 0.9
6 20 F R 21 Y N 3 2.7 1.49 2.6 3.0
7 22 M LR 19 Y Y 3 3.8 1.17 2.4 3.8
8 30 F R 13 Y N 5 5.2 1.26 6.5 14
9 43 F R 12 Y N 4 3.1 0.45 3.7 4.4
10 16 M R 17 Y Y 5 3.8 1.11 7.5 8.8
11 32 F R 30 Y N 3 5.1 3.43 8.4 2.8
12 36 M L 25 Y Y 4 5.2 1.48 11.6 6.5
13 21 F L 14 Y N 3 6 0.47 14.7 1.1
14 21 F R 14 Y N 8 3.7 1.1 16.9 9.3
15 29 F R 17 Y N 4 2.5 1.31 5.4 2.7
16 41 F R 19 Y N 3 4.6 0.72 7.4 4.4
17 24 M R 21 Y N 3 5.1 1.88 8.9 2.9
18 31 F R 15 Y N 6 6.1 1.18 24.2 3.8
19 21 M R 15 Y Y 4 2.8 0.72 5.4 5.8
20 19 F R 21 Y Y 3 4.9 2.53 5.2 6.0
Mean 30 18 T:20 T:8 4 4.4 9.39 4.64

aReproduced with permission from Jiang et al. (2019).

Electrode localization.

After implantation, electrodes were located by aligning postimplant CT to preoperative 3D T1-weighted structural MRI with ∼1 mm3 voxel size (Dykstra et al., 2012), using 3D Slicer (RRID:SCR_005619). This allows visualization of individual contacts with respect to HC cross-sectional anatomy, which was interpreted in reference to the atlas of Duvernoy (1988). The assignment of depth contacts to aHC/pHC was made with the posterior limit of the uncal head as boundary (Poppenk et al., 2013; Ding and Van Hoesen, 2015). Recordings were obtained from 32 HC contacts: 20 anterior (11 left) and 12 posterior (7 left). In 4 patients, HC recordings were bilateral (3 anterior and 1 posterior); and in 8 patients, ipsilateral aHC/pHC were both recorded. The distance of each hippocampal contact from the anterior limit of the hippocampal head was obtained in Freesurfer (RRID:SCR_001847). The CT-visible cortical contacts were then identified as previously described (Jiang et al., 2019), to ensure that activity recorded by bipolar transcortical pairs is locally generated (Mak-McCully et al., 2015). Electrode contacts were rejected from analysis if they were involved in the early stages of the seizure discharge or had frequent interictal activity or abnormal spontaneous LFPs. From the total of 2844 contacts implanted in the 20 patients, 366 transcortical pairs (18.3 ± 4.7 per patient) were accepted for further analysis. Polarity of the pairs was adjusted, if necessary, to “pial surface minus white matter” according to MRI localization, confirmed with decreased high γ power during surface-negative DSs (see below).

Freesurfer (Dale et al., 1999; Fischl et al., 1999a, 2004) was used to reconstruct from individual MRI scans the cortical pial and inflated surfaces, as well as automatic parcellation of the cortical surface into anatomical areas (Desikan et al., 2006), after a sulcal-gyral alignment process. An average surface for all 20 patients was then generated to serve as the basis of all 3D maps. While each cortical SEEG electrode contact's location was obtained through direction correlation of CT and MRI as described earlier in this section, we obtained the cortical parcellation labels corresponding to each contact by morphing the right-anterior-superior-oriented anatomical coordinates from individual surfaces to the average surface space (Fischl et al., 1999b). In addition, the nearest vertices on the average surface to the morphed coordinates would be identified for subsequent plotting of 2D projections onto the left lateral view (see Fig. 4). For the 2D projections only, to optimize visualization (i.e., minimize multiple contact overlap) while preserving anatomical fidelity, positions of contacts with significant HC-NC GE correlation were allowed to shift within a 5 mm radius. All visualizations were created with custom scripts in MATLAB 2016b (The MathWorks).

Figure 4.

Figure 4.

GEs across NC tend to occur with consistent latencies from HC-SSRs (A) and HC-SSs (B). A, In N2, with pHC-SSRs as reference, NC-GEs tend to occur in the following order: TB starts → DS peaks ∼= SS starts → US peaks; NC-US peaks tend to co-occur with HC-SSRs. Similar results for pHC in N3 and for aHC in N2/N3 can be found in Extended Data Figure 4-1, and equivalent results for pHC in N2 with only the first SSR in each HC-SS as trigger can be found in Extended Data Figure 4-3. B, In N2, with pHC-SS as reference, NC-GEs tend to occur in the following order: TB starts → DS peaks → SS starts → US peaks. NC-SSs tend to co-occur with HC-SSs, and widespread NC-USs tend to follow. Similar results for pHC in N3 and for aHC in N2/N3 can be found in Extended Data Figure 4-2. A, B, Top rows contain histograms of peak latencies for HC-NC channel pairs with significant temporal correlations between HC-GEs and NC-GEs. Each count is the peak latency of a particular HC-NC channel pair. Bottom rows contain maps of peak latency between HC-GEs and NC-GEs for cortical channels. Circles represent where NC-GE relationships with HC-SSRs/SSs are significant. Plus signs indicate nonsignificant channels. The intensity of each circle corresponds to the strength of HC-SSR to NC-GE coupling estimated. The color of each circle representss peak latency: Red represents NC-GEs before HC-SSRs. Blue represents NC-GEs after HC-SSRs. Green represents NC-GEs co-occurring with HC-SSRs (i.e., within 50 ms of each other). Both hemispheres and medial and lateral cortical sites are superimposed in each plot. Additional statistical results on the nonhomogeneity of HC-coupled NC site distributions can be found in Extended Data Figure 4-4, Extended Data Figure 4-5, and Extended Data Figure 4-6.

Data collection and preprocessing.

Continuous recordings from SEEG depth electrodes were made with cable telemetry system (JE-120 amplifier with 128 or 256 channels, 0.016–3000 Hz bandpass, Neurofax EEG-1200, Nihon Kohden) across multiple nights (Tables 1, 2) over the course of clinical monitoring for spontaneous seizures, with 1000 Hz sampling rate. The total NREM sleep durations vary across patients; while some difference is expected given intrinsic variability of normal human sleep duration (Carskadon and Dement, 2010) and sleep deprivation in clinical environment, we also confirmed that the percentages of NREM in total sleep from 28 sleeps across 16 of our patients were comparable to (i.e., within 2 SD of) normative data (Moraes et al., 2014) in terms of N2 and N3 durations, with no significant difference in GE occurrence rates between normative and other sleep (Jiang et al., 2019). Recordings were anonymized and converted into the European Data Format. Subsequent data preprocessing was performed in MATLAB (RRID:SCR_001622); the Fieldtrip toolbox (Oostenveld et al., 2011) was used for bandpass filters, line noise removal, and visual inspection. Separation of patient NREM sleep/wake states from intracranial LFP alone was achieved by previously described methods using clustering of first principal components of δ-to-spindle and δ-to-γ power ratios across multiple LFP-derived signal vectors (Gervasoni et al., 2004; Jiang et al., 2017), with the addition that separation of N2 and N3 was empirically determined by the proportion of DSs that are also part of slow oscillations (at least 50% for N3) (Silber et al., 2007), since isolated DSs in the form of K-complexes are predominantly found in Stage 2 sleep (Cash et al., 2009).

Table 2.

Hippocampal SEEG electrode coverage and occurrence rates of hippocampal GEs

Patient no. L aHC L pHC R aHC R pHC SWR rate, N2 aHC (/min) SWR rate, N2 pHC (/min) SWR rate, N3 aHC (/min) SWR rate, N3 pHC (/min) SSR rate, N2 aHC (/min) SSR rate, N2, pHC (/min) SSR rate, N3, aHC (/min) SSR rate, N3, pHC (/min) HC-SS rate, N2, aHC (/min) HC-SS rate, N2, pHC (/min) HC-SS rate, N3, aHC (/min) HC-SS rate, N3, pHC (/min)
1 x 11 19 6.3 9.4 5.8 9
2 x 1.6 0.67 1.7
3 x x 11 12 24 25 1.4 3.4 1.4 5.6 2.8 5.4 2.6 9.3
4 x x x 10 4.3 11 4.3 1.4 3.8 1.1 1.6 2.6 5.5 2.4 3.2
5 x x 21 3.7 38 14 1.3 7 1.9 9.9 1.4 4 1.8 7.2
6 x x 9 12 15 17 4.6 18 5.2 22 6.1 10 7.6 14
7 x 11 13 0.27 0.26 0.52 0.43
8 x 19 20 1.2 1.3 1.4 1.9
9 x x 13 7.9 16 7.2 1.1 7.2 1.5 11 2.2 5.9 2.8 9.3
10 x 13 18 2.7 2.2 2.7 2.7
11 x x x 7.3 7.7 12 12 0.44 1.1 0.49 1.5 1.5 2.4 1.1 2.8
12 x 5.5 7 13.4 19 12 17
13 x x x 13 6.2 19 4.9 2.6 8 2.4 8.2 3.6 9.8 3 13
14 x 11 15 7 7.9 11 12
15 x x 3.6 7.2 7.1 8.6 7.2 7.6
16 x 9.4 11 1.6 2.2 2.2 3
17 x 23 24 0.75 0.89 1.4 1.4
18 x 7.8 10 1.2 1.1 1.9 1.9
19 x x 12 2.6 19 6.1 7.7 14 9.7 16 6.6 9.7 9.3 12
20 x 11 12 1.3 3 2.9 5.6
Mean 12 7 18 11 2.1 8.2 2.8 10 2.8 7.5 3.5 9.8
SD 1.1 1.0 1.6 1.8 0.44 1.4 0.62 1.9 0.36 0.87 0.59 1.3
Hippocampal GE selection.

To identify hippocampal spindles, we applied a previously reported spindle detector (Mak-McCully et al., 2017) to hippocampal LFP signals from NREM: a 10–16 Hz bandpass (zero-phase shift filter with transition bands equal to 30% of the cutoff frequencies) was applied to the LFP data, and the analytic amplitude of the bandpassed signal was convolved with an average Tukey window of 600 ms. A channel-wise cutoff set at mean ± 2 SD was applied to identify local maxima that would be “peaks” of putative spindles, with spindle edges defined at 50% of peak amplitude. The putative HC-SS segments were accepted for subsequent analyses if they were >400 ms, had <14 dB power in the nearby frequency bands (4–8 Hz and 18–30 Hz), and contained at least 3 unique oscillation peaks with 40–100 ms gaps.

Hippocampal SWRs were identified by starting with the top 20% of peaks in root-mean-square amplitude of 60–120 Hz LFP over a moving 20 ms window, then requiring that putative SWR events: (1) be >200 ms apart; (2) have >3 distinct ripple peaks in the HC LFP signal (low-passed at 120 Hz); and (3) be associated with an LFP matching hand-marked sharp-waves in the same subject and channel (Jiang et al., 2019). RMS peaks potentially arising from artifacts or epileptiform activity were rejected by detecting sharp LFP transients and setting a threshold that separated hand-marked HC-SWR from interictal events (Jiang et al., 2019). Before the SWR similarity evaluation, putative ripples were also examined for whether a given ripple center falls within a hippocampal spindle duration in the same signal; if positive, such ripples were then deemed SSR. Since a single ripple could qualify as both SWR and SSR, such a ripple would be denoted as SXR where appropriate.

Cortical GE selection.

Automatic cortical TB detection was performed as previously described (Gonzalez et al., 2018): a 5–9 Hz zero-phase shift bandpass filter was applied to the LFP data, and a channel-wise cutoff set at mean ± 3 SD was applied to each channel's Hilbert envelope (smoothed with 300 ms Gaussian kernel) to identify local maxima that would be “peaks” of putative TBs, and make start/stop edge cutoffs at mean ± SD. The resulting TBs were only accepted for further analyses if their durations fall between 400 and 1000 ms, with their number-of-zero-crossings-based frequency estimate falling within the bandpass frequency range as well.

Automatic cortical spindle (SS) detection was performed in the same matter as previously described for hippocampal spindles. DSs and USs were identified as follows: for each cortical LFP signal in NREM, a zero-phase filter from 0.1 to 4 Hz was applied. Consecutive zero crossings of opposite slope separated by 250–3000 ms were then selected as delineating putative GEs. For each putative GE, the amplitude peak between zero crossing was computed; only the top 10% of peaks were retained. The polarity of each signal was inverted, if necessary, to assure that DSs were negative, as confirmed with a high γ (70–190 Hz) power decrease exceeding 1 dB within ±250 ms of the negative DS peaks.

Experimental design and statistical analysis.

Statistical tests that involve multiple comparisons in this and subsequent Materials and Methods subsections all have α = 0.05 after FDR correction (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995). The FDR correction procedure was implemented as follows: for a total of N hypotheses, each with corresponding p values Pi (iN), which are sorted in ascending order to identify Pk, k being the largest i for which Pi ≤ (i/N) × α. All hypothesis with p values ≤ Pk would then be rejected. The striking positive skewness both in general and in specific categories of the NC-GE histograms (Extended Data Fig. 3-1) suggests high variability in co-occurrence rates of NC-GEs with HC-SSs and with HC-SSRs. Therefore, to account for sampling error, we created linear mixed-effect (LME) models for several analyses as described below.

We compared the occurrence rates of HC-SSRs/SSs from different NREM stages (N2 vs N3) and from different sources (aHC vs pHC) with both paired t tests and LME models to account for patient-wise variability. For LME, our base models have either HC-SSR or HC-SS rates as the response variable, with AP (aHC/pHC) and stage (N2/N3) as categorical predictor variables. The full model includes interaction between predictor variables and all possible random slope/intercept terms (GE rate ∼ 1 + AP × stage + (AP | patient ID) + (stage | patient ID) + (1 | patient ID)), with reduced models' fits being compared with the full model fit via likelihood ratio tests. The best-fit models for both HC-SSRs and HC-SSs turned out to be the following: HC-GE rate ∼ 1 + AP × stage + (AP | patient ID) + (1 | patient ID).

Similarly, we compared the occurrence rates of HC-SWRs with those of HC-SSRs in different NREM stages and from different hippocampal origins, while accounting for random effect due to patient identity. Our full LME model has HC-GE rates as the response variable, with three categorical predictor variables and all possible random slopes, plus random intercept due to patient identity. By stepwise removal of individual terms from the full model and comparing each reduced model with the one before it via likelihood ratio tests, we arrived at the best fit model of GE rate ∼ 1 + GE type × AP × stage + (GE type | patient ID).

We also evaluated whether a significant difference between SSR/SWR ratio in the first versus second half of NREM sleep could be found while accounting for variance due to patient identity. Our base LME model has SSR/SWR ratio as the response variable, and “sleep halves” as the categorical predictor variable. After comparing models with random intercept and/or slope with base model, we found the best fit to be the base model (ratio ∼ 1 + sleep halves).

For time-frequency analysis in Figure 1, spectral content of the LFP data from hippocampal channels was evaluated using EEGLAB (RRID:SCR_007292) routines that applied wavelet transforms (Delorme and Makeig, 2004). Spectral power was calculated over 1–200 Hz across each individual time period (“trial”) centered around HC-SWR ripple centers, HC-SSR ripple centers, or HC-SS onsets found in NREM by convoluting each trial's signal with complex Morlet wavelets, and averages were then taken across trials. The resulting time-frequency matrices were normalized with respect to the mean power at each chosen frequency and masked with two-tailed bootstrap significance at α = 0.05, with the pretrigger times (−2000 ms to −1500 ms) as baseline.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

HC-SSs coordinate ripples and are found predominantly in pHC. A, Overlaid average waveforms of HC-SWR (A1), of ripples not coupled to sharpwaves (A2), and of HC-SSR (A3) across hippocampal contacts from 32 different patients, with one representative patient bolded in black for clarity. The non-normalized, across-patient mean LFP peak-to-peak amplitude of the sharp wave (A, black trace) is 127.1 μV, much greater than the corresponding peak amplitudes of 8.284 and 14.79 μV in B and C, respectively. B, Example LFP trace of a ∼14 Hz HC-SS (red section) from bipolar recording in the pHC (B1), with expanded time-base plot of the spindle shown in B2; 80–90 Hz ripples occur preferentially at some peaks of the spindle waves. C, Example averaged time-frequency plot triggered on the center peak of HC-SSs from the same channel in B (C1, n = 1086) and the corresponding average LFP (C2). D, Event-related spectral power is contrasted for SWRs (D1), SSRs (D2), and SSs (D3) from the same HC site, with corresponding average LFP traces below the time-frequency plots. The high-frequency ripple burst is seen for SWRs and SSRs but not prominently so in SSs. Prolonged spindle activity is seen for SSRs and SSs but not SWRs. E, HC-SWRs preferentially occur in aHC, whereas HC-SSs/SSRs prefer pHC. Each panel marks mean rates of HC-SWR/SSR/SS occurrence across patients in N2 (E1) or N3 (E2), for aHC and pHC. F, Example state plots showing the separation of NREM sleep Stages N2 and N3 from waking/N1/REM in ∼6.5 h LFP recording, using the first principal component derived from vectors (one per cortical bipolar SEEG channel) of frequency power ratios (0.5–3 Hz over 0.5–16 Hz) (Gervasoni et al., 2004; Jiang et al., 2017). SWR rate (F1) and SSR rate (F2) are color coded with red intensity, and N2/N3 periods are marked with orange/green horizontal lines, respectively. Light blue arrow indicates the beginning of NREM sleep. Similar illustration for HC-SSs rate over time can be found in Extended Data Figure 1-1. E, Error bars indicate SEM.

Peristimulus time histograms were constructed for each HC-NC channel pair, separately for each GE (TB, SS, DS, and US), and for each sleep-stage (N2 or N3), each histogram comprising the occurrence times of a given NC-GE during the ±2 s interval surrounding midpoints of HC-SSRs and onsets of HC-SSs (for examples, see Fig. 3). Together, there were 598 unique HC-NC pairs (458 ipsilateral, 140 contralateral), and 16 histograms for each pair (one for each sleep stage, times each of the 4 GE types, for HC-SSs or SSRs), for a total of 9568 histograms. On average, each of these histograms plots the occurrence times of 1386 ± 2565 NC-GEs (3.86 skewness) with respect to 861 ± 1581 HC-SSRs (3.57 skewness) and to 1022 ± 1642 HC-SSs (3.61 skewness). Overall, a total of 13.2 million NC-GE events, 4.12 million HC-SSRs, and 4.89 million HC-SSs were plotted.

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

NC-GEs in relation to HC-SSRs (A) and HC-SSs (B), for example channel pairs. Each row of histograms is for a different type of NC-GE: TB onsets, DS peaks, SS onsets, and US peaks. Right, Example LFP trace containing the corresponding NC-GE (green dashed line boxes). Each column of plots shows example histograms with significant temporal correlations between HC-SSs/SSRs and NC-GEs from the same NC region in (from left to right) frontal, temporal, parietal, and occipital lobes. Magenta vertical lines indicate trigger (HC-SS/SSR) location for the peristimulus histograms. Orange bars represent the time ranges with both peak NC-GE occurrence rate and significant correlation. Yellow horizontal bars represent the average expected NC-GE counts from randomized control distributions. A summary of histogram contents can be found in Extended Data Figure 3-1.

The significance of peaks and troughs in each histogram was tested by comparing them with distributions derived from histograms constructed under the null hypothesis of no relationship between the NC-GEs and HC-SS/SSRs using the following randomization procedure. Null-hypothesis histograms (N = 1000) were constructed of NC-GE occurrences relative to a number of random times equal to the number of HC-SSs/SSRs (for examples, see Fig. 3). For each 200 ms time bin with 100 ms overlap comprising the 4 s trial duration, the actual counts are then compared with the distribution under the null hypothesis, followed by FDR correction. Similar peristimulus histograms and randomization tests were also constructed with aHC-SWRs as triggers and pHC-SWRs/SSRs as tallied events (see Fig. 2A).

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Temporal relations of ripples and spindles, within and between hippocampal locations. A, Peristimulus time histograms with aHC-SWRs as triggers and either pHC-SSRs (A1) or pHC-SWRs (A2) as tallied events, with HC-SXR excluded from A1. In N2, pHC-SSRs tend to follow aHC-SWRs by ∼350 ms (A, green arrows), and aHC/pHC-SWRs tend to co-occur within ±200 ms. Similar results for N3 can be found in Extended Data Figure 2-1F, G. B, Histograms of ripple counts across all HC-SS in N2 (N3 results are in Extended Data Figure 2-1H. Not shown are the HC-SSs with ≥5 ripples (n = 256 in N2, max 9). C, PAC within HC between HC-SS phase and ripple amplitude (bandpassed between 50 and 150 Hz). D, The time course of intrahippocampal ERPAC between spindle (10–16 Hz) phase and ripple (60–120 Hz) amplitude, computed over all HC-SSs in NREM across all patients, separately for aHC (D1) and pHC (D2). Events are aligned at HC-SS starts (red vertical lines). Green line segments indicate the time segments with significant coupling (p < 0.001). The same pattern is observed if pHC events are randomly subselected to have the same number of events involved as aHC (Extended Data Figure 2-1E).

The latencies of the largest significant peaks, identified in the individual histograms constructed as described above, were used to create summary histograms for each GE, sleep stage, and HC-SS/SSR origin in aHC versus pHC. These are plotted in Figure 4 and tabulated in Table 3. Each of the 16 histograms of histogram peaks (N2/N3 × aHC/pHC × 4 NC-GEs) for HC-SSRs summarizes the significant latencies from 78 ± 41 HC-NC channel pair histograms, comprising a total of 1.37 × 105 ± 1.45 × 105 HC-NC events (range 7.18 × 103 to 4.04 × 105). Each of the 16 histograms of histogram peaks for HC-SSs summarizes the significant latencies from 126 ± 63 HC-NC channel pair histograms, comprising a total of 2.46 × 105 ± 2.07 × 105 HC-NC events (range 1.24 × 104 to 6.23 × 105). To test whether, overall, a given type of cortical GE significantly precedes or follows HC-SSs/SSRs, two-tailed binomial tests with chance probability of 0.5 were performed on the number of channel pairs with peak latencies in the 2000 ms before versus the 2000 ms after the reference HC-SSs/SSRs. Since certain GEs, such as cortical θ, tend to yield peak latencies centered around zero, we also tested with Kolmogorov–Smirnov tests whether, overall, for a given NC-GE, the distribution of peak latencies significantly related to HC-SSs/SSRs differs from chance.

Table 3.

Relation of NC-GE to HC-SSR and HC-SS occurrence timesa

NC-GE type Sleep stage HC longitudinal axis NC-GE versus HC-SSR related (KS test, p value) Peak latency from HC-SSR (ms), mean Peak latency from HC-SSR (ms), DistPeak Non-zero latency from HC-SSR (binomial p value) NC-GE versus HC-SS related (KS test, p value) Peak latency from HC-SS (ms), mean Peak latency from HC-SS (ms), DistPeak Non-zero latency from HC-SS (binomial p value)
TB onsets N2 Anterior 0 −398 −741 0 0 −164 −392 0
Posterior 0.002 −386 −1074 0.011 0 −455 −698 0
N3 Anterior 0.129 123 206 1 0 −96 −338 0.006
Posterior 0.654 −131 −346 0.388 0 −512 −646 0
DS peaks N2 Anterior 0 −185 −575 0 0 55 −65 0.232
Posterior 0 38 31 0.519 0 105 −167 0.001
N3 Anterior 0.216 −95 −426 0.117 0 148 −57 1
Posterior 0 −288 −489 0.001 0 −64 −184 0
SS onsets N2 Anterior 0 −16 −163 0.213 0 80 73 0.086
Posterior 0 −211 −353 0 0 −19 −106 0.368
N3 Anterior 0.017 −32 −142 0.728 0.003 −10 12 0.377
Posterior 0 −241 −425 0 0 −45 −32 0.013
US peaks N2 Anterior 0 35 93 0.221 0 348 534 0
Posterior 0 68 62 0.643 0 165 212 0
N3 Anterior 0.013 −131 −180 0.609 0 338 568 0
Posterior 0 −49 −71 0.699 0 199 111 0

aSeparate values and statistical significance tests are shown for NREM (sleep stages N2 vs N3), aHC versus pHC, and for different NC GEs (TBs, SSs, DS, USs). Tests indicate whether there was a significant association between the times of occurrence of the HC-SSRs/HC-SSs and GEs (fourth and eighth columns); if the NC-GEs occurred significantly before the HC-SSRs/HC-SSs (negative numbers) or before after, when measuring the peak latency as the mean value across cortico-HC pairs (fifth and ninth columns), or as the peak of an extreme value distribution fitted over the ±2000 ms histogram-of-histograms in Figure 4A and Extended Data Figure 4-1 (sixth column) or in Figure 4B and Extended Data Figure 4-2 (10th column). The seventh and 11th columns show whether there is a significant difference in the number of NC-GEs occurring 2000 ms before versus 2000 ms after the HC-SSRs/HC-SSs. Extremely small (<0.0001) p values are represented by 0 for clarity. While NC-DS was significantly earlier than pHC-SSR in N3, but not in N2, it should be noted that NC-DS distribution appeared bimodal for N2 pHC-SSR, which likely distorted the mean and distribution peak estimations: the larger pretrigger peak of the NC-DS latency distribution for N2 pHC-SSR, like the NC-DS distribution peak in N3, preceded that of the NC-SS latency distribution (∼700 ms pre-SSR for NC-DS vs 353 ms for NC-SS) (Fig. 4A).

To evaluate the phase relationship between cortical and hippocampal spindle oscillations, we computed phase-locking values (PLVs) (Lachaux et al., 1999) between each cortical and hippocampal channel pairing over 3 s trials centered on all hippocampal spindle event starts in NREM (N2 + N3). Only hippocampal GEs that overlapped with the cortical spindle for at least 160 ms were included, to ensure that the cortical and hippocampal spindles overlap for at least 1 spindle cycle, thus allowing more accurate estimates of phase-locking. We also computed PLV for the same cortical-hippocampal channel pairs over the same number of trials centered on random times in NREM to create a baseline estimate; and for each nonoverlapping 50 ms time bin, a two-sample t test was performed between the actual PLV and the baseline estimate, with the resulting p values undergoing FDR correction. A given channel pair would be considered significantly phase-locking if: (1) >40 trials were used in the PLV computation, since small sample size is known to introduce bias (Aydore et al., 2013); or (2) at least 3 consecutive time bins yield post-FDR p values <0.05.

To evaluate phase-amplitude coupling (PAC) between cortical spindle phase and hippocampal ripple amplitude, we adopted a Modulation Index method as follows (Tort et al., 2010): for each HC-NC channel pair with significant SS PLV, we first computed instantaneous phase and amplitude through Hilbert transform for each trial (as previously obtained for PLV analysis), with NC-SS phase angles in 10 degree bins to create a corresponding mean amplitude distribution across trials. For statistical evaluation of observed PAC, we then computed the Modulation Index (i.e., dividing the Kullback-Leibler distance of the observed amplitude distribution from the uniform distribution by log (N), N being the number of trials) for this channel pair, and performed permutation tests by generating 200 additional Modulation Indices with random pairings of the NC-SS phase and HC ripple amplitude (so that phase from one trial is matched to amplitude from another trial). The permutation test results were then FDR-corrected.

For intrahippocampal coupling between ripple-frequency amplitude (70–100 Hz) and spindle (10–16 Hz) phase, we applied previously published methods for evaluating event-related PAC (ERPAC) and estimating subsequently ERPAC significance over time from intracranial LFP data (Voytek et al., 2013). The events were NREM hippocampal spindle starts, and each ERPAC trial covers −1000 to 1500 ms perievent. Each trial was also marked as either overlapping with NC-SS (by more than one spindle cycle) or not. Statistical evaluation of significant coupling was done using permutation tests that kept the actual analytic amplitude and phase values at each time point, but randomized the trial labels 1000 times so that the amplitude values are matched to phase values from random trials. The resulting p values for each time point then underwent FDR correction. To generate the comodulogram in Figure 2C, we concatenated the hippocampal data across spindle events and applied previously published PAC algorithm (Colgin et al., 2009) implemented within the Pactools software package (Dupré la Tour et al., 2017) under Python 3.6 (RRID:SCR_008394).

To explore the anatomical distribution of HC-NC GE co-occurrences, we tested the following: (1) whether the distribution of significantly SSR- or HC SS-coupled NC sites was different from chance for a given HC site; (2) whether the significantly different from chance distributions of NC sites from 1 would also differ between aHC/pHC site pairs; and (3) whether the NC sites showing significant HC-NC correlation with one NC-GE type tended to also show correlation with another NC-GE type.

For 1, we performed a χ2 test of homogeneity for each HC site with regard to each GE, that is, for a given patient with multiple NC sites, a 2 × N contingency table was made for each NC site, N being the number of NC sites, one row of table containing the number of NC-GE overlapping with (i.e., occurring within 1500 ms of) HC-SS/SSR for each NC, the other containing the number of NC-GE that do not. A significant χ2 would therefore indicate a significant nonrandom distribution for that particular HC site and NC-GE type.

For 2, we performed Wilcoxon signed rank tests on the data obtained in 1 to evaluate whether for a given NC-GE type (for which 1 yielded significance for both members of an HC site pair), the proportions of GE from significantly coupled NC sites that overlapped with aHC-SSRs/SSs differ from the proportions for pHC-SSR/SS. Each test required that both members of a HC site pair have a significant χ2 test result from 1.

We further explored the spatial distribution of HC-NC relationship by tallying the proportion of significant HC-NC channel pairs across different NC regions, with respect to different NC-GE types and different HC-SS/SSR sources (Table 4, Table 5). To characterize the apparent variability in Tables 4 and 5, we performed four-way ANOVAs to compare the main effects of GE type (TB, SS, DS, US), NREM stage (N2 vs N3), aHC or pHC origin of HC-SS/SSR, and NC ROIs (coverage listed in Extended Data Table 4-1) as well as the two-way and three-way interaction effects (Table 6, Table 7). To evaluate the differences in NC-HC coupling between SWRs and SSRs, we combined the SSR data with data from the equivalent analysis in our study on SWR (Jiang et al., 2019), and performed another four-way ANOVA with the following factors: SSR/SWR as the participant of NC-HC coupling, NREM stage, aHC/pHC origin of HC-SWR/SSR, and NC ROIs.

Table 4.

Numbers of HC-NC channel pairs by NC ROIs, and proportions of HC-NC channel pairs with significant NC-GE/HC-SSR relationships across NC ROIsa

NC-GE type Total no. of channel pairs
Proportion of channels showing significant NC-GE co-occurrence with HC-SSR
TB
DS
SS
US
N2
N3
N2
N3
N2
N3
N2
N3
ROI aHC pHC aHC pHC aHC pHC aHC pHC aHC pHC aHC pHC aHC pHC aHC pHC aHC pHC
Lateral occipital 15 15 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.067 0.267 0.333 0.067 0.200 0.133 0.133 0.133 0.067 0.200 0.267 0.133 0.067
Medial temporo-occipital 31 26 0.129 0.231 0.032 0.115 0.452 0.385 0.129 0.385 0.129 0.231 0.097 0.154 0.355 0.538 0.129 0.385
Lateral parietal 74 51 0.135 0.176 0.081 0.039 0.297 0.451 0.203 0.353 0.216 0.549 0.041 0.373 0.473 0.529 0.176 0.353
Medial occipito-parietal 34 27 0.118 0.259 0.059 0.037 0.176 0.630 0.206 0.370 0.324 0.630 0.059 0.556 0.412 0.556 0.176 0.333
Lateral temporal 86 54 0.128 0.241 0.035 0.019 0.314 0.444 0.081 0.222 0.186 0.333 0.070 0.148 0.430 0.574 0.151 0.167
Insula 11 6 0.273 0.333 0.182 0.000 0.636 0.500 0.273 0.167 0.091 0.500 0.000 0.167 0.364 0.667 0.364 0.333
Paracentral 29 16 0.207 0.313 0.069 0.063 0.345 0.625 0.172 0.313 0.276 0.313 0.034 0.375 0.448 0.625 0.138 0.313
Orbitofrontal 23 6 0.174 0.000 0.174 0.000 0.348 1.000 0.087 0.000 0.261 0.667 0.130 0.333 0.478 0.833 0.261 0.167
Prefrontal 49 14 0.204 0.214 0.102 0.143 0.510 0.929 0.224 0.500 0.265 0.714 0.245 0.571 0.469 0.929 0.245 0.429
Cingulate 13 6 0.077 0.333 0.077 0.167 0.385 0.500 0.385 0.500 0.308 0.667 0.308 0.500 0.462 0.667 0.231 0.667
Total no. of channel pairs across NC 365 221
Average proportions 0.144 0.210 0.081 0.065 0.373 0.580 0.183 0.301 0.219 0.474 0.112 0.324 0.409 0.618 0.200 0.321
SDs 0.072 0.116 0.056 0.056 0.125 0.212 0.094 0.148 0.077 0.196 0.092 0.174 0.081 0.171 0.071 0.158

aDetails on Freesurfer parcellations included in each ROI can be found in Extended Data Table 4-1.

Table 5.

Numbers of HC-NC channel pairs by NC ROIs, and proportions of HC-NC channel pairs with significant relationships between NC-GEs and HC-SSs

NC-GE type Total no. of channel pairs
Proportion of channels showing significant NC-GE co-occurrence with HC-SS
TB
DS
SS
US
N2
N3
N2
N3
N2
N3
N2
N3
ROI aHC pHC aHC pHC aHC pHC aHC pHC aHC pHC aHC pHC aHC pHC aHC pHC aHC pHC
Lateral occipital 15 15 0.200 0.133 0.133 0.000 0.200 0.600 0.067 0.467 0.133 0.200 0.133 0.133 0.467 0.333 0.400 0.600
Medial temporo-occipital 31 26 0.194 0.269 0.161 0.231 0.613 0.500 0.194 0.423 0.194 0.192 0.129 0.154 0.581 0.538 0.258 0.538
Lateral parietal 74 51 0.432 0.412 0.176 0.176 0.757 0.706 0.311 0.608 0.324 0.706 0.095 0.353 0.797 0.627 0.365 0.804
Medial occipito-parietal 34 27 0.324 0.370 0.147 0.185 0.529 0.630 0.265 0.556 0.324 0.667 0.147 0.556 0.765 0.630 0.353 0.667
Lateral temporal 86 54 0.442 0.463 0.163 0.148 0.733 0.722 0.349 0.444 0.221 0.444 0.070 0.093 0.791 0.722 0.302 0.389
Insula 11 6 0.364 0.333 0.182 0.000 0.727 0.833 0.273 0.500 0.364 0.333 0.000 0.000 0.545 0.500 0.182 0.167
Paracentral 29 16 0.414 0.438 0.138 0.188 0.655 0.500 0.241 0.563 0.172 0.688 0.103 0.188 0.586 0.750 0.138 0.438
Orbitofrontal 23 6 0.739 1.000 0.435 0.167 0.783 1.000 0.565 0.167 0.261 0.833 0.087 0.333 0.870 0.833 0.478 0.500
Prefrontal 49 14 0.531 0.714 0.367 0.286 0.694 0.857 0.449 0.571 0.367 0.714 0.163 0.571 0.694 0.857 0.490 0.714
Cingulate 13 6 0.385 0.500 0.385 0.333 0.692 0.667 0.692 0.833 0.385 1.000 0.308 0.500 0.923 0.667 0.538 0.833
Total no. of channel pairs across NC 365 221
Average proportions 0.402 0.463 0.229 0.171 0.638 0.701 0.341 0.513 0.274 0.578 0.124 0.288 0.702 0.646 0.350 0.565
SDs 0.150 0.230 0.111 0.101 0.162 0.151 0.174 0.159 0.086 0.258 0.075 0.194 0.144 0.151 0.126 0.193
Table 6.

Summary of four-way ANOVA comparing associations between HC-SSRs and NC-GEsa

Source F (df) p Post hoc analysis (if any) and corresponding p value (Tukey's range test)
Main effect GE 64.7 (3,27) 1.87 × 10−12 TB versus SS TB versus DS TB versus US SS versus DS SS versus US DS versus US
2.119 × 10−7 3.828 × 10−9 3.777 × 10−9 0.0048 0.0001 NS
Stage 151.6 (1,27) 1.37 × 10−12 NA
AP 100.5 (1,27) 1.34 × 10−10 NA
ROI 12.5 (9,27) 1.55 × 10−7 LT CG OF PF PC MTO MOP IS LP LO
LO versus NS 7.149 × 10−7 0.0003 2.024 × 10−7 0.0014 0.0478 0.0004 0.0005 0.0034 NA
PF versus 7.566 × 10−7 NS 0.0541 NA 0.0153 0.0004 0.0514 0.0413 0.0064 2.024 × 10−7
Two-way interaction GE × stage 6.5 (3,27) 0.0018 TB versus SS TB versus DS TB versus US SS versus DS SS versus US DS versus US
N2 9.110 ± 10−5 6.217 × 10−8 6.005 × 10−8 0.0029 0.0001 NS
N3 0.0008 9.315 × 10−5 1.733 × 10−5 NS NS NS
GE × AP 9.0 (3,27) 0.0003 aHC versus pHC TB DS SS US
NS 0.0002 4.101 × 10−7 0.0001
GE × ROI 1.4 (27,27) 0.2098 NA
Stage × AP 6.6 (1,27) 0.0160 aHC versus pHC N2 N3
1.312 × 10−8 8.236 × 10−5
Stage × ROI 2.9 (9,27) 0.0160 N2 versus N3 LT CG OF PF PC MTO MOP IS LP LO
0.0051 NS 3.528 × 10−6 0.0060 0.0117 NS NS 0.0030 NS NS
AP × ROI 2.8 (9,27) 0.0187 aHC versus pHC LT CG OF PF PC MTO MOP IS LP LO
NS 0.0063 NS 0.0004 NS NS 0.0039 NS NS 1.0000
Three-way interaction GE × stage × AP 0.1 (3,27) 0.9321 NA
GE × stage × ROI 1.1 (27,27) 0.3676 NA
GE × AP × ROI 1.3 (27,27) 0.2497 NA
Stage × AP × ROI 1.8 (9,27) 0.1165 NA

aFor each HC-NC electrode pair and sleep stage (N2 vs N3), the significance of the NC-GE occurrence times relative to the HC-SS onset was determined (see Fig. 3A; Materials and Methods). We then tallied the proportion of significant channel pairs according to their HC location (aHC, pHC), NC location (see Extended Data Table 4-1; Fig. 5A), and GE type (TB, DS, SS, US). These proportions were then entered into ANOVA. LO, Lateral occipital; LT, lateral temporal; CG, cingulate; OF, orbitofrontal; PF, prefrontal; PC, paracentral; MTO, medial temporo-occipital; MOP, medial occipito-parietal; IS, insula; LP, lateral parietal; NS, not significant (for post hoc Tukey's range tests, p > 0.1).

Table 7.

Summary of four-way ANOVA comparing associations between HC-SSs and NC-GEsa

Source F (df) p Post hoc analysis (if any) and corresponding p value (Tukey's range test)
Main effect GE 90.4 (3,27) 3.37 × 10−14 TB versus SS TB versus DS TB versus US SS versus DS SS versus US DS versus US
NS 3.843 × 10−9 3.787 × 10−9 3.840 × 10−9 3.786 × 10−9 NS
Stage 242.5 (1,27) 5.19 × 10−15 NA
AP 54.6 (1,27) 6.04 × 10−8 NA
ROI 24.9 (9,27) 7.77 × 10−11 LT CG OF PF PC MTO MOP IS LP LO
LO versus 0.0052 1.295 × 10−7 1.567 × 10−7 1.583 × 10−7 0.0208 NS 0.0002 NS 1.815 × 10−5 NA
MOP versus NS 0.0017 0.0279 0.0294 NS 0.0267 NA 0.0480 NS 0.0002
LP versus NS 0.0217 NS NS NS 0.0021 NS 0.0041 NA 1.815 × 10−5
CG versus 8.018 × 10−5 NA NS NS 1.895 × 10−5 2.822 × 10−7 0.0017 4.092 × 10−7 0.0217 1.295 × 10−7
Two-way interaction GE × stage 0.2 (3,27) 0.9128 NA
GE × AP 10.9 (3,27) 0.0001 aPH versus pHC TB DS SS US
1.0000 0.0085 4.203 × 10−7 0.1645
GE × ROI 2.0 (27,27) 0.0354 LT CG OF PF PC MTO MOP IS LP LO
TB versus SS NS NS NS NS NS NS NS NS NS NS
TB versus DS NS 0.0144 NS NS NS NS NS 0.0029 0.0333 NS
TB versus US NS 0.0071 NS NS NS NS 0.0054 NS 0.0050 0.0089
SS versus DS 0.0040 NS NS NS NS NS NS 0.0005 NS NS
SS versus US 0.0060 NS 0.0392 NS NS 0.0197 NS NS NS 0.0293
DS versus US NS NS NS NS NS NS NS NS NS NS
Stage × AP 1.1 (1,27) 0.3008 NA
Stage × ROI 7.1 (9,27) 3.28 × 10−5 N2 versus N3 LT CG OF PF PC MTO MOP IS LP LO
2.696 × 10−5 NS 9.627 × 10−7 0.0047 0.0003 NS 0.0824 1.304 × 10−5 0.0032 NS
AP × ROI 1.8 (9,27) 0.1213 NA
Three-way interaction GE × stage × AP 11.1 (3,27) 0.0001 N2 aHC N2 pHC N3 aHC N3 pHC
NC-SS, N2 pHC 7.634 × 10−6 NA 5.141 × 10−7 1.653 × 10−5
NC-DS, N3 aHC 1.038 × 10−5 7.725 × 10−7 NA 0.0196
NC-US, N3 aHC 9.516 × 10−7 1.190 × 10−5 NA 0.0016
GE × stage × ROI 1.0 (27,27) 0.4610 NA
GE × AP × ROI 1.7 (27,27) 0.0907 NA
Stage × AP × ROI 3.0 (9,27) 0.0139 Mostly NS (e.g., for orbitofrontal, p = 1.138 × 10−5 for N2 pHC vs N3 aHC, and p = 4.444 × 10−6 for N3 pHC vs N3 aHC)

aFor each HC-NC electrode pair and sleep stage (N2 vs N3), the significance of the NC-GE occurrence times relative to the HC-SS onset was determined (see Fig. 3B; Materials and Methods). We then tallied the proportion of significant channel pairs according to their HC location (aHC, pHC), NC location (see Extended Data Table 4-1; Fig. 5A), and GE type (TB, DS, SS, US). These proportions were then entered into ANOVA. LO, Lateral occipital; LT, lateral temporal; CG, cingulate; OF, orbitofrontal; PF, prefrontal; PC, paracentral; MTO, medial temporo-occipital; MOP, medial occipito-parietal; IS, insula; LP, lateral parietal; NS, not significant (for post hoc Tukey's range tests, p > 0.1).

Similarly, given the lateralization of human hippocampal function, we were interested in examining whether ipsilateral HC-NC channel pairs would show different relationships from contralateral pairs across GE types, NREM stages, or NC ROIs. Due to a sparse representation of contralateral channel pairs for individual ROIs, we calculated proportions for larger regions by combining both HC and NC sites: aHC and pHC channels were combined into a single HC category, and the 10 NC ROIs used in previous analysis were collapsed into two: “frontocentral” and “nonfrontal” (Extended Data Table 4-1). We then computed for each ROI the proportion of NC channels significantly coupled to HC-SSs/SSRs (Table 8), and performed four-way ANOVA as previously described, with the ipsilateral/contralateral factor replacing the aHC/pHC factor. All post hoc analyses for both ANOVAs were performed with Tukey's range test. For each ANOVA, we checked the normality assumption by conducting the Lilliefors test on the residuals.

Table 8.

Proportions of HC-NC channel pairs with significant GE-SSR or GE-HC SS relationships across NC, separated by ROI, NREM stage, NC-GE type, and ipsilateral/contralateral HC-NC pairings

HC event ROI/SWR source Proportion of channels with significant NC-GE and HC-SS/SSR co-occurrence
TB
DS
SS
US
N2
N3
N2
N3
N2
N3
N2
N3
Ipsilateral Contralateral Ipsilateral Contralateral Ipsilateral Contralateral Ipsilateral Contralateral Ipsilateral Contralateral Ipsilateral Contralateral Ipsilateral Contralateral Ipsilateral Contralateral
SSR Frontocentral 0.206 0.133 0.099 0.133 0.518 0.467 0.234 0.333 0.362 0.200 0.255 0.200 0.553 0.467 0.270 0.200
Nonfrontal 0.169 0.138 0.052 0.049 0.436 0.228 0.225 0.179 0.300 0.260 0.169 0.098 0.495 0.382 0.228 0.171
SS Frontocentral 0.560 0.467 0.305 0.267 0.716 0.600 0.475 0.467 0.433 0.333 0.206 0.267 0.738 0.800 0.461 0.400
Nonfrontal 0.381 0.358 0.179 0.114 0.704 0.569 0.404 0.317 0.375 0.317 0.169 0.130 0.710 0.618 0.453 0.374
Code accessibility.

All custom scripts would be available upon request by contacting the corresponding author and would be delivered through the UCSD RDL-share service.

Results

In this paper, we quantified HC-SSs and HC-SSRs in humans, their relationship to HC-SWRs and NC-GEs, and their variation within different HC regions and sleep stages. We confirmed that ripples phase-modulated by local SSs (i.e., HC-SSRs) do occur in the human hippocampus, but they are preferentially located in the pHC sites. Like HC-SWRs (Jiang et al., 2019), HC-SS/SSRs were strongly related to the NC-GEs in a spatiotemporally diverse manner, with a particularly strong relationship in Stage N2 between pHC-SSs/SSRs and NC-TBs, DSs, SSs, and USs. The relative latencies are consistent with the order we have established in earlier studies of NC and thalamic GEs (Mak-McCully et al., 2017; Gonzalez et al., 2018) (i.e., TBs→ DSs→SSs/USs, and HC-SSs tends to coincide with NC-SSs). Preferential co-occurrence, strong coherence, and phase-locking were noted between pHC-SSs and NC-SSs in the posterior parietal and retrosplenial cortices.

Characterization of human HC-SSs and SSRs in NREM

We identified human HC-SWRs, SSRs, and SSs from intracranial recordings and aligned them with simultaneously recorded cortical events in NREM sleep, to determine whether their occurrences are related and have a consistent order, and whether their relations change between NREM Stages N2 and N3, or between aHC and pHC sites. Morphologically normal ripples were isolated from >24 h continuous recordings in 20 SEEG patients with intractable focal epilepsy, with anterior (in 17 patients) and/or pHC contacts (in 11 patients), and related to sleep NC-GEs in 12–30 bipolar recording channels per patient. Some ripples were found to co-occur with classical sharpwaves (Fig. 1A1; with across-patient mean LFP peak-to-peak amplitude of the sharp wave at 127.1 μV), and others did not (Fig. 1A2,A3; with peak amplitudes of 8.284 and 14.79 μV, respectively). The latter could sometimes be observed as HC-SSRs nestled within HC-SSs (Fig. 1B,C), as previously reported (Staresina et al., 2015). Time-frequency analysis triggering on HC-SWR ripple centers, SSR ripple centers (with all ripple centers found in the same HC-SSs included as separate trials), and HC-SS onsets (Fig. 1D) showed that, while HC-SWRs tended to be surrounded by a broad-band power decrease, HC-SSRs and HC-SSs did not. Similar to NC-SSs and HC-SWRs, HC-SSRs and HC-SSs in humans were mostly absent in waking (Fig. 1F; Extended Data Fig. 1-1E). Notably, HC-SSRs and HC-SSs did not appear to concentrate in NREM to the same extent as SWRs, in agreement with a recent report on human HC-SSs in REM (Lestra et al., 2018). However, whether these putative HC-SS hits in wake/REM are truly HC-SSs would be difficult to determine based on our NREM-based detection criteria, and further exploration of this phenomenon would be outside the scope of our current study.

We also found that, while HC sites produce both HC-SWRs and HC-SSs/HC-SSRs (Table 2), the occurrence rates (no./min) of HC-SSRs were remarkably lower than those of HC-SWRs in aHC (p = 1.602 × 10−7 in N2, p = 7.682 × 10−8 in N3; two-tailed paired t tests); no similar difference was observed for pHC, however (p = 0.4911 in N2; p = 0.8038 in N3; two-tailed paired t tests) (Fig. 1E; Extended Data Fig. 1-1A–D. To verify that the relative imbalance of aHC SSRs and SWRs could not be explained by interpatient variance alone, we built LME models with HC-GE occurrence rates as response variable, and GE type (SWRs or SSRs), aHC/pHC, and NREM stage (N2/N3) as categorical predictor variables, with patient identity as random effect. We found that, indeed, there is a significant interaction effect of GE type × aHC/pHC on the occurrence rate of HC-GEs (p = 7.246 × 10−7).

HC-SSs/SSRs tended to occur more often in pHC than in aHC (p = 0.0018/0.0002 in N2, p = 0.0027/0.0004 in N3, two-tailed two-sample t tests) (Fig. 1E; Extended Data Fig. 1-1A–D). Transitional forms (“SXR”) that were detected as both SWRs and SSRs constituted a minority of SSRs (21.4% in N2 aHC, 26.9% in N3 aHC, 9.7% in N2 pHC, 13.0% in N3 pHC), and the proportion of SXR among SSRs was greater in aHC than in pHC (p = 0.0045 for N2, p = 0.0060 for N3, two-tailed two-sample t tests), in line with the SWR rate reduction in pHC. Notably, while cortical spindles tend to occur more often in N2, we observed an increase of pHC-SSs/SSRs in N3 (p = 0.0061 and p = 0.0088), but not for aHC-SSs/SSRs (p = 0.0857 and p = 0.0752; two-tailed paired t tests). To verify that differences in HC-SS/SSR occurrence rates across hippocampal origin and/or NREM stages could not be explained by interpatient variance alone, we built LME models with HC-GE occurrence rates as response variable, with aHC/pHC and NREM stage as categorical predictor variables and patient identity as random effect. As expected, hippocampal longitudinal origin showed a significant effect on HC-GE rate (p = 0.0003 and p = 1.925 × 10−5 for SSRs and HC-SSs), whereas the effect of NREM stage on HC-GE rate was nonsignificant (p = 0.4785 and p = 0.2522 for SSRs and HC-SSs). While there was no significant interaction effect between aHC/pHC and NREM stage for HC-SSRs (p = 0.1349), HC-SSs did show a significant interaction (p = 0.0324).

NREM Stage N3, associated with DSs and USs (which are in turn associated with SWRs), tends to be relatively more concentrated in earlier sleep cycles, whereas Stage N2, associated with SSs and thus SSRs, tends to occur in later cycles. Consequently, we hypothesized that the HC-SSR to HC-SWR ratio would increase over the course of the night. We calculated in each NREM sleep segment the ratio of SSR counts to SWR counts, and compared these ratios in the first and second half of sleep. We found that, while both SWR and SSR occurrence rates decreased in the second half, the extent of the decrease was much more pronounced for HC-SWRs (Extended Data Fig. 1-1F). Indeed, the SSR/SWR ratio increased significantly from the first half of NREM sleep to the second half (0.67 vs 0.88, p = 0.0363, one-tailed paired t test). However, when we accounted for patient-wise variability by introducing an LME model, we found the effect of early/late NREM on SSR/SWR ratio to be nonsignificant (p = 0.5667).

Given the presence of both HC-SWRs and HC-SSRs in the same hippocampus (Table 2), and the preference of HC-SSRs to occur in pHC compared with aHC (Fig. 1E), we investigated whether a consistent relationship exists between SWRs and SSRs in aHC and pHC. In 2 of 8 subjects with both aHC and pHC contacts in the same HC, we found significant elevations in pHC-SSR counts at 100–500 ms following aHC-SWR triggers (p < 7.811 × 10−12 for all panels in Fig. 2A1 [N2]; Extended Data Fig. 2-1F [N3]; one-sample t test, with SXR excluded), in contrast to the usual ±200 ms co-occurrence of pHC-SWRs with aHC-SWRs (Fig. 2A2; Extended Data Fig. 2-1G).

Since HC-SSs could co-occur with multiple ripples in different spindle cycles (Fig. 1B), we tallied the numbers of HC ripples within the duration of HC-SSs. Up to 9 ripples in N2 (or 10 in N3) could occur within one HC-SS (Fig. 2B; Extended Data Fig. 2-1H–J. Given the potential locality of HC ripples (Patel et al., 2013) and the SS phase modulation of high γ amplitude (Fig. 2C), these ripple counts might be underestimates if each spindle cycle could contain a ripple. More than half of the HC-SSs overlapped with at least one ripple from the same HC site. Among the population of HC-SSs with at least one co-occurring ripple, >60% had two or more ripples within the SS duration.

Time-frequency plots locked to HC spindle peaks yielded ripple power increases that appeared modulated by spindle frequency phase (Fig. 1D); this was further supported by intrahippocampal PAC (Fig. 2C). The time course of this coupling was examined by computing ERPAC between spindle (10–16 Hz) phase and ripple (60–120 Hz) amplitude, using previously published methods (Voytek et al., 2013). All HC-SSs in NREM across all patients were included in the computation, separated by longitudinal origin (aHC/pHC), with aligned events being HC-SS starts. As we expected from the PAC measure (Fig. 2C), we found significant event-related modulation of hippocampal ripple amplitude by HC-SS phase (Fig. 2D), with the effect being much more prominent in pHC (two-tailed paired t test over mean ERPAC values from 0 to 1000 ms after trigger, p < 2.20 × 10−16, Cohen's d = 1.74).

Co-occurrence of HC-SSs and HC-SSRs in anterior and posterior, or left and right HC

Although initial descriptions of SWRs in rodents emphasized their co-occurrence throughout the entire extent of both hippocampi (Buzsáki et al., 1992), more detailed studies showed that isolated SWRs are more common (Patel et al., 2013). The dominant and nondominant hippocampal formations of humans are thought to be specialized for verbal and visuospatial memory, respectively (Glosser et al., 1998), which might predict distinct memory traces and thus low bilateral co-occurrence of HC-SSs/SSRs. Analogously, the extent to which HC-SSRs and/or HC-SSs co-occur within the same hippocampus and/or bilaterally ought to be investigated.

We found that, for patients (n = 8) with both aHC and pHC contacts in the same HC, the overlap (i.e., co-occurrence within 50 ms) between aHC-SSRs and pHC-SSRs (with N2 and N3 combined) was strikingly low, at 1.7 ± 2.1%, though still greater than the mean overlap percentage derived from chance (p = 0.0173, one-tailed paired t test). Similarly, while the overlap between aHC-SSs and pHC-SSs was significantly greater than chance (p = 0.0010, one-tailed paired t test), the mean percentage remains low (3.7 ± 3.2%). Similarly, low overlap percentages were found when using data from N2 or N3 alone (Extended Data Fig. 2-1A).

We also evaluated HC-NC GE overlap in patients (n = 4) with bilateral (left and right) HC contacts, placed in either both anterior or both pHC. In these patients, co-occurrence of SSRs between HC in different hemispheres (with N2 and N3 combined) was minuscule (0.41 ± 0.47%). This was much less than that usually observed between two contacts within the same HC, and not significantly greater than chance-derived overlap percentages (p = 0.1506, one-tailed paired t test). Co-occurrence of HC-SSs was also rare but more frequent than that of HC-SSRs (1.0 ± 0.66%). This small percentage nonetheless was significantly greater than chance (p = 0.0062, one-tailed paired t test). Similar overlap percentages were found when using data from N2 or N3 alone (Extended Data Fig. 2-1B).

Thus, in humans, a small but significant proportion of HC-SSRs and HC-SSs co-occur in both aHC and pHC, and only a minuscule (but still greater than chance for HC-SSs) proportion of HC-SSs and HC-SSRs co-occur in both left and right hippocampi. Furthermore, by varying the co-occurrence criterion range between 25 and 5000 ms, we observed greater-than-chance co-occurrence rates for aHC- and pHC-SSRs/SSs (as well as for left and right HC SSRs/SSs) initially (Extended Data Fig. 2-1A–D), but the actual co-occurrence rate tapered off exponentially (i.e., showed linear decay in log-log plots) toward chance level beyond 1000 ms. Remarkably, the co-occurrence likelihood of aHC + pHC and bilateral HC-SSs/SSRs remains in an above-chance plateau until the time window in which co-occurrence is measured expands beyond 500–1000 ms for bilateral HC or for aHC/pHC. Since the typical significant latency (as estimated in Fig. 3) between NC-GEs and HC-SSs/SSRs is comparable, HC-GEs from different HC regions could interact with disparate cortical regions at different times.

Associations of cortical GEs with HC-SSRs and HC-SSs

To examine the relationship between HC-SSs/SSRs and cortical activity, different cortical GEs (NC-GEs, including TBs, DSs, USs, and SSs) were identified in simultaneous recordings from all lobes. These recording channels were differential derivations between adjacent contacts spanning the cortical ribbon, and were thus assured of being locally generated. Detection algorithms have all been extensively validated in our previous studies (Mak-McCully et al., 2015, 2017; Gonzalez et al., 2018). To display the regularities in our data, we constructed histograms of the occurrence times of each NC-GE (TB onset, SS onset, DS peak, US peak) in relation to HC event triggers [HC-SWR ripple center, HC-SSR ripple center (multiple ripples within a single HC-SS counted as separate trials), and HC-SS onset] between each pair of HC (n = 32) and NC (n = 366) channels (n = 20 patients). Separate analyses were performed for Stages N2 versus N3, for aHC versus pHC, and for HC-NC pairs in the same versus opposite hemispheres. Detailed breakdown of histogram contents by GE types and HC-SS/SSR sources (N2 vs N3, aHC vs pHC, ipsilateral vs contralateral) can be found in Extended Data Fig. 3-1. For both HC-SSs and HC-SSR, across all NC-GE types, in both N2 and N3, the HC-NC associations appeared to be more frequently involving pHC than aHC, as reflected in the elevation of NC-GE counts (Extended Data Fig. 3-1; this agrees with our earlier observation that HC-SSs and HC-SSRs predominantly occurred in pHC, and is in contrast with the distribution observed for SWRs Fig. 1E).

Of the 598 total HC-NC channel pairs, 555 (93%) of all pairs (429, 94% of the ipsilateral pairs and 126, 90% of the contralateral pairs) had at least one histogram showing a significant relationship between the HC-SSs/SSRs and a NC-GE at p < 0.0.05 after FDR correction (randomization test, see Experimental design and statistical analysis), suggesting widespread NC-GE involvement with HC-SSs/SSRs. The percentages of significant ipsilateral and contralateral pairs were not significantly different (Fisher's exact test, p = 0.1888). Of the entire set of 4784 histograms involving HC-SSRs and NC-GEs, 1247 (26%) showed a significant association; for those involving HC-SSs and NC-GEs, 2033 (43%) showed a significant association, which is significantly greater than the proportion of histograms coupled to HC-SSRs (p = 1.385 × 10−64, Fisher's exact test). The significant histograms (for examples, see Fig. 3) include all types of NC-GEs from channels across all cortical lobes. For HC-SSRs, the peak of the histogram represented a mean increase of 157%, or median increase of 86.7% above baseline (measured as the mean height of nonsignificant bins), with SD of 250%, and positive skew value of 6.40. For HC-SSs, the peak of the histogram represented a mean increase of 147%, or median increase of 91.3% above baseline, with SD of 220%, and positive skew value of 9.19.

We summarized these data further by constructing histograms of peak latencies from the histograms described above. Specifically, we determined the latency of peak occurrence rate relative to HC-SSR or HC-SS time for each significant HC-NC-GE histogram, and plotted these as a histogram for each NC-GE type, separately for N2 versus N3 and aHC versus pHC (Fig. 4; Extended Data Fig. 4-1; Extended Data Fig. 4-2. As noted above, N2 and pHC sites had more HC-SSRs and HC-SSs. Results were also summarized by plotting each cortical channel after morphing to a standardized brain (Fig. 4; Extended Data Fig. 4-1; Extended Data Fig. 4-2. In the summary histograms, significant temporal relationships were found between HC-SSRs and each of the NC-GE types. As shown in Table 3, we tested for the presence of a significantly nonrandom distribution of NC-GE peak times with respect to HC-SSRs (column 4), and found significant results for both N2 and N3, and both aHC and pHC, for NC-SSs and NC-USs. NC-TBs and HC-SSRs were significantly related in N2, but not in N3, possibly due to NC-TBs being more abundant and more related to NC-DSs in N2 (Gonzalez et al., 2018). NC-DSs and HC-SSRs were significantly related in N2 for both aHC and pHC, but only for pHC in N3, which may reflect the preference of HC-SSRs to occur in pHC. Similarly, in the summary histograms, significant temporal relationships were found between HC-SS and each of the NC-GE types. We also tested for the presence of a significantly nonrandom distribution of NC-GE peak times with respect to HC-SSs (Table 3, column 8); we found significant results for both N2 and N3, and both aHC and pHC, for NC-TBs, DSs, SSs, and USs.

We also examined whether the peak latency in the summary histograms between HC-SSs/SSRs and NC-GEs differed significantly from zero, and computed both the mean of the included HC-NC channel pairs (Table 3, columns 5 and 9) and the center of a distribution fitted over the peak latencies (Table 3, columns 6 and 10). Based on the fitted distribution centers in N2, NC-TB onsets significantly preceded aHC-SSRs by 741 ms, NC-DS peaks significantly preceded by 575 ms, NC-SS onsets preceded by 163 ms, and NC-US peaks followed by 93 ms. Generally, similar results could be found for pHC-SSRs in N2/N3, based on both mean peak latencies and fitted distribution peak centers (Table 3): at least among the NC-GEs that preceded HC-SSRs, there appeared to be a reliable sequence of TBs→DSs→SSs for both aHC and pHC in N2. Similarly, for pHC in N3, the TB→DS→SS sequence persisted. Finally, while NC-USs were significantly coupled to HC-SSRs across N2 and N3 for both aHC and pHC, there were no significant biases to precede or follow HC-SSRs, indicating that NC-USs tended to co-occur with HC-SSRs and were likely to be the last in the sequence of NC-GE types we observed (i.e., TBs→DSs→SSs→USs). This sequence appears also in relation to HC-SSs (Table 3, columns 8–11). These observations are consistent with the sequence previously established in earlier studies of NC and thalamic GEs (Mak-McCully et al., 2017; Gonzalez et al., 2018).

HC-NC GE co-occurrence patterns are anatomically selective

Having established that SSRs in particular HC sites are significantly associated with GE in NC sites, we asked whether co-occurring HC-SSRs/NC-GEs were randomly distributed across NC locations for a given HC site (Extended Data Fig. 4-4). Across different GEs, for ∼70% of the HC sites, their SSRs co-occurred with GEs in a nonrandom distribution of NC sites in N2 (χ2 test of homogeneity). Similar results were obtained for HC-SSs in N2 but were less striking in N3 (Extended Data Fig. 4-4; Extended Data Fig. 4-5).

Given that HC-NC co-occurrences are anatomically nonrandom (i.e., selective), we further determined whether they differed between simultaneously recorded aHC and pHC sites by performing Wilcoxon signed rank tests to evaluate whether for a given GE type, the proportions of GEs from significantly coupled NC sites that overlapped with aHC-SSRs/SSs differ from the proportions for pHC-SSRs/SSs. Each test required that both members of a HC site pair have a significant χ2 test result from 1. In total, 8 aHC/pHC pairs were examined. We found that, for all the qualified aHC/pHC pairs, the distributions of significantly associated NC channels for aHC and for pHC differed significantly (Extended Data Fig. 4-6). Thus, different anatomical distributions of NC are significantly coupled with simultaneously recorded aHC and pHC.

Differences in the NC-GE to HC-SSR relationship across NC regions, NREM stages, HC sites, and GEs

While the summaries in Figure 4 and Table 3 gave us an overview of NC-GE and HC-SS/SSR relations, we noted that there appeared to be regional differences across NC in the strength of temporal correlations. We also considered the possibility that “hot spots” may exist in NC and contribute to the widespread positive skewness in the distributions of GE counts across individual HC-NC GE histograms (Extended Data Fig. 3-1). We therefore explored the spatial distribution of HC-NC relationship further by tallying the proportion of significant HC-NC channel pairs across different NC regions, with respect to different NC-GE types and different HC-SSR/SS sources (Table 4, Table 5). We then performed two four-way ANOVAs (one for HC-SSRs, another for HC-SSs) to compare the main effects of NC-GE type (TB, SS, DS, US), NREM stage (N2 vs N3), aHC or pHC origin of HC-SS/SSR, and NC ROIs (Fig. 5A; coverage listed in Extended Data Table 4-1), as well as the two-way and three-way interaction effects. All post hoc analyses were performed with Tukey's range test. We checked the normality assumption by conducting the Lilliefors test on the residuals, and the null hypothesis of normality could not be rejected (p = 0.2058 for HC-SSRs, p = 0.0935 for HC-SSs). Both the ANOVA and post hoc analyses' results (F statistics and p values) are summarized in Table 6 (for HC-SSRs) and Table 7 (for HC-SSs).

Figure 5.

Figure 5.

Strength of HC-NC association shows both correlativity and variability across GE types, NREM stages, HC-SS/SSR sources, and NC regions. A, Map of NC ROIs used for analysis (Extended Data Table 4-1). B, C, Stacked bar graph of mean significant (evaluated as in Fig. 3) HC-NC channel pair proportions with regard to all channels found in each ROI. B, Data were obtained by averaging across GE types for each SSR origin (different NREM stages and HC contact positions). C, Similarly, data were obtained by averaging across NC-GE types for each HC-SS origin. D, E, Global (i.e., across all NC ROIs) proportions of significant HC-NC channel pairs for HC-SSRs (D) and for HC-SSs (E). B–E share the same color legend. ant, aHC; post, pHC.

For the ANOVA performed over proportions of NC channels showing significant NC-GE to HC-SSR temporal correlations, all four main effects were significant. The main effect for GE was due to the mean proportions of SSR-responsive HC-NC channel pairs differing between all GE type pairs, except NC-DSs and NC-USs. The mean proportions were 0.1252 for TBs, 0.2822 for SSs, 0.3591 for DSs, and 0.3874 for USs (Fig. 5D). The NREM stage main effect was due to more HC-NC channel pairs being significantly related to HC-SSR in N2 (mean proportion of 0.3784) than in N3 (0.1985). The aHC/pHC main effect was due to more of NC being related to pHC-SSRs (0.3617) than to aHC-SSRs (0.2152), consistent with our previous observation of higher HC-SSR density in pHC than in aHC. The ROI main effect was due to a stronger HC-NC association by prefrontal locations, followed by cingulate, orbitofrontal, and medial occipitoparietal cortices; in contrast, lateral occipital and lateral temporal regions had the weakest associations, particularly in N3 (Fig. 5B; Table 6).

Except for GE × ROI, all two-way interaction effects from the SSR ANOVA were significant. None of the three-way interaction effects were significant. The lack of significance for GE × ROI interaction suggests that, overall, the spatial pattern of HC-NC association does not differ across NC-GE types. The GE × NREM stage interaction was due to NC-SS interacting less with HC-SSRs than do USs and DSs, in N2 but not in N3. The GE × aHC/pHC interaction was due to the absence of a pHC advantage for TBs in N3. For SSs, in particular, pHC proportions were approximately twice as large as aHC proportions in N2, and approximately 3 times as large in N3 (Fig. 5D). The NREM stage × aHC/pHC interaction was due to a relatively greater difference in HC-NC coupling by pHC versus aHC during in N2 than N3 (0.1841 vs 0.1090) (Fig. 5D). The NREM stage × NC ROI interaction was due to some locations (orbitofrontal, lateral temporal, prefrontal, paracentral, and insula) showing stronger coupling during N2 than N3, whereas others (generally more posterior) were approximately equal (Fig. 5B). Finally, the aHC/pHC × NC ROI interaction was due to a significant aHC/pHC preference limited to cingulate, prefrontal, and medial occipitoparietal regions (Fig. 5B), although both aHC-SSRs and pHC-SSRs appeared to engage the whole cortex (Fig. 3A; Fig. 4A; Extended Data Fig. 4-1).

In a companion paper (Jiang et al., 2019), we performed a four-way ANOVA equivalent to the one above, but for HC-SWRs. To compare the previous results with SWRs to the current results with SSRs, we included both datasets in a five-way ANOVA with factors of SWRs/SSRs, NC-GE type (TB, DS, SS, US), NREM stage (N2/N3), aHC/pHC, and ROI. The normality assumption was checked (p = 0.1478, Lilliefors test on residuals). We found that HC-SWR/NC-GE coupling was more likely than HC-SSR/NC-GE coupling (SWR/SSR main effect, F(1,27) = 117.4, p = 2.471 × 10−11). Coupling of SWRs/SSRs to NC-GEs significantly interacted with the type of NC-GE (F(3,27) = 51.57, p = 2.621 × 10−11), the NREM stage (F(1,27) = 7.27, p = 0.0119), and whether the SWRs/SSRs were generated in aHC versus pHC (F(1,27) = 4.57, p = 0.0417). However, the SWR/SSR × NC ROI interaction showed a trend (F(9,27) = 2.11, p = 0.0650). Notably, post hoc tests revealed that the SWR/SSR × NC-GE interaction effect was due to HC-SSRs preferentially coupling with NC-SSs (p = 0.0013), whereas other NC-GE types are more strongly associated with HC-SWRs (p = 1.591 × 10−4 for TBs, p = 6.027 × 10−8 for DSs, p = 6.616 × 10−8 for USs). Overall, these results are consistent with the special relationship observed between pHC-SSRs and NC-SSs.

Differences in the NC-GE to HC-SS relationship across NC regions, NREM stages, HC sites, and NC-GE types

We repeated the analysis described above for HC-SSRs, but for HC-SSs, and summarized the results in Table 7. For the ANOVA performed over proportions of NC channels with significant NC-GE to HC-SS temporal correlation, all four main effects were significant. The GE main effect was due to a larger proportion of responsive HC-NC channel pairs for DSs (0.5484) and USs (0.5658) than for TBs (0.3165) and SSs (0.3160) (Fig. 5E). The NREM stage main effect was due to more HC-NC coupling in N2 (0.5507) than N3 (0.3226). The aHC/pHC main effect was due to more HC-NC coupling by pHC (0.4907) than aHC (0.3826), consistent with our previous observation of higher HC-SS density in pHC than in aHC (Fig. 1E; Extended Data Fig. 1-1A,B). All the above effects were comparable to their counterparts in the previous ANOVA regarding HC-SSRs, with the exception that NC-SSs were significantly more related to HC-SSRs than NC-TBs were, but no such significant difference was observed for HC-SSs overall. Thus, HC-SSRs could involve a subset of HC-SSs with preferential coupling to NC-SSs. The ROI main effect was due to greater coupling by NC regions, such as the cingulate, versus low levels by other regions (e.g., lateral occipital) (Fig. 5C). These results and post hoc analysis agree with their counterparts from the HC-SSR ANOVA.

The GE × aHC/pHC interaction was due to aHC/pHC differences being confined to DSs and SSs. For NC-SSs, in particular, pHC proportions were approximately twice as large as aHC proportions in N2, and more than twice as large in N3 (Fig. 5E). The GE × ROI interaction was due to a greater coupling of certain NC-GE types in particular ROIs. The insula, for instance, had higher coupling to DSs (0.5833) and USs (0.3485) than TBs (0.2197) and SSs (0.1742). The NREM stage × NC ROI interaction was a larger coupling in N2 than N3, especially in orbitofrontal and insula ROIs, compared with the small N2/N3 difference in, for example, cingulate and lateral occipital (Fig. 5C). Notably, for both HC-SSR and HC-SS ANOVAs, all ROIs involving occipital cortices (medial temporo-occipital, medial occipitoparietal, and lateral occipital) showed consistent associations with HC across N2 and N3, whereas orbitofrontal, prefrontal, paracentral, lateral temporal, and insula ROIs showed preferential associations in N2.

The GE × NREM stage × aHC/pHC interaction is presented in Figure 5E, where, for example, N2 pHC dominated for NC-SS relations. The NREM stage × aHC/pHC × NC ROI interaction is illustrated in Figure 5C, where N2 pHC proportion was dominant in some ROIs (e.g., orbitofrontal), whereas others appeared more balanced (e.g., cingulate, lateral occipital). Overall, with minor exceptions, the NC-GE coupling pattern with HC-SS is similar to that of HC-SSRs.

Relationships between NC-GEs and HC-SS/SSRs in ipsilateral versus contralateral HC-NC channel pairs

Given the well-known lateralization of human hippocampal function (Glosser et al., 1998), we were interested in examining whether ipsilateral HC-NC channel pairs would show different relationships from contralateral pairs across NC-GE types, NREM stages, or NC ROIs. Due to a sparse representation of contralateral channel pairs for individual ROIs, we combined the 10 NC ROIs used in previous analysis into two: “frontocentral,” which included the previous cingulate, orbitofrontal, prefrontal, and paracentral ROIs; and “nonfrontal,” which included the previous lateral temporal, medial temporo-occipital, medial occipitoparietal, insula, lateral occipital, and lateral parietal ROIs. We then computed for each ROI the proportion of NC channels significantly coupled to HC-SSRs/HC-SSs (Table 8) and tallied the numbers of NC channels with or without significant HC-NC associations for each NC-GE type across N2 and N3 (Table 9).

Table 9.

Numbers and proportions of ipsilateral and contralateral NC channels showing significant HC-NC GE couplinga

HC event Stage (aHC and pHC combined) N2
N3
Ipsilateral
Contralateral
Ipsilateral
Contralateral
NC-GE type No. of significant channels No. of nonsignificant channels Proportion No. of significant channels No. of nonsignificant channels Proportion No. of significant channels No. of nonsignificant channels Proportion No. of significant channels No. of nonsignificant channels Proportion
SSR TB 81 367 0.1808 19 119 0.1377 30 418 0.0670 8 130 0.0580
DS 207 241 0.4621 35 103 0.2536 102 346 0.2277 27 111 0.1957
SS 143 305 0.3192 35 103 0.2536 88 360 0.1964 15 123 0.1087
US 230 218 0.5134 54 84 0.3913 108 340 0.2411 24 114 0.1739
Mean over NC-GE types 165 283 0.3689 36 102 0.2591 82 366 0.1831 19 120 0.1341
SS TB 196 252 0.4375 51 87 0.3696 98 350 0.2188 18 120 0.1304
DS 317 131 0.7076 79 59 0.5725 191 257 0.4263 46 92 0.3333
SS 176 272 0.3929 44 94 0.3188 81 367 0.1808 20 118 0.1449
US 322 126 0.7188 88 50 0.6377 204 244 0.4553 52 86 0.3768
Mean over NC-GE types 253 195 0.5642 66 73 0.4747 144 305 0.3198 34 104 0.2464

aOverall, across all GEs, the proportions of significant channels were lower for contralateral than for ipsilateral.

We checked the normality assumption for ANOVA by conducting the Lilliefors test on the residuals; while the null hypothesis of normality could not be rejected for HC-SSs (p = 0.5000), the null hypothesis was rejected for SSRs (p = 0.0030). Nevertheless, we could observe in Table 9 consistent reduction in the proportions of NC channels showing significant temporal correlation with HC-SSRs, by 13%–45% for each NC-GE type (separately in N2 or N3) from ipsilateral to contralateral, with NC-TB in N3 showing the least drop and DS in N2 showing the most. Similarly, for HC-SSs, we observed a consistent reduction across NC-GE types in both N2 and N3; with the exception of NC-TBs in N3 (40% drop for HC-SSs vs 13% for HC-SSRs), the ipsilateral-to-contralateral drop (11%–20%) in HC-NC association proportions was smaller with respect to HC-SSs than to HC-SSRs. This suggests that HC-NC communication for spindles preferentially takes place ipsilaterally, and our subsequent PAC analyses appeared to support this (Fig. 6), as we would describe in detail later in Results.

Figure 6.

Figure 6.

Parietal spindles phase-lock to hippocampal spindles and modulate hippocampal ripple activity. A, HC-SS phase-locking with NC-SS tends to be localized along the longitudinal axis (A1 vs A2) and prefers ipsilateral HC-NC channel pairs (A3 vs A4). Each panel shows two traces of PLV. Blue trace represents average PLV computed over 3 s trials with overlapping HC-SSs and NC-SSs, centered on the starts of HC-SSs. Black trace represents average PLV computed over an equal number of pseudo-trials centered on random time points in NREM (N2 + N3). Lt, Left; Rt, right. B, PAC between the phase of NC-SS (10–16 Hz) in parietal lobe and the amplitude of HC ripple-range activity (60–120 Hz). Each histogram bin covers 10 degrees of phase. B1, B2, Channels are the same as those in A2 and A4, respectively. C, 2D projection maps of medial (C1) and lateral (C2) NC channels showing significant spindle phase-locking with HC. Each blue circle represents a significant NC channel, with color intensity corresponding to peak PLV amplitude. Plus signs indicate channels with no significant phase-locking. Projection maps that separate left and right hemispheres can be found in Extended Data Figure 6-1.

We performed four-way ANOVA as previously described for NC-GE to HC-SS associations, with the ipsilateral/contralateral factor replacing the aHC/pHC factor. All post hoc analyses were performed with Tukey's range test. All four main effects were significant, but none of the interaction effects was significant. As our previous ANOVA already revealed significant main effects for NC-GE type and NREM stage, we focused our attention on the remaining two factors (i.e., ipsilateral/contralateral and combined NC ROI), which provided results beyond those explored in the previous ANOVAs. The main effect of ipsilateral/contralateral channel pair types yielded F = 12.55, p = 0.0383 and, together with mean proportions of 0.454 for ipsilateral and 0.400 for contralateral, suggested that more of NC were related to ipsilateral HC-SSs than to contralateral HC-SSs, although the difference was modest. The main effect of ROIs yielded F = 28.90, p = 0.0126. Since only two ROIs were included in this ANOVA, this result suggested that the more frontal/cingulate parts of NC were significantly more likely to show significant associations between NC-GEs and HC-SSs than the posterior regions (mean proportion of 0.468 for frontocentral vs 0.386 for nonfrontal).

HC-SSs phase-lock to NC-SSs, and NC-SSs modulate HC ripple activity

A previous report of human HC-SSs suggested that scalp SSs could phase-lock with HC-SSs, which modulated ripple activity (Staresina et al., 2015). We observed earlier in the results that hippocampal ripples could indeed co-occur with and appeared modulated by HC-SSs (Fig. 2C,D), and we found widespread NC channels showing significant HC-NC SS temporal correlations, particularly in N2 with respect to pHC (Figs. 4B, 5E). To further evaluate spindle-mediated HC-NC coupling, we computed PLVs over 3 s trials where NC-SSs overlapped with HC-SSs for at least one cycle (for examples, see Fig. 6A). After PLV computation, for each nonoverlapping 50 ms time bin, a two-sample t test was performed between the actual PLV and the baseline estimate, with the resulting p values undergoing FDR correction. A given HC-NC channel pair would be considered significantly phase-locking if: (1) >40 trials were used in the PLV computation, since small sample size is known to introduce bias (Aydore et al., 2013); or (2) at least 3 consecutive time bins yield post-FDR p values <0.05. Since HC-SS modulates ripple activity, NC-SS phase-locking with HC-SS would modulate HC ripples by inference; we nonetheless also performed PAC analysis between NC-SS phase and HC ripple amplitude in HC-NC channel pairs with significant SS phase-locking (for examples, see Fig. 6B), and estimated the significance of observed PAC by computing average Modulation Indices across trials (each with the same HC-NC SS overlap criterion as used for PLV) and comparing them with bootstrapped baselines (Tort et al., 2010).

We found that significant HC-NC SS phase-locking (over 10–16 Hz, with maximum PLV ranging from 0.3 to 0.9) could be found across widespread NC regions (Fig. 6C; Extended Data Fig. 6-1). Maximum PLV tended to be reached at ∼300–700 ms after HC-SS onsets. We observed a higher density of NC channels showing significant PLV with pHC than with aHC. In particular, 37% of NC channels phase-locked with HC; and out of this channel population, 67% phase-locked with pHC (vs 33% with aHC) in N2, and 70% phase-locked with pHC (vs 30% with aHC) in N3. For both N2 and N3, this pHC preference among NC channels with significant SS PLV was significant (Fisher's exact tests, p = 4.991 × 10−9 for N2, p = 2.377 × 10−7 for N3). While HC-NC SS phase-locking was observed across NC, only a small proportion had sustained large PLV (e.g., Fig. 6A2,A4). Specifically, only 5% of all NC channels had a peak HC-NC SS PLV value >0.4; in this small population of high PLV channels, ∼70% were parietal (7 in precuneus, 5 in angular gyrus, 4 in intraparietal sulcus). In addition, for patients with both aHC and pHC contacts in the same hippocampus, NC channels phase-locked to pHC tended not to show significant phase-locking with aHC as well (e.g., Fig. 6A), suggesting that HC-NC coupling mediated by spindle phase could be strongly localized along the HC longitudinal axis, which appears in agreement with the rare overlap between aHC- and pHC-SSRs/SSs reported in the beginning of Results.

Similar to the result above with high PLV channel pairs being a small subset of all channels with significant PLV, NC-SS phase to HC ripple amplitude PAC was only observed in approximately one-fourth of all channels with HC-NC-SS phase-locking. Again, these channels were mainly (∼70%) parietal (6 in precuneus, 3 in angular gyrus, 3 in intraparietal sulcus). Interestingly, only half of the high SS PLV channel pairs showed significant NC-SS to HC ripple PAC also.

Remarkably, for the population of NC channels from the 8 patients with multiple HC contacts in the same hippocampi, 15 of the 16 (94%) NC channels showing significant SS phase-locking with aHC also phase-lock with pHC significantly in N2, whereas conversely only 15 of the 43 (35%) NC channels showing significant SS phase-locking with pHC also phase-lock with aHC; in N3, this aHC/pHC imbalance in percentages of concurrently coupled NC sites was preserved (7 of 10, 70% with aHC vs 7 of 24, 29% with pHC). Thus, if aHC phase-locks SS with an NC site, then pHC does also, but not vice versa.

Strong ipsilateral preference for phase-locked HC-NC channel pairs was also observed in the patients with bilateral pHC contacts, with example PLV plots shown in Figure 6A, where the right precuneus preferentially coupled with right pHC. Within the channel pairs showing significant SS PLV involving aHC, in N2, only 5% were contralateral (thus 95% were ipsilateral); in N3, all of the channel pairs with significant PLV involving aHC were ipsilateral. Similarly, for phase-locking channel pairs involving pHC, 89% were ipsilateral in N2 and 90% were ipsilateral in N3. This strong preference for ipsilateral HC-NC SS phase-locking is larger than the preference for ipsilateral co-occurrence of HC-SSs and HC-SSRs with NC-GEs described above.

Discussion

This study quantifies the characteristics of ripples that co-occur with hippocampal spindles (HC-SSRs) in humans. We previously demonstrated that human SWRs (HC-SWRs) resemble animal HC-SWRs in their density, NREM concentration, waveform, and intrahippocampal topography (Jiang et al., 2019). HC-SSRs and HC-SWRs were distinct in important ways: HC-SSRs were dominant in pHC, whereas HC-SWRs were in aHC. Both HC-SSRs and HC-SWRs were coupled with all types of cortical GEs (TBs, DSs, USs, and SSs) in widespread bilateral locations (Fig. 7). However, HC-SSRs were tightly phase-locked with SSs in ipsilateral inferior parietal and precuneate sites, whereas HC-SWRs showed a preference for prefrontal, central, and cingulate cortices. HC-SSRs generally occurred during the NC-SSs/USs, whereas HC-SWRs generally occurred ∼400 ms earlier, during the NC-DSs. A single HC-SSR is commonly adjacent to multiple ripples on successive peaks of the same HC-SSs, separated by ≥50 ms, whereas single ripples on HC-SWRs were separated by ∼5 s. These differences suggest that HC-SSRs and HC-SWRs may potentially play complementary roles in memory consolidation, although further support from additional behavioral studies is needed, as our current study lacks behavioral assessments that could clearly demonstrate the involvement of HC-SS/SSR in memory consolidation.

Figure 7.

Figure 7.

Proposed HC-SWR/SSR interactions with NC-GEs in the context of previous findings of NC-HC-thalamic interactions during GEs (Mak-McCully et al., 2017; Gonzalez et al., 2018; Jiang et al., 2019). A, The sequence begins with a spontaneous NC-TB. Firing patterns are hypothesized to project to the HC and help inform the context in which they arise (A1). Locally, the NC-TB may lead to the NC-DS (A2). B, The NC-DS in turn may contribute to the broadband decrease in HC-LFP preceding SWR (B1) and trigger a thalamic-DS (B2). C, The HC-SWR occurs at this critical juncture (Jiang et al., 2019), contributing recent memory traces to the NC as it reboots from the NC-DS (C1); meanwhile, the thalamic-DS enables the h and T currents to generate a thalamic-SS (C2). D, The pHC-SSR follows, perhaps projecting more detailed recent memory information during multiple ripples (D1). The pHC-SSRs are tightly phase-locked to SS in inferior and medial parietal cortices (present study), projected from the thalamus (D2). Increased firing and Ca2+ influx during the NC-SS and underlying NC-US may provide an ideal cellular environment for memory consolidation. Experimental evidence is lacking for pHC-SSRs serving as markers for replay, and the evidence for aHC-SWRs is mainly from rodents.

Human HC-SSs have previously been recorded in NREM (Montplaisir et al., 1981; Malow et al., 1999; Andrillon et al., 2011; Carpentier et al., 2017) and REM sleep (Lestra et al., 2018). We observed SSs in HC with no ictal or interictal involvement, and HC-SSs are observed in animals (Miyawaki and Diba, 2016), suggesting that they represent healthy activity. Our confirmation of phase modulation of HC ripples by HC-SSs, and our demonstration of their coordination with NC-GEs provides a possible framework for HC-NC information transfer, as previously suggested (Siapas and Wilson, 1998; Sirota et al., 2003; Staresina et al., 2015) Antony et al., 2018). However, despite our efforts to eliminate contamination by epileptiform events, our study is limited by the possibility of pathological restructuring, and the possible role of SSRs in memory replay is unconfirmed in mechanistic studies.

Animal studies have described varying temporal relationships between HC-SWRs and NC-SSs. The possibility that NC-SSs play an initiating role is implied by the observation that SSs can reach the hippocampus via entorhinal cortex and phase-modulate HC firing (Sirota et al., 2003; Isomura et al., 2006). However, others found HC-SWRs to precede NC-SSs (Siapas and Wilson, 1998; Peyrache et al., 2011). This apparent discrepancy could reflect the fact that, while HC-SWRs tend to precede NC-SSs (Jiang et al., 2019), HC-SSRs tend to co-occur with NC-SSs (Fig. 4A; Extended Data Fig. 4-1). Since some of the reports cited above detected high-frequency power without verifying a co-occurring sharpwave waveform, it is possible that they studied a mixed population of SWRs and SSRs.

Clemens et al. (2011) reported that ripples (including both interictal spikes and physiological activity) recorded medially to the parahippocampal gyrus are phase-modulated by parietal scalp spindles. Similarly, Staresina et al. (2015) found a 5% increase in HC ripple band power ±250 ms to scalp spindles. However, the current report appears to provide the first evidence that human intracranially recorded NC-SS phase-lock with HC-SS and show PAC with ripples. HC and parietal spindles may become phase-locked via projections from CA1 to area 7a (Clower et al., 2001), thalamohippocampal (Amaral and Cowan, 1980), and/or thalamocortical projections (Mak-McCully et al., 2017) (Fig. 7B–D).

An important consequence of this co-occurrence and phase-locking of HC-SWRs with NC-SSs is that, if HC-SWRs mark replay firing, then this information would be projected to NC at a propitious time (Fig. 7D). In humans, NC-SSs (Hagler et al., 2018), and especially NC-SSs/USs, increase firing (Csercsa et al., 2010; Mak-McCully et al., 2017; Gonzalez et al., 2018). In rodents, NC-SSs (Seibt et al., 2017), and especially NC-SSs/USs (Niethard et al., 2018), are associated with strong Ca2+ influx in NC pyramidal cell apical dendrites, and increased plasticity (Steriade and Timofeev, 2003). Behavioral consolidation in rodents depends on NC-SS/US coupling with HC-SWRs (Maingret et al., 2016; Latchoumane et al., 2017). Similarly, scalp SS and SS-US complexes are associated with consolidation in humans (Marshall et al., 2006; Mednick et al., 2013; Niknazar et al., 2015).

The distinctive behavioral contributions of ventral/dorsal HC in rodents, or aHC/pHC in humans, have been characterized in multiple ways. In rodents, ventral HC lesions impair contextual emotional learning, corresponding to its amygdala, orbitofrontal, and hypothalamic connections, whereas dorsal lesions impair spatial learning, corresponding to its retrosplenial and parietal connections (Fanselow and Dong, 2010). Although ventral HC neurons also exhibit place fields, they are less frequent and larger than dorsal HC (Strange et al., 2014), and the smaller ventral HC dentate gyrus may support coarser pattern separation (Poppenk et al., 2013). The anatomical contrasts between ventral and dorsal HC in rodents have been confirmed with resting-state BOLD connectivity measures in humans (Ranganath and Ritchey, 2012). Functional imaging studies suggest greater aHC involvement in learning the gist of events, and pHC the details, perhaps consistent with the broader encoding by its cells in rodents (Poppenk et al., 2013; Strange et al., 2014).

The preponderance of SSRs in dorsal HC has not been reported in rodents. Indeed, SWRs are more dense and stronger in dorsal than ventral rodent HC (Patel et al., 2013). One possible interpretation is that pHC-SSRs may support a faculty that is particularly well developed in humans. The cortical areas (angular gyrus, precuneus, and retrosplenial cortex) strongly phase-locked with pHC-SSR, as well as the HC itself, are all associated with episodic memory, which Tulving and Markowitsch (1998) have argued is unique to humans. Lesions restricted to the HC but sparing adjoining temporal cortical areas severely impair episodic memory with relatively preserved nonepisodic declarative memories (Vargha-Khadem et al., 1997). Similarly, angular gyrus lesions impair recollective experience without necessarily impairing familiarity judgments (Papagno, 2018). Recollective experience is correlated with hemodynamic activation in these areas (Gilmore et al., 2015), as well as unit increases (Rutishauser et al., 2018), possibly indicating a role in storing details of episodic memories (Rugg and King, 2018; but see Davis et al., 2018). Recollection is also associated with a parietal positive event-related potential, which is dependent on HC integrity (Smith and Halgren, 1989), and correlated across trials with pHC and retrosplenial BOLD activation (Hoppstädter et al., 2015). HC stimulation can induce intense feelings that current experience is being relived (déjà vu) as well as vivid recollections of previous experiences (Halgren et al., 1978; Curot et al., 2017). These considerations raise the possibility that pHC-SSRs are more prominent in humans because they help support autobiographical memory; further investigations with behavioral assessments are necessary to test this hypothesis.

The ratio of hippocampal to neocortical pyramidal cells is ∼1:33 in rats and ∼1:2246 in humans (Korbo et al., 1990; West and Gundersen, 1990; West et al., 1991; Pakkenberg and Gundersen, 1997). Thus, the HC→NC fanout is extremely broad in humans. Yet, a central feature of HC-dependent memory is its ability to unify arbitrary elements, and this is presumed to require the HC to interact with all or most of the NC (McNaughton, 2010). Indeed, we have shown that the HC-SWRs co-occur with NC-SSs/USs across widespread cortical areas in all lobes (Jiang et al., 2019). Furthermore, HC-SWRs only occur ∼12 times per minute; thus, most cortical activity would develop in the absence of HC input from any particular location. Nonetheless, HC-SWRs tend to occur at the time of the NC-DSs, that is, at the beginning of the NC-SSs/USs (Fig. 7B), and thus can set the stage for subsequent elaboration of the NC↔ NC network. Therefore, the wide HC→NC fanout and low HC-SWR density, as well as its timing, might be expected to limit the influence of HC input to setting the general context of NC network evolution rather than its precise content (Fig. 7C).

In contrast to aHC-SWRs, pHC-SSRs produce multiple ripple bursts and thus replay patterns to a focal cortical target during the process of NC cell assembly. Multiple ripples commonly occur over the course of a single pHC-SSR, with their centers separated by ≥50 ms, compared with HC-SWRs, which are separated by ∼5 s. Furthermore, in contrast to the NC-GE co-occurrence of HC-SWRs with widespread NC areas, pHC-SSRs restrict their very high level of phase-locking during putative replay to a focal target in area 7a. Finally, the pHC-SSR input to NC is sustained during the SSs/USs when cell-firing dendritic calcium levels are optimal for synapse formation, as opposed to a single punctate input from HC-SWRs at the beginning of the SSs/USs. Thus, while the HC-SWR input appears well suited to provide guiding input to widespread NC areas comprising the “gist” of a recent event, the pHC-SSRs may be better-suited to impress specific details through repeated inputs to focal NC areas (Fig. 7C,D).

In conclusion, during NREM sleep, the human hippocampus generates ripples and may potentially provide specific information to the neocortex in two distinct LFP and anatomical contexts: aHC-SWR and pHC-SSR. Both are coordinated with thalamocortical waves, which occur in a typical order TBs→DSs→SSs/USs, but aHC-SWRs occur with the DSs and pHC-SSRs with SSs/USs (Fig. 7). aHC is anatomically connected with limbic and prefrontal sites, and aHC-SWRs are preferentially coupled with prefrontal and cingulate NC-GEs (Jiang et al., 2019). pHC is connected to parahippocampal, retrosplenial, precuneus, and inferior parietal cortices, and pHC-SSRs are strongly phase-locked with these areas. The low rate of aHC-SWRs, their broad bilateral anatomical fanout, and the preference of this fanout for frontolimbic sites are consistent with the contextual contribution sometimes assigned to aHC (or ventral hippocampus in rodents). In contrast, the rapid repeated ripples during pHC-SSRs, phase-locked with focal ipsilateral parietal loci, are consistent with a more detail-oriented consolidation process if, indeed, pHC-SSRs are found to participate in replay.

Supplementary Material

Figure 1-1

Additional information on HC-SS/SSR distributions over the longitudinal axis of HC and over behavioral states. A-D. Distribution of HC-SS and HC-SSR occurrence rates in different NREM stages across the HC longitudinal axis. Each circle represents data from one hippocampal contact. Red circles indicate contacts in aHC; blue circles indicate those in pHC. Yellow highlights mark the contacts from interictal-free patients. Pt.: patient. E. Example state plot showing the separation of NREM sleep from waking/REM in ∼8-hour LFP recording and HC-SS occurrence rate (color coded with red intensity) over time. N2/N3 periods are marked with orange/green horizontal lines, respectively. Light blue arrow marks the beginning of NREM sleep. F. Mean rates of HC-SWR and HC-SSR in the first half (“Early”) versus second half (“Late”) of NREM sleep across all nights and patients. Download Figure 1-1, EPS file (6.7MB, eps)

Figure 2-1

Co-occurrence likelihood distributions over different time windows for bilateral hippocampi and along the HC longitudinal axis. A-B. For both bilateral HC site pairs and Ant-HC/Post-HC site pairs, HC-SSR (A) and HC-SS (B) co-occurrence likelihood approaches chance as time window for co-occurrence expands. For bilateral HC, high variability in the ratio between actual and chance proportions (of overlapping HC-SS/SSR) was observed, likely due to the relatively small sample size. Blue filled circles mark (wrt. left y-axis) the ratios of actual over chance overlap proportions, and orange/brown unfilled circles mark (wrt. right y-axis) the corresponding actual (orange) and chance (brown) proportions. All axes for each panel are logarithmic. C. Combined data from A show that HC-SSR co-occur between different HC locations (i.e. across both hemispheres or between aHC and pHC of the same hemisphere), at a low rate (<4%) that falls to chance before 1 s of separation. D. Similar findings for HC-SS after combining data from B. E. The time-course of intra-hippocampal event-related phase-amplitude coupling (ERPAC) between spindle (10-16 Hz) phase and ripple (60-120 Hz) amplitude, computed over HC-SS in NREM across all patients for pHC, where pHC events are randomly sub-selected to have the same number of events involved as aHC (Fig. 2D1). Events are aligned at HC-SS starts (red vertical lines). Green line segments mark the time segments with significant coupling (p < 0.001). F-G, Peri-stimulus time histograms in N3 with aHC-SWR as triggers and either pHC-SSR (F) or pHC-SWR (G) as tallied events, with HC-SXR excluded from F; pHC-SSR tend to follow aHC-SWR by ∼350 ms (marked with green arrows in A), and aHC/pHC-SWR tend to co-occur within ±200 ms. H. Histograms of ripple counts across all HC-SS in N3. Not shown are the HC-SS with ≥5 ripples (n=211, max 10). In both N2 (Fig. 2B) and N3, only ∼10% of the HC-SS had 3 ripples or more nestled within, possibly owing to less prominent ripples in LFP signal being under the detection threshold. I. Histogram of temporal intervals between adjacent ripples within the same HC-SS, peaking between 100 and 150 ms, with the median of the distribution at 234 ms. Each histogram bin covers 50 ms. The distance between adjacent SSR within the same HC-SS peaks at between 100 and 150 ms, with the median inter-ripple distance being 234 ms. J. Auto-correlogram of ripple occurrence times, triggering on ripples within HC-SS, over ±1000 ms, with 50 ms bins. Red vertical line marks the centers of each SSR. The drop in correlogram counts between 0 and 100 ms agrees with ripple modulation at spindle frequency (Fig. 1C, Fig. 2C-D), although the absence of counts between 0 and ±50 ms is partially accounted for due to the ripple detection criteria we used (see “Hippocampal graphoelement selection” in Methods). Download Figure 2-1, EPS file (6.7MB, eps)

Figure 3-1

Summary of mean number of events in NC-GE to HC-SSR/SS histograms for individual channel pairs. Each cell is formatted as follows: Mean±Standard Deviation (Skewness). Ipsi: ipsilateral. Contra: contralateral. Download Figure 3-1, PDF file (199.8KB, pdf)

Figure 4-1

DSs, SSs, and USs across NC tend to occur with consistent latencies from HC-SSR in N3. A-C. Top rows: Histograms of peak latencies for HC-NC channel pairs with significant temporal correlations between HC-SSR and NC-GE. Each count is the peak latency of a particular HC-NC channel pair. Bottom rows: maps of peak latency between HC-SSR and NC-GE for cortical channels. Circles indicate where GE-SSR relationships are significant, while plus signs represent non-significant channels. The intensity of each circle corresponds to the strength of GE-SSR coupling estimated (as in Fig. 3), and the color of each circle indicates peak latency: red for NC-GE before HC-SSR, blue for NC-GE after HC-SSR, and green for NC-GE co-occurring with HC-SSR (i.e. within 50 ms of each other). Both hemispheres and medial and lateral cortical sites are superimposed in each plot. A. In N2, with aHC-SSR as reference, NC-GE tend to occur in the following order: TB starts → DS peaks ∼= SS starts → US peaks. B. In N3, with aHC SSR as reference, NC-GE tend to occur in the following order: DS peaks → SS starts → US peaks. C. In N3, with pHC SSR as reference, NC-GE tend to occur in the following order: DS peaks → SS starts → US peaks. In both A and B, US peaks tend to co-occur with HC-SSR. Download Figure 4-1, TIF file (21.8MB, tif)

Figure 4-2

GEs across NC tend to occur with consistent latencies from HC-SS in N3. A-C. Top rows: Histograms of peak latencies for HC-NC channel pairs with significant temporal correlations between HC-SS and NC-GE. Each count is the peak latency of a particular HC-NC channel pair. Bottom rows: maps of peak latency between HC-SS and NC-GE for cortical channels. Circles indicate where GE-SS relationships are significant, while plus signs represent non-significant channels. The intensity of each circle corresponds to the strength of NC-GE to HC-SS coupling estimated (as in Fig. 3), and the color of each circle indicates peak latency: red for NC-GE before HC-SS, blue for NC-GE after HC-SS, and green for NC-GE co-occurring with HC-SS (i.e. within 50 ms of each other). Both hemispheres and medial and lateral cortical sites are superimposed in each plot. A. In N2, with aHC-SS as reference, NC-GE tend to occur in the following order: TB starts → DS peaks → SS starts → US peaks. B. In N3, with aHC SS as reference, NC-GE tend to occur in the following order: DS peaks → SS starts → US peaks. C. In N3, with pHC SS as reference, NC-GE tend to occur in the following order: TB starts → DS peaks → SS starts → US peaks. In A-C, NC-SS tend to co-occur with HC-SS, and widespread NC-US tend to follow. Download Figure 4-2, TIF file (21.8MB, tif)

Figure 4-3

HC-SSR to NC-GE associations are preserved when limiting HC-SSR to only the first SSR within each HC-SS. A. Event-related spectral power for first-in-SS HC-SSR (same patient source as Fig. 1D), with corresponding average LFP trace below. B. Top row: Histograms of peak latencies for HC-NC channel pairs with significant temporal correlations between first-in-SS HC-SSR and NC-GE. Each count is the peak latency of a particular HC-NC channel pair. Bottom row: maps of peak latency between first-in-SS HC-SSR and NC-GE for cortical channels. Circles indicate where GE-SSR relationships are significant, while plus signs represent non-significant channels. The intensity of each circle corresponds to the strength of GE-SSR coupling estimated (as in Fig. 3), and the color of each circle indicates peak latency: red for NC-GE before HC-SSR, blue for NC-GE after HC-SSR, and green for NC-GE co-occurring with HC-SSR (i.e. within 50 ms of each other). Both hemispheres and medial and lateral cortical sites are superimposed in each plot. Similar to Fig. 4A, in N2, with pHC-SSR as reference, NC-GE tend to occur in the following order: TB starts → DS peaks → SS starts → US peaks. Download Figure 4-3, TIF file (12.3MB, tif)

Figure 4-4

NC-GE synchronized with HC-SSR are not randomly distributed across sites. P-values for chi-square tests of homogeneity (α = 0.05 post FDR-correction applied over each column), conducted for each HC site with regard to each NC-GE type’s coupling to HC-SSR. Cells marked 2.20E-016 are placeholders for p < 2.2 x 10-16. # signif.: number of HC sites with a non-homogeneous anatomical distribution of significantly coupled NC sites. Out of 32 x 4 = 128 distributions of NC channels showing NC-GE to HC-SSR association in N2, 89 were significantly different from chance for HC-SSR in N2, compared to 46 in N3. The proportion of significant distributions was significantly greater in N2 (Fisher’s exact tests, p = 2.754 x 10-8). The percentages of pHC sites coupled to non-homogeneous NC site distributions (75-92% in N2, 42-67% in N3) were greater than the percentages of aHC sites (55-65% in N2, 15-35% in N3) for all NC-GE types. Download Figure 4-4, PDF file (320.4KB, pdf)

Figure 4-5

NC-GE synchronized with HC-SS are not randomly distributed across sites. P-values for chi-square tests of homogeneity (α = 0.05 post FDR-correction applied over each column), conducted for each HC site with regard to each NC-GE type’s coupling to HC-SS. Ant.: Ant-HC. Post: Post-HC. Cells marked 2.20E-016 are placeholders for p < 2.2 x 10-16. # signif.: number of HC sites with a non-homogeneous anatomical distribution of significantly coupled NC sites. Out of 32 x 4 = 128 distributions of NC channels showing NC-GE to HC-SS association in N2, 109 were significantly different from chance for HC-SSR in N2, compared to 62 in N3. The proportion of significant distributions was significantly greater in N2 (Fisher’s exact tests, p = 2.212 x 10-10). The pHC percentages (83-100% in N2, 42-67% in N3) and aHC percentages (70-95% in N2, 40-60% in N3) were both very high, consistent with a selective interaction with parietal areas. Download Figure 4-5, PDF file (321.4KB, pdf)

Figure 4-6

Coupled NC sites differ between simultaneously recorded aHC and pHC sites. Proportions of NC-GE from significantly coupled NC sites that overlapped with aHC-SSR/SS differ from the proportions for pHC-SSR/SS. Wilcoxon signed rank tests were performed on the data obtained for Figs. 4-4 and 4-5. Each test (whose p-value was tabulated above) required that both members of a HC site pair must have significant chi-square test result from Fig. 4-4 (for HC-SSR) or Fig. 4-5 (for HC-SS); otherwise, the corresponding cell would be filled with NA. # signif.: number of significant tests (in total number of qualified patients). Download Figure 4-6, PDF file (200.7KB, pdf)

Figure 6-1

2d projection maps of left hemisphere medial (A), left hemisphere lateral (B), right hemisphere medial (C), and right hemisphere lateral (D) NC channels showing significant spindle phase-locking with HC. Each blue circle marks a significant NC channel, with color intensity corresponding to peak PLV amplitude. Plus signs mark channels with no significant phase-locking. Download Figure 6-1, TIF file (10.7MB, tif)

Table 4-1

ROIs for statistical analyses of spatio-temporal differences across NC regions in HC-NC relationships. Download Table 4-1, PDF file (202.9KB, pdf)

Footnotes

This work was supported by the U.S. Office of Naval Research's Multidisciplinary University Research Initiatives Program N00014–16-1–2829, National Institute of Mental Health RF1 MH117155 and R01 MH111437, and National Institute of Biomedical Imaging and Bioengineering R01 EB009282. We thank John Gale, Qianqian Deng, Charles Dickey, Darlene Evardone, Zach Fitzgerald, Chris Gonzalez, Don Hagler, Milan Halgren, Erik Kaestner, Rachel Mak-McCully, Adam Niese, Burke Rosen, and T. G. Venti for support.

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

References

  1. Amaral DG, Cowan WM (1980) Subcortical afferents to the hippocampal formation in the monkey. J Comp Neurol 189:573–591. 10.1002/cne.901890402 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Andrillon T, Nir Y, Staba RJ, Ferrarelli F, Cirelli C, Tononi G, Fried I (2011) Sleep spindles in humans: insights from intracranial EEG and unit recordings. J Neurosci 31:17821–17834. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2604-11.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Antony JW, Piloto L, Wang M, Pacheco P, Norman KA, Paller KA (2018) Sleep spindle refractoriness segregates periods of memory reactivation. Curr Biol 28:1736–1743.e4. 10.1016/j.cub.2018.04.020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Axmacher N, Elger CE, Fell J (2008) Ripples in the medial temporal lobe are relevant for human memory consolidation. Brain J Neurol 131:1806–1817. 10.1093/brain/awn103 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Aydore S, Pantazis D, Leahy RM (2013) A note on the phase locking value and its properties. Neuroimage 74:231–244. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.02.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Battaglia FP, Sutherland GR, McNaughton BL (2004) Hippocampal sharp wave bursts coincide with neocortical “up-state” transitions. Learn Mem 11:697–704. 10.1101/lm.73504 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y (1995) Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J R Stat Soc Ser B Methodol 57:289–300. [Google Scholar]
  8. Bragin A, Penttonen M, Buzsáki G (1997) Termination of epileptic afterdischarge in the hippocampus. J Neurosci 17:2567–2579. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-07-02567.1997 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bragin A, Engel J Jr, Wilson CL, Fried I, Buzsáki G (1999) High-frequency oscillations in human brain. Hippocampus 9:137–142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Buzsáki G. (2015) Hippocampal sharp wave ripple: a cognitive biomarker for episodic memory and planning. Hippocampus 25:1073–1188. 10.1002/hipo.22488 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Buzsáki G, Horváth Z, Urioste R, Hetke J, Wise K (1992) High-frequency network oscillation in the hippocampus. Science 256:1025–1027. 10.1126/science.1589772 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Carpentier N, Cecchin T, Koessler L, Louis-Dorr V, Jonas J, Vignal JP, Carpentier M, Szurhaj W, Bourgin P, Maillard L (2017) Stereo-electroencephalography identifies N2 sleep and spindles in human hippocampus. Clin Neurophysiol 128:1696–1706. 10.1016/j.clinph.2017.06.248 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Carskadon MA, Dement WC (2010) Monitoring and staging human sleep. In: Principles and practice of sleep medicine (Kryger MH, Roth T, Dement WC, eds), Ed 5, pp 16–26. St. Louis: Elsevier Saunders. [Google Scholar]
  14. Cash SS, Halgren E, Dehghani N, Rossetti AO, Thesen T, Wang C, Devinsky O, Kuzniecky R, Doyle W, Madsen JR, Bromfield E, Eross L, Halász P, Karmos G, Csercsa R, Wittner L, Ulbert I (2009) The human K-complex represents an isolated cortical down-state. Science 324:1084–1087. 10.1126/science.1169626 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Clemens Z, Mölle M, Eross L, Jakus R, Rásonyi G, Halász P, Born J (2011) Fine-tuned coupling between human parahippocampal ripples and sleep spindles. Eur J Neurosci 33:511–520. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07505.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Clower DM, West RA, Lynch JC, Strick PL (2001) The inferior parietal lobule is the target of output from the superior colliculus, hippocampus, and cerebellum. J Neurosci 21:6283–6291. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-16-06283.2001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Colgin LL, Denninger T, Fyhn M, Hafting T, Bonnevie T, Jensen O, Moser MB, Moser EI (2009) Frequency of γ oscillations routes flow of information in the hippocampus. Nature 462:353–357. 10.1038/nature08573 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Csercsa R, Dombovári B, Fabó D, Wittner L, Eross L, Entz L, Sólyom A, Rásonyi G, Szucs A, Kelemen A, Jakus R, Juhos V, Grand L, Magony A, Halász P, Freund TF, Maglóczky Z, Cash SS, Papp L, Karmos G, et al. (2010) Laminar analysis of slow wave activity in humans. Brain J Neurol 133:2814–2829. 10.1093/brain/awq169 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Curot J, Busigny T, Valton L, Denuelle M, Vignal JP, Maillard L, Chauvel P, Pariente J, Trebuchon A, Bartolomei F, Barbeau EJ (2017) Memory scrutinized through electrical brain stimulation: a review of 80 years of experiential phenomena. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 78:161–177. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.04.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Dale AM, Fischl B, Sereno MI (1999) Cortical surface-based analysis: I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. Neuroimage 9:179–194. 10.1006/nimg.1998.0395 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Davis SW, Wing EA, Cabeza R (2018) Contributions of the ventral parietal cortex to declarative memory. Handb Clin Neurol 151:525–553. 10.1016/B978-0-444-63622-5.00027-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Delorme A, Makeig S (2004) EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J Neurosci Methods 134:9–21. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Desikan RS, Ségonne F, Fischl B, Quinn BT, Dickerson BC, Blacker D, Buckner RL, Dale AM, Maguire RP, Hyman BT, Albert MS, Killiany RJ (2006) An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage 31:968–980. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Destrieux C, Bourry D, Velut S (2013) Surgical anatomy of the hippocampus. Neurochirurgie 59:149–158. 10.1016/j.neuchi.2013.08.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Diekelmann S, Born J (2010) The memory function of sleep. Nat Rev Neurosci 11:114–126. 10.1038/nrn2762 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Ding SL, Van Hoesen GW (2015) Organization and detailed parcellation of human hippocampal head and body regions based on a combined analysis of cyto- and chemoarchitecture. J Comp Neurol 523:2233–2253. 10.1002/cne.23786 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Duvernoy H. (1988) The human hippocampus: an atlas of applied anatomy. Munich: Bergmann-Verlag. [Google Scholar]
  28. Dupré la Tour T, Tallot L, Grabot L, Doyère V, van Wassenhove V, Grenier Y, Gramfort A (2017) Non-linear auto-regressive models for cross-frequency coupling in neural time series. PLOS Comput Biol 13:e1005893. 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005893 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Dykstra AR, Chan AM, Quinn BT, Zepeda R, Keller CJ, Cormier J, Madsen JR, Eskandar EN, Cash SS (2012) Individualized localization and cortical surface-based registration of intracranial electrodes. Neuroimage 59:3563–3570. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.046 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Fanselow MS, Dong HW (2010) Are the dorsal and ventral hippocampus functionally distinct structures? Neuron 65:7–19. 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.11.031 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Fischl B, Sereno MI, Dale AM (1999a) Cortical surface-based analysis: II: Inflation, flattening, and a surface-based coordinate system. Neuroimage 9:195–207. 10.1006/nimg.1998.0396 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Fischl B, Sereno MI, Tootell RB, Dale AM (1999b) High-resolution intersubject averaging and a coordinate system for the cortical surface. Hum Brain Mapp 8:272–284. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Fischl B, van der Kouwe A, Destrieux C, Halgren E, Ségonne F, Salat DH, Busa E, Seidman LJ, Goldstein J, Kennedy D, Caviness V, Makris N, Rosen B, Dale AM (2004) Automatically parcellating the human cerebral cortex. Cereb Cortex 14:11–22. 10.1093/cercor/bhg087 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Gervasoni D, Lin SC, Ribeiro S, Soares ES, Pantoja J, Nicolelis MA (2004) Global forebrain dynamics predict rat behavioral states and their transitions. J Neurosci 24:11137–11147. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3524-04.2004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Gilmore AW, Nelson SM, McDermott KB (2015) A parietal memory network revealed by multiple MRI methods. Trends Cogn Sci 19:534–543. 10.1016/j.tics.2015.07.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Girardeau G, Zugaro M (2011) Hippocampal ripples and memory consolidation. Curr Opin Neurobiol 21:452–459. 10.1016/j.conb.2011.02.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Girardeau G, Benchenane K, Wiener SI, Buzsáki G, Zugaro MB (2009) Selective suppression of hippocampal ripples impairs spatial memory. Nat Neurosci 12:1222–1223. 10.1038/nn.2384 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Glosser G, Deutsch GK, Cole LC, Corwin J, Saykin AJ (1998) Differential lateralization of memory discrimination and response bias in temporal lobe epilepsy patients. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 4:502–511. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Göldi M, van Poppel EA, Rasch B, Schreiner T (2019) Increased neuronal signatures of targeted memory reactivation during slow-wave up states. Sci Rep 9:2715. 10.1038/s41598-019-39178-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Gonzalez CE, Mak-McCully RA, Rosen BQ, Cash SS, Chauvel PY, Bastuji H, Rey M, Halgren E (2018) Theta bursts precede, and spindles follow, cortical and thalamic downstates in human NREM sleep. J Neurosci 38:9989–10001. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0476-18.2018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Gonzalez-Martinez J, Bulacio J, Alexopoulos A, Jehi L, Bingaman W, Najm I (2013) Stereoelectroencephalography in the “difficult to localize” refractory focal epilepsy: early experience from a North American Epilepsy Center. Epilepsia 54:323–330. 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2012.03672.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Hagler DJ Jr, Ulbert I, Wittner L, Eross L, Madsen JR, Devinsky O, Doyle W, Fabó D, Cash SS, Halgren E (2018) Heterogeneous origins of human sleep spindles in different cortical layers. J Neurosci 38:3013–3025. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2241-17.2018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Halgren E, Walter RD, Cherlow DG, Crandall PH (1978) Mental phenomena evoked by electrical stimulation of the human hippocampal formation and amygdala. Brain J Neurol 101:83–117. 10.1093/brain/101.1.83 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Headley DB, Kanta V, Paré D (2017) Intra- and interregional cortical interactions related to sharp-wave ripples and dentate spikes. J Neurophysiol 117:556–565. 10.1152/jn.00644.2016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Hoppstädter M, Baeuchl C, Diener C, Flor H, Meyer P (2015) Simultaneous EEG-fMRI reveals brain networks underlying recognition memory ERP old/new effects. Neuroimage 116:112–122. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.05.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Isomura Y, Sirota A, Ozen S, Montgomery S, Mizuseki K, Henze DA, Buzsáki G (2006) Integration and segregation of activity in entorhinal-hippocampal subregions by neocortical slow oscillations. Neuron 52:871–882. 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.10.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Jiang X, Shamie I, Doyle W, Friedman D, Dugan P, Devinsky O, Eskandar E, Cash SS, Thesen T, Halgren E (2017) Replay of large-scale spatiotemporal patterns from waking during subsequent NREM sleep in human cortex. Sci Rep 7:17380. 10.1038/s41598-017-17469-w [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Jiang X, Gonzalez-Martinez J, Halgren E (2019) Coordination of human hippocampal sharpwave ripples during NREM sleep with cortical theta bursts, spindles, downstates and upstates. Advance online publication. Retrieved Sep 17, 2019. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2857-18.2019. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2857-18.2019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Korbo L, Pakkenberg B, Ladefoged O, Gundersen HJ, Arlien-Søborg P, Pakkenberg H (1990) An efficient method for estimating the total number of neurons in rat brain cortex. J Neurosci Methods 31:93–100. 10.1016/0165-0270(90)90153-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Lachaux JP, Rodriguez E, Martinerie J, Varela FJ (1999) Measuring phase synchrony in brain signals. Hum Brain Mapp 8:194–208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Latchoumane CV, Ngo HV, Born J, Shin HS (2017) Thalamic spindles promote memory formation during sleep through triple phase-locking of cortical, thalamic, and hippocampal rhythms. Neuron 95:424–435.e6. 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.06.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Lestra P, Peter-Derex L, Salin P, Comte JC (2018) Hippocampus and prefrontal cortex exhibit spindle-like waves during REM sleep. Neurophysiol Clin 48:135–136. 10.1016/j.neucli.2018.05.019 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  53. Maingret N, Girardeau G, Todorova R, Goutierre M, Zugaro M (2016) Hippocampo-cortical coupling mediates memory consolidation during sleep. Nat Neurosci 19:959–964. 10.1038/nn.4304 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Mak-McCully RA, Rosen BQ, Rolland M, Régis J, Bartolomei F, Rey M, Chauvel P, Cash SS, Halgren E (2015) Distribution, amplitude, incidence, co-occurrence, and propagation of human K-complexes in focal transcortical recordings. eNeuro 2:0028–15.2015. 10.1523/ENEURO.0028-15.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Mak-McCully RA, Rolland M, Sargsyan A, Gonzalez C, Magnin M, Chauvel P, Rey M, Bastuji H, Halgren E (2017) Coordination of cortical and thalamic activity during non-REM sleep in humans. Nat Commun 8:15499. 10.1038/ncomms15499 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Malow BA, Carney PR, Kushwaha R, Bowes RJ (1999) Hippocampal sleep spindles revisited: physiologic or epileptic activity? Clin Neurophysiol 110:687–693. 10.1016/S1388-2457(99)00008-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Marshall L, Helgadóttir H, Mölle M, Born J (2006) Boosting slow oscillations during sleep potentiates memory. Nature 444:610–613. 10.1038/nature05278 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. McNaughton BL. (2010) Cortical hierarchies, sleep, and the extraction of knowledge from memory. Artif Intell 174:205–214. 10.1016/j.artint.2009.11.013 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  59. Mednick SC, McDevitt EA, Walsh JK, Wamsley E, Paulus M, Kanady JC, Drummond SP (2013) The critical role of sleep spindles in hippocampal-dependent memory: a pharmacology study. J Neurosci 33:4494–4504. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3127-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Miyawaki H, Diba K (2016) Regulation of hippocampal firing by network oscillations during sleep. Curr Biol 26:893–902. 10.1016/j.cub.2016.02.024 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Mölle M, Yeshenko O, Marshall L, Sara SJ, Born J (2006) Hippocampal sharp wave ripples linked to slow oscillations in rat slow-wave sleep. J Neurophysiol 96:62–70. 10.1152/jn.00014.2006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Montplaisir J, Leduc L, Laverdière M, Walsh J, Saint-Hilaire JM (1981) Sleep spindles in the human hippocampus: normal or epileptic activity? Sleep 4:423–428. 10.1093/sleep/4.4.423 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Moraes W, Piovezan R, Poyares D, Bittencourt LR, Santos-Silva R, Tufik S (2014) Effects of aging on sleep structure throughout adulthood: a population-based study. Sleep Med 15:401–409. 10.1016/j.sleep.2013.11.791 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Nádasdy Z, Hirase H, Czurkó A, Csicsvari J, Buzsáki G (1999) Replay and time compression of recurring spike sequences in the hippocampus. J Neurosci 19:9497–9507. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-21-09497.1999 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Niethard N, Ngo HV, Ehrlich I, Born J (2018) Cortical circuit activity underlying sleep slow oscillations and spindles. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 115:E9220–E9229. 10.1073/pnas.1805517115 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Niknazar M, Krishnan GP, Bazhenov M, Mednick SC (2015) Coupling of thalamocortical sleep oscillations are important for memory consolidation in humans. PLoS One 10:e0144720. 10.1371/journal.pone.0144720 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. O'Neill J, Pleydell-Bouverie B, Dupret D, Csicsvari J (2010) Play it again: reactivation of waking experience and memory. Trends Neurosci 33:220–229. 10.1016/j.tins.2010.01.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Oostenveld R, Fries P, Maris E, Schoffelen JM (2011) FieldTrip: open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data. Comput Intell Neurosci 2011:156869. 10.1155/2011/156869 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Pakkenberg B, Gundersen HJ (1997) Neocortical neuron number in humans: effect of sex and age. J Comp Neurol 384:312–320. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Papagno C. (2018) Memory deficits. In: The parietal lobe: handbook of clinical neurology, pp 377–393. Amsterdam: Elsevier. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Patel J, Schomburg EW, Berényi A, Fujisawa S, Buzsáki G (2013) Local generation and propagation of ripples along the septotemporal axis of the hippocampus. J Neurosci 33:17029–17041. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2036-13.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Peyrache A, Khamassi M, Benchenane K, Wiener SI, Battaglia FP (2009) Replay of rule-learning related neural patterns in the prefrontal cortex during sleep. Nat Neurosci 12:919–926. 10.1038/nn.2337 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Peyrache A, Battaglia FP, Destexhe A (2011) Inhibition recruitment in prefrontal cortex during sleep spindles and gating of hippocampal inputs. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 108:17207–17212. 10.1073/pnas.1103612108 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Poppenk J, Evensmoen HR, Moscovitch M, Nadel L (2013) Long-axis specialization of the human hippocampus. Trends Cogn Sci 17:230–240. 10.1016/j.tics.2013.03.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Ramirez-Villegas JF, Logothetis NK, Besserve M (2015) Diversity of sharp-wave ripple LFP signatures reveals differentiated brain-wide dynamical events. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 112:E6379–E6387. 10.1073/pnas.1518257112 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Ranganath C, Ritchey M (2012) Two cortical systems for memory-guided behaviour. Nat Rev Neurosci 13:713–726. 10.1038/nrn3338 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Rugg MD, King DR (2018) Ventral lateral parietal cortex and episodic memory retrieval. Cortex 107:238–250. 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.07.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Rutishauser U, Aflalo T, Rosario ER, Pouratian N, Andersen RA (2018) Single-neuron representation of memory strength and recognition confidence in left human posterior parietal cortex. Neuron 97:209–220.e3. 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.11.029 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Seibt J, Richard CJ, Sigl-Glöckner J, Takahashi N, Kaplan DI, Doron G, de Limoges D, Bocklisch C, Larkum ME (2017) Cortical dendritic activity correlates with spindle-rich oscillations during sleep in rodents. Nat Commun 8:684. 10.1038/s41467-017-00735-w [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Siapas AG, Wilson MA (1998) Coordinated interactions between hippocampal ripples and cortical spindles during slow-wave sleep. Neuron 21:1123–1128. 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80629-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Silber MH, Ancoli-Israel S, Bonnet MH, Chokroverty S, Grigg-Damberger MM, Hirshkowitz M, Kapen S, Keenan SA, Kryger MH, Penzel T, Pressman MR, Iber C (2007) The visual scoring of sleep in adults. J Clin Sleep Med 3:121–131. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Sirota A, Csicsvari J, Buhl D, Buzsáki G (2003) Communication between neocortex and hippocampus during sleep in rodents. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 100:2065–2069. 10.1073/pnas.0437938100 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Skaggs WE, McNaughton BL (1996) Replay of neuronal firing sequences in rat hippocampus during sleep following spatial experience. Science 271:1870–1873. 10.1126/science.271.5257.1870 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Skaggs WE, McNaughton BL, Permenter M, Archibeque M, Vogt J, Amaral DG, Barnes CA (2007) EEG sharp waves and sparse ensemble unit activity in the macaque hippocampus. J Neurophysiol 98:898–910. 10.1152/jn.00401.2007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Smith ME, Halgren E (1989) Dissociation of recognition memory components following temporal lobe lesions. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 15:50–60. 10.1037/0278-7393.15.1.50 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Staresina BP, Bergmann TO, Bonnefond M, van der Meij R, Jensen O, Deuker L, Elger CE, Axmacher N, Fell J (2015) Hierarchical nesting of slow oscillations, spindles and ripples in the human hippocampus during sleep. Nat Neurosci 18:1679–1686. 10.1038/nn.4119 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Steriade M, Timofeev I (2003) Neuronal plasticity in thalamocortical networks during sleep and waking oscillations. Neuron 37:563–576. 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00065-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Strange BA, Witter MP, Lein ES, Moser EI (2014) Functional organization of the hippocampal longitudinal axis. Nat Rev Neurosci 15:655–669. 10.1038/nrn3785 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Tort AB, Komorowski R, Eichenbaum H, Kopell N (2010) Measuring phase-amplitude coupling between neuronal oscillations of different frequencies. J Neurophysiol 104:1195–1210. 10.1152/jn.00106.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Tulving E, Markowitsch HJ (1998) Episodic and declarative memory: role of the hippocampus. Hippocampus 8:198–204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Vargha-Khadem F, Gadian DG, Watkins KE, Connelly A, Van Paesschen W, Mishkin M (1997) Differential effects of early hippocampal pathology on episodic and semantic memory. Science 277:376–380. 10.1126/science.277.5324.376 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Voytek B, D'Esposito M, Crone N, Knight RT (2013) A method for event-related phase/amplitude coupling. Neuroimage 64:416–424. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.09.023 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. West MJ, Gundersen HJ (1990) Unbiased stereological estimation of the number of neurons in the human hippocampus. J Comp Neurol 296:1–22. 10.1002/cne.902960102 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. West MJ, Slomianka L, Gundersen HJ (1991) Unbiased stereological estimation of the total number of neurons in the subdivisions of the rat hippocampus using the optical fractionator. Anat Rec 231:482–497. 10.1002/ar.1092310411 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Wilson MA, McNaughton BL (1994) Reactivation of hippocampal ensemble memories during sleep. Science 265:676–679. 10.1126/science.8036517 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Figure 1-1

Additional information on HC-SS/SSR distributions over the longitudinal axis of HC and over behavioral states. A-D. Distribution of HC-SS and HC-SSR occurrence rates in different NREM stages across the HC longitudinal axis. Each circle represents data from one hippocampal contact. Red circles indicate contacts in aHC; blue circles indicate those in pHC. Yellow highlights mark the contacts from interictal-free patients. Pt.: patient. E. Example state plot showing the separation of NREM sleep from waking/REM in ∼8-hour LFP recording and HC-SS occurrence rate (color coded with red intensity) over time. N2/N3 periods are marked with orange/green horizontal lines, respectively. Light blue arrow marks the beginning of NREM sleep. F. Mean rates of HC-SWR and HC-SSR in the first half (“Early”) versus second half (“Late”) of NREM sleep across all nights and patients. Download Figure 1-1, EPS file (6.7MB, eps)

Figure 2-1

Co-occurrence likelihood distributions over different time windows for bilateral hippocampi and along the HC longitudinal axis. A-B. For both bilateral HC site pairs and Ant-HC/Post-HC site pairs, HC-SSR (A) and HC-SS (B) co-occurrence likelihood approaches chance as time window for co-occurrence expands. For bilateral HC, high variability in the ratio between actual and chance proportions (of overlapping HC-SS/SSR) was observed, likely due to the relatively small sample size. Blue filled circles mark (wrt. left y-axis) the ratios of actual over chance overlap proportions, and orange/brown unfilled circles mark (wrt. right y-axis) the corresponding actual (orange) and chance (brown) proportions. All axes for each panel are logarithmic. C. Combined data from A show that HC-SSR co-occur between different HC locations (i.e. across both hemispheres or between aHC and pHC of the same hemisphere), at a low rate (<4%) that falls to chance before 1 s of separation. D. Similar findings for HC-SS after combining data from B. E. The time-course of intra-hippocampal event-related phase-amplitude coupling (ERPAC) between spindle (10-16 Hz) phase and ripple (60-120 Hz) amplitude, computed over HC-SS in NREM across all patients for pHC, where pHC events are randomly sub-selected to have the same number of events involved as aHC (Fig. 2D1). Events are aligned at HC-SS starts (red vertical lines). Green line segments mark the time segments with significant coupling (p < 0.001). F-G, Peri-stimulus time histograms in N3 with aHC-SWR as triggers and either pHC-SSR (F) or pHC-SWR (G) as tallied events, with HC-SXR excluded from F; pHC-SSR tend to follow aHC-SWR by ∼350 ms (marked with green arrows in A), and aHC/pHC-SWR tend to co-occur within ±200 ms. H. Histograms of ripple counts across all HC-SS in N3. Not shown are the HC-SS with ≥5 ripples (n=211, max 10). In both N2 (Fig. 2B) and N3, only ∼10% of the HC-SS had 3 ripples or more nestled within, possibly owing to less prominent ripples in LFP signal being under the detection threshold. I. Histogram of temporal intervals between adjacent ripples within the same HC-SS, peaking between 100 and 150 ms, with the median of the distribution at 234 ms. Each histogram bin covers 50 ms. The distance between adjacent SSR within the same HC-SS peaks at between 100 and 150 ms, with the median inter-ripple distance being 234 ms. J. Auto-correlogram of ripple occurrence times, triggering on ripples within HC-SS, over ±1000 ms, with 50 ms bins. Red vertical line marks the centers of each SSR. The drop in correlogram counts between 0 and 100 ms agrees with ripple modulation at spindle frequency (Fig. 1C, Fig. 2C-D), although the absence of counts between 0 and ±50 ms is partially accounted for due to the ripple detection criteria we used (see “Hippocampal graphoelement selection” in Methods). Download Figure 2-1, EPS file (6.7MB, eps)

Figure 3-1

Summary of mean number of events in NC-GE to HC-SSR/SS histograms for individual channel pairs. Each cell is formatted as follows: Mean±Standard Deviation (Skewness). Ipsi: ipsilateral. Contra: contralateral. Download Figure 3-1, PDF file (199.8KB, pdf)

Figure 4-1

DSs, SSs, and USs across NC tend to occur with consistent latencies from HC-SSR in N3. A-C. Top rows: Histograms of peak latencies for HC-NC channel pairs with significant temporal correlations between HC-SSR and NC-GE. Each count is the peak latency of a particular HC-NC channel pair. Bottom rows: maps of peak latency between HC-SSR and NC-GE for cortical channels. Circles indicate where GE-SSR relationships are significant, while plus signs represent non-significant channels. The intensity of each circle corresponds to the strength of GE-SSR coupling estimated (as in Fig. 3), and the color of each circle indicates peak latency: red for NC-GE before HC-SSR, blue for NC-GE after HC-SSR, and green for NC-GE co-occurring with HC-SSR (i.e. within 50 ms of each other). Both hemispheres and medial and lateral cortical sites are superimposed in each plot. A. In N2, with aHC-SSR as reference, NC-GE tend to occur in the following order: TB starts → DS peaks ∼= SS starts → US peaks. B. In N3, with aHC SSR as reference, NC-GE tend to occur in the following order: DS peaks → SS starts → US peaks. C. In N3, with pHC SSR as reference, NC-GE tend to occur in the following order: DS peaks → SS starts → US peaks. In both A and B, US peaks tend to co-occur with HC-SSR. Download Figure 4-1, TIF file (21.8MB, tif)

Figure 4-2

GEs across NC tend to occur with consistent latencies from HC-SS in N3. A-C. Top rows: Histograms of peak latencies for HC-NC channel pairs with significant temporal correlations between HC-SS and NC-GE. Each count is the peak latency of a particular HC-NC channel pair. Bottom rows: maps of peak latency between HC-SS and NC-GE for cortical channels. Circles indicate where GE-SS relationships are significant, while plus signs represent non-significant channels. The intensity of each circle corresponds to the strength of NC-GE to HC-SS coupling estimated (as in Fig. 3), and the color of each circle indicates peak latency: red for NC-GE before HC-SS, blue for NC-GE after HC-SS, and green for NC-GE co-occurring with HC-SS (i.e. within 50 ms of each other). Both hemispheres and medial and lateral cortical sites are superimposed in each plot. A. In N2, with aHC-SS as reference, NC-GE tend to occur in the following order: TB starts → DS peaks → SS starts → US peaks. B. In N3, with aHC SS as reference, NC-GE tend to occur in the following order: DS peaks → SS starts → US peaks. C. In N3, with pHC SS as reference, NC-GE tend to occur in the following order: TB starts → DS peaks → SS starts → US peaks. In A-C, NC-SS tend to co-occur with HC-SS, and widespread NC-US tend to follow. Download Figure 4-2, TIF file (21.8MB, tif)

Figure 4-3

HC-SSR to NC-GE associations are preserved when limiting HC-SSR to only the first SSR within each HC-SS. A. Event-related spectral power for first-in-SS HC-SSR (same patient source as Fig. 1D), with corresponding average LFP trace below. B. Top row: Histograms of peak latencies for HC-NC channel pairs with significant temporal correlations between first-in-SS HC-SSR and NC-GE. Each count is the peak latency of a particular HC-NC channel pair. Bottom row: maps of peak latency between first-in-SS HC-SSR and NC-GE for cortical channels. Circles indicate where GE-SSR relationships are significant, while plus signs represent non-significant channels. The intensity of each circle corresponds to the strength of GE-SSR coupling estimated (as in Fig. 3), and the color of each circle indicates peak latency: red for NC-GE before HC-SSR, blue for NC-GE after HC-SSR, and green for NC-GE co-occurring with HC-SSR (i.e. within 50 ms of each other). Both hemispheres and medial and lateral cortical sites are superimposed in each plot. Similar to Fig. 4A, in N2, with pHC-SSR as reference, NC-GE tend to occur in the following order: TB starts → DS peaks → SS starts → US peaks. Download Figure 4-3, TIF file (12.3MB, tif)

Figure 4-4

NC-GE synchronized with HC-SSR are not randomly distributed across sites. P-values for chi-square tests of homogeneity (α = 0.05 post FDR-correction applied over each column), conducted for each HC site with regard to each NC-GE type’s coupling to HC-SSR. Cells marked 2.20E-016 are placeholders for p < 2.2 x 10-16. # signif.: number of HC sites with a non-homogeneous anatomical distribution of significantly coupled NC sites. Out of 32 x 4 = 128 distributions of NC channels showing NC-GE to HC-SSR association in N2, 89 were significantly different from chance for HC-SSR in N2, compared to 46 in N3. The proportion of significant distributions was significantly greater in N2 (Fisher’s exact tests, p = 2.754 x 10-8). The percentages of pHC sites coupled to non-homogeneous NC site distributions (75-92% in N2, 42-67% in N3) were greater than the percentages of aHC sites (55-65% in N2, 15-35% in N3) for all NC-GE types. Download Figure 4-4, PDF file (320.4KB, pdf)

Figure 4-5

NC-GE synchronized with HC-SS are not randomly distributed across sites. P-values for chi-square tests of homogeneity (α = 0.05 post FDR-correction applied over each column), conducted for each HC site with regard to each NC-GE type’s coupling to HC-SS. Ant.: Ant-HC. Post: Post-HC. Cells marked 2.20E-016 are placeholders for p < 2.2 x 10-16. # signif.: number of HC sites with a non-homogeneous anatomical distribution of significantly coupled NC sites. Out of 32 x 4 = 128 distributions of NC channels showing NC-GE to HC-SS association in N2, 109 were significantly different from chance for HC-SSR in N2, compared to 62 in N3. The proportion of significant distributions was significantly greater in N2 (Fisher’s exact tests, p = 2.212 x 10-10). The pHC percentages (83-100% in N2, 42-67% in N3) and aHC percentages (70-95% in N2, 40-60% in N3) were both very high, consistent with a selective interaction with parietal areas. Download Figure 4-5, PDF file (321.4KB, pdf)

Figure 4-6

Coupled NC sites differ between simultaneously recorded aHC and pHC sites. Proportions of NC-GE from significantly coupled NC sites that overlapped with aHC-SSR/SS differ from the proportions for pHC-SSR/SS. Wilcoxon signed rank tests were performed on the data obtained for Figs. 4-4 and 4-5. Each test (whose p-value was tabulated above) required that both members of a HC site pair must have significant chi-square test result from Fig. 4-4 (for HC-SSR) or Fig. 4-5 (for HC-SS); otherwise, the corresponding cell would be filled with NA. # signif.: number of significant tests (in total number of qualified patients). Download Figure 4-6, PDF file (200.7KB, pdf)

Figure 6-1

2d projection maps of left hemisphere medial (A), left hemisphere lateral (B), right hemisphere medial (C), and right hemisphere lateral (D) NC channels showing significant spindle phase-locking with HC. Each blue circle marks a significant NC channel, with color intensity corresponding to peak PLV amplitude. Plus signs mark channels with no significant phase-locking. Download Figure 6-1, TIF file (10.7MB, tif)

Table 4-1

ROIs for statistical analyses of spatio-temporal differences across NC regions in HC-NC relationships. Download Table 4-1, PDF file (202.9KB, pdf)


Articles from The Journal of Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Society for Neuroscience

RESOURCES